Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine Online Supplemental Material (1) Detailed description of primary and ancillary measures (2) Testing inflammation as a potential mediator (3) Table S1: Baseline sample characteristics for the primary study variables (4) Table S2. Significance test and effect sizes for the loneliness by supplementation group interaction predicting cognitive function scores (5) Table S3. Significance test for the main effect of supplementation group predicting cognitive function scores (6) Table S4: Planned contrasts examining the effect of supplementation for people who were more or less lonely (7) Table S5: Residual change in cognitive function scores from pre to post supplementation. (8) References for online supplemental material Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine Detailed Description of Primary and Ancillary Measures The California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edition (CVLT-II) is a well-established measure of verbal episodic memory [1,2]. Indices of immediate free recall, short-delay free recall, and long-delay free recall measured verbal episodic memory. Across 5 different trials, participants heard a list of the same 16 words and were immediately asked to recall the entire list. The total number of words recalled across the learning trials served as a measure of immediate free recall. After hearing a separate word list, participants were asked to recall words from the original list, assessing short-delay free recall (which could be affected by retroactive interference from the second list). Participants were also asked to remember words from the original list after a 20-minute delay, assessing long-delay free recall. Alternate versions of the test were administered at each visit to minimize practice effects, and the order of the test versions was determined randomly. Scoring was completed using CVLT-II Comprehensive Scoring System software, with higher numbers indicating better verbal episodic memory [1]. Three tests from the Wechsler Memory Scale – Third Edition (WMS-III) assessed working memory [3]. The Digit Span and Letter-Number Sequencing tasks measured verbal working memory whereas the Spatial Span task measured visuospatial working memory. To complete the digit span task, participants were given an increasingly long list of numbers and were asked to recall them either forward or backwards. The Letter-Number Sequencing test consisted on a list of mixed letters and numbers; participants were asked to recall both in ascending order. For the Spatial Span task, participants watched an experimenter touch a series of blocks with different numbers on them and were asked to repeat the same sequence. All three tasks were scored according to standard WMS-III instructions, with higher numbers reflecting better working memory [3]. Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine The Trail Making task was used to measure executive function [4]. Participants were given a sheet with numbers dispersed on the page and were asked to connect the numbers in numeric order (Part A). Next they were given a sheet with numbers and letters on the page and were asked to connect both in ascending order, alternating between letters and numbers (Part B). To obtain a clean measure of executive function, the time participants took (in seconds) to complete Part A was subtracted from Part B [4]. Higher numbers correspond to better executive function. The Controlled Oral Word Association Task test assessed verbal fluency and processing speed [5]. Participants were given a time limit to generate as many words as possible that started with a certain letter. The total number of words participants generated across three letter trials was used to compute processing speed; higher numbers reflect better verbal fluency and processing speed. The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression (CES-D) Scale is one of the most commonly used measures of depressive symptoms [6]. The CES-D has good discriminant validity, construct validity, and test-retest reliability. The CES-D was included to account for potential relationships among depression, loneliness, and cognitive function [7–9]. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index measured sleep quality over the past month (PSQI) [10]. The PSQI can distinguish between people with and without sleep disturbances, indicating acceptable discriminant validity. The PSQI provided a way to disentangle sleep quality from the link between loneliness and cognitive function [11,12]. Activity levels were measured with the Community Healthy Activities Model Program for Seniors questionnaire, a well-validated measure of physical activity among middle-aged and older adults [CHAMPS: 13]. High levels of exercise are associated with better cognitive function Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine [14]. Accordingly, the exercise index allowed us to examine the links between loneliness and cognitive function, independent of activity levels. To assess social integration versus isolation, participants completed the Social Network Index Interview [SNII; 15]. The SNII, a commonly used measure of social integration, asks participants to indicate whether or not they are in different types of relationships and have different social roles. This measure was included to disentangle the presence/absence of a relationship (i.e., social integration) versus the perceived quality of those relationships (i.e., loneliness).1 Participants answered questions about their age, marital status, weekly average alcohol consumption, current medication use, and education. Years of education was used as an index of SES because some people in our sample did not work outside of the home. In addition, education is less vulnerable to current economic conditions than income and job status. We also assessed participants’ body mass index (BMI: kg/m2). 1 The SNII was administered 4 weeks after supplementation began (i.e.,12 weeks before trial completion), at the same time as the loneliness measure. As noted in eTable 1, there were no differences in social integration across groups. Accordingly, the SNII was used as a baseline measure of social integration. Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine Testing Inflammation as a Potential Mediator The primary paper that resulted from the parent RCT demonstrated that omega-3 supplementation caused reductions in inflammation over time [16]. Specifically, serum interleukin-6 (IL-6) decreased by 10% and 12% in the 1.25 g/d and 2.50 g/d imega-3 groups, respectively, compared to a 36% increase in the placebo group. Similarly, the 1.25 g/d and 2.50 g/d omega-3 groups showed modest 0.2% and 2.3% changes in serum tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-α), compared to a 12% increase in the placebo group. Loneliness is linked to elevated inflammation and inflammation may promote cognitive difficulties [17,18]. Accordingly, we explored whether decreased inflammation may partially explain why omega-3 supplementation attenuated loneliness-related episodic memory problems in the current study. To test this possibility, we ran a series of analyses exploring the links among loneliness, omega-3 supplementation, inflammation, and episodic memory. Similar to the primary analyses, we controlled for marital status BMI, activity levels, and baseline levels of the corresponding outcome. The interaction between loneliness and supplementation group was unrelated to post-supplementation inflammation IL-6 and TNF-α, F(2,114) = 1.35, p = .264 and F(2,119) = 0.81, p = .446 respectively. In addition, changes in both IL-6 and TNF-α were unrelated to post-supplementation immediate and long-delay free recall, independent of baseline levels, all p values > .134. Collectively, these results suggest that changes in inflammation did not explain why lonely participants benefited from omega-3 supplementation. Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine eTable 1. Baseline sample characteristics for the primary study variables. Characteristic Full Sample Placebo 1.25 g/d 2.50 g/d p-value Mean(SD) Mean(SD) Mean(SD) Mean(SD) Loneliness 37.86(9.83) 37.82(10.42) 36.00(8.29) 39.88(10.54) .177 BMI 30.59(4.50) 29.76(4.73) 31.83(4.66) 30.18(3.88) .065 Hours of activity/week 11.11(5.83) 10.09(4.96) 13.04(6.66) 10.20(5.35) .021 Social integration 7.89(1.86) 7.58(2.04) 7.94(1.48) 8.16(2.03) .334 Sleep quality 5.47(2.85) 5.40(2.47) 5.26(2.66) 5.76(3.37) .689 Alcoholic drinks/week 1.57(2.69) 1.63(3.06) 1.26(1.69) 1.80(3.10) .616 Depressive symptoms 7.58(8.10) 7.70(9.08) 7.70(8.69) 7.35(6.44) .972 CVLT – immediate 56.77(9.82) 58.07(9.29) 54.60(11.30) 57.62(8.51) .189 CVLT – short delay 11.89(2.77) 12.26(2.80) 11.18(3.05) 12.22(2.33) .107 CVLT – long delay 12.58(2.58) 12.96(2.36) 11.73(2.94) 13.05(2.24) .026 Digit Span 17.50(3.94) 17.02(3.94) 17.09(3.86) 18.43(3.94) .162 Letter-Number Sequencing 11.03(2.36) 10.61(1.92) 10.63(1.99) 11.89(2.90) .013 Spatial Span 16.26(2.81) 16.30(2.40) 15.57(2.80) 16.95(3.09) .063 Trail Making (B-A) 37.23(23.34) 35.87(18.68) 43.30(31.93) 32.42(14.87) .074 Controlled Oral Word Association Task 41.47(11.37) 42.37(9.52) 37.33(10.12) 44.78(13.13) .005 NOTE: The p-value refers to the test of between group differences at baseline, which were tested with unadjusted models. Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine eTable 2. Significance test and effect sizes for the loneliness by supplementation group interaction predicting cognitive function scores Model 1 Model 2 F df p value R2 F df p value R2 CVLT – immediate 2.69 2,121 .072 .02 5.05 2,117 .008 .04 CVLT – short delay 0.45 2,121 .637 .01 0.90 2,117 .411 .01 CVLT – long delay 2.74 2,120 .068 .02 3.38 2,116 .038 .03 Digit Span 0.19 2,122 .830 .00 0.13 2,118 .881 .00 Letter-Number Sequencing 1.54 2,122 .219 .01 1.15 2,118 .322 .01 Spatial Span 0.84 2,122 .436 .01 0.88 2,118 .419 .01 Trail Making (B-A) 1.33 2,121 .269 .01 0.96 2,117 .386 .01 Controlled Oral Word Association Task 0.24 2,122 .787 .00 0.22 2,118 .802 .00 Outcome NOTE: The R2 reflects the proportion of variance explained in the outcome by the interaction between loneliness and omega-3 supplementation group. Model 1 included the baseline cognitive function scores, loneliness, group, and the loneliness by group interaction as predictors. Model 2 also included marital status, BMI, and activity levels, as reported in the primary analyses. Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine eTable 3. Significance test for the main effect of supplementation group predicting cognitive function scores Characteristic Placebo 1.25 g/d 2.50 g/d F df p-value Mean(SE) Mean(SE) Mean(SE) CVLT – immediate 58.05(1.11) 54.32(1.36) 58.29(1.25) 3.60 2,117 .030 CVLT – short delay 12.38(0.36) 11.98(0.45) 12.62(0.41) 0.72 2,117 .490 CVLT – long delay 12.87(0.30) 12.59(0.38) 13.25(0.34) 1.09 2,116 .341 Digit Span 18.09(0.39) 17.98(0.47) 18.43(0.45) 0.37 2,118 .695 Letter-Number Sequencing 11.02(0.27) 11.05(0.33) 11.46(0.31) 0.74 2,118 .479 Spatial Span 16.84(0.34) 16.60(0.41) 16.35(0.39) 0.49 2,118 .615 Trail Making (B-A) 35.67(2.31) 35.09(2.87) 35.84(2.63) 0.03 2,117 .974 Controlled Oral Word Association Task 42.35(0.98) 43.45(1.21) 43.41(1.12) 0.38 2,118 .683 NOTE: The means, standard errors (SE), and test statistics were derived from the model reported in the primary analyses. These analyses included baseline cognitive function scores, marital status, BMI, and activity levels, loneliness, group, and the loneliness by group interaction as predictors. Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine eTable 4. Planned contrasts examining the effect of supplementation on immediate and long-delay recall for people who were more or less lonely Outcome Loneliness Score (a) (b) (c) (a) vs (b) (a) vs (c) (b) vs (c) Placebo 1.25 g/d 2.50 g/d p value p value p value Less lonely (-1 SD) 60.84(1.54) 56.37(1.68) 56.57(1.82) .041 .063 .929 More lonely (+1 SD) 55.30(1.52) 52.29(2.12) 59.99(1.42) .245 .022 .002 Less lonely (-1 SD) 13.47(0.42) 12.68(0.46) 12.79(0.51) .184 .278 .861 More lonely (+1 SD) 12.28(0.41) 12.51(0.58) 13.70(0.39) .743 .011 .079 CVLT – immediate CVLT – long delay NOTE: The values presented in columns a, b, and c are the estimated marginal means and standard errors of the mean for each group, which correspond to the values plotted in Figures 1 and 2. Each estimated marginal mean was obtained from the adjusted model presented in the primary analyses for people who were one standard deviation above and below the mean of loneliness. Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine eTable 5. Residual change in cognitive function scores from pre to post supplementation. Outcome Supplementation Group Loneliness Score Pre to Post Supplementation Change Mean(SE) Less lonely (-1 SD) 4.31(1.54) More lonely (+1 SD) -1.23(1.52) Less lonely (-1 SD) -0.16(1.68) More lonely (+1 SD) -4.24(2.12) Less lonely (-1 SD) 0.04(1.82) More lonely (+1 SD) 3.46(1.42) Less lonely (-1 SD) 0.92(0.42) More lonely (+1 SD) -0.27(0.41) Less lonely (-1 SD) 0.13(0.46) More lonely (+1 SD) -0.04(0.58) Less lonely (-1 SD) 0.24(0.51) More lonely (+1 SD) 1.15(0.39) Placebo CVLT - immediate 1.25 g/d omega-3 2.50 g/d omega-3 Placebo CVLT – long delay 1.25 g/d omega-3 2.50 g/d omega-3 NOTE: Only the cognitive functions measures with significant loneliness by group interactions are reported. The values were derived from the adjusted models presented in the primary analyses. Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine References for Online Supplemental Material [1] Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. The California Verbal Learning Test Second Edition. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2000. [2] Woods SP, Delis DC, Scott JC, Kramer JH, Holdnack JA. The California Verbal Learning Test – second edition: Test-retest reliability, practice effects, and reliable change indices for the standard and alternate forms. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2006;21:413–420. [3] Wechsler D. WMS-III: Wechsler memory scale administration and scoring manual. Psychological Corporation; 1997. [4] Reitan RM. Trail Making Test: Manual for administration and scoring. Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory; 1986. [5] Benton A, Hamsher K. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. 2nd ed. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates; 1989. [6] Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1977;1:385–401. [7] McDermott LM, Ebmeier KP. A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. J Affect Disord 2009;119:1–8. [8] Jaremka LM, Andridge RR, Fagundes CP, Alfano CM, Povoski SP, Lipari AM, Agnese DM, Arnold MW, Farrar WB, Yee LD, Carson III WE, Bekaii-Saab T, Martin Jr EW, Schmidt CR, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. Pain, depression, and fatigue: Loneliness as a longitudinal risk factor. Health Psychol in press; [9] Jaremka LM, Fagundes CP, Glaser R, Bennett JM, Malarkey WB, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. Loneliness predicts pain, depression, and fatigue: Understanding the role of immune dysregulation. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013;38:1310–1317. [10] Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF 3rd, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989;28:193–213. [11] Blackwell T, Yaffe K, Ancoli-Israel S, Scheider JL, Cauley JA, Hillier TA, Fink HA, Stone KL. Poor sleep is associated with impaired cognitive function in older women: The Study of Osteoporotic Fractures. J Gerontol Ser -Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;61:405–410. [12] Kurina LM, Knutson KL, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT, Lauderdale DS, Ober C. Loneliness is associated with sleep fragmentation in a communal society. Sleep 2011;34:1519–1526. [13] Stewart AL, Mills KM, King AC, Haskell WL, Gillis D, Ritter PL. CHAMPS physical activity questionnaire for older adults: outcomes for interventions. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2001;33:1126–1141. Supplemental Digital Content 1 for Psychosomatic Medicine [14] Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: A metaanalytic study. Psychol Sci 2003;14:125–130. [15] Cohen S. Social supports and physical health: Symptoms, health behaviors, and infectious disease. In: Cummings EM, Greene AL, Karraker KH, editors. Life-span developmental psychology: Perspectives on stress and coping. Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1991. p. 213–234. [16] Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Belury MA, Andridge R, Malarkey WB, Hwang BS, Glaser R. Omega3 supplementation lowers inflammation in healthy middle-aged and older adults: A randomized controlled trial. Brain Behav Immun 2012;26:988–995. [17] Rafnsson SB, Deary IJ, Smith FB, Whiteman MC, Rumley A, Lowe GDO, Fowkes FGR. Cognitive decline and markers of inflammation and hemostasis: The Edinburgh Artery Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2007;55:700–707. [18] Jaremka LM, Fagundes CP, Peng J, Bennett JM, Glaser R, Malarkey WB, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. Loneliness promotes inflammation during acute stress. Psychol Sci 2013;24:1089– 1097.