Examination Number: 7879463 Age-Related Changes in False Recognition An ERP Study Master of Science in Psychological Research Methods The University of Edinburgh 2010 – 2011 1 Examination Number: 7879463 Disclosure “I have read and understood The University of Edinburgh guidelines on Plagiarism and declare that this written dissertation is all my own work except where I indicate otherwise by proper use of quotes and references.” Acknowledgements I would like to thank the PPLS EEG Pilot Scheme and the MSc Psychological Research Methods course whom both funded this study and without which this research would not be possible. I would also like to thank my family and my supervisor who both gave me excellent guidance at every stage of my thesis. 2 Examination Number: 7879463 Abstract Episodic memory function is well known to decline with age and there is evidence to suggest seniors prone to forget events compared to younger adults (Aizpurua & Koutstaal, 2010). What’s more, seniors are inclined to falsely ‘remember’ events which did not happen. For example, seniors are more affected by misleading post-event information (e.g. lures), remembering that information as having occurred alongside the original event (Roediger & Geraci, 2007; Chan & McDermott, 2007; Koutstaal, 2006; Aizpurua & Koutstaal, 2010). This thesis compares two hypotheses of false memory, the Cognitive Control Hypothesis and the Memory Specificity Hypothesis using an event-related potential (ERP) index of true recollection, an early left parietal old/new effect (~400 to 800ms) and adapting a paradigm used by Koutstaal (2006) which successfully demonstrated high false recognition in participants using picture stimuli. Participants undertook two explicit forced choice tasks: unintentionally studying picture stimuli by answering a size-judgment task and later a recognition test where they could answer either “old” (for repeated stimuli), “similar” (lures) or “new” (novel). Results supports previous behavioural findings by demonstrating seniors are significantly worse at discriminating between repeated and novel stimuli than younger adults, despite similar end performances with respect to repeated and novel stimuli. In addition, evidence shown here supports the suggestion that seniors are inclined to falsely “remember” events which did not happen, by misleading post-event information, compared to younger adults. An exploratory analysis of left parietal old/new effects from collected ERP data revealed unexpected conclusions. False alarms to lure stimuli (by answering “old”) showed significantly greater magnitudes compared to all other critical conditions in both age groups. This is not supported by previous research. However, younger participants showed greater correct responses to old items with greater magnitudes than correctly rejected new items which is supported by previous research. While neither hypothesis could be accepted based on the information gathered, the study design worked well and could easily be repeated. Future research replicating this study and combining it with other behavioural, ERP and fMRI results with undoubtedly yield a better understanding of the underlying processes involved cognitive ageing. 3 Examination Number: 7879463 Contents Introduction Page 1 – 13 Memory in Normal Ageing Page 1 – 4 Brain Regions of Interest Page 4 – 7 Cognitive Control Hypothesis Page 7 Memory Specificity Hypothesis Page 7 – 8 Expected ERP Indices Page 8 – 10 Thesis Aims, Questions and Expectations Page 10 – 12 Method Page 13 – 18 Participants Page 13 -14 Neuropsychological Tests Page 14 Design Page 14 Materials Page 14 – 15 Procedure Page 15 – 18 ERP Recording and Pre-processing Page 18 Results Page 19 – 28 Neuropsychological Test Performance Page 19 Behavioural Analysis Strategy Page 20 Behavioural Task Performance Page 20 Accuracy analysis Page 20 Response times analysis Page 22 Discrimination and response bias analysis Page 23 ERP Analysis Strategy Page 24 -25 ERP Data Page 25 – 28 4 Examination Number: 7879463 Discussion Page 29 Neuropsychological Test Findings Page 29 Behavioural Findings Page 30 ERP Findings Page 31 General Discussion Page 32 Conclusion Page 36 References Page 37 – 50 Appendix A (Consent forms) Page 51 – 54 Appendix B (EEG procedure) Page 55 Appendix C (Information sheets for experiment tasks) Page 56 - 59 Appendix D (Hand-charts for EEG experiment) Page 60-63 Appendix E (Pilot results) Page 64 - 68 5 Examination Number: 7879463 Introduction Episodic memory is an essential part of cognition and necessary for many abilities including decision making, categorisation, speech and planning. Improved episodic performance enhances these abilities and offers an evolutionary advantage as the ability to learn and remember allows the consequence of actions to be predicted from previous experience, with better adaptation to changing situations (Aizpurua & Koutstaal, 2010; Shing et al., 2010; Gronlund, Carlson & Tower, 2007; Dickerson & Eichenbaum, 2009). However, episodic memory function is well known to decline with age (Aizpurua & Koutstaal, 2010). Compared to other skills, failings in memory are the commonest complaints of seniors (Newson & Kemps, 2006; Yassa et al., 2010; Chan & McDermott, 2007). While there is evidence demonstrating seniors are prone to forget specific events. There is also evidence that seniors are inclined to falsely ‘remember’ events which did not happen. For instance, seniors are more affected by misleading post-event information, remembering that information as having occurred alongside the original event (Roediger & Geraci, 2007; Chan & McDermott, 2007; Koutstaal, 2006; Aizpurua & Koutstaal, 2010). This thesis will investigate the impact of normal ageing on memory retrieval using a recognition paradigm under electroencephalogram (EEG) conditions developed from Koutstaal’s (2006) study which demonstrated enhanced false memory in seniors. This thesis will explore the background to recognition memory, the theory behind the Cognitive Control and Memory Specificity Hypotheses and the event-related potential (ERP) indicator I will be used to compare the two hypotheses. Memory in Normal Ageing In memory research, an important concept is ‘Multiple Memory Systems’. This theory assumes that memory is not one single entity but divided into separate categories and sub-categories which seemingly similar, differ in the type of information they process (Sherry & Schacter, 1987; Moscovitch & Winocur, 1995; Squire, 2004). This study focuses on episodic memory, “[A system] recently evolved, late-developing and early-deteriorating past-orientated” giving an individual “[conscious awareness] of an earlier experience in a certain situation at a certain time”, allowing mental time-travel to previous events to re-live experiences (Tulving, 1972; 1993; 2002 p.5). This unique aspect of ‘mental time-travel’ separates it from other memory systems (Wheeler, Struss & Tulving, 1997). Research shows that episodic, prospective and working memory noticeably decline with increased age compared to other putative memory systems such as semantic memory, an individual’s memory for facts and knowledge (Duverne, Habibi & Rugg, 2008; Grady & Craik, 2000). 6 Examination Number: 7879463 For example remembering that the capital of Scotland is Edinburgh, is preserved through life if it is frequently used while memory for specific information, for example remembering the situation surrounding being taught that fact, declines more rapidly (Luo & Craik, 2008). Episodic memory can be split into four broad theoretical areas: an initial transformation of sensory information into a representation (event) required for storage (i.e. encoding); the strengthening of that event over a period of time (i.e. consolidation); the storage of the event and the recall of that event (i.e. retrieval) (Tulving, 1972; Mayes & Roberts, 2001). The aspect of retrieval and recognition memory, the ability to decide if a presented item has been previously experienced and the impact of normal ageing upon them is the focus for this thesis. Seniors are less able to correctly classify incoming information as either known or novel and are prone to forget specific events (Roediger & Geraci, 2007). In addition there is also evidence that seniors are prone to ‘remember’ events which did not happen. For instance, misleading post-event information will affect seniors more than younger adults, with seniors attributing this post-event information as having occurred alongside the original event (Aizpurua, Garcia-Bajos & Migueles, 2009; Mitchell, Johnson & Mather, 2003; Multhaup, De Leonardis & Johnson, 1999; Chan & McDermott, 2007). This increase in false memory in seniors was demonstrated in paradigms such as the false-fame paradigm (Multhaup, 1995; Davidson & Glisky, 2002). In this instance participants would study lists consisting of names of people participants were told were non-famous. At test, participants were asked to identify the famous names in a new list. This list would contain famous names, non-famous names and names from the previous list. Seniors demonstrated increased false-fame errors compared to younger adults (Multhaup, 1995). Seniors’ susceptibility to false recognition has been shown in drawings (Koutstaal, 2006), complex scenes (Gutchess et al., 2005), word-pair tasks (Grady & Craik, 2000; Castel & Craik, 2003; Hay & Jacoby, 1999), sentences (Bayen, 1999) and in one of the most widely used paradigms, the Deese/Roediger-McDermott (DRM) task (Roediger & McDermott, 1995). The DRM task includes word lists (e.g. boat, whale, blue, waves) where a majority of the words are linked by a semantically related “lure” word (in this case ‘ocean’). Participants learn the lists presented (study phase) and are later asked to recall the word lists (test phase). Seniors will falsely remember the non-presented lure word more frequently than younger adults with a high confidence that it appeared in the original list (Norman & Schacter, 1997; Wiese & Daum, 2006; Luo & Craik, 2008; Chan & McDermott, 2007). In order to assess why seniors’ were more susceptible to lure items Koutstaal & Schacter (1997) used a picture recognition paradigm. Here, participants studied multiple different pictures from various categories (e.g. boats, shoes, cats) along with unrelated pictures not belonging to any category (e.g. 7 Examination Number: 7879463 a snow sled, a hand bell). At test, participants made “old” versus “new” judgments on whether pictures were shown before or novel from a list of previously studied items, related lure pictures and unrelated pictures. Seniors showed more false recognition to the lure pictures than younger adults. Seniors also showed similar correct “old” responses to young for pictures from large categories and impaired correct “new” responses for unrelated lure pictures. This type of performance suggests that within recognition memory seniors have a preserved access to information about general similarities but impaired access to distinctive information (Koutstaal & Schacter, 1997; Schacter et al., 1998). Research into recognition memory tends to take a dual-process account where recognition memory performance is determined by two separate processes: recollection, which is context-dependent so that specific details about a previous encounter are retrieved and familiarity occurring when the encounter is recognised but without context (e.g. you meet a person and recognise their face but cannot remember the name or when/where you saw that person) (Mecklinger & Jäger, 2009; Eichenbaum, Yonelinas & Ranganath, 2007; Shing et al., 2010). Both familiarity and recollection are thought to aid the correct judgement on whether a newly presented item/event is truly novel or not (Wiese & Daum, 2006; Davidson & Glisky, 2002; Cheng & Rugg, 2004; Shing et al., 2010). While the exact mechanisms underlying seniors’ performance in recognition tests such as the DRM are still to be explained, differences can be explained by two general theories. The first is an impaired ability in a mechanism during encoding that binds together different aspects of memory into a coherent representation and at retrieval separates aspects of memory into a distinct event (i.e. an “associative” deficit). Here, seniors would show less detailed recollection in a recognition task. The second possible impairment is a “strategic/monitoring” deficit causing seniors to over generalize in their search criteria at encoding and fail to correctly verify/evaluate the relevant information at retrieval (Shing et al., 2010; Davidson & Glisky, 2002; Schacter et al., 1998). In this case seniors would fully recollect information but the information would be less specific (due to impaired strategic encoding) or would be incorrectly utilized (due to impaired strategic retrieval). Gallo, Bell, Beier & Schacter (2006) propose that seniors deficits in strategic recollection are due to two types of monitoring processes. There is evidence that both are used for the successful rejection of lure items and that both processes are diminished in seniors (Chan & McDermott, 2007; Gallo et al., 2006; Gallo, 2004). The first is a diagnostic monitoring process, the distinctiveness heuristic and the second is a disqualifying process called recall-to-reject. Recall-to-reject can be defined as an editing process which aids in avoiding “lure” material by recalling the lures’ instantiating study item (i.e. “I didn’t see this item because I remember a similar one previously shown”) while the 8 Examination Number: 7879463 distinctiveness heuristic is defined as aiding avoiding lures because of a failure to recall expected information (i.e. “I didn’t see this item because I’d remember if I had”) (Odegard & Lampinen, 2005; Gallo et al., 2006; Dodson & Schacter, 2002). Associative deficits can be accounted for by a failure of pattern separation, defined as a mechanism for separating partially overlapping patterns of activation to successfully retrieve one specific episodic event separate from other patterns/events (Gilbert, Kesner & Lee, 2001; Toner, Pirogovsky, Kirwan & Gilbert, 2009). When this pattern separation fails, high levels of overlap occur between items creating a sustained or increased memory for what items have in common (i.e. gist) but impairing the successful recall of specific, distinctive items. Consequently participants will be more likely to falsely recollect items (Schacter et al., 1998). Brain Regions of Interest Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies present growing evidence that both the frontal lobes (FLs) and the medial temporal lobes (MTLs) contribute to age-related changes in recognition memory (Davidson & Glisky, 2002; Moscovitch & Winocur, 1995). Research on brain regions involved with memory began with patient H.M. who suffered severe epileptic fits and underwent surgery to remove his hippocampus and alleviate seizures. Although seizure activity decreased, the surgery left him unable to commit new information to memory. His symptoms included difficulties in recognising faces and a severe functional impairment as he could no longer remember when or what his last meal was, his age or even his carers (Corkin, 2002). These drastic changes following surgery for patient H.M. and other amnesic patients such as Clive Wearing and Jon focused research on the hippocampus and parahippocampal area (i.e. the medial temporal lobes, MTLs) (Squire, Wixted & Clark, 2007; Baddeley, 2001; Moscovitch & Winocur, 1995). Evidence of the significance of the MTLs with respect to episodic memory has accumulated, however, the importance of the MTL with respect to normal age-related deficits in recognition is still debated. Research from post-mortem examinations and in vivo structural neuroimaging (e.g. MRI and positron emission tomography; PET) suggests age-related morphological changes (i.e. anatomical and physiological degradation such as cortical thinning and gyral atrophy) are prominent in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Blatter et al., 1995; Raz et al., 1997; 2005; Sowell et al., 2003; Persson et al., 2006). There is also evidence of age-related morphological changes in the MTL (particularly the hippocampus), however, this is not consistent. The PFC consistently undergoes the steepest decline (Dennis & Cabeza, 2008, p.3). Current evidence suggests that these morphological changes, 9 Examination Number: 7879463 particularly in the PFC, compel our brains to reorganise their functions (Persson et al., 2006). Seniors’ decreased performance in DRM and other recognition tasks suggests that FL age-related changes are linked with failing memory in the elderly (Moscovitch & Winocur, 1995). Evidence suggests that with standard neuropsychological testing of frontal lobe function seniors consistently perform worse than younger adults (Koutstaal, 2006; West, 1996). ). In addition, in situations where seniors do not consistently perform less well in FL tests, those that show low frontal lobe performance have increased levels of false recognition (Butler, McDaniel, Dornburg, Prince & Roedigger, 2004). Although PFC damage has not been shown to directly incur severe amnesic symptoms, deficiencies occur within executive functioning (i.e. a set of cognitive abilities that control and regulate other abilities and behaviours) (West, 1996; 2000, West & Schwarb, 2006). This is thought to impact upon retrieval strategies and may account for differences between younger populations and seniors during recognition tasks (Naveh-Benjamin, Brac & Oded, 2007; Chan & McDermott, 2007; West & Schwarb, 2006; Wiese & Daum, 2006). Other studies have found links between FL function and susceptibility to use general memory, “gist” (Koutstaal, 2006; Aizpurua et al., 2009). Multhaup (1995) demonstrated in the false-fame paradigm that by encouraging seniors’ participants to use more stringent decision criteria, where participants categorized names (whether famous vs. non-famous) in terms of their source, seniors’ false recognition performance matched younger adults performance. Strategic manipulations in Multhaup’s (1995) study and others demonstrate that improving FL/executive function can decrease false recognition (Davidson & Glisky, 2002). However, there is also evidence pointing away from FL deficits. For example, Chan & McDermott (2007) tested recollection using a DRM paradigm. In this case neuropsychological tests indicating frontal lobe functioning and age-related factors were shown to be independent contributors to recollection performance. They concluded that another factor, separate from frontal lobe function, must contribute to seniors’ heightened susceptibility to false memories. Aizpurua & Koutstaal’s (2010), also using standard neuropsychological tests of FL function, demonstrated that seniors’ FL function was not significantly correlated with successful recollection. Furthermore, other studies have shown that despite encouragement to make stringent decision strategies in both recall-toreject and distinctiveness heuristic monitoring processes this does not fully reduce false recognition levels in seniors to that of younger adults (Koustaal & Cavendish, 2006; Koustaal, Schacter, Galluccio & Stofer; 1999; Gallo et al., 2006). Associative deficits, specifically pattern separation, linked to MTL structures may be contributing to seniors’ decreased performance in recognition tasks. PET and fMRI studies have demonstrated that 10 Examination Number: 7879463 successful recollection of materials is correlated with increased hippocampus activity in the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal as well as cerebral blood flow (CBF) (Suzuki, Johnson & Rugg, 2011a; 2011b; Rugg, Fletcher, Frith, Frackowiak & Dolan, 1997; Cabeza & Nyberg, 1997; 2000; Kapur, Friston, Frith & Frackowiak, 1995). Furthermore, there is evidence suggesting the dentate gyrus (DG) sub-region in the hippocampus and its CA1/3 mossy fibre projections supports pattern separation (Gilbert et al., 2001; Kesner, 2007; Myers & Scharfman, 2009; Rolls, 1996; Bakker, Kirwan, Miller & Stark, 2008). In senior adults decreased performance in recognition can be linked to decreases in the hippocampus and specifically the dentate gyrus. (Toner et al., 2009; Wilson, Ikonen, Gallagher, Eichenbaum & Tanila, 2005). When pattern separation fails, high levels of overlap occur between items creating a sustained or increased memory for what items have in common (i.e. gist) but impairs the successful recall of specific, distinctive items. Consequently participants will be more likely to falsely recollect items (Schacter et al., 1998). Popular theories of consolidation such as the ‘Standard Model’ (Squire & Zola-Morgan, 1991) and the ‘Multiple Trace Theory’ (Nadel & Moscovitch, 1997) can account for the pattern separation hypothesis. They suggest that the fundamental features of each memory representation are consolidated in the hippocampus which initially binds different aspects of memory, throughout the brain; such that no single location contains a complete record of the specific episodic event (Squire, 1992; 1995; Dudai, 2004; Nadel & Moscovitch, 1997). When part of an event is retrieved, a process of spreading activation occurs through the subset of that event’s features until the rest of the constituent features are recalled (Schacter et al., 1998; Dudai, 2004). Within the pattern separation hypothesis, if a recognition test item has been incorrectly ‘connected’ to other memory aspects or when retrieved spreading activation has not fully occurred, then correct pattern separation will be unsuccessful. In summary, there is evidence for both MTL and FL involvement in recognition memory, false memory and age-related changes in these areas. Due to its monitoring processes, the FL can be considered to impact on the reliance of familiarity processes while the MTL can be considered to impact both familiarity itself and recollection processes (Shing et al., 2010). This is supported by the roles FLs and MTLs have been accorded in the literature. Medial temporal lobe function can be described as associative with the hippocampus a core component, allowing for pattern completion to generate successful recollection while frontal lobe function monitors the input and output from the MTLs by using a distinctiveness heuristic and recall-to-reject in order to conclude which stimuli presented are novel and which are known (Moscovitch & Winocur, 1995; Davidson & Glisky, 2002). While pattern separation, recall-to-reject and the distinctiveness heuristic are not the only processes involved in recognition memory, they are fundamental to this thesis which focuses on ageing changes and specifically in false memory: all have been independently shown to be impaired in 11 Examination Number: 7879463 seniors when falsely recognising information in DRM and other tasks (Chan & McDermott, 2007; Moscovitch & Winocur, 1995; Koutstaal, 2006; Toner et al., 2009; Wilson et al., 2005). In addition, due to the relatively automatic role of the hippocampus (and general MTL) within retrieval, binding constituent features together into a coherent memory trace, pattern separation is considered to be relatively quick. On the other hand, the strategic role of the FL appear to be effortful; possibly selfactivated spontaneously or by instruction and consequently slower (for review see Shing et al., 2010; Rugg & Curran, 2007). Evidence for the hypotheses of pattern separation, recall-to-reject and the distinctiveness heuristic can be grouped into two broad theories of decreased recognition performance in senior adults where false recognition increases: the Cognitive Control Hypothesis (West, 1996; 2000; Gallo et al., 2006; Koutstaal, 2006) and the Memory Specificity Hypothesis (Shing, Werkle-Bergner, Li & Lindenberger, 2008; Luo & Craik, 2008). Cognitive Control Hypothesis The Cognitive Control Hypothesis (CCH) is based on research on FL function and proposes that FL age-related decline is the main component to a decline in successful recognition. In false recognition paradigms seniors demonstrate strategic deficits (e.g. in distinctiveness heuristic processes and recall-to-reject processes) and consequently show an over-reliance on familiarity, performing less well than their younger counterparts. Seniors performance on frontal lobe neuropsychological tests should also predict their performance in false memory paradigms: with seniors who perform worse in the neuropsychological test being more likely to be misled by lure items (Dodson & Schacter, 2002; West, 1996; West, 2000; Gallo et al., 2006). Memory Specificity Hypothesis The Memory Specificity Hypothesis (MSH) is based on both the MTL and FL function. It assumes that seniors will perform lower than younger adults with both strategic deficits (due to FL age-related changes) where recall-to-reject and distinctiveness heuristic monitoring are impaired long with a pattern separation deficit (due to MTL changes; Toner et al., 2009; Wilson et al., 2005). For example, while the CCH proposes seniors’ will make false recognition errors based on less specific information 12 Examination Number: 7879463 from an overwhelming feeling of familiarity or the inappropriate use of gist, the MSH instead proposes that seniors will also experience convincing false recollections (Kroll, Knight, Metcalfe, Wolf & Tulving, 1996; Dodson, Bawa & Slotnick, 2007). Seniors will be more likely than young adults to fully recall false memories by combining features of different episodes (items) causing seniors to falsely recall with high confidence. Previous research supports this concept of confident false recollection in seniors (Dodson, Bawa & Krueger, 2007; Lyle & Johnson, 2006; Chua, Schacter & Sperling, 2009). Expected ERP Indices Despite the amount of behavioural evidence, results from behavioural studies are limited in making firm conclusions on the processes underlying recognition memory, whether they be FL-based or MTL-based (Wolk et al., 2009). For example, previous research has shown seniors performing similarly to younger subjects on recognition memory tasks (Yonelinas, 2002). However, alternative cognitive and neural processes may underlie this equivalent performance. By using techniques such as event-related potentials (ERPs), which has excellent temporal resolution, age-related differences in behavioural tasks can be related to neural and cognitive processes (Wolk et al., 2009). Previous studies have frequently identified three event-related potentials (ERP) which in relation to recognition memory: an early (~300-500ms) right frontal effect and mid-frontal effect (FN400) along with a later (~400-800ms) left parietal effect (Curran, 2004; Curran & Cleary, 2003; Wilckens, Tremel, Wolk & Wheeler, 2011; Ally, Simons et al., 2008; Vilberg & Rugg, 2008; 2009; Rugg, Otten & Henson, 2002; for review see Mitchell & Johnson, 2009). All waveforms effects show recollected items (‘old’) demonstrating a more positive deflection than correctly rejected novel items (‘new’), termed old/new effects and are found in a variety of recognition tests that use both word and picture stimuli, including word-plurality, temporal source recognition, word-modality and word-pairs (Yonelinas, 2002; Guerin & Miller, 2009; Donaldson & Rugg, 1998; Wilding, 2000; Trott, Friedman, Ritter, Fabiani & Snodgrass, 1999). The FN400 old/new effect, is similar to established N400 waveforms but has different scalp topography and the right frontal effect are both generally accepted to reflect familiarity processes in recognition memory (Kutas & Federmeier, 2011; Mayes & Roberts, 2001; Paller, Voss & Boehm, 2007; Wilding & Rugg, 1997; Rugg et al., 2002; Hayama, Johnson & Rugg, 2008). This conclusion is supported by the post-retrieval monitoring nature of strategic (i.e. executive) frontal lobe dependent processes that are therefore seen after automatic recollection effects (Shing et al., 2010; 13 Examination Number: 7879463 Mitchell & Johnson, 2009; Rugg & Curran, 2007). Conversely, the left parietal “old/new” effect has been repeatedly associated with successful recollection of materials (Wilding, 2000; Rugg & Curran, 2007; Ally, Simons et al., 2008). While parietal involvement in recollection may be at first thought unusual, parietal regions have long been linked with the MTLs (Wilding & Rugg, 1996; 1997; Rugg, Schloerscheidt & Mark, 1998; for review see Mitchell & Johnson, 2009; Wagner, Shannon, Kahn & Buckner, 2005). In Wilding & Rugg (1996; 1997) and Wilding’s (2000) study’s participants listened to words spoken aloud either in a male or female voice. When participants were tested, presented lists of words in which they made “old” (repeated) or “new” (novel) judgements and when answering “old” making a source (context) judgement on the gender of the speaker, correct source judgments were linked to left parietal old/new effects. Here, correct source judgements would elicit increased left parietal activity in comparison to incorrect source judgements or new judgements. Since correct source judgments are dependent on correct recollection it follows that the left parietal old/new effect can be linked with recollection (Cheng & Rugg, 2004). Other evidence investigating depths of processing, where the meaning of an item has been deepened (i.e. adding ‘meaning’ to an item compared to a simply sensory analysis such as colour, shape), has been linked to left parietal old/new effects. Items, which have added meaning in recollection, show left parietal effects (Gonsalves & Paller, 2000; Rugg, Allen & Birch, 2000). Further evidence from “remember” vs. “know” procedures strengthen this theory. In this case “old” judgements will require participants to make a “remembered/know” decision, when answering “old” they would also state whether the memory was due to a feeling of familiarity (“know”) or they remembered the context surrounding the item (“remembered”). When participants choose “remembered” for an “old” response it would most likely be a recollection response. Correct “remembered” responses consistently reveal left parietal effects (Wolk et al., 2006; Vilberg & Rugg, 2007; 2008; 2009; Rugg et al., 1998; Cheng & Rugg, 2004). Conversely right frontal and FN400 old/new effects have been linked to familiarity responses (Wilckens et al., 2011; Curran & Cleary, 2003; Curran, 2004; for review see Rugg & Curran, 2007; Wolk et al., 2006). Age-related differences between the three ERP effects have only come to light recently and are not consistent. Some studies have shown little to no age-differences between any effects (Li, Morcom & Rugg, 2004; Mark & Rugg, 1998; Gutchess, Leuji & Federmeier, 2007). However, studies that do show age-related differences have shown both abnormal or reduced old/new effects both at frontal and parietal locations (Yonalinas, 2002). A study by Trott, Friedman, Ritter & Fabiani (1997) and Trott, Friedman, Ritter, Fabiani & Snodgrass (1999) found reduced and abnormal frontal activation in 14 Examination Number: 7879463 seniors compared to younger adults linked to unsuccessful retrieval which provides support that frontal lobe dysfunction, specifically strategic deficits, are linked with poorer recognition performance in seniors (for review see Friedman, 2000). Studies also tend to show little age-related differences in left parietal old/new effect (Morcom & Rugg, 2004; Duverne, Motamedinia & Rugg, 2009; Trott et al., 1997; 1999; Friedman, 2000). While all three old/new effects could be analysed in this study, due to time constraints only the left parietal area will be analysed. It’s recollective inclination throughout the literature will form the basis of this thesis’ hypotheses. Thesis Aims, Questions and Expectations The primary objective of this thesis will be to assess whether the Cognitive Control Hypothesis or the Memory Specificity Hypothesis, more accurately reflects the age-related changes that cause decreased successful recollection in seniors along with increased false recognition of lure items. In order to assess this four ERP conditions will be analysed: correctly identified repeated items (Hits, H), correctly rejected lure items (CL), correctly rejected novel items (CN) and false alarms to lure items (FA). This study aims to further investigate age-related decline in recognition by adapting Koutstaal’s (2006) false recognition experiment, which demonstrated high false recognition performances in young and senior adults using picture stimuli. In addition, Ally, Waring et al. (2008) and Morcom & Rugg (2004) found old/new ERP effects were strongest in picture conditions, reflected by an increased behavioural performance for pictures than words. If true, an ERP study based on picture stimuli would be more likely to elicit old/new effects and thus contrast the CCH and MSH hypotheses. Koutstaal’s (2006) study used picture stimuli in a randomly cued recognition test with item-specific and category-based judgements (see figure 1). Participants would implicitly encode pictures by using a size-judgement task by indicating after each picture whether the real-world counterpart of the object would fit into a small box (example box provided at the start), after which participants would make “old” versus “new” recognition judgments on repeated, novel and similar (i.e. lure) pictures. Moreover, participants were instructed to make their “old/new” decisions for each picture based on whether the picture was seen before (item-specific) or another picture from the same category was seen before. Koutstaal (2006) found that, like previous experiments, seniors could move between 15 Examination Number: 7879463 item and category levels, flexibly making use of gist memory. However, seniors’ performance decreased when presented with lure items (i.e. the sitting duck in Figure 1) compared to younger adults. In this study participants will undergo a self-paced, forced-choice picture recognition task using pictures from Koutstaal’s study along with new, original pictures. Representation of Koutstall (2006) paradigm and example stimuli used. Study Phase Stimuli Test Phase Stimulus Correct Type Response Category Repeated Old Item Repeated Old Category Similar Old Item Similar New Category Novel New Item Novel New Instruction Stimuli Figure 1. Schematic representation of study and recognition tasks used by Koutstaal (2006). Participants at study would implicitly memorise items by performing a size-judgment task. At test, previous items would be grouped by either category or single-item: using this indication participants would need to make a judgment on whether the item presented was “old”, “similar” or “new”. NB: Pictures in Koutstaal’s (2006) study were presented in colour. 16 Examination Number: 7879463 In both the CCH and the MSH younger adults will show similar left parietal old/new (recollection) effects for both Hits and CL as they will be using strategic processing (i.e. ‘recall-to-reject’ and the ‘distinctiveness heuristic’) to correctly recall original stimuli (Duverne et al., 2009; Trott et al., 1997; 1999; Friedman, 2000; Yonalinas, 2002). They will also show similar left parietal effects for FA and CR as both conditions use familiarity process: in the CR condition young adults will correctly use familiarity to correctly reject novel items while in the FA condition they will over-rely on this nonspecific feeling of familiarity and incorrectly accept the lure item as “old” (Dodson & Schacter, 2002; Odegard & Lampinen, 2005; Gallo et al., 2006). In this respect: Hits = CL > FA = CR However, in seniors if the CCH holds true they will encounter a failure in strategic processing. Seniors will tend to not recollect lures due to impairments in recall-to-reject and/or the distinctiveness heuristic and thus will not reject lures (Gallo et al., 2006; Dodson & Schacter, 2002). Instead seniors will over-generalise original stimuli and incorrectly accept lures on a familiarity basis. With CR seniors will also not recollect items, instead rejecting new items due to their unfamiliarity. When seniors correctly identify lure stimuli as similar they will not recollect original item, instead rejecting the lure due to its unfamiliarity (Koutstaal, 2006, Odegard & Lampinen, 2005). Consequently a left parietal old/new (recollection) effect will not be seen FA, CR or CL. Such that: Hits > CL = FA = CR If the Memory Specificity Hypothesis is accurate then seniors will encounter a failure in pattern separation where high levels of overlap will occur between items such that the correct recollection of specific, distinctive items will be impaired (Toner et al., 2009; Gilbert et al., 2001). Seniors will instead fully and confidently recollect items, however, they will not recollect them specifically enough to differentiate lure items from repeated items (Chua et al., 2009; Lyle & Johnson, 2006; Dodson et al., 2007). Such that: Hits = CL = FA > CR 17 Examination Number: 7879463 Method Participants Behavioural Pilot Study: Thirteen participants were from two age groups of 18-35 years (N=9) and 62-79 years (N=4). Participants were either university students or retired professionals. Other inclusion criteria were: an ability to use a computer and read English, normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The experimental procedure for the pilot study was approved by the Psychology Research Ethics Committee of the University of Edinburgh (PREC). All participants were naïve to the design and aims of the study. Pilot participants received £6 in payment. ERP Study: Forty-six participants were assessed within the two age groups of 18-35 years (N=22) and 62-79 years (N=24). The younger group had an average age of 25 (standard deviation, SD = 3.66) years and the older group of 71 years (SD = 5.98). A summary of participant characteristics is listed in Table 1. Additional exclusion criteria were applied; right handedness was required and an of health conditions believed to influence their neurological status or future ERP recording. Health problems excluded, included: drug and alcohol abuse, vascular conditions, brain surgery, open heart and bypass surgery, diabetes with complications, head injuries incurring more than an overnight hospital admission or a psychiatric illness requiring repeated treatment. Participants in the ERP study also completed a standardized battery of neuropsychological tests which investigated cognitive functions associated with memory, ageing or both (shown in the neuropsychological tests section). No participants were excluded due to low scores in the neuropsychological tests. However, 22 participants were excluded from the group ERP analysis (seniors N=9; young adults N=15) . This was due to computer malfunction (N=3); bad quality EEG data (N=5) and inadequate behavioural performance which resulted in too few trials in one of the critical experimental conditions (repeated hits; correct novel rejections; correct lure rejection and lure false alarms: N=14). 18 Examination Number: 7879463 The experimental procedure for the ERP study was approved by the Psychology Research Ethics Committee of the University of Edinburgh (PREC). ERP participants were paid £25 and were naïve to the design and aims of the study. Neuropsychological Tests These tests were completed in a separate session within 1 week of the EEG experiment. The neuropsychological tests included: the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein & McHugh, 1975), a brief assessment of general cognitive functioning used a general screening where a minimum of 25/30 was required for the inclusion in the study (Mitchell, 2009); the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) (Zigmond & Snaith, 1983), a basic measure of anxiety and depression; the Trail Making Task (TMT; Reitan, 1958; 1992), a measure of processing speed, mental flexibility and executive function; the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR; Wechsler, 2001), a measure of verbal IQ; two sub-test of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV; Wechsler, 2010) called Digit Span, a measure of attention, and Symbol Coding, a measure of visual perception, speed and executive function. Design The experiment used a within-items and between-subjects design; participants undertaking two explicit forced choice tasks based on picture items developed from Koustaal’s (2006) study. Participants would unintentionally study picture stimuli by answering a size-judgment task. They could answer “yes” or “no” but this factor was disregarded during analysis, as it was irrelevant to our study aims. After studying the items (each shown once) participants would undergo a recognition test and could answer either “old” (repeated), “similar” (lures) or “new” (novel) to picture stimuli. Materials Behavioural Pilot Study: Consent forms were used (Appendix A) along with a personal laptop; an MSI X610 with a 15.6 inch screen size and 1366x768 resolution. Six programmes were designed using Cogent2000 software (version 1.29: see reference Cogent 2000) running on MATLAB (version 7.10.0 R2010a: MATLAB) which both ran the experiment and recorded responses. 19 Examination Number: 7879463 ERP Study: In addition to consent forms (Appendix A), basic information forms were used in the EEG procedure (Appendix B). The basic information forms included the health screening questionnaire and questions on: age, gender, education status, vision status and native language. Two information forms were designed which laid out instructions to the size-judgement task (i.e. study phase) and the memory recognition task (i.e. test phase) (Appendix C). A small shoebox (approx. 8 inches by 16 inches by 5 inches) was used to help participants visualise how large the space was for objects to possibly fit into for the size-judgement task. A hand-chart (Appendix D) was also used to show participants how to place their hands when undertaking the procedure. One programme was developed using the software package Psychtoolbox (version 3.0.9: Psychtoolbox; Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997) running of MATLAB (version 7.10.0 R2010a: MATLAB) which ran the experiment. Procedure The paradigm comprised of two explicit forced choice tasks based on picture stimuli; a study phase size-judgment task and a test phase recognition task where pictures could either be repeated from the study phase, novel or similar items (i.e. “lures”) (see figure 1). The pool of stimuli and expected performance were evaluated in the behavioural pilot study which led to the following procedure being developed (Appendix E). In total 1,014 picture stimuli were used as trials in the EEG experiment; of which 611 were updated or duplicated stimuli from Koutstaal’s (2006) study. These pictures were detailed line drawings or coloured photographs based on common objects and animals. Trials were counterbalanced so that each item (excluding practice stimuli) was presented in repeated, similar and novel conditions at study and test. This resulted in six versions of the programme being developed. Each version contained 324 trials at study and 484 at test. Within both tasks 4 trials were used as fillers. Two fillers were shown at the start and two were shown at the break and if a participant paused the programme. At study 160 items were shown as to-be-repeated trials and 160 were trials shown only once in the experiment. Within the test phase 160 trials were repeated items from the study phase; 160 trials were novel items, and 160 trials were items matched as “similar” to trials at study which were not repeated. Trials which were a matched “similar” pair would have the same title (e.g. light bulb) but 20 Examination Number: 7879463 would be different brands, breed of animal, colours, orientations, one could be a line drawing and the other a photograph, etc. An example of this design can be seen in Figure 2. Design and Picture Stimuli Phase of experiment STUDY TEST Type of Item Stimulus Type Correct Response to Item Old Repeated Old New Lure Similar New Novel New Figure 2. A schematic representation of the study and recognition tasks: Participants were shown items which they would make a size-judgement decision and implicitly study and in a later recognition task (test) they would decide whether items had been repeated, were only similar to original items or completely novel. Twenty items presented as practice for the size-judgement task and 30 items presented for the recognition memory task. Practice programmes were designed to be consistent with the two tasks. At study, 10 items were to-be-repeated and 10 shown only once. At test practice, 10 items were repeated trials from the practice study phase, 10 items were novel trials and 10 items were lure trials. Each trial (including fillers) was interspersed with a fixation point “+”. Trials were self-paced. Participants could respond either with the picture on-screen or after it had been displayed where the black “+” sign show. At study, pictures were displayed for a maximum of 2 seconds with maximum response time where a black “+” sign was displayed for 7 seconds. At test, pictures were displayed for 500ms with a maximum response time of 5 seconds. After a participant response the 21 Examination Number: 7879463 sign started black, turned red and then disappeared to show the next picture. This response-stimulus interval was 1 second with the black “+” was displayed for 100ms changing to the red for 500ms. The background to all screens was white. Text was in Arial font, size 40. All pictures were shown in white box 150mm by 135mm. Pictures also covered approximately the same area inside this box. At the start of the experiment participants underwent EEG preparations after which they were seated 90cm in front of the computer monitor. They were raised so that they would be eye-level with the fixation point to ensure minimal eye movement. Participants were randomly allocated, viewing one of the six versions of the study/test programmes. The experiment was counterbalanced with a approximately equal distribution of participants doing each version. Instructions at the start of the both tasks (Appendix C) informed participants how to complete those tasks. The study phase was first. Here participants were given the “Picture Decision Task” instructions and after reading them, the experimenter also explained the task. The objective was to view the pictures on screen and after each decide whether its real-world counterpart would fit inside the small box presented. Two keys were shown on the keypad “A” and “L”. Both keys were covered by white stickers to differentiate them from the rest of the keypad. Using a hand-chart (Appendix D) participants were shown which key marked “YES” and which key marked “NO”. This was counterbalanced so that approximately half of participants pressed “A” for “YES”; “L” for “NO” and vice versa. All participants were informed they could press the “P” button to pause the programme if they needed a break. Participants were also advised to stay as relaxed as possible while keeping head and eye movements to a minimum and focusing on the picture or “+” when appropriate. Participants were given the opportunity to ask questions after which the box and instruction sheets were removed and the practice study phase programme was started. Once the practice trials were completed participants were again given an opportunity to ask questions before the study phase was started. At the end of the study phase all participants had a 10 to 15 minute break. Participants were next given the instruction sheet for the test phase (Appendix C). The objective was to watch pictures on screen, for each picture they would decide whether it was repeated (“old”), similar or novel (“new”). It was explained that a similar picture would be one in where a picture on the first task had the same name (i.e. dog, pitcher, deck chair), however, it would be different (i.e. coloured differently; be different breeds of animal; different orientations, etc). Participants were shown the hand-chart demonstrating buttons to be used for “OLD”, “SIMILAR” and “NEW” (Appendix D). As at study, response keys were marked with white stickers over the “C”, “B” and “M” 22 Examination Number: 7879463 keys. Keys were counterbalanced so that approximately half of participants would press “C” for old; “B” for similar and “M” for new while the other half would press “M” for old; “B” for similar and “C” for new. Once the recognition task was completed the EEG cap was removed and participants were paid, debriefed and thanked. All participants completed the procedure. ERP Recording and Pre-processing Participants wore the 10-20 electrode cap configuration with 64 channels. Six other channels were placed: one on the horizontal canthus of each eye, and one vertically above and below the right eye, in order to track eye movements and blinks, along with two on the mastoids to be used as a linked reference. The initial EEG recording was done at a ~1kHz sampling rate with a BioSemi amplifier. Low pass filtering was conducted at ~100Hz using a ADC’s decimation filter with a 5th order sinc response and -3dB point at the 1/5th of the sampling rate on raw participant files. After data collection, an average of the two mastoid electrodes served as a reference and epoched data was further filtered with high-pass filtering at 0.3Hz and low-pass filtering at 20Hz. Blink artefacts, vertical and horizontal eye movements were analysed using independent component analysis (ICA) and were automatically removed using ADJUST (Mognon, Jovicich, Bruzzone & Buiatti, 2010). ERPs intervals were initially calculated at a -2000ms to 2500ms with a -2000ms baseline then narrowed to -500ms to 2500ms with a -500 baseline for each subject in each stimuli category (repeated, lure and novel) and response type (old, similar and new). EEG pre-processing, ERP construction and statistical analysis of ERP magnitude and topography was conducted in EEGLAB (Delorme & Makeig, 2004; Delorme, Sejnowski & Makeig, 2007). Before participants started the study and test tasks they were asked to keep head and eye movements to a minimum while focusing on the picture and “+” when they were on screen. An opportunity to ask questions was then given after which the instruction sheets were removed and the practice test phase programme was started. After the practice trials participants were given the opportunity to ask the experimenter any questions they had about the task presented before the test phase programme was started. 23 Examination Number: 7879463 Results Neuropsychological Test Performance The results from the neuropsychological test battery are summarized in Table 1. This table includes p-values from independent t-test using age-group as a between-group factor. Results indicate significant differences in the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading with seniors’ performing at a higher level; the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Digit Span sub-test, forward and backwards, with younger participants performing higher in both parts; the Trail Making Task part A and B with younger participants faster, and the anxiety component of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression scale where younger participants were identified as more anxious than senior participants. Table 1. Participant characteristics and performance on the neuropsychological test battery Younger (n = 17) Senior (n = 16) t-test P -28.08 < .001 9 Female, 7 Male - - 3, 5, 8* - - M SD M SD Age 24.7 3.5 71.7 5.9 Gender Distribution 9 Female, 8 Male 2, 1, 14* Education Distribution MMSE Digit Span 29.1 1.4 28.9 1.8 0.4 n.s. Forwards 7.2 1.1 8.4 0.6 -2.4 < .001 Backwards 5.5 1.6 6.7 1.1 -2.4 < .05 44.8 4 48 2.2 -3.84 < .05 A 25.2 8.6 32.1 9.7 -2.17 < .05 B 49.7 23.3 80.2 41.1 -2.64 < .05 6.4 2.6 5.5 2.4 0.3 n.s. Anxiety 6.2 3.6 3.9 1.9 -2.18 < .05 Depression 2.7 2.1 2.5 2.2 0.35 n.s. WTAR TMT Digit Symbol HADS Mean (M) and standard deviation (SD) for younger and senior adults including significant results. T-test performed at 25 degrees of freedom. Note: n.s. means ‘not significant’ (p > .05) *Numbers correspond to Secondary schooling, 1st Degree, 2nd Degree 24 Examination Number: 7879463 Behavioural Analysis Strategy The dependent measures analysed were: proportion in answering old/similar/new to repeated/lure/novel stimuli and time duration from mean response time (RT) per condition using analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Greenhouse-Geisser as a test of sphericity. Following Toner et al. (2009) differences between the two age groups were assessed on correctly answered trials (H, CL and CR). After this group differences within error responses on lure trials was as FA (old responses for lure trials) is also a critical condition in this thesis’ hypotheses. Measures of accuracy and response bias were also analysed using several well known indexes (Pr, PrSim, Pattern separation, Br) to further understand age-related differences in the ability to discriminate between repeated and novel stimuli. Experimental task performance Accuracy analysis The mean proportion of old, similar and new responses to repeated, lure and novel stimuli for both age-groups are shown in Figure 3. A 2x3 ANOVA was conducted with age-group as a between-group factor and trial type (old, similar and new) of correct responses as a between-subject factor. This revealed a significant main effect of trial type (F(1.8, 55.9) = 36.77, p < .001), a main effect of group (F(1, 31) = 9.01, p < .05), shown in Figure 4. Mean proportion of responses for repeated, lure and novel stimuli Repeated Stimuli Lure Stimuli Novel Stimuli Figure 3. The mean proportion of old, similar and new responses (including standard errors; SE) to repeated, lure and novel stimuli. Note: error bars are +/- 2 SE. 25 Examination Number: 7879463 Accuracy according to proporitons of correct trial type by group Figure 4. The mean proportion of trial type (correct old/similar/new responses) by group. Note: error bars are +/- 2 SE. An independent t-tests based on trial type (old, similar and new) of correct responses by age-group showed significant differences in the similar response condition (t(31) = 3.33, p < .05) with younger participants performing better (M = 0.48, SD = 0.14) than senior participants (M = 0.33, SD = 0.12) and a trend effect in correct old responses (t(31) = 2.02, .05 < p < .1) where younger participants looked to be more accurate (M = 0.71, SD = 0.13) than senior participants (M = 0.62, SD = 0.14). Three follow-up mixed-model ANOVAs were calculated to compare age-group (young vs. senior) by the 3 pairs of trial types (old vs. similar; similar vs. new; old vs. new correct responses). The first 2x2 mixed-model ANOVA factors age-group and trial type (old vs. similar) showed a significant main effect of trial type (F(1, 31) = 84.41, p = .001) and a significant main effect of group (F(1, 31) = 11.23, p = .001). Here, younger adults performed better (Mold = 0.71, SDold = 0.13; Msimilar = 0.48, SDsimilar = 0.14) than senior participants (Mold = 0.61, SDold = 0.14; Msimilar = 0.33, SDsimilar = 0.12) and all participants were more accurate for old items (M = 0.66, SD = 0.14) than for similar items (M = 0.41, SD = 0.15). A 2x2 mixed-model ANOVA with age-group and trial type (simlar vs. new) showed a significant main effect of group (F(1, 31) = 6.29, p = .05), a significant main effect of correct response type (F(1, 31) = 41.89, p = .001) and a significant interaction (F(1, 31) = 4.74, p = .05). Here, younger adults performed better in the lure condition with a higher proportion of correct (similar) responses (Msimilar = 0.48, SDsimilar = 0.14 vs. Msimilar = 0.33, SDsimilar = 0.12) while senior adults performed beter in the novel condition with a higher proportion of correct (new) responses (Mnew = 0.65, SDnew = 0.13 vs. Mnew = 0.64, SDnew = 0.13). All participants performed higher with correct new responses (M = 0.65, SD = 26 Examination Number: 7879463 0.14) than with correct similar responses (M = 0.41, SD = 0.15). A 2x2 ANOVA with age-group as a between-subject factor and correct response type (old vs. new) showed no significant effects. To compare differences in types of error for lure stimuli between age-groups a mixed-model 2x2 ANOVAs was calculated with factors age-group (young vs. senior) and error type (old vs. new responses). Results showed only a significant main effect between group (F(1, 31) = 10.95, p = .05) where youger participants showed fewer errors in both conditions (Mold error = 0.26, SDold error = 0.09; Mnew error = 0.26, SDnew error = 0.13) than senior participants (Mold error = 0.33, SDold error = 0.13; Mnew error = 0.34, SDnew error = 0.13). Response times analysis The mean reaction times (RT) of old, similar and new responses to repeated, lure and novel stimuli for both age-groups can be seen in Figure 5. RT were measured in a 2x3 ANOVA factors age-group (young vs. senior) and trial type (old, similar and new) for correct responses. This revealed no significant main effects. However, independent t-tests of trial type (old, similar and new) in correct responses by age-group showed significant differences for old trials (t(31) = -2.87, p < .05) with younger participants answering faster (M = 1102, SD = 156) than senior participants (M = 1296, SD = 229) and significant age differences for similar trials (t(31) = -3.69, p < .001) with younger participants answering faster (M = 1364, SD = 238) than senior participants (M = 1696, SD = 277). Mean Reaction Time of responses for repeated, lure and novel stimuli Repeated Stimuli Lure Stimuli Novel Stimuli Figure 5. The reaction times for old, similar and new responses (including standard errors; SE) to repeated, lure and novel stimuli. Note: error bars are +/- 2 SE. 27 Examination Number: 7879463 Three follow-up mixed-model ANOVAs were calculated to compare RT differences between agegroup (young vs. senior) by the 3 pairs of trial types (old vs. similar; similar vs. new; old vs. new correct responses). A 2x2 ANOVA with factors of age-group and trial type (old vs. similar) showed a significant main effect of age-group (F(1,31) =14.94, p = .001) and a significant main effect of trial type (F(1,31) = 64.57, p = .001). Here, younger adults responded faster (Mold = 1102, SDold = 156; Msimilar = 1365, SDsimilar = 237) than senior participants (Mold = 1296, SDold = 229; Msimilar = 1695, SDsimilar = 277) with all participants responding faster in the old response condition (M = 1196, SD = 215) than in the similar response condition (M = 1525, SD = 304). A 2x2 ANOVA with factors age-group and trial type (simlar vs. new) showed a significant main effect of group (F(1,31) = 6.67, p = .05), a significant main effect of correct response type (F(1,31) = 32.39, p = .001) and a significant interaction between trial type and group (F(1,31) = 11.76, p = .05). Here, younger adults responded faster (Msimilar = 1365, SDsimilar = 237; Mnew = 1304, SDnew = 289) than senior participants (Msimilar = 1695, SDsimilar = 277; Mnew = 1453, SDnew = 303) with all participants responding faster in the new response condition (M = 1377, SD = 300) than in the similar response condition (M = 1525, SD = 304). A 2x2 ANOVA with factors agegroup as and trial type (old vs. new) showed a significant main effect of trial type (F(1,31) = 19.36, p = .001) where both groups were faster in the old response condition (M = 1196, SD = 215) compared to the new response condition (M = 1377, SD = 300) and a significant group effect (F(1,31) = 4.97, p = .05) where younger participants responded faster (Mold = 1102, SDold = 156; Mnew = 1304, SDnew = 289) than senior participants (Mold = 1296, SDold = 229; Mnew = 1453, SDnew = 303) when correctly answering old and new. To compare differences in types of error for lure stimuli between age-groups a mixed-model 2x2 ANOVAs was calculated with factors age-group (young vs. senior) and error type (old vs. new responses). Results showed no significant effects. Discrimination and response bias analysis Accuracy discrimination between stimuli and response bias was calculated using proportion results for each response condition, displayed in Table 2. To measure discrimination between repeated and novel stimuli the discrimination index Pr (H - old response to novel items) and d’ (Snodgrass & Corwin, 1988; Ally, Simons et al., 2008) was used. An independent t-test with age-group as a between-group factor both Pr and d’ revealed significant differences in age-group with younger adults showing enhanced discrimination compared to seniors. In addition a discrimination index analogous to Pr for H versus FA was calculated (PrSim) along with an index for pattern separation (PS 28 Examination Number: 7879463 where CL - similar response to novel items) was calculated using an independent t-test with agegroup as a between-group factor (Toner et al., 2009). Both PrSim and PS revealed significant agegroup differences with seniors showing poorer ability to discriminate between similar vs. old items and similar vs. new items compared to younger adults. Response bias (Br) was also measured (Snodgrass & Corwin, 1988) using an independent t-test using age-group as a between-group factor. No significant differences was shown between the two age-groups. Table 2. Discrimination and response bias Younger (n = 15) Senior (n = 12) t-test P 0.14 2.69 < .05 1.18 0.64 2.82 < .05 0.15 0.28 0.13 3.37 < .05 0.21 0.14 0.11 0.1 2.42 < .05 0.2 0.13 0.25 0.13 -1.05 n.a. M SD M SD Pr 0.63 0.16 0.49 d’ 2.08 1.12 PrSim 0.45 PS Br Mean (M) and standard deviation (SD) for younger and senior adults including significant result for Pr (H vs. “old” response to novel items), PrSim (Hits vs. FA), PS (CL vs. “similar” response to novel items) and response bias (Br). Within response bias positive values of C indicate a conservative bias while negative values indicate a liberal bias. Note: n.a. means ‘not significant’. T-test performed at 25 degrees of freedom. ERP Analysis Strategy Due to the small group sizes, an exploratory analysis of magnitude over the left parietal scalp (shown in Figure 6) was conducted. This analysis included data from eight electrodes: TP7, CP5, CP3, CP1, P1, P3, P5 and P7. The time interval of 450 to 800ms was chosen based both on previous research (Curran, 2004; Rugg & Curran, 2007) and on visual inspection of the grand mean average waveforms for young adults and seniors (Figure 7 and 8). The four critical conditions which will be compared are: correct hits (H), correct lure rejections (CL), correct new rejections (CR) and false alarms to lure items where participants answer “old” (FA). First, a non-parametric permutation on the mean ERP waveforms was conducted in the time interval -200 to 1500ms (alpha = 0.05,uncorrected for multiple comparisons; Delorme & Makeig, 2004). Further post-hoc pair-wise comparisons (alpha = 0.05, uncorrected for multiple comparisons) using non-parametric permutations were computed to analyse the differences between the critical conditions for each age-group in the 450 to 800ms time interval: H vs. CR; H vs. CL; H vs. FA; CR vs. CL; CR vs. FA and CL vs. FA (Delorme & Makeig, 2004). 29 Examination Number: 7879463 64 electrode cap with electrodes used for analysis of left parietal effects Anterior TP7 CP5 P7 P5 CP3 CP1 P3 P1 Posterior Figure 6. Area of interest on the left parietal scalp consisting of TP7, CP5, CP3, CP1, P1, P3, P5, P7 electrodes. ERP Data Grand average ERP waveforms are shown for each group from the selected electrode sites in Figures 7 and 8. The mean number of trials in each condition yielding each subject’s waveforms is shown in Table 3. Table 3. Trial numbers for each condition used in ERP analysis Younger (n = 15) Senior (n = 9) M Range M Range H 110 55 – 136 98 66 – 128 FA 38 19 – 69 48 21 – 90 CL 71 41 – 105 48 30 – 72 CR 97 46 – 141 96 57 – 144 Mean and ranges for trial numbers in correct old responses (H); lure false alarms where participants answered “old” (FA); lure correct rejections where participants answered “similar” (CL); and new correct rejections conditions. The ERP analysis was guided by both previous research and visual inspection of the mean ERP waveforms in the selected left parietal electrodes beginning with a non-parametric permutation on 30 Examination Number: 7879463 the mean ERP waveforms in the time interval 450 to 800ms (alpha = 0.05), significant areas shown in Figure 7 and 8. Results showed significant differences between the 4 critical conditions in the young participant data (electrodes: TP7, CP3, CP1, P1, P3, P5) and in the senior participant data (electrodes: TP7, CP3, CP1, P5 and P7) at multiple points. This can be seen in Figures 7 and 8. Mean ERP waveforms from left parietal in younger adults CP5 TP7 CR H FA CL CP3 P3 CP5 TP7 P7 CP3 P5 P3 CP1 P1 P5 P7 CP1 P1 P7 0 500 1000 Time from Stimulus Onset (ms) 0 500 1000 Time from Stimulus Onset (ms) 0 500 1000 Time from Stimulus Onset (ms) Figure 7. Mean ERP waveforms (young adults) for the selected electrodes on left parietal scalp varying by critical condition (CR = novel correct rejection; H = correct repeated hits; FA = lure false alarms and CL = correct lure rejection). Note: boxes indicate regions of significant difference (time window 450 to 800ms only). 31 Examination Number: 7879463 Mean ERP waveforms from left parietal in senior adults CP5 TP7 CR H FA P7 CL CP3 P3 CP5 TP7 CP1 P1 P5 P7 P5 CP3 CP1 P3 P1 P7 0 500 1000 Time from Stimulus Onset (ms) 0 500 1000 Time from Stimulus Onset (ms) 0 500 1000 Time from Stimulus Onset (ms) Figure 8. Mean ERP waveforms (senior adults) the selected electrodes in left parietal scalp varying by critical condition (CR = novel correct rejection; H = correct repeated hits; FA = lure false alarms and CL = correct lure rejection). Note: boxes indicate regions of significant difference (time window 450 to 800ms only). To further analyse differences found in general non-parametric permutations, specific post-hoc pairwise comparisons were computed between the critical conditions (H, CL, CR and FA) in the 450 to 800ms time interval for both groups. While effects are weak due to the small number of participants, differences have been noted between critical conditions. In young adults a pair-wise comparison of H vs. CR showed the most significant difference in the time window. Differences were demonstrated in electrodes TP7, CP3, CP1, P1, P3 and P5, specifically in the 600 to 800ms region where H were seen as a more positive-going waveform compared to CR. 32 Examination Number: 7879463 A comparison of H vs. FA showed significant differences in the TP7, CP1 and P1 electrode between 550 and 800ms, however, H in this case showed more negative-going waveforms than FA. A comparison of H vs. CL showed little significant differences with TP7 (450-500ms) and P1 (750800ms). In these cases CL showed more positive-going waveforms. A comparison of CR vs. FA showed significant differences in the P5, P3 and P1 electrode from 450 to 500 and 600 to 750ms. A comparison of CR vs. CL revealed differences in CP5, CP3, P3 and P7 electrodes from 600 to 700ms with CL showing more positive-going waveforms. A comparison of CL vs. FA showed little difference between conditions, CP1 and P1 electrodes showed (450 to 470ms) CL as more positive-going waveforms than FA. Taking into account the greatest differences between conditions, in young adults: FA > H = CL > CR The same method of pair-wise comparisons was conducted for senior participant data. A pair-wise comparison of H vs. CR showed in- significant difference between the two conditions. A comparison of H vs. FA showed small areas of time of significant differences between the two conditions, in TP7, CP3, P1, P3 and P7 where FA waveforms were shown to be more positive-going. A comparison of H vs. CL showed very little differences between the two conditions. A comparison of CR vs. FA revealed little significant differences. In TP7 some differences were found at 530-550 and 680 to 800ms where FA were shown to be more positive-going waveforms. A comparison of CR vs. CL showed significant differences in small areas across the TP7 electrode where CL was more positive-going than CR. A final comparison of CL vs. FA revealed significant differences at CP3, P1, P3 and P5 where FA was more positive-going than CL. Taking into account the greatest differences between conditions, in seniors: FA > H = CL = CR 33 Examination Number: 7879463 Discussion This study explored whether the Cognitive Control Hypothesis (CCH) or the Memory Specificity Hypothesis (MSH) more accurately reflects age-related changes which have demonstrated decreased successful recollection in senior adults compared to younger adults (Roediger & Geraci, 2007; Chan & McDermott, 2007; Aizpurua & Koutstaal, 2010; Schacter et al., 1998). The findings from the neuropsychological tests, behavioural and ERP analyses along with implications are discussed in the following sections. Neuropsychological test findings The results from neuropsychological tests revealed both expected and unexpected performances in the two groups. Significant age-related differences were found in the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (Wechsler, 2001) where seniors’ demonstrated better performances than younger adults. This is an expected result as vocabulary and verbal IQ are not generally shown to decrease with age (Salthouse, 2009; 2010). In addition, scores in the Trail Making Task revealed seniors to be significantly slower than younger adults. This is also expected as decreased processing speed is one of the most commonly viewed age-related effects (Tombaugh, 2004; Salthouse, 2010; Park et al., 2002). Unexpectedly, results from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-IV; Wechsler, 2010) sub-test, Digit Span, showed significant age-related differences in both forward and backwards assessments. In both cases, seniors performed better than younger adults. Conversely, previous research has found younger adults demonstrate greater Digit Span performance than seniors (Park et al., 2002). This suggests that the two age-groups were un-matched when performing the recognition experiment. When evaluating any potential ERP effects, age-group matching would support conclusions (see “General Discussion” section). In addition, results from the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale revealed younger adults as significantly more anxious than seniors. This also reveals a mismatching between groups which may potentially effect ERP conclusions. The differences between anxiety levels may be attributable to the timing of the ERP experiment. The majority of younger participants were MSc students at the University of Edinburgh and at the time of testing will all be in the midst of their final year thesis writing. Consequently, the timing of the experiment may have influenced these scores. 34 Examination Number: 7879463 Behavioural findings This study supports previous behavioural findings in respect to recognition memory and ageing in which seniors are significantly worse at discriminating between repeated and novel stimuli than younger adults, despite similar end performances with respect to repeated and novel stimuli (Yonelinas, 2002; Wolk et al., 2009). In addition, evidence shown here supports the suggestion that seniors are inclined to falsely “remember” events which did not happen, by misleading post-event information (i.e. lure items), compared to younger adults (Roediger & Geraci, 2007; Chan & McDermott, 2007; Koutstaal, 2006; Aizpurua & Koutstaal, 2010). However, seniors were also just as likely to falsely reject lure items as completely “new”. When investigating age-differences in correct responses, results showed younger adults made significantly less errors overall than senior participants. This demonstrated age-related differences in the DRM method and supports previous research (Koutstaal, 2006; Norman & Schacter, 1997; Wiese & Daum, 2006; Luo & Craik, 2008; Chan & McDermott, 2007). Significant differences in Pr and d’ scores (correct old responses vs. incorrectly answering “old” to a novel item) also showed that seniors are unable to distinguish repeated and novel items as well as younger participants, despite performance being affected only in response to lure items (Toner et al., 2009). This suggests that age-related differences is not due to a general decline in recognition memory but rather a specific impairment in monitoring strategies (i.e. recall-to-reject and the distinctiveness heuristic) and/or pattern separation. Senior adults may be over-relying on a non-specific sense of familiarity, inappropriately using gist information and/or be susceptible to a bias in pattern completion rather than pattern separation. This would allow for senior participants to perform proficiently when identifying repeated or novel items but would significantly impair performance in correctly identifying lure items, such as found here. An analysis of the types of error made by participants to lure items (incorrectly answering “old” or “new”) revealed that senior participants made significantly more errors compared to younger adults supporting previous research (Toner et al., 2009; Koutstaal, 2006). However, no significant effects were found for error type. Neither group specifically answered “old” or “new” when making lure errors, instead both occurred. Using the index PrSim, it was found that significant differences occurred between groups in the ability to discriminate between repeated and lure items, with seniors significantly worse than younger adults. This suggests that while seniors show deficit in the ability to discriminate between items, they will both falsely recognise the recognise the item as “old” along with falsely rejecting the item as completely “new”. To analyse if there was group differences in pattern separation an index called PS was used, this compared the proportion of correct 35 Examination Number: 7879463 responses to lures items (CL) to the false alarms where participants answered “old” to lure items (FA). This revealed significant differences between groups in the ability to use pattern separation with seniors displaying significantly decreased pattern separation inefficiency and supports previous findings (Toner et al., 2009; Yassa et al., 2010; Wilson et al., 2005). However, both groups revealed positive means such that both groups showed a bias towards pattern separation rather than pattern completion. This does not support previous research. In addition, an index (Br) was used to assess response bias between groups. This was not significant, meaning age-related differences found between the two groups cannot be simply attributed to different response criteria. When analysing reaction time (RT) data younger participants were shown to be faster in correctly answering repeated and lure items than seniors. A general slowing of processing speed demonstrated in the Trail Making Task may account for some difference, however, if so then agerelated RT differences should be present also in response to novel items. In addition, when noted previous work has generally found significant differences in CR, not in H or CL (Li et al., 2004; Morcom, Li & Rugg, 2007). However, since difference in accuracy scores is only seen in the lure condition, while RT is seen in both lure and repeated conditions, it is likely that any ERP results are not due to disparity in RT. ERP findings Due to the small number of participants whose ERP data could be evaluated an exploratory analysis comparing magnitude differences between each of the critical conditions, within-groups pair-wise comparisons were conducted. The strongest ERP effect was found in younger adults where H>CR (correct repeated items > correct novel rejections). Since H are more likely to be due to true recollection instead of a feeling of familiarity, H>CR demonstrates a connection between recollection and the left parietal scalp as shown in previous research (Yonelinas, 2002; Guerin & Miller, 2009, Wilding, 2000; Trott et al., 1999; Li et al., 2004). Within younger adults, the conclusion H = CL > CR is unsurprising as both H and CL (correctly rejected lures) would show greater left parietal magnitudes than CR as they would depend on recollective processes more than familiarity processes. However, an unexpected conclusion is FA > H = CL > CR. Pair-wise comparisons also demonstrated this on multiple electrodes across the left parietal region (TP7, CP1, P1, P3 and P5). This suggest that FA is associated with recollection more than H/CL. An original assumption in order to fully contrast the Cognitive Control Hypothesis and Memory specificity Hypothesis was that FA would show less left parietal magnitude due to familiarity effects (Duverne et al., 2009; Trott et al., 1997; 1999; Friedman, 2000; Yonalinas, 2002). It also doesn’t support previous findings in younger adults where H > FA (Gutchess et al., 2007). It is possible that with increased ERP data FA magnitudes will not be 36 Examination Number: 7879463 significantly different from H or CL or may be equivalent to CR as was originally predicted. However, the results from the sample gathered shows significant increases. It is possible that FA elicit recollection effects as younger adults demonstrate failures of pattern separation when falsely recognising lure stimuli as “old”. However, PS (the pattern separation index) demonstrated positive results in younger adults indicative of a bias towards pattern separation compared to pattern completion. This suggests that pattern separation where full recollection is made (with an incorrect/less detailed item) cannot be the full explanation of the significant magnitude difference between FA and the other critical conditions. Results from Senior ERP data also revealed unexpected conclusions: FA > H = CL = CR. While direct group comparisons were not conducted there appeared to be a diminished effects as H, CL and CR all equated one another. Whereas diminished ERPs have been found in senior adults compared to younger adults, the majority of research show H, similar to younger adults, to be greater in magnitude over the left parietal scalp than CR (Wolk et al., 2009). In addition, the same question of FA demonstrating significantly greater magnitudes than all other conditions was apparent in senior ERP data. Significant age-related differences were found in the PS index revealing that older adults were less efficient at patter separation than younger adults. However, similar to younger adults, PS demonstrated positive results which suggested a bias towards separation compared to pattern completion. Consequently it is unlikely that a pattern separation deficit can account for the significant differences between FA and the other critical conditions. Comparisons of CR and CL in senior ERP data revealed no significant differences. If true, this would suggest support for the CCH. Accompanying the CCH hypothesis, CR are unlikely to be due to recollection, instead seniors will reject new items due to their unfamiliarity. Also, when seniors correctly identify lure stimuli as similar they will not recollect original item, instead rejecting the lure due to its unfamiliarity (Dodson & Schacter, 2002; West, 1996; West, 2000; Gallo et al., 2006). However, in the CCH hypothesis FA will also not show recollection effects (i.e. H > FA). However, the data suggest that FA > H. This would mean that lures are recollected and so incorrectly answered as “old” while correct repeated items are correctly answered “old” but by using more familiarityrelated processes, similar to correctly rejecting novel items. General Discussion The primary purpose of the experiments was to evaluate which of the two hypotheses (MSH or CCH) more suitably explained age-related differences found in recognition memory. With regard to this aim neither hypothesis at this time can be adopted. There is evidence for inefficient pattern 37 Examination Number: 7879463 separation in seniors adults compared to younger adults within the behavioural data. There is also evidence of diminished ERP effects in seniors (i.e. where H = CL > CR in young, in seniors H = CL = CR) such that seniors correctly reject lures by use of familiarity measures (similar to correctly rejecting novel items) which is in accordance with the CCH, however, not all the assumptions in the CCH are met (i.e. H > CR). In particular, neither hypothesis can accurately explain the conclusion that both age groups show a significant increase magnitude for FA than all other critical conditions (H, CL and CR). Nevertheless, the ERP results posed here are exploratory in nature only. None of the effects shown, including the largest effect of H > CR in younger adults, remains significant once correcting for multiple comparisons. More data will be needed to ensure the conclusions here are genuine results. This should be done both by increasing the number of participants and increasing the number of trials per condition (i.e. >460). While piloting this experiment allowed for an estimation on the number of trials needed, participants in the experiment generally performed better than pilot participants. Consequently trials in the FA condition were particularly low in many participants. In keeping with ethical principles all participants were offered relaxation periods within the recognition test and between the study and test phase of the experiment . In addition, no participants complained of fatigue. In future studies, it is expected that the expansion of study/test items for each condition could easily be achieved. Once a larger sample of data is gathered, statistical group comparisons with ERP effects can be computed along with topographic maps which can investigate potential negative waves from other scalp regions which may be diminishing any ERP effects found here. It is also worth noting when comparing groups (i.e. seniors and younger adults) disparities in performance may impact upon ERP analysis (Li et al., 2004; Morcom et al., 2007; Duverne et al., 2008; Wolk et al., 2009; Friedman, 2003). In this study performance was equated for novel items but not for lure items and a trend effect was evident for repeated items. In addition, significant differences within the Trail Making Task where seniors performed better than younger adults suggest unequal age-matching. As performance declines on a recognition test, there is likely a greater number of H, CR and CL that are correlated with ‘lucky guesses’ (Wolk et al., 2009). This can effectively dilutes response categories meaning that more responses are not due to recollection or familiarity-related processes. This creates an apparent diminution of age-related retrieval effects (Li et al., 2004; Morcom et al., 2007; Wolk et al., 2009; Friedman, 2003). This may be relevant for the decreased left parietal effects shown in the senior group of this study (i.e. where H = CL = CR). 38 Examination Number: 7879463 Additionally, there is an important difference in the method between this study and similar studies which demonstrate left parietal old/new effects. This study investigates age-related differences using an item recognition task while the majority of previous studies involve source memory (Trott et al., 1997; 1999; Li et al., 2004; Morcom et al., 2007; Mark & Rugg, 1998). Due to the method of source memory tasks, H will be more select as participants will have to answer correctly on the item and its surrounding context. This means that compared to item recognition tasks such as this where participants simply answer correctly on the item, source memory tasks H conditions will be correlated more with true recollection, not guesses (Wolk et al., 2009). Furthermore, source memory tasks are more likely to show significant left parietal old/new effects. Consequently the diminished ERP effects found in the senior group in this study (i.e. where H = CL = CR) could be partially due to a different methodology. Moreover, other item recognition studies have also show diminished ERP effects in comparison to younger adults (Ally, Simons et al., 2008; Swick & Knight, 1997; Wolk et al., 2009; Walhovd et al., 2006). This suggests that in item recognition tasks, retrieval may associated with less detailed recollection and familiarity-processes (Wolk et al., 2009). Despite the limitations in the ERP data, the design of the experiment worked well. Behavioural data clearly demonstrated that seniors are significantly worse at differentiating between repeated and novel stimuli compared to younger adults. Seniors were also significantly worse at differentiating repeated and lure stimuli and demonstrated significantly greater inefficiency when performing pattern separation. Thereby replicating and extending previous work in this field (Roediger & Geraci, 2007; Chan & McDermott, 2007; Koutstaal, 2006; Aizpurua & Koutstaal, 2010; Toner et al., 2009; Yassa et al., 2010; Wilson et al., 2005). The design of the experiment could also easily be repeated. In future experiments it would be worth ensuring greater generalisbility by using a wider sample of the population. All participants in this study were students and recently graduates from the University of Edinburgh or otherwise from volunteer groups within the University. Further replication of our work with a wider, larger sample of the population with the application of added trial numbers would help enhance our results and findings. It would also be beneficial to investigate possible frontal ERP effects such as the FN400 and right frontal effect. This could not be done within this study due to time constraints, however, further analyses can be done using this study with data collected from frontally located electrodes. Finally, using the conclusions derived in this study allows for the development of better techniques for investigating age-related decline in recognition and increases in false memory. False recognition can have important implications in everyday life as in eyewitness identifications, where one persons actions can be mistakenly attributed to an innocent party (Schacter, 1999). One famous account of 39 Examination Number: 7879463 this involved a well-respected psychologist called Donald Thompson who was accused of rape on the basis of a detailed eyewitness testimony from the victim. It later transpired that the victim had been watching a programme where Thompson had been interviewed before the event occurred and had mistakenly replaced the rapists image with the image of Thompson from the television. While Thomson was fortunate to have airtight alibi, this will not always be the case (Thompson, 1988; Schacter, 1999). If false recollection is caused by pattern separation, the MSH suggests that seniors will recall the erroneous information with high confidence (Dodson et al., 2007; Lyle & Johnson, 2006; Chua et al., 2009). If true, false recollections will be even more problematic as high confidence can influence others believing the erroneous information, as shown in mock-juries (Cutler, Penrod & Dexter, 1990). In addition, high rates of “gist-based” processing suggested by the CCH may over the course of time may cumulatively make it more difficult to access specific information and compound problems in instances such as depression (Koutstaal & Cavendish, 2006; Williams & Dritschel, 1992; Tun et al., 2003). Furthermore, research into the processes underlying false recognition will support dementia research which current goal is to search for early markers for disease onset and to dedifferentiate these markers with normal, healthy ageing (Toner et al., 2009; Yassa et al., 2010). 40 Examination Number: 7879463 Conclusion This study provides behavioural evidence that seniors discriminate poorly between repeated and novel stimuli compared to younger adults including being more likely to mislabel post-event information as “old”. An exploratory analysis of left parietal old/new effects from collected ERP data revealed unexpected conclusions where false alarms to lure stimuli showed significantly greater magnitudes compared to all other critical conditions in both age groups. This is not supported by previous research. However, younger participants showed greater correct responses to old items with greater magnitudes than correctly rejected new items which is supported by previous research. All ERP analysis are tentative and will need more data in both groups to statistically analyse potential left parietal effects with confidence along with calculating other necessary analyses such as between-group ANOVA and topographical scalp maps. While neither hypothesis could be accepted based on the information gathered, the study design worked well and could easily be repeated. Future research replicating this study and combining it with other behavioural, ERP and fMRI results with undoubtedly yield a better understanding of the underlying processes involved cognitive ageing. 41 Examination Number: 7879463 References Aizpurua, A. & Koutstaal, W. (2010). Aging and flexible remembering: contributions of conceptual span, fluid intelligence, and frontal functioning. Psychology and Aging, 25(1), 193-207. Aizpurua, A., Garcia-Bajos, E. & Migueles, M. (2009). Memory for actions of an event: older and younger adults compared. The Journal of General Psychology, 136(4), 1-14. Ally, B.A., Simons, J.S., McKeever, J.D., Peers, P.V. & Budson, A.E. (2008). Parietal contributions to recollection: electropsychological evidence from aging and patients with parietal lesions. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 1800-1812. Ally, B.A., Waring, J.D., Beth, E.H., McKeever, J.D., Milberg, W.P. & Budson, A.E. (2008). Aging memory for pictures: using high-density event-related potentials to understand the effect of aging on the picture superiority effect. Neuropsychologia, 46(2), 679-689. Baddeley, A.D. (2001). The concept of episodic memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 356(1413), 1345–1350. Bakker, A., Kirwan, C.B., Miller, M. & Stark, C.E.L. (2008). Pattern separation in the human hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus. Science, 319(5870), 1640-1642. Bayen, U.J. (1999). Aging and source monitoring of characters in literary texts. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 6, 187-200. Blatter, D.D., Bigler, E.D., Gale, S.D., Johnson, S.C., Anderson, C.V., Burnett, B.M., Parker, N., Kurth, S. & Horn, S.D. (1995). Quantitative volumetric analysis of brain MR: normative database spanning 5 decades of life. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 16(2), 241-251. Brainard, D.H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 443-446. Butler, K.M., McDaniel, M.A., Dornburg, C.C., Prince, A.L. & Roediger, H.L. (2004). Age differences in veridical and false recall are not inevitable: the role of frontal lobe function. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(5), 921-925. 42 Examination Number: 7879463 Cabeza, R. & Nyberg, L. (1997). Imaging Cognition: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI tudies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(1), 1-26. Cabeza, R. & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging Cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI tudies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(1), 1-47. Castel, A.D. & Craik, F.I.M. (2003). The effects of aging and divided attention on memory for item and associative information. Psychology and Aging, 18(4), 873-885. Chan, J.C.K. & McDermott, K.B. (2007). The effects of frontal lobe functioning age on veridical and false recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(4), 606-611. Chau, E.F., Schacter, D.L. & Sperling, R.A. (2009). Neural basis for recognition confidence in younger and older adults. Psychological Aging, 24(1), 139-153. Cheng, S-k. & Rugg, M.D. (2004). A event-potential study of two kinds of source judgement error. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 113-127. Cogent 2000 (Version 1.29) [Computer software developed by the Cogent 2000 team at FIL, the ICN and Cogent Graphics developed by John Romaya at the LON at the Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience]. Retrieved from http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent.php Corkin, S. (2002). What’s new with the amnesic patient H.M.? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 153160. Curran, T. (2004). Effects of attention and confidence on the hypothesized ERP correlates of recollection and familiarity. Neuropsychologica, 42, 1088-1106. Curran, T. & Cleary, A.M. (2003). Using ERPs to dissociate recollection from familiarity in picture recognition. Cognitive Brain Research, 15, 191-205. Cutler, B.L., Penrod, S.D. & Dexter, H.R. (1990). Juror sensitivity to eyewitness identification evidence. Law and Human Behaviour, 14(2), 185-191. 43 Examination Number: 7879463 Davidson, P.R. & Glisky, E.L. (2002). Neuropsychological correlates of recollection and familiarity in normal aging. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioural Neuroscience, 2(2), 174-186. Delorme, A. & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including an independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134, 921. Delorme, A., Sejnowski, T. & Makeig, S. (2007). Enhanced detection of artifacts in EEG data using higer-order statistics and independent component analysis. NeuroImage, 34, 1443-1449. Dennis, N.A. & Cabeza, R. (2008). Neuroimaging of healthy cognitive aging. In F.I.M. Craik & T.A. Salthouse (Eds.) The Handbook of Aging and Cognition (3rd Ed.), (pp. 1-54). New York, USA: Psychology Press. Dickerson, B.C. & Eichenbaum, H. (2009). The episodic memory system: neurocircuitry and disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 86-104. Dodson, C.S., Bawa, S. & Krueger, L.E. (2007). Aging, metamemory, and high-confidence errors: a misrecollection account. Psychology & Aging, 22(1), 122-133. Dodson, C.S., Bawa, S. & Slotnick, S.D. (2007). Aging, source memory, and misrecollections. Journal of Experimetal Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(1), 169-181. Dodson, C.S. & Schacter, D.L. (2002). When false recognition meets metacognition: the distinctiveness heuristic. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 782-803. Donaldson, D.I. & Rugg, M.D. (1998). Recognition memory for new associations: electrophysiological evidence for the role of recollection. Neuropsychologia, 36(5), 377-395. Dudai, Y. (2004). The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 51-86. 44 Examination Number: 7879463 Duverne, S., Habibi, A. & Rugg, M.D. (2008). Regional specificity of age effects on the neural correlates of episodic retrieval. Neurobiology of Aging, 29, 1902-1916. Durverne, S., Motamedinia, S. & Rugg, M.D. (2009). Effects of age on the neural correlates of retrieval cue processing are modulated by task demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(1), 117. Eichenbaum, H., Yonelinas, A.R. & Ranganath, C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 30, 123-152. Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E. & McHugh, P.R. (1975). Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198. Friedman, D. (2000). Event-related brain potential investigations of memory and aging. Biological Psychology, 54, 175-206. Friedman, D. (2003). Cognition and aging: a highly selective overview of event-related potential (ERP) data. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(5), 702-720. Gallo, D.A. (2004). Using recall to reduce false recognition: diagnostic and disqualifying monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30(1), 120-128. Gallo, D.A., Bell, D.M., Beier, J.S. & Schacter, D.L. (2006). Two types of recollection-based monitoring in younger and older adults: recall-to-reject and the distinctiveness heuristic. Memory, 14(6), 730741. Gilbert, P.E., Kesner, R.P. & Lee, I. (2001). Dissociating hippocampal subregions: a double dissociation between dentate gyrus and CA1. Hippocampus, 11, 626-636. Gonsalves, B. & Paller, K.A. (2000). Brain potentials associated with recollective processing of spoken words. Memory & Cognition, 28(3), 321-330. Grady, C.L. & Craik, F.I.M. (2000). Changes in memory processing with age. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 224-231. 45 Examination Number: 7879463 Gronlund, D., Carlson, C. & Tower, D. (2007). Episodic memory. In Durso, F. (Eds.), Handbook of applied cognition (pp. 111-136). John Wiley & Sons: Chiechester, Sussex. Guerin, S.A. & Miller, M.B. (2009). Lateralization of the parietal old/new effect: an event-related fMRI study comparing recognition memory for words and faces. Neuroimage, 44(1), 232-242. Gutchess, A.H., Leuji, Y. & Federmeier, K.D. (2007). Event-related potentials reveal age differences in the encoding and recognition of scenes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(7), 1089-1103. Gutchess, A.H., Welsh, R.C., Hedden, T., Bangert, A., Minear, M., Liu, L.L. & Park, D.C. (2005). Aging and the neural correlates of successful picture encoding: frontal activations compensate for decreased medial-temporal activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(1), 84-96. Hay, J.F. & Jacoby, L.L. (1999). Seperating habit and recollection in young and older adults: effects of elaborative processing and distinctiveness. Psychology and Aging, 14(1), 122-134. Hayama, H.R., Johnson, J.D. & Rugg, M.D. (2008). The relationship between the right frontal old/new effect and post-retrieval monitoring: specific or non-specific? Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1211-1223. Kapur, N., Friston, K.J., Frith, C.D. & Frackowiak, R.S.J. (1995). Activation of the human hippocampal formation during memory for faces: a PET study. Cortex, 31(1), 99-108. Kesner, R.P. (2007). A behavioural analysis of dentate gyrus function. Progress in Brain Research, 163, 567-576. Koutstaal, W. (2006). Flexible remembering. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(1), 84-91. Koutstaal, W. & Cavendish, M. (2006). Using what we know: consequences of intentionally retrieving gist versus item-specific information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 32(4), 778-791. Koutstaal, W. & Schacter, D.L. (1997). Gist-based false recognition of pictures of older and younger adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(4), 555-583. 46 Examination Number: 7879463 Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D.L., Galluccio, L. & Stofer, K.A. (1999). Reducing gist-based false recognition in older adults: encoding and retrieval manipulations. Psychology & Aging, 14(2), 220-237. Kutas, M. & Federmeier, K.D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621-647. Kroll, N.E.A., Knight, R.T., Metcalfe, J., Wolf, E.S. & Tulving, E. (1996). Cohesion failure as a source of memory illusions. Journal of Memory & Language, 35, 176-196. Li, J., Morcom, A.M. & Rugg, M.D. (2004). The effects of age on the neural correlates of successful episodic retrieval: an ERP study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioural Neuroscience, 4(3), 279-293. Luo, L. & Craik, F.I.M. (2008). Aging and memory: a cognitive approach. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 53(6), 346-353. Lyle, K.B. & Johnson, M.K. (2006). Importing perceived features into false memories. Memory, 14(2), 197-213. Mark, R.E. & Rugg, M.D. (1998). Age effects on brain activity associated with episodic memory retrieval: an electrophysiological study. Brian, 121, 861-873. MATLAB & Simulink Student Version (Version 7.10.0 R2010a) [Software developed by MathWorks] Retrieved from http://www.mathworks.com/products/matlab/?s_cid=global_nav Mayes, A.R. & Roberts, N. (2001). Theories of episodic memory. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 356(1413), 1395-1408. Mecklinger, A. & Jäger, T. (2009). Episodic memory storage and retrieval: insights from electrophysiological measures. In F. Rösler, C. Ranganath, B. Röder, & R.H. Kluwe (Eds.), Neuroimaging in human memory: Linking cognitive processes to neural systems (pp. 357-382). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. 47 Examination Number: 7879463 Mitchell, A.J. (2009). A meta-analysis of the accuracy of the mini-mental state examination in the detection of dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, 411-431. Mitchell, K.J. & Johnson, M.K. (2009). Source monitoring 15 years later: what have we learned from fMRI about the neural mechanisms of source memory? Psychological Bulletin, 135(4), 638-677. Mitchell, K.J. & Johnson, M.K. & Mather, M. (2003). Source monitoring and suggestibility to misinformation: adult age-related differences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 107-119. Mognon, A., Jovicich, J., Bruzzone, L. & Buiatti, M. (2010). ADJUST: An automatic EEG artefact detector based on the joint use of spatial and temporal features. Psychophysiology, 48(2), 229-240. Morcom, A.M., Li, J. & Rugg, M.D. (2007). Age effects on the neural correlated of episodic retrieval: increased cortical recruitment with matched performance. Cerebral Cortex, 17(11), 2491-2506. Morcom, A.M. & Rugg, M.D. (2004). Effects of age on retrieval cue processing as revealed by ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1525-1542. Moscovitch, M. & Winocur, G. (1995). Frontal lobes, memory, and aging. Annuals of the New York Academy of Science, 769, 119-150. Multhaup, K.S. (1995). Aging, source, and decision criteria: when false fame errors do and do not occur. Psychology and Aging, 10(3), 492-497. Multhaup, K.S., De Leonardis, D.M. & Johnson, M.K. (1999). Source memory and eyewitness suggestibility in older adults. The Journal of General Psychology, 126(1), 74-84. Myers, C.E. & Scharfman, H.E. (2009). A role for hilar cells in pattern separation in the dentate gyrus: a computational approach. Hippocampus, 19(4), 321-337. Nadel, L. & Moscovitch, M. (1997). Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 217-227. 48 Examination Number: 7879463 Naveh-Benjamin, M., Brac, T.K. & Oded, L. (2007). The associative memory deficit of older adults: the role of strategy utilization. Psychology & Aging, 22(1), 202-208. Newson, R.S. & Kemps, E.B. (2006). The nature of subjective cognitive complaints of older adults. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 63(2), 139-151. Norman, K.A. & Schacter, D.L. (1997). False recognition in younger and older adults: exploring the characteristics of illusory memories. Memory & Cognition, 25(6), 838-848. Odegard, T.N. & Lampinen, J.M. (2005). Recollection rejection: gist cuing of verbatim memory. Memory & Cognition, 33(8), 1422-1430. Paller, K.A., Voss, J.L. & Boehm, S.G. (2007). Validating neural correlated of familiarity. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 11(6), 243-250. Pelli, D.G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437-442. Park, D.C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N.S., Smith, A.D. & Smith, P.K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 299-320. Persson, J., Nyberg, L., Lind, J., Larsson, A., Nilsson, L.-G., Ingvar, M. & Buckner, R.L. (2006). Structure-function correlates of cognitive decline in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 907-915. Psychtoolbox (Version 3.0.9) [The Psychophysics Toolbox is a collection of MATLAB functions allowing for greater control of your computer to test observes with stimuli]. Retrieved from http://psychtoolbox.org/wikka.php?wakka=HomePage Raz, N., Gunning, F.M., Head, D., Dupuis, J.H., McQuain, J., Briggs, S.D., Loken, W.J., Thornton, A.E. & Acker, J.D. (1997). Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cerebral Cortex, 7, 268-282. 49 Examination Number: 7879463 Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K.M., Kennedy, K.M., Head, D., Williamson, A., Dahle, C., Gerstorf, D., Acker, J.D. (2005). Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1676-1689. Reitan, R.M. (1958). Validity of the trail making test as an indication of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271-276. Reitan, R.M. (1992). Trail Making Test: Manual for administration and scoring. San Antonio, USA: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory. Roediger, H.L. & McDermott, K.B. (1995). Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 803814. Roediger, H.L. & Geraci, L. (2007). Aging and the misinformation effect: a neuropsychological analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(2), 321-334. Rolls, E.T. (1996). A theory of hippocampal function in memory. Hippocampus, 6, 601-320. Rugg, M.D., Allan, K. & Birch, C.S. (2000). Electrophysiological evidence for the modulation of retrieval orientation by depth of study processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 664-678. Rugg, M.D. & Curran, T. (2007). Event-related potentials and recognition memory. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 11(6), 251-257. Rugg, M.D., Fletcher, P.C., Frith, C.D., Frackowiak, R.S.J. & Dolan, R.J. (1997). Brain regions supporting intentional and incidental memory: a PET study. NeuroReport, 8, 1283-1287. Rugg, M.D., Otten, L.J. & Henson, N.A. (2002). The neural basis of episodic memory: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 357, 1097-1110. Rugg, M.D., Schloerscheidt, A.M. & Mark, R.E. (1998). An electropsysiological comparison of two indices of recollection. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 47-69. 50 Examination Number: 7879463 Salthouse, T.A. (2009). When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiological Aging, 30(4), 507-514. Salthouse, T.A. (2010). Selective review of cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 754-730. Schacter, D.L. (1999). The seven sins of memory: insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 54(3), 182-203. Schacter, D.L., Norman, K.A. & Koutstaal, W. (1998). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 289-318. Sherry, D.F. & Schacter, D.L. (1987). The evolution of multiple memory systems. Psychological Review, 94, 439-454. Shing, Y.L., Werkle-Bergner, M.W., Brehmer, Y., Müller, V., Li, S. & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Episodic memory across the lifespan: the contributions of associative and strategic components. Neuroscience & Biobehavioural Reviews, 34, 1080-1091. Shing, Y.L., Werkle-Bergner, M., Li, S.-C. & Lindenberger, U. (2008). Associative and strategic components of episodic memory: a life-span dissociation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(3), 495-513. Snodgrass, J.G. & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117(1), 34-50. Sowell, E.R., Peterson, B.S., Thompson, P.M., Welcome, S.E., Henkenius, A.I., Toga, A.W. (2003). Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nature Neuroscience, 6(3), 309-315. Squire, L.R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, mokeys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195-231. 51 Examination Number: 7879463 Squire, L.R. (1995). Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a neurobiological perspective. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5, 169-177. Squire, L.R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82, 171-177. Squire, L.R., Wixted, J.T. & Clark, R.E. (2007). Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: a new perspective. Nature Reviews of Neuroscience, 8(11), 872-883. Squire, L.R. & Zola-Morgan, S. (1991). The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science, 253(5026), 1380-1386. Suzuki, M., Johnson, J.D. & Rugg, M.D. (2011a). Recollection-related hippocampal activity during continuous recognition: a high-resolution fMRI study. Hippocampus, 21, 575-583. Suzuki, M., Johnson, J.D. & Rugg, M.D. (2011b). Decrements in hippocampal activity with item repetition during continuous recognition: an fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(6), 1522-1532. Swick, D. & Knight, R.T. (1997). Event-related potentials differentiate the effects of aging on word and nonword repetition in explicit and implicit memory tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(1), 123-142. Tombaugh, T.N. (2004). Trail making test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 203-214. Thompson, D.M. (1988). Context and false recognition. In G.M. Davies & D.M. Thompson (Eds.). Memory in Context: Context in Memory (pp. 285-304). England, UK: Wiley. Toner, C.K., Pirogovsky, E., Kirwan, B. & Gilbert, P.E. (2009). Visual object pattern separation deficits in nondemented older adults. Learning & Memory, 16, 338-342. Trott, C.T., Friedman, D., Ritter, W. & Fabiani, W. (1997) Item and source memory: differential age effects revealed by event-related potentials. NeuroReport, 8(15), 3373-3378. 52 Examination Number: 7879463 Trott, C.T., Friedman, D., Ritter, W., Fabiani, W. & Snodgrass, J.G. (1999). Episodic priming and memory for temporal source: event-related potentials reveal age-related differences in prefrontal functioning. Psychology and Aging, 14(3), 390-413. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381-403). New York, USA: Academic Press. Tulving, E. (1993). What is episodic memory? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2(3), 67-70. Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic Memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, pp1-25. Tun, P.A., Wingfield, A., Rosen, M.J. & Blanchard, L. (1998). Response latencies for false memories: gist-based processing in normal aging. Psychology & Aging, 13(2), 230-241. Vilberg, K.L. & Rugg, M.D. (2007). Dissociation of the neural correlates of recognition memory according to familiarity, recollection, and amount of recollected information. Neuropsychologia, 45(10), 2216-2225. Vilberg, K.L. & Rugg, M.D. (2008). Memory retrieval and the parietal cortex: a review of evidence from a dual-process perspective. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 1787-1799. Vilberg, K.L. & Rugg, M.D. (2009). Functional significance of retrieval-related activity in lateral parietal cortex: evidence from fMRI and ERPs. Human Brain Mapping, 30(5), 1490-1501. Walhovd, K.B., Fjell, A.M., Reinvang, I., Lundervold, A., Fischl, B., Quinn, B.T., Makris, N. & Dale, A.M. (2006). The frontal and structural significance of the frontal shift in the old/new ERP effect. Brain Research, 1081, 156-170. Wagner, A.D., Shannon, B.J., Kahn, I. & Buckner, R.L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 445-453. Wechsler, D. (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR). San Antonio, USA: The Psychological Corporation. 53 Examination Number: 7879463 Wechsler, D. (2010). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale: Technical and interpretive manual (3rd Ed.). San Diego, USA: Academic Press. West, R. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory of cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120(2), 272-292. West, R. (2000). In defense of the frontal lobe hypothesis of cognitive aging. Journal off the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 727-729. West, R. & Schwarb, H. (2006). The influence of aging and frontal function on the neural correlates of regulative and evaluative aspects of cognitive control. Neuropsychology, 20(4), 468-481. Wheeler, M., Struss, D. & Tulving, E. (1997). Towards a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 121(3), 331-354. Wilding, E.L. (2000). In what way does the parietal ERP old/new effect index recollection? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 35, 81-87. Wilding, E.L. & Rugg, M.D. (1996). An event-related potential study of recognition memory with and without retrieval of source. Brain, 119, 889-905. Wilding, E.L. & Rugg, M.D. (1997). An event-related potential study of memory for words spoken aloud or heard. Neuropsychologia, 35(9), 1185-1195. Williams, J.M.G. & Dritschel, B.H. (1992). Categoric and extended autobiographical memories. In M.A. Conway, D.C. Rubin, H. Spinnler & W.A. Wagenaar (Eds.). Theoretical Perspectives on Autobiographical Memory (pp. 391-410). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic. Wilson, I.A., Ikonen, S., Gallagher, M., Eichenbaum, H. & Tanila, H. (2005). Age-associated alterations of hippocampal place cells are subregion specific. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(29), 6877-6886. Wilckens, K.A., Tremel, J.J., Wolk, D.A. & Wheeler, M.E. (2011). Effects of task-set adoption on ERP correlates of controlled and automatic recognition memory. NeuroImage, 55(3), 1384-1392. 54 Examination Number: 7879463 Wiese, H. & Daum, I. (2006). Frontal positivity discriminates true from false recognition. Brain Research, 1075, 183-192. Wolk, D.A., Sen, N.M., Chong, H., Riis, J.L., McGinnis, S.M., Holcomb, P.J. & Daffner, K.R. (2009). ERP correlates of item recognition memory: effects of age and performance. Brain Research, 23, 218231. Wolk, D.A., Schacter, D.L., Lygizos, M., Sen, M., Holcomb, P.J., Daffner, K.R. & Budson, A.E. (2006). ERP correlates of recognition memory: effects of retention interval and false alarms. Brain Research, 1096, 148-162. Yassa, M.A., Lacy, J.W., Stark, S.M., Albert, M.S., Gallagher, M. & Stark, C.E.L (2010). Pattern separation deficits associated with increased hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus activity in nondemented older adults. Hippocampus, 21(9), 968-679. Yonelinas, A.P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: a review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441-517. Zigmond, A.S. & Sniath, R.P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67, 361-370. 55 Examination Number: 7879463 Appendix A – Consent forms for behavioural piloting and the ERP study BEHAVIOURAL EXPERIMENT Dear participant, Please read the following information carefully. Invitation To Take Part In A Research Study You are being asked to take part in a research study in which we will examine differences between younger and older adults in their ability to make decisions about and recognise pictures. Time Commitments This particular experiment will require up to 1 hour to complete (including reading of the instructions). Right To Withdraw During the experiment you have the right to withdraw at any time; you are under no obligation to continue if you do not wish to and it does not matter if you have already started the tasks. If you wish to withdraw we will immediately stop the procedure and any data that you have produced up to that moment will be destroyed. Risks There are no known risks for you in this study. This experiment conforms to the British Psychological Society guidelines and ethical constraints for human experimental psychology. Confidentially/Anonymity The data we collect does not contain any personal information, and there is no way to link your name to the data you provided. Cost, Reimbursement And Compensation You will be paid £6 for this experiment. 56 Examination Number: 7879463 For Further Information About This Research: If there is anything not covered on this sheet which you would like to know, please do not hesitate to contact us. We would be more than happy to discuss it further with you. To participate or for more information, please Lindsey Robb Dr. Alexa Morcom Psychology Department University of Edinburgh 7 George Square Edinburgh, EH8 9JZ email Lindsey Robb at s0678794@sms.ed.ac.uk or phone on 07795576194. Alternatively, if it is not possible to contact Lindsey Robb, you can contact: As an informed participant in this experiment, I understand that: Dr. Alexa Morcom (Principal Investigator & 1. 2. 3. supervisor) My participation is voluntary and I may cease to take part in this experiment at any time, without penalty. Email: alexa.morcom@ed.ac.uk I am aware of what my participation involves. Tel:participation 0131 651 1907 There are no risks involved in the of this study. 4. This is a research project, not an assessment of me individually, and I will not be identified in any results. 5. All my questions about the study have been satisfactorily answered. I have read and understood the above information and give consent to participate. Participant’s Signature:___________________________ Date:__________ I have explained the above and answered any questions asked by the participant. Researcher’s Signature:__________________________ Date:__________ Thank you for agreeing to participate in this study. 57 Examination Number: 7879463 EEG (Electroencephalogram) EXPERIMENT Dear participant, Please read the following information carefully. Invitation To Take Part In A Research Study You are being asked to take part in a research study in which we will examine differences between younger and older adults in their ability to make decisions about and recognise pictures. EEG (Electroencephalogram) reaction times and accuracy will be recorded during the experiment. Time Commitments This particular experiment will require around up to 2 hours to complete (including reading of the instructions and cap preparation). Right To Withdraw During the experiment you have the right to withdraw at any time; you are under no obligation to continue if you do not wish to and it does not matter if you have already started the tasks. If you wish to withdraw we will immediately stop the procedure and any data that you have produced up to that moment will be destroyed. Risks There are no known risks for you in this study. This experiment conforms to the British Psychological Society guidelines and ethical constraints for human experimental psychology. EEG Recording The EEG recording involves you to wear an elastic electrode cap so we can record the electrical activity of your brain. The gel applied to the electrodes means that you may want to wash your hair afterwards: we have shower facilities for this. We will also ask you to sit as still as possible and avoid eye movements during the EEG recording. These help us to record good quality data. Before we start EEG recording, you will have time to familiarise you with each procedure. Confidentially/Anonymity The data we collect does not contain any personal information, and there is no way to link your name to the data you provided. Cost, Reimbursement And Compensation You will be paid £25 for this experiment. 58 Examination Number: 7879463 For Further Information About This Research: If there is anything not covered on this sheet which you would like to know, please do not hesitate to contact us. We would be more than happy to discuss it further with you. To participate or for more information, please Lindsey Robb Dr. Alexa Morcom Psychology Department University of Edinburgh 7 George Square Edinburgh, EH8 9JZ email Lindsey Robb at s0678794@sms.ed.ac.uk or phone on 07795576194. Alternatively, if it is not possible to contact Lindsey Robb, you can contact: As an informed participant in this experiment, I understand that: Dr. Alexa Morcom (Principal Investigator & 1. 2. 3. supervisor) My participation is voluntary and I may cease to take part in this experiment at any time, without penalty. Email: alexa.morcom@ed.ac.uk I am aware of what my participation involves. Tel:participation 0131 651 1907 There are no risks involved in the of this study. 4. This is a research project, not an assessment of me individually, and I will not be identified in any results. 5. All my questions about the study have been satisfactorily answered. I have read and understood the above information and give consent to participate. Participant’s Signature:___________________________ Date:__________ I have explained the above and answered any questions asked by the participant. Researcher’s Signature:__________________________ Date:__________ Thank you for agreeing to participate in this study. 59 Examination Number: 7879463 Appendix B – Basic Information form EEG (Electroencephalogram) EXPERIMENT Basic Information Participant Number: ........................ Gender: Male ........................ Female ........................ Age: ........................ Handedness (for writing): ........................ Years of Education: ........................ Native Language: ........................ Normal Vision: Yes ........................ If not: Correction: No ........................ Glasses ........................ Contact lenses ........................ None ........................ Health questions Please indicate by ticking the box if you have or have had any of the following: Head injury requiring more than overnight hospital admission Diabetes with complications Brain surgery, or open heart and bypass surgery Stroke, vascular or ischaemic incident, or major vascular surgery A psychiatric illness requiring repeated treatment or hospital admission (e.g. depression, schizophrenia) Problems with drug or alcohol abuse Are you currently Experiencing any illness or undergoing any medical treatment that could potentially interfere with your thinking or concentration? If the answer to any of these is Yes, please include relevant details or tell the experimenter in confidence: ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ ........................ 60 Examination Number: 7879463 Appendix C – Task information forms Picture Decision Task In this experiment, your first task will be to view pictures in the centre of the screen. After viewing each picture, you will need to decide whether the realworld counterpart would fit inside a small box (this will be shown to you at the start). An example of the type of item you will be shown in the experiment is below: [Note: This item will be in your practice session and will not be repeated in the actual task.] You will answer the question by pressing the right or the left button. The experimenter will inform you about which button you will press will be the “YES” and which button will be the “NO” answer. Your main task is to answer as accurate as possible. 61 Examination Number: 7879463 There will be some practice trials in order to familiarise you with the procedure. If you have any questions, please ask the experimenter now. Both of the picture decision tasks will be recorded using EEG. To ensure good quality data try to remain as relaxed as possible and reduce head movements. It would also be beneficial to focus only on the centre of the screen which is indicated by the black/red plus “+” sign or the pictures shown. 62 Examination Number: 7879463 Memory Task In this second task, you will view a second set of pictures. After viewing each picture you will need to decide whether the picture you saw was also in the first task. If the picture was exactly the same in the first task, you should answer “old” indicating you have seen it before. This picture below was seen as an example in the first information sheet. If it had been in the first task then you could label it as “old”. If the picture was similar to one shown in the first task (e.g. both were pictures of puppies, however, they were different breeds), then you should answer “similar” indicating they were different versions of the same item. An example of this could be: 63 Examination Number: 7879463 [Note: If the first picture was of, for example, a lampshade and the other a lightbulb this doesn’t mean they are in the same category. Only if they were both lampshades or lightbulbs then they would be “similar”.] If the picture is a new picture of which no “similar” items have been shown before – then you should answer “new” indicating that you have not seen this item or any similar items before. For example: Your main task is to answer as accurately as possible, but also try to respond as quickly as you can whilst still being accurate. If you have any questions, please ask the experimenter now. As in the first task it is important to remain as relaxed as possible to obtain the best EEG recording. Please focus as far as you can in the centre of the screen as indicated by the plus “+” sign, as it’s important to move your eyes around as little as possible, although you can blink normally. 64 Examination Number: 7879463 65 Examination Number: 7879463 66 Examination Number: 7879463 67 Examination Number: 7879463 68