ENHANCING LONG JUMP PERFORMANCE THROUGH ATTENTIONAL FOCUS A Thesis Presented to the faculty of the Department of Kinesiology and Health Science California State University, Sacramento Submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE in Kinesiology by Eric Ryan Fremd SPRING 2013 ENHANCING LONG JUMP PERFORMANCE THROUGH ATTENTIONAL FOCUS A Thesis by Eric Ryan Fremd Approved by: __________________________________, Committee Chair Dr. William H. Edwards __________________________________, Second Reader Dr. Michael Wright ____________________________ Date ii Student: Eric Ryan Fremd I certify that this student has met the requirements for format contained in the University format manual, and that this thesis is suitable for shelving in the Library and credit is to be awarded for the thesis. __________________________, Graduate Coordinator ___________________ Dr. Michael Wright Date Department of Kinesiology and Health Science iii Abstract of ENHANCING LONG JUMP PERFORMANCE THROUGH ATTENTIONAL FOCUS by Eric Ryan Fremd Statement of Problem Most track and field events have not received much attention by researchers investigating attentional focus. The long jump, being one of the track events that has been neglected in previous studies is in need of research to find which attentional focus produces the greatest performances (Partridge, Porter, & Wu 2012). Studies on attentional focus have mostly been done on skills that manipulate an object in some manner (Nolan, Porter, Ostrowski, & Wu, 2010). Conducting research on the long jump, a skill that requires no object manipulation, and is a whole body movement, will add new empirical evidence to the field of attentional focus. The findings could potentially be of great use to practitioners and coaches for instructing the long jump and possibly other track events as well (Nolan, Porter, Ostrowski, & Wu, 2010). Sources of Data An external focus of attention has been shown to produce greater results than an internal focus of attention. The Constrained Action Hypothesis explains that when a performer adopts an external focus of attention, a more automatic control process is used iv that leads to faster and more reflexive movements. Adopting an internal focus of attention will cause the natural automatic control processes to be interrupted by consciously attempting to control ones own movements causing decreases in the performance of movement (Shea & Wulf, 2001). Adopting an external focus of attention has been seen to induce various performance enhancements in a multitude of tasks when compared to an internal focus of attention. These include increased agility, jump height and distance, force production, muscular efficiency, and others. (Nolan, Ostrowski, porter, & Wulf, 2010; Dufek & Wulf, 2009; Dufek, Lozano, Pettigrew, & Wulf, 2010; Anton, Porter, & Wu, 2011). Research needed to investigate whether these performance enhancements will be seen in the long jump has not been reported. Adding attentional focus research findings to the coaching literature for track and field is needed (Partridge, Porter, Wu, 2010). Conclusions Reached Analysis revealed that there were no significant differences between participants’ performance when employing natural, internal, or external focus conditions. The nullhypotheses were accepted as showing no difference between day 1 and day 2 performances, as well as no differences between the external and internal focus of attention condition performances. _______________________, Committee Chair Dr. William H. Edwards _______________________ Date v ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to acknowledge my parents Tom & Sharon Fremd for all of their love and support throughout my life and through graduate school. With out the two of you none of this could have been possible. I would also like to acknowledge my fiancé Crystal for always being supportive and encouraging of all the goals that I am striving to achieve. Acknowledgement must also be paid to all of my friends, mentors, coaches, and professors for all you have done for me to help me reach this point. Lastly, I would like to acknowledge and thank Dr. Edwards for all of the guidance you have given me throughout Graduate school and throughout the process of completing this thesis. Thank you all. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Page Acknowledgments.................................................................................................................... V Chapter 1. INTRODUCTION ……………...……………………………………………………….. 1 Purpose of the Study ..................................................................................................... 1 Problem ......................................................................................................................... 4 Purpose ......................................................................................................................... 4 Hypothesis .................................................................................................................... 5 Delimitations ................................................................................................................. 5 Limitations .................................................................................................................... 5 Definitions of Terms ..................................................................................................... 6 2. REVIEW OF LITERATURE ............................................................................................. 7 Constrained Action Hypothesis ................................................................................... 8 Early Attentional Focus Research ................................................................................. 9 Agility Tasks ............................................................................................................... 12 Force Production ......................................................................................................... 14 Electromyography ....................................................................................................... 17 Peak Forces ................................................................................................................. 18 Distance Effects .......................................................................................................... 19 Expert and Elite Performers ........................................................................................ 23 3. METHODS ....................................................................................................................... 28 Participants................................................................................................................. 28 vii Instruments ................................................................................................................. 28 Procedures................................................................................................................... 29 Design & Analysis ...................................................................................................... 31 Results........................................................................................................................ 32 4. RESULTS ........................................................................................................................... 32 Analysis of Data.......................................................................................................... 32 5. DISCUSSION ...................................................................................................................... 35 Recommendations for Future Study ........................................................................... 39 Appendix A. Questionnaire .................................................................................................... 42 Appendix B. Track & Field Dynamic Warmup ...................................................................... 43 References ............................................................................................................................... 44 viii 1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION Purpose of the Study Instructional strategies have a strong impact on motor learning and the performance of skills (Munzert & Zentgraf, 2009). The type of instructions and how they are given influence the effectiveness of what is being taught. The retention, performance and transfer of the skills being learned are all affected by how the skills are being instructed (Hodges & Williams, 2005). In sport, being able to learn a skill quickly and correctly is of high importance. There is never a great amount of time that can be devoted to repeatedly teaching and learning a single skill. For coaches, simply using proper instructional strategies can improve their athletes’ performance and increase the efficiency of their training (Fathi, Sarmi, & Zarghami, 2012). Applying instructional strategies that have been found to be the most effective through research are not always applied by practitioners. In fact, research on skill acquisition has not been given as much support as other science fields by many coaches and practitioners (Ford & Williams, 2009). This reluctance to apply what researchers have found to be beneficial instructional strategies could reflect coaches’ unwillingness to accept what happens in a laboratory as transferring to real world performance implications (Ericsson & Williams, 2007). Without coaches and practitioners being up to date on what instructional strategies are the most efficient and useful, it is unlikely that their learners or athletes will be able to achieve their best performance. (Partridge, Porter, & Wu, 2010). 2 The instructions that elicit a learners focus of attention have a profound effect on the performance and retention of the motor skill (Wulf, 2007). There are various components on which athletes can focus. For instance, when a golf swing is being performed the learner could attend his or her focus to the swing of the club, the movement of the arms, the force put into the ground by the legs, the trajectory of the ball, the intended destination of the ball, or making contact with the ball. When the learners attend their focus to the appropriate components, their performance will be enhanced (Wulf, 2007). The opposite is also true. Certain instructions can elicit a focus that can lead to performance and learning decrements (Munzert & Zentgraf, 2009). In sport, it is seen as the coaches job to provide instructions that will most benefit their athletes’ performance or learning of a skill. Without knowledge and understanding of the most current scientific findings regarding instructions and the focus that they elicit, coaches can unknowingly cause decrements in their athletes’ performance or learning (Ford & Williams, 2009). Recently, motor learning research has looked at the effects that instructions have on a performers’ focus of attention. When performing a task, performers can either focus their attention internally or externally to themselves (Wulf, 2007, p. 2). The relatively new research that has been done in this area has found through multiple studies that there is a significant difference in various measures when a performer focuses his or her attention internally or externally. When instructions are aimed to induce an internal focus of attention, the performer will focus on his or her own bodies’ movements. These movements could be the coordination pattern of how to swing their arm, when to extend 3 the knee, or to extend at the wrist. Any instructions given that try to aim the performer’s attention toward his or her own body’s movement is considered to be an internal focus of attention. Instructions that aim to guide a performer’s focus of attention externally concentrate on environmental landmarks or on the effects of a movement. Examples of this type of focus are focusing on the path of a projectile, the inside of the rim of a basketball hoop, a spot on the ground, or a desired landing spot for a jump. As long as the performer’s attention is directed away from his or her body to the environment, or to a movement effect, it is considered an external focus of attention. An external focus of attention has recently become more accepted by many researchers, because it has been found through a number of studies to be beneficial for performance and learning. However, there are still researchers who believe that when learning a skill, an internal focus of attention is still the best way to maximize the learning of the skill (Wulf, 2007, p. 2). A skill that requires high levels of coordination and is considered a difficult motor skill to master is the long jump (Lee, Lishman, & Thomson, 1982). The long jump is a track and field event where the performer starts with a run up approach, and jumps from a designated take off location, with the goal of obtaining the greatest distance possible (Fowler, Lees, Grahmn-Smith, 1994). Most track and field coaching literature assumes that the technical instruction given to the athlete will be in an internal fashion. This may be due to an emphasis that track and field coaches have put on biomechanics. With an event like the long jump that requires a great amount of coordination at high rates of 4 speed, there should be more coaching literature available with recent research findings on the most efficient and beneficial motor learning techniques (Partridge, Porter, & Wu 2012). Problem Most track and field events (other than the discus) have not received much attention by researchers investigating attentional focus. The long jump, being one of the track events that has been neglected in previous studies is in need of research to find which attentional focus produces the greatest performances (Partridge, Porter, & Wu 2012). Studies on attentional focus have mostly been done on skills that manipulate an object in some manner (Nolan, Porter, Ostrowski, & Wu, 2010). Conducting research on the long jump, a skill that requires no object manipulation, and is a whole body movement, will add new empirical evidence to the field of attentional focus. The findings could potentially be of great use to practitioners and coaches for instructing the long jump and possibly other track events as well (Nolan, Porter, Ostrowski, & Wu, 2010). Purpose The purpose of this study was to examine whether an external focus of attention while performing a long jump would produce greater performances than an internal focus of attention. The study also examined whether a natural focus of attention would produce better results while performing a long jump than would an external or internal focus of attention would. 5 Hypothesis There will be no significant difference in the participants performance of the long jump between the natural, external and internal focus of attention conditions. Delimitations The delimiting factors for this study were that athletes had to be collegiate athletes (junior college or university), have had a minimum of 2 years of competitive experience, and had no existing injuries. The study taking place over 2 consecutive days could potentially affect the results of the study. Specialized footwear was not provided to the participants, which could have had an affect on performance. This study had a small sample size due to the long jump being an event that does not typically see a lot of participants when compared to other events like the sprints. Finding a large amount of willing participants in the greater Sacramento area was not possible. The number of participants was 4. The use of a measuring tape was also a delimiting factor because of the chance of human error while measuring the jump distances. Limitations A limiting factor for this study was that it was conducted outside, in an uncontrolled environment. The weather could have affected the participants’ performances from trial to trial in negative or positive ways. Not being able to supervise what the participants did outside of the study was a limiting factor. Any physical activity outside of the study could not be accounted for. Nutrition and sleep were not accounted for, which could have affected the performances of the participants. The participants motivation while participating in the study may have also been a limiting factor. 6 Definitions of Terms Attentional focus: The focal point of an individuals concentration of a specific aspect of his or her movements or of the environment External focus of attention: Attending ones focus to features in the environment or a movement effect. Internal focus of attention: Attending one’s focus to his or her own body during a movement Long Jump: A track and field event where the performer starts with a run up approach, and jumps from a designated take off location, with the goal of obtaining the greatest distance possible Constrained Action Hypothesis: Focusing on the movements of ones body causes conscious disruption of natural control processes that would normally regulate movements effectively and efficiently. When an individual focuses on the movement effect a more automatic type of control is used. This allows reflexive and unconscious processes to take control of the body’s movements to a greater degree. Focusing on the movement effect results in greater performance and learning. 7 Chapter 2 REVIEW OF LITERATURE When learning a motor skill there are three stages of learning that take place, as proposed by Fitts (1964) and Fitts and Posner (1967). In the first or “cognitive” stage of learning, a good amount of thought goes into performing the task. The learner will tend to try various movement strategies, attempting to find the one that will bring him or her the most success. The cognitive effort that the learner typically puts into performing the task is typically of the step-by-step process of how to physically perform the task. For complex skills, it has been seen that the learner spends a great amount of time in this learning stage, because the performance of the movements are typically uncoordinated, slow, and of great variance. (Wulf, 2007, p.3) After the learner has developed an essential movement strategy, and has become more consistent with performing the task, he or she is considered to be in the second or “associative” stage of learning. Learners in this stage will devote less attention to the step-by-step actions of the skill, and will have some components of the skill automatically controlled. In order to move into the next and final stage of learning, the autonomous stage, the learner has to have had a great amount of practice. When a learner reaches this stage, his or her movements are almost exclusively automatic, requiring very little conscious attention. The learner will show greater coordination in the movement and be much more efficient in executing the task (Wulf, 2007, p. 4). These three stages of learning are seen in the theory of explaining why an internal or external focus of attention can have a significant effect on acquisition and performance of a skill 8 Constrained Action Hypothesis The constrained action hypothesis, proposed by Wulf, McNevin, and Shea (2001), aims to explain the differences between the two attentional foci. The theory proposes that when an individual directs his or her focus inward, toward his or her movements, it tends to interrupt natural processes that control and regulate the coordination of movements. By consciously attempting to control their movements to meet the desired movement outcome, learners disrupt the automatic processes that would otherwise control their movements with greater success and efficiency. In comparison, when attention is directed outward toward the environment or to a movement‘s effect, a more automatic control process allows faster and more reflexive control of a movement. As a byproduct, performance and learning are increased (McNevin, Shea, & Wulf 2001). According to the constrained action hypothesis, when a learner adopts an internal focus, their performance and learning ability resemble that of a learner who is more in the cognitive stage than any other stage of learning. On the other hand, when an external focus of attention is adopted, the learner’s performance is more automatic, resembling the associative, or autonomous stages of learning (Wulf, 2007, p. 114). The higher movement production efficiency, and lower muscular energy expenditure that is seen in the autonomous stage of learning, has also been found in studies for beginning learners who adopt an external focus of attention (Wulf, 2007, p. 4 & 116). 9 Early Attentional Focus Research The significance of attentional focus was first seen in a study done by Wulf, Hob, and Prinz (1998) that involved the use of a ski simulator. The objective of this study was to make the largest oscillatory movements possible and to move as fast as possible. During this study the researchers gave one group of participants the instructions to “try and push the platform with the outer foot and focus on that foot”. The second group was instructed to “focus on the force they exert upon the wheels (which were directly under their feet). The first group was deemed the “internal” group, and the second group was deemed the “external group (Hob, Prinz, & Wulf, 1998). A third group was given no instructions, which was used as the control group. The researchers reasoning was that because of the minuet difference in the focus of attention, if there was any benefits in performance and retention of the motor skill for the external focus of attention, the effect will be a robust one. Participants performed 8 trials of 90seconds for two days in a row. After a day’s rest a retention test was given, where the researches’ did not give any instructions on where to focus attention, as was done on the previous days to elicit specific attentional foci. For the very first 90 second trial of day 1, all of the participants showed similar performances. Toward the end of day one and through the end of day 2, the external focus group demonstrated higher improvements in performance compared to the internal focus group and the control group. The average amplitudes of movement of the ski simulator for the external group after day 1 was 12cm greater than the internal group, and 6cm greater than the control group. The retention test showed that the participants who 10 had previously been instructed to adopt an external focus again outperformed the internal and control groups. They yielded amplitudes 7cm greater than the internal group, and 6cm greater than the control group. These experimental results effectively demonstrated that the external groups performance and learning of the skill were improved when compared to the other two groups. One of the interesting findings from this study was the similarity of the retention test scores of the internal focus group and the control group. Although both were less efficient when compared to the external groups retention scores, an internal focus was not significantly less efficient than the control groups scores. This would suggest that instructions that elicit an internal focus of attention produce the same learning effect of a skill than no attentional instructions would (Wulf, 2007, p. 39). A follow up study was done by Wulf and Shea (1999). Their main purpose for conducting the follow up study was to find out if the results that were found in the ski simulator study could be replicated in a different task. This study used a stabilometer task, where the participants objective was to maintain balance and keep the platform as horizontal as possible. The study took place over 3 days, with the first 2 days used as practice days, and the third day used as a retention test day. Four groups were involved in the study. Two of the four groups received either internal or external focus instructions. The internal group was instructed to focus on keeping their feet horizontal, while the external group was instructed to focus on keeping two markers on the platform just in front of their feet horizontal. The other two groups received the same internal or external instructions, but also received concurrent feedback on a computer screen of their performance during each trial. The internal-feedback group was told that the pink line on 11 the monitor should be thought of as their feet. The external-feedback group was told the lines represented the markers in front of their feet on the platform surface. The results of this follow up study were in agreement with the first ski simulator study. Learning was more effective when the participants were instructed to have an external focus of attention compared to an internal focus of attention. It also suggests that when the performers attention is focused on something more distal to themselves, the learning effect will be enhanced to a greater degree than a more proximal external focus. In another experiment, Wulf and two of her colleagues, investigated whether or not the results that were found in the previous studies would transfer to sports skills (Lauterbach, Toole, Wulf, 1999). They studied participants who had no previous experience with golfing. Participants attempted to hit a target with a 90cm diameter that was 15m away. Around the target there were four concentric circles which were used to give scores on concerning the accuracy of each shot. Before the participants were allowed to start their trials, they were given 10 minutes of basic instructions and demonstrations on how to stand and how to grip the golf club. During this time they were also allowed to practice swinging the club. After the initial practice time, one group of participants was instructed to focus on the swing of their arms (internal), and the other group was instructed to focus on the swing of the golf club (external). The results showed that even from the initiation of the trials, the external instructions group demonstrated greater accuracy compared to the internal instructions group. The internal group did show improvement from the beginning to the end to the practice trials, but showed less accuracy than the external group. This trend continued to the retention test as well which 12 indicates that the external focus group was able to perform and learn the skill more effectively than the internal focus group. The findings of this preliminary study of attentional focus used for sports skills showed that minor adjustments in instructional wording to induce an external focus do indeed enhance the learning and performance of sports skills in addition novel balance tasks (Wulf, 2007, p. 44). Agility tasks In a more recent study, attentional focus was again studied for the benefits of adopting an external focus, but this time agility performance was tested. The study was conducted by several prominent researchers in the field of attentional focus (Nolan, Ostrowski, Porter, & Wulf, 2010). They used an agility “L run” to investigate if the advantages of an external focus would be seen in agility tasks. Twenty participants with no formal sprint training had to sprint through the “L” course consisting of two 5m sections making a right angle. The participants performed one condition per day over a 3 day span. The instructions that all groups received were to “run through he course as quickly as you can with maximum effort”. The external and internal groups received additional instructions that the control group did not. The internal group was told to “focus on moving their legs as rapidly as possible. For the turn, focus on planting their foot as firmly as possible”. The external groups additional instructions were to “focus on running to the cone as rapidly as possible. For the turn, focus on pushing off the ground as forcefully as possible”. 13 The results of the study showed that the movement times of the external condition were significantly faster than that of the other two groups. The control condition and the internal condition did not show any significant difference between their performances. In addition to the performance results, the researchers had the participants answer questionnaires after each trial for each condition inquiring what the participants actually focused on while performing the task. This was done in order to check if they used the focus they were instructed to use or not. The results of the manipulation check showed that when the participants were in the control condition they focused externally 13% of the time, internally 10% of the time, and “other 77% of the time. While in the internal condition the participants indicated that the focused internally 76% of the time, externally 1%, and “other” 23% of the time. Under the external condition participants focused externally 67% of the time, internally 2%, and “other 31% of the time. These questionnaire results indicated that the instructions given induced the desired attentional responses for a majority of the trials for the external and internal conditions. The control conditions instructions were intended to induce a neutral attentional focus. Under the control condition instructions the participants focused their attention elsewhere 77% of the time. Using the data from the manipulation check, the researchers concluded that the instructions that were given produced the desired attentional focus. This further validates the advantages of using instructions that induce an external focus of attention to produce greater performances, demonstrating that the instructions were the actual cause of the increased performances. 14 Force Production Through multiple studies finding showing that an external focus of attention produces increased movement efficiency, it has been theorized that maximal force production would be increased as well (Dufek, Granados, Wulf, & Zachry, 2007). Under this premise. Dufek et al. (2007) conducted a study to investigate whether an external focus of attention increases vertical jump height compared to an internal focus of attention. The vertical jump was chosen because it requires the production of maximal forces to propel the body off the ground (Dufek & Wulf, 2009). The results of the study showed that the participants who adopted an external focus of attention had both increased jump height and vertical center-of-mass displacement compared to participants in the internal condition. These results indicated that an external focus of attention could cause increased force production and performances. A follow up study was done by Dufek and Wulf (2009), to further investigate the causes of the differences in jump height between the external and internal focus conditions. The researchers had 10 physically active men and women perform maximal vertical jumps, using a vertec measurement device. To record and measure the participants vertical ground reaction forces, they performed the jumps on a force platform. A motion capture automated tracking system with reflective markers that were placed on the participants lower body was used to capture the kinematic data of the vertical jumps. The participants were instructed to jump straight up and touch the highest vertec rung they could with the tips of their fingers. Each of the 10 participants performed 10 jumps under the external and internal condition. The researchers’ instructions for the 15 participants while under the internal condition were to “concentrate on the tips of their fingers, reaching as high as possible during the jumps”. The instructions while under the external condition were to “concentrate on the rungs of the vertec, reaching as high as possible”. Between each of the trials the researchers gave the participants several instructional reminders to keep their focus on the intended attentional condition. The results of the study showed that the external focus condition produced greater heights and greater center of mass displacement than did the internal focus condition. The impulses created under the external condition were on average 21.5 Ns greater than the internal condition. The joint moments were also higher overall under the external condition. These findings provide the mechanical reasons for difference in performance between the external and internal groups. The researchers stated that possible reasons for the external conditions greater force production could be from a more effective coordination pattern between the agonist and antagonist muscle groups. Without inappropriate co-contraction, the results would be greater maximal force production and greater heights jumped. The opposite of this would be true for the internal condition. Inappropriate co-contraction and inefficient movement efficiency would decrease the force produced and the height jumped (Dufek & Wulf, 2009). Also based of a study by Vanezis and Lees (2005) the researchers of this study concluded that in their study the participants performances while under the external focus closely resembled that of a skilled jumper, and the opposite for the internal condition performances. The researchers suggested that this was due to the appropriate coordination within and between the muscles producing the movement. 16 While these studies showed that an external focus of attention lead to higher vertical jump performances, which is a whole body movement that requires maximum force production, Porter et al. (2010) found it necessary to research a horizontal whole body movement requiring maximum force production. They decided to use a standing long jump, which has frequently been used to test and predict athletic performance. The main reason they chose to do a study using the standing long jump was because it is a whole body movement that does not involve the manipulation of an object, which had not been done in research on attentional focus (Nolan, Porter, Ostrowski, & Wu, 2010). The researchers recruited participants who had never received any formal jump training to participate in the study. The 120 participants were split in half into either an internal condition group or an external condition group. Each participant completed 5 jump trials under their condition. The instructions that the internal group received were “when you are attempting to jump as far as possible, I want you to focus your attention on extending your knees as rapidly as possible”. The external conditions groups instructions were “When you are attempting to jump as far as possible, I want you to focus your attention on jumping as far past the start line as possible”. The results of this study showed that the external condition, which had instructions that focused the participants attention to an external part of the environment, produced significantly farther jump distances than the internal condition whose instructions directing the participants’ attention inward to their bodies movements. The external condition group produced jumps on average that were 10.05cm further than the internal condition group produced. The researchers felt that results of their study on a 17 whole body movement add to the generalizability of using an external focus of attention on skills to increase performance. It also shows that coaches and practitioners should pay attention to the wording of their instructions to make sure that they are helping to elicit an external focus of attention. If they accomplish this, they will increase the probability of improving their athletes physical abilities. Electromyography The benefits of an external focus of attention over an internal focus of attention have shown to produce greater performance results over a multitude of studies. The underlying mechanisms that have been proposed to cause the seen advantages of an external focus of attention have been gaining validity through EMG testing. Higher efficiency of muscular contractions has been one of the theorized mechanisms causing the seen advantages of an external focus of attention. Research on an isometric plantar flexion has supported this theory. An external focus, causes lower EMG activity in the antagonist muscle by reducing co-contraction activity (Healy, Lohse, & Sherwood, 2011). This reduction of activity in the antagonist muscle group allows for an efficient restriction free contraction. An internal focus of attention has been shown to cause a significantly greater amount of activity in the antagonist muscle group. This decreases the efficiency of the movement and lowers performance outcomes (Healy, Lohse, & Sherwood, 2011). Small alterations in instructions, have a profound effect on the efficiency and performance of a movement. Giving instructions that induce an internal focus of attention like telling an individual to focus on their leg muscles to produce force, 18 instead of telling them to push against the platform will greatly decrease efficiency and subsequently performance of a movement (Healy, Lohse, & Sherwood, 2011). It has also been seen through research that non-isometric and explosive movements show more efficient EMG activity in the limbs causing movement. A follow up study investigating attentional focus used for jumping for height, gave instructions with a minor adjustment. Participants in the internal condition were told to focus on their finger tips when jumping and reaching for the height markers. The external group had the slight alteration of being instructed to focus on the height markers. According to Dufek, Lozano, Pettigrew, and Wulf (2010), small alterations in instructions that will cause an external focus of attention instead of an internal focus will lead to greater efficiency and performance. The cause of the greater efficiency is seen through lower EMG activity in the active muscles of a movement. Expert performers have been shown to have more efficient muscular activity than do non-expert performers. The opposite is true for adopting an internal focus of attention (Healy, Lohse, & Sherwood, 2011). Peak Forces Another popular theory of why an external focus of attention produces greater performance results is that it allows the individual to produce higher peak forces. With the use of a force platform researchers studied the peak forces along with the distances achieved during a standing long jump using both internal and external focus conditions (Brown, Porter, 7 Wu, 2011). As seen in previous studies the external focus condition outperformed the internal condition, but what other studies and researchers theorized about force production being greater for the external group was not found to be 19 significant. These findings lend additional support to the findings in the study done by Dufek et al. (2010), that the increased performance of the external focus of attention is due to a more efficient muscle contraction pattern. Aside from this mechanism of increased performance, Brown et al. (2011) proposed other plausible mechanisms for the increased performances. The increased performances of the external condition could be due to a more efficient timing component which allowed the force application to be maximized compared to the internal conditions force application timing. Another plausible reason for the external conditions increased performance could be that the external instructions, which focused the participants attention to an environmental benchmark, allowed for a more effective projectile angle (Brown, Porter, & Wu, 2011). The researchers responsible for the study recognized that further study is needed to validate their theories for the possible mechanisms of the increased performance of the external condition. Their findings do, however add to the empirical and scientific evidence of the advantageous effects an external focus of attention on the performance of a wide variety of motor skills (Brown, Porter, 7 Wu, 2011). Distance Effects The advantageous effects of adopting an external focus of attention has also been seen to have an even greater effect on performance as the external focus of attention moves further away from the performers body (McNevin et al., 2003). The proposed cause of the increases in performance and learning are seen with more distal external focus condition is that the automacity of the movement is progressed to a greater degree (McNevin et al., 2003). The distance effects of attentional focus have not been researched 20 as extensivly as internal versus external focus of attention has, but a fair amount of generalizability has been seen within the studies that have researched the phenomenon (McKay & Wulf, 2012). The tasks that have been used to research the distance effects have been a stabilometer task, golf, and dart throwing, all of which showed consistent results that the more distal the external focus of attention is, the greater the performance and learning results (McKay & Wulf, 2012). An interesting thought among some researchers and practitioners is that learners who do not prefer an external focus of attention might not see the increased benefits in performance and retention of a skill as do learners preferring an external focus (Wulf, 2007, p. 71). This problem has sparked studies to investigate whether there is a difference in preference of focus of attention for performance and learning. In a study by Wulf et al. (2001) participants were given the option to practice, and then chose between an internal and external focus of attention. They were told to either focus on keeping their feet horizontal to the ground, or to keep markers on the platform horizontal to the ground. Over half of the participants who initially chose to focus internally, had changed their preference to the external focus by the retention test. Those participants who had remained with the internal focus were outperformed by all of the participants who adopted the external focus of attention (Park, Shea, & Wulf, 2001). Focusing attention on a more distal effect rather than proximal has also been shown to produce greater performance and learning results (Bell & Hardy, 2009; (McNevin, Shea, & Wulf, 2003). A learner’s preference of a more distal or proximal external focus of attention has also been shown to not be significant. McKay and Wulf 21 (2012) performed a study to look into whether this was true. They had participants throw darts under a proximal and distal focus to allow them to choose which focus they preferred. Once they chose their preference, they had to throw under each condition. What was found was that most participants preferred the distal effect to the proximal focus from the start. Under the distal focus they preformed significantly higher than under the proximal focus. These results showed that preference again did not matter for performance. The greater distal focus condition produced greater performances than the more proximal focus, even when the participant preferred the more proximal focus (McKay & Wulf, 2012). A possible mechanism for the increased performance of the distal focus could come from a more global and effective movement pattern. Focusing on the trajectory of the dart could have caused performers’ attention to the details of their movements more than focusing on the bulls-eye of the dart board. The findings of this particular study and skill might transfer to more complex skills as well. Beginners who are learning a complex skill with various degrees of freedom might benefit from a more proximal external focus of attention, because this focus might allow the performer to relate the movement more closely to the movement patterns that control the movement. Expert performers of advanced and complex skills, however, could benefit from a more distal focus of attention because they already have the motor pattern developed and only need to evoke the proper action (McKay & Wulf, 2012). As it has been observed, these skills that all have required the manipulation of an object in some manner, all benefit from a more distal external focus. They were all skills 22 that differed in movement pattern and showed a good amount of generalizability. What these skills did not show, however, was whether or not a more distal external focus of attention over a more proximal focus will show the same advantageous results for a power based movement with no object manipulation (Anton, Porter, & Wu, 2011). To test if the advantages would transfer to a power based movement with no object manipulation, a standing long jump was used in a recent study. The instructions used to elicit the distal focus condition were to focus attention on a target that was 3m away from the participant. A cue guiding the participant’s focus to the ground near their feet was used to elicit the proximal focus condition. It was found that both external conditions outperformed the control condition, which was only given general instructions designed to elicit no specific focus of attention. The distal external focus also resulted in significantly further jump distances than the proximal external focus condition (Anton, Porter, & Wu, 2011). This shows than a more distal external focus of attention is superior to a more proximal focus and a neutral focus of attention for the performance of a power based non-object manipulating skill. Possible underlying reasons for why the proximal focus performed to a lower degree compared to the external focus, is that the more proximal a performers focus is, the more it will cause performers to take a greater active control over the action, which will constrain the movement efficiency (McNevin, Shea, & Wulf, 2003). 23 Expert and Elite Performers Most of the studies that have been done on attentional focus have used novice or beginner level participants with respect to the skill that was being studied. For a majority of these studies with the novice participants, an external focus of attention showed to be the most beneficial form of attentional focus. There have been a few studies that have used beginner and expert performers. In a study reported by Su and Wulf (2007) beginner and expert golfers both showed enhanced performance when adopting an external focus of attention. The expert performers showing enhanced performance is the more profound finding of this study for a few reasons. It is thought that an expert at a specific skill will have already developed an automated motor program for that skill (Wulf, 2008). By focusing on anything involved with the movement, even if external, it could interfere with the preexisting automated movement of the expert performer. Contradictory to that theory, for the skilled performers in the Su and Wulf (2007) study performance was enhanced for the expert performers when using an external focus. What these findings show, is that high level performers can still see the advantageous effects of using an external focus of attention. The participants that were used in the previous study were considered experts, but not quite elite performers. To enhance the research on focus of attention and expert performers Gabriele Wulf (2008) used a balance task with performers from the Cirque du Soleil show. These performers are considered to have elite levels of balance, which they display during their performances by executing complex and very challenging balance tasks under pressure. The task that Wulf had them perform was from a previous study 24 that she did with novice performers (Shea et al., 2007). The previous study’s results showed that an external focus of attention produced greater performance and learning than the internal or control condition. In both study’s the participants were given instructions to focus on minimizing movements of their feet for the internal condition, or minimize movements of the disk for the external condition. The control condition was not given any specific instructions, to allow the participants to adopt their own focus. Given that the elite balance performers already had high degrees of automacity for very complex balance tasks, an improvement through the use of an external focus of attention would not be expected (Wulf, 2008). This expectation was found to be true. When the acrobats were allowed to use their own focus, without any outside instructions, they showed a greater frequency of postural adjustment. A greater frequency is typically seen in higher degrees of automacity within the performance of a skill. Under the external and internal conditions the participants did not show any differences in postural sway as when under the control condition, but this is a less significant measure compared to the frequency of postural adjustment. Instructing the participants that had elite levels of balance skills to focus on an internal or external focus of attention resulted in a degradation of performance when compared to their own focus. This degradation of performance is an effect of reverting to a more active postural control (Wulf, 2008). When the elite performers were allowed to focus under the condition that they were accustomed to and that had become an automated motor program they were able to dissipate disturbances in balance more rapidly and efficiently. What this study demonstrated is that when a performer has 25 reached an elite skill level, and a task’s directions are below their current control level a performance, degradation will occur (Wulf, 2008). For elite performers, instructions should be aimed at movement effects that are currently higher than what they have mastered. Doing this should avoid performance degradations and possibly enhance performance even further (Wulf, 2008). Coaching or working with elite athletes is a rarity when compared to working with good or above average athletes. Since most coaches and practitioners will not be coaching or working with elite athletes it is important to know if the results that were seen with the elite balancers are also seen with experts of a task that is not quite elite? Stoate and Wulf (2011) decided to investigate this with a study of expert swimmers. They wanted investigate whether swimmers who have put in years of practice would perform better under their own normal conditions or an external or internal condition. A benefit of this study in comparison to other reported studies is that the task used was the same task that would be used in a sports setting (Stoate & Wulf, 2011). The instructional differences were, again like most studies, very minuet to show the significance of the performance differences. The external group was instructed to “focus on pushing the water back” and the internal group was instructed to “focus on pulling your hands back”. It was found that the difference in performance between the control condition and the external condition was insignificant. However, both conditions were significantly faster than the performances of the internal group. It seems that these results show that in expert but non-elite performers, there are no decrements in performance when adopting an external focus of attention, but there is no significant increase in performance either. 26 This could be due to the movement already being so highly automated and efficient that an increase in performance will not occur (Stoate & Wulf, 2011). An interesting finding through the use of a manipulation check questionnaire was that when the control group responded to what they were attending to, a majority of their replies had an external focus characteristic. This could be an indication of why the external and control groups performances did not show a significant difference between them (Stoate & Wulf, 2011). The evidence of an external focus of attention enhancing motor skill performance for a variety of tasks is becoming greater in number. The evidence for it being the best focus of attention for experts and elite athletes is inconsistent. There also seems to be a disconnect between what has been found in research and what is being used by coaches and practitioners. Despite a majority of the research findings on attentional focus, a recent study of elite track and field athletes indicated that a vast majority of their instruction from coaches is aimed at an internal rather than external focus (Partridge, Porter, & Wu, 2012). The study also showed that a majority of the cues during competition that they receive and think about are of an internal focus of attention. What this shows is that there is a need for more studies on actual sport events or tasks, so that greater empirical evidence is available for coaches to see how attentional focus directly affects the event they are coaching. Track and field specifically could benefit greatly from further research on attentional focus over the different events in the sport. Most of the coaching literature and education programs for track and field have vast amounts of information on biomechanics and the physical training for the different events, but lacks in motor 27 learning information (Partridge, Porter, Wu, 2012). Further research needs to be done on the different events, as well as different skill levels in track and field. This will allow coaches to make the best decision for what type of instructions to give their athletes to allow for the greatest performances possible. 28 Chapter 3 METHODS Participants The participants in this study were 5 junior college male long jumpers ranging in age from 17 to 22 years attending Sacramento City College. To be included in the study, the participants had to have a minimum of two years of competitive long jumping experience, and to have had at least one semester’s worth of collegiate training. The participants were considered experts based on their training and experience. However, the participants were not considered elite performers. The participants were recruited for this study by the researcher. A general explanation of the task was given to the participant, but the exact purpose of the study was left unknown. If a participant was interested in volunteering for the study, he was provided an informed consent form explaining the study in more detail. The participant’s identity was protected by having a number assigned to each participant that only the researcher knew and keep in a secure location. Instruments The instruments that were used for this study were a long jump runway with an all weather surface, a long jump board, a sand pit, and a tape measure to measure the long jump distance. A printed spreadsheet was used to record the data onsite, which was transferred to a computer after the data had been collected. A questionnaire was used to determine what attentional focus was inherently used by the participants (see appendix). 29 Procedures The study took place in Hughes Stadium at Sacramento City College prior to the beginning of the competitive track and field season. The study spanned 2 days. Day 1 consisted of all the participants taking 4 long jumps from a 12 step approach without being given any instructions that would sensitize them to a specific attentional focus. The instructions that the participants received on day 1 were “You will be performing a full long jump while attempting to jump the greatest distance possible”. Prior to the beginning of the trials, the participants were put through the same warm up. The warm up was a typical warm-up sequence typically done by track and field athletes. The exact warm-up is located in the appendix. Participants were also given time after the warm up to practice their approach run. They were allowed no more than 3 practice attempts. Once all warm-up and practice time ended, the jumpers were given a 5-minute rest period to recover. During the 5-minute rest period, the participants were given the instructional statement from above. In addition to those instructions the participants were also instructed to refrain from talking to the other participants while the trials were taking place to have them avoid discussing their performance and foci. This prevented the athletes from being able to discuss what they were thinking about before and during their performance. This allowed for the participant’s natural focus of attention to be used without any outside influences. The jumpers were also given the order in which they would be jumping. Each participant was given a 1-minute period to begin their jump after the researcher indicated it was all clear to jump. This is the same amount of time that jumpers are allotted during competitions to initiate their jump. Once the participant 30 completed his jump and exited the sand pit, the researcher measured the distance from the front of the take off board and the furthest back marking in the sand made by the participant. The distance was recorded under that participant’s assigned number. On day 2 of the study the participants were put through the same warm-up and practice procedures that took place on day1. The participants were again given the same initial instructions, as well as the instructions to refrain from talking to one another. The measuring and recording of data was also the same for day 2 of the study. The difference from day 1 to day 2 was that the participants were split in half to make 2 groups. Group 1 was given instructions that invoked an internal focus of attention for their first 2 trials, and group 2 was given instructions that invoked an external focus of attention for their first 2 trials. For the second 2 trials, group 1 was given instructions that invoked an external focus of attention, and group 2 was given instructions that invoked an internal focus of attention. These instructions were given individually to each participant directly prior to each trial. The instructions for the internal focus of attention were “As you make contact with the board, focus on forcefully pushing off with your leg”. The instructions for the external focus of attention were “As you make contact with the board, focus on forcefully pushing off of the board”. The instructions that were given to each participant were kept unknown to the other participants at all times. After the trials were complete for day 1 a questionnaire was given to the participants to determine what they paid attention to before, during, and after their performances. This questionnaire was used to show what focus of attention the 31 participants naturally used without outside instructions. A copy of the questionnaire is located in the appendix. Design & Analysis Independent t-tests were used as the statistical analysis for the study. An independent t-test was performed for the day 1 results for group 1 against their results from day 2 under both instructional conditions. Another independent t-test was performed in the same way for group 2. These analyses compared the performances of the participants natural focus of attention with their performances under an internal and external focus of attention. A third independent t-test was performed for all of the external focus of attention jumps versus all of the internal focus of attention jumps from day 2. This was used to determine whether an internal or external focus of attention produced greater performances overall. The confidence level of the analysis was set at 0.1. 32 Chapter 4 RESULTS Recent research has demonstrated the benefits of an external focus of attention on the performance of a number of different sport and motor skills. No research to date however, has investigated the possible benefits of an external attentional focus by athletes performing a long jump. This study investigated the comparative effects of internal and external attentional focus when performing the long lump. Five Junior College long jumpers were given 4 jumps on two consecutive days. On the first day no instructions were given to the participants. The second day the participants were divided into two groups and given instructions that provided either an internal focus of attention condition or an external focus of attention condition for half of their jumps. The groups received their instructions in reverse orders. The purpose of the study was to examine whether an external focus of attention while performing a long jump will produce greater performances than an internal focus of attention, or whether a natural focus of attention would produce better results while performing a long jump than would an external or internal focus of attention. Analysis of Data A paired t-test was performed for the jumps from day 1 versus the internal and external jumps from day 2 for Group 1. The mean distance for Group 1’s jumps from day 1 was 5.7037m. On day 2 of the study, Group 1 jumped under the internal focus condition first, and the external focus condition second. The mean distance for the internal focus of attention condition on day 2 was 5.7875m. The mean distance for the 33 external focus of attention condition from day 2 was 5.73m. The mean distance jumped for both attentional foci from day 2 were greater than the natural focus of the participants from day 1 for group 1 (see Table 1.1). Table 1.1 Group 1 t-test value P-Value Mean of Day 1 / Mean of Day 2 SD of Day 1 / SD of Day 2 Day 1 vs. Day 2 INT -0.1922 0.5715 5.7037 / 5.7875 0.6985 / 0.7183 Day 1 vs. Day 2 EXT -0.0626 0.5234 5.7037 / 5.73 0.6985 / 0.6776 A comparison of the day 1 and day 2 mean average jumping scores for group 1 for the external attentional focus condition resulted in acceptance of the null-hypothesis of no difference between day 1 and day 2 performance (t= -0.0626, p= 0.5234). The same comparison, but with the internal attentional focus condition also resulted in acceptance of the null-hypothesis of no difference between day 1 and day 2 performances (t= 0.1922, p=0.5715) The jumps from Group 2 were also tested in the same manor as Group 1. The mean distance for Group 2’s jumps from day 1 was 5.4408m. On day 2 of the study Group 2 jumped under the external focus condition first, and the internal focus condition second. The mean distance for the internal focus of attention condition on day 2 was 5.4483m. The mean distance for the external focus of attention condition from day 2 was 5.5083m (see table 1.2). Table 1.2 34 Group 2 t-test value P-Value Mean of Day 1 / Mean of Day 2 SD of Day 1 / SD of Day 2 Day 1 vs. Day 2 EXT -0.4797 0.6717 5.4408 / 5.5083 0.3048 / 0.269 Day 1 vs.Day 2 INT -0.0401 0.515 5.4408 / 5.4483 0.3048 / 0.4047 A comparison of the day 1 and day 2 mean average jumping scores for group 2 for the external attentional focus condition resulted in an acceptance of the nullhypothesis of no difference between day 1 and day 2 performance (t= -0.4797, p= 0.6717). The same comparison, but with the internal attentional focus condition also resulted in the acceptance of the null-hypothesis of no difference between day 1 and day 2 performances (t= -0.0401, p=0.5150). Another t-test was performed comparing all of the external focus of attention condition jumps versus all of the internal focus of attention condition jumps. The mean value for the external condition jumps was 5.597m, and the mean value for the internal condition jumps was 5.6194m (see Table 1.3). The comparison between the internal and external focus of attention condition mean average-jumping scores resulted in an acceptance of the null-hypothesis of no difference between the two conditions (t= -0.0951, p= 0.5355). Table 1.3 EXT vs. INT t-test value P-Value Mean of EXT/ Mean of INT SD of EXT / SD for INT All EXT vs. All INT -0.0951 0.5355 5.597 / 5.6194 0.4542 / 0.5621 35 Chapter 5 DISCUSSION Numerous studies have demonstrated the benefits of employing an external focus of attention for performing a variety of tasks and sports skills. However, no previous study has investigated the use of an external focus of attention when performing the long jump. The purpose of the present study, therefore, was to examine whether an external focus of attention while performing a long jump would produce greater performances than would an internal focus of attention. The null-hypothesis of no difference was tested to compare differences between subjects performing the long jump employing either an external or an internal focus of attention condition. To investigate which attentional focus condition would produce greater performances, junior college long jumpers were given four 12-step approach long jumps on two consecutive days. On the first day the participants were given no instructions that would elicit a specific attentional focus. This served as the baseline test. On the 2nd day of the study, the participants were given instructions for two of their jumps that would elicit an internal focus of attention, which were “As you make contact with the board, focus on forcefully pushing off with your leg”. For their other 2 jumps, the participants were given instructions that would elicit an external focus of attention, which were “As you make contact with the board, focus on forcefully pushing off of the board”. The participants randomly divided into two groups. Group 1 received Internal focus instructions first and external instructions second on day 2. Group 2 was given external and then internal focus of attention instructions on day 2. 36 To analyze the results of the study, t-tests were calculated for the day 1 results versus the day 2 external and internal focus condition results for both groups. Another ttest was calculated for all internal focus jumps versus all external focus of attention jumps. There were no significant differences between participants’ performance when employing natural, internal, or external focus conditions. The null-hypothesis was accepted as showing no difference between day 1 and day 2 performances, as well as no differences between the external and internal focus of attention condition performances. Even though no significant differences were found, potentially meaningful trends were observed. The mean of the jumps from day 2 for both groups and for both attentional foci conditions were greater than means for the jumps from day 1 of the study. Another trend that was observed was that for the first 2 jumps of day 2, no matter which of the attentional focus conditions was employed, performance was greater than the second two jumps taken on day 2. The data also revealed that the mean of all of the internal focus of attention condition jumps was slightly greater than the mean from all of the external focus of attention condition jumps. However, the differences between the means throughout all categories were statistically insignificant. The results of this study are inconsistent with the majority of findings from previous research on attentional focus. An external focus of attention has been shown to allow participants to produce higher amounts of force, as well as greater efficiency (Dufek & Wulf, 2009; Dufek, Lozano, Pettigrew, & Wulf, 2010). Standing long jump and jump for height performances have also been shown to be enhanced when an external focus of attention is used (Nolan, Porter, Ostrowski, & Wu, 2010; Dufek & Wulf, 2009). 37 Previous studies with expert performers have shown that an external focus of attention and the performers natural focus of attention produce better results than when performing under an internal focus of attention (Stoate & Wulf, 2011). In this study, in which the participants were considered experts, subjects did perform slightly better, on average, under internal focus of attention conditions. They also performed better when given instructions eliciting a specific attentional focus compared to when they were not given any attentional focus instructions. These trends could be due to several different reasons. For both groups, day 2 performances were greater than those for day 1 distance, regardless of attentional focus. This could be due to the participants improving technically from day 1 of the study or getting into a better rhythm. The participants also jumped further on the first 2 jumps of day 2 no matter which attentional focus condition they were using first. This may have been due to the effect of fatigue. Even though the participants improved on their overall average distance jumped on day 2, during their 3rd and 4th jumps they may have experienced fatigue detracting from their performance regardless of which attentional focus condition was adopted. Being on site during the research trials, the researcher observed fatigue setting in during the last two jump attempts for the participants. This was thought to be accounted for prior to the study taking place. It was believed that the participants, being trained long jumpers, would be able to perform adequately when jumping two days in a row (which is common in track & field competitions) without signs of fatigue. What was overlooked was that the participants were not yet in full competitive condition. The study took place the week prior to the start of the participants’ 38 competitive season, which may have been too early in their training year to have developed a capacity for jumping 8 times over two consecutive days. This may have accounted for the small differences observed between the internal and external focus of attention conditions. Another possible reason for the insignificant difference between the performances of the attentional focus conditions could be that the distance affect for the external focus instructions was not large enough. It was the researchers attempt to make the differences between the internal and external instructions as small as possible, so that if there were a difference in performances, this would demonstrate the robustness of the attentional focus effects (Hob, Prinz, & Wulf, 1998). With hindsight, the external focus of attention instructions should have had a greater distance effect than the board, which was directly under the participants’ foot. Previous research has shown that the greater the distance effect the greater advantage an external focus of attention has on performance (McNevin et al., 2003). With the distance effect of the external instructions used being so close to the performers body, it may be likely that the participants took greater active control over their actions and thus constrained their movement efficiency (McNevin, Shea, & Wulf, 2003). A possible reason for the slightly greater performances while under the internal focus of attention condition compared to the external focus of attention condition could also be because the athletes were more familiar with an internal focus orientation. The questionnaires that were completed by the participants indicated that all but two naturally used an internal focus of attention. The other two participants indicated that they equally 39 distributed their focus of attention between an internal and external focus. This may be because a majority of track & field coaches give cues that induce an internal focus of attention (Partridge, Porter, & Wu, 2012). It has been speculated that this is because the track & field coaching education literature puts a greater focus on biomechanics and the physiology of training than it does on motor learning. This could very well be true. However, it is possible that through trial and error track & field coaches have embraced cues and instructions that elicit an internal focus of attention because they see that their athletes respond to it better. Perhaps in track & field, where very finite and precise movements can make a large difference in performance, an internal focus of attention is needed to achieve the desired technical results. However, looking at the results of this study, which did not show a significant difference between the two attentional foci, it is hard to determine which attentional focus is the more advantageous. It is less difficult to determine that giving long jumpers instructions, whether they are internal or external, will produce more advantageous results than withholding instructions. Recommendations for Future Study This study was approached as an exploratory study for attentional focus and the long jump. No previous study had been done on the long jump, or on any movement skill similar to it. It is for this reason that such a low number of participants were used for the study. Due to the low participant numbers and the lack of any significant differences being found, it is difficult and not appropriate to infer specific conclusions and recommendations based upon findings from this study. However, an ancillary objective 40 of the study was to create a baseline of attentional focus instructions for the long jump that could be used and improved upon in future studies, as well as in the field. In future studies involving attentional focus and the long jump, perhaps a greater distance effect could be used for the external focus of attention instructions. This would allow the participants to more clearly differentiate the movement effect from their body’s own movement, which would allow for a more automatic and reflexive movement (McNevin, Shea, & Wulf, 2003). This study’s external focus instructions directed the participants focus to the board that they were taking off from, which may have been too close to differentiate from the movements of their own body. Expanding the time between the two days of the study is also be recommended. Allowing the participants a greater rest period than 24 hours would allow for greater dissipation of any fatigue that was caused by the first day of the study. Another recommendation would be to expand the study to a multiple week study, where the participants have trials 2 to 3 days a week. This would allow for more data points to be gathered, which would allow for the power of any difference between the attentional foci to be seen more clearly. Using novice performers rather than expert performers would also be a worthwhile option for future researchers to consider. Expert performers are difficult to obtain in the large numbers needed to be able to show the power of the differences between the focus conditions. Having a greater number of participants would help in avoiding issues that could arise from participants drop out of the study, something from which this study suffered. Another benefit to using novice performers is that it would 41 allow for greater performance increases to occur, because unlike expert performers, novice performers have yet to automate the skill being performed. This would aid in showing the robustness of the different attentional focus instructions. The results of this study did not show any significant difference in the participants’ long jump performances between the different attentional focus conditions. However, the majority of the research on attentional focus still shows that an external focus of attention is more beneficial than an internal or natural focus of attention. It is worthwhile to dedicate further research to attentional focus and the long jump, as well as other track and field events. With the performance benefits that attentional focus has been seen to have through research on performance, it is important to further explore what benefits it may have on the long jump and other track and field events as well. 42 Appendix A Questionnaire #1 What did you concentrate on prior to and/or during your jumps? 1) How your body, legs and/or arms were moving or should move 2) Locations or areas in the surrounding environment 3) Equally distributed between the two 43 Appendix B Track & Field Dynamic Warm Up 2 x Jog forward 40m / Backwards 40m 2 x Side Slide w/arms 20m / Change sides 20m 1 x Skip 20m / Fast Skip 20m 1 x Skip 20m / High Skip 20m YOGA HAMSTRING STRETCH 1 X 30 SECONDS EACH LEG 2 x Caroche 20m and run 20 m 15 x Each Leg Single Leg Hops on Grass 1 x 30 Seconds High Knee Runs in place QUAD STRETCHES (HEEL TO BUTT 20 SECONDS EACH LEG) 2 x “A” Skips 20m and run 20m 2 x “B” Skips 20m and run 20m HAMSTRING STRETCH (20 SECONDS EACH LEG) 2 x Quick Foot/Bound 20m and run 20m 2 x Back Hamstring Run 20m and run 20m 90 SECONDS STRETCH OF CHOICE 2 x Straight Leg Bounds 20m and run 20m 2 x 40m Full Effort Sprint (walk back) 44 References Anton, P. M., Porter, J. M., WU, W. F. W. (2011). Increasing the Distance of an External Focus of Attention Enhances Standing Long Jump Performance. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 0(0), 1-5. Brown, L. E., Porter, J. M., Wu, W. F. W. (2011). Effect of Attentional Focus Strategies on Peak Force and performance in the Standing Long Jump. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 10, 520-525. Chow, J. Y., Davids, K., Peh, S. Y. (2011). Focus of Attention and it’s Impact on Movement Behavior. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 14, 70-78. Dufek, J. S., Granados, C., Wulf, G., Zachry, T. (2007). Increases in Jump-and-Reach Height Through an External Focus of Attention. Int J Sports Sci & Coaching, 2, 275-284. Dufek, J. S., Lozano, L., Pettigrew, C., Wulf, G. (2010). Increased Jump Height and Reduced EMG Activity with an External Focus. Human Movement Science 29, 440-448. Dufek, J. S., Wulf, G. (2009). Increased jump height with an External Focus Due to Enhanced Lower Extremity Joint Kinetics. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41(5), 401-409. Ericsson, A. K., Williams, M. A. (2007). Capturing Naturally Occurring Superior Performance in the Laboratory: Translational Research on Expert performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 1393), 115-123. 45 Fathi, I., Saemi, E., Zarghami, M. (2012). External Focus of Attention Enhances Discus Throwing Performance. Kinesiology, 44(1), 47-51. Fitts, P. M. (1964). Perceptual-Motor Skills Learning. In A.W. Melton (Ed.), Categories of Human Learning (pp. 243-285). New York: Academic Press. Fitts, P. M., Posner, M. I., (1967). Human Performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole. Ford, P. R., Williams, M. R. (2009). Promoting a Skills-Based Agenda in Olympic Sports: The Role of Skill-Acquisition Specialist. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27(13), 1381-1392. Fowler, N., Graham-Smith, P., Lees, P. (1994). A Biomechanical Analysis of the Last Stride, Touchdown, and Takeoff Characteristics of the Men’s Long Jump. Journal of Applied Biomechanics 10, 61-78. Francesconi, D. (2011). Implicit and Explicit Learning in Motor Cognition: issues for Movement Education. The International Journal of Sport and Society, 2(1), 1-7. Graham-Smith, P., Lees, A. (2005). A Three-Dimensional Kinematic Analysis of the Long Jump Take-Off. Journal of Sports Sciences, 23(9), 891-903. Healy, A. F., Lohse, K. R., Sherwood, D. E. (2011). Neuromuscular Effects of Shifting the Focus of Attention in a Simple Force Production Task. Journal of Motor behavior, 43(2), 173-179. Hob, M., Prinz, W., Wulf, G. (1998). Instructions for Motor Learning: Differential Effects of Internal Versus External Focus of Attention. Journal of Motor behavior, 30, 169-179. 46 Hodges, N. J., Williams, M. A. (2004). Practice, Instruction and Skill Acquisition in Soccer: Challenging tradition. Journal of Sports Sciences, 23(6), 637-650. Lauterbach, B., Toole, T., Wulf, G. (1999). Learning Advantages of an External Focus of Attention in Golf. Research Quarterly of Exercise and Sport, 70, 120-126. Lees, A., Vanezis, A. (2005). A Biomechanical Analysis of Good and Poor Performers of the Vertical Jump. Ergonomics, 48, 1594-1603. Masters, R. S. W., Maxwell, J. P., Poolton, J. M., Raab, M. (2005). Benefits of an External Focus of Attention: Common Coding or Conscious Processing? Journal of Sports Science, 224(1), 89-99. McKay, B., Wulf, G. (2012). A Distal External Focus Enhances Novice Dart Throwing Performance. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 10(2), 149156. McNevin, N. H., Shea, C. H., Wulf, G. (2001). The Automacity for Complex Motor Skill Learning as a Function of Attentional Focus. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A(4), 1143-1154. McNevin, N. H., Shea, C. H., Wulf, G. (2003). Increasing the Distance of an External Focus of Attention Enhances Learning. Psychological Research, 67, 22-29. Munzert, J., Zentgraf, K. (2009). Effects of Attentional-Focus Instructions on Movement Kinematics. Psychology of Sport and Science Exercise, 10, 520-525. 47 Nolan, R. P., Ostrowski, E. J., Porter, J. M., Wulf, G. (2010). Directing Attention Externally Enhances Agility performance: A Qualitative and Quantitative Analysis of The efficacy of Using Verbal Instructions to Focus Attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 1(216), 1-7. Nolan, R. P., Ostrowski, E. J., Porter, J. M., Wu, W. F. W. (2010). Standing Long-Jump Performance is Enhanced When Using an External Focus of Attention. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 24(7), 1746-1750. Park, J. H., Shea, C. H., Wulf, G. (2001). Attention in Motor Learning: Preferences for and Advantages of an External Focus. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 72, 335-334. Partridge, J. A., Porter, J. M., Wu, W. F. W. (2012). Focus of Attention and verbal Instructions: Strategies of Elite Track & Field Coaches and Athletes. Sports Science Review, 19(3-4), 77-89. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and Action Planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129-154. Shea, C. H., Wulf, G. (1999). Enhancing Motor Learning Through External-Focus Instructions and Feedback. Human Movement Science, 18, 553-571. Shea, C. H., Tollner, T., Wulf, G. (2007). Attentional Focus Effects as a Function of Task Difficulty. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 257-264 Stoate, I., Wulf, G. (2011). Does the Attentional Focus Adopted by Swimmers Affect Their Performance? International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching, 6(1), 99108. 48 Su, J., Wulf, G. (2007). An External Focus of Attention Enhances Golf Shot Accuracy in Beginners and Experts, Research Quarterly for Exercise and sport, 78, 384-389. Wulf, G. (2003). Attentional Focus in Motor Skill Learning: Do Females Benefit from an External Focus? Women in Sport & Physical Activity Journal 12(1), 1-37. Wulf, G. (2007). Attention and Motor Skill Learning. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Wulf, G. (2008). Attentional Focus Effects in Balance Acrobats. American Alliance for Health, Physical Education, and Dance 79(3), 319-325.