Running head: GENDER AND FRIENDSHIP IN SOCIAL BEHAVIOR The Impact of Gender and Friendship Research in the Field of Social Behavior Beatrice L. Rosen Emory University 1 GENDER AND FRIENDSHIP IN SOCIAL BEHAVIOR 2 The Impact of Gender and Friendship Research in the Field of Social Behavior More than 40 published studies and articles, dating back to 1974, have contributed to the firmly established finding that men’s same-sex friendships are less intimate and supportive than women’s (Bank & Hansford, 2000). Bank and Hansford (2000) tested six possible variables to explain this robust finding: lack of parental models for friendship, emotional restraint, homophobia, masculine self-identity, competitive striving, and role conflicts. Emotional restraint and homophobia toward gay men were the two predominant variables accredited with these gender effects on both intimacy and support in best friendships (Bank & Hansford, 2000). Masculine self-identity was a variable that moderated the relationship between gender and intimate – but not supportive – friendship. On the other hand, same-sex parents with close friendships moderated the relationship between gender and supportive – but not intimate – friendship (Bank & Hansford, 2000). I argue that Bank and Hansford’s (2000) article is one of the most important to come from psychological research because of the impact that the study’s findings have had on a variety of investigated and applied topics in the realm of social behavior. Since 2002 and November 2012 the study has been cited in at least 29 journal articles, and I believe it will continue to be incredibly influential over time, for interpersonal friendships permeate our daily lives. Lewis (1978) sparked initial empirical investigations into sex differences in intimate friendships through his own study of emotional intimacy and friendship. Lewis (1978) found that although most males report a higher number of same-sex friendships than women do, most of these are not close, intimate, or characterized by self-disclosure. This could be because men may experience obstacles to emotional intimacy out of the demands of traditional male roles in society, such as pressures to compete, homophobia, aversion to vulnerability, and lack of GENDER AND FRIENDSHIP IN SOCIAL BEHAVIOR 3 adequate role models (Lewis, 1978). Stokes, Fuehrer, and Childs (1980) verified Lewis’s findings in their study regarding gender differences in self-disclosure to various target persons. One of these target persons included intimates, and females were more willing than males to disclose to intimates. Stokes’ et al. (1980) findings further verified a tendency for men to avoid emotional intimacy with one another because of competition and homophobia. To ensure the existence of sex differences found in the nature of interactions with friends, Caldwell and Peplau (1982) examined if men and women varied in quantitative aspects of friendship. This includes the number of friends, the amount of time spent with friends, or the value placed on intimate friendships. Results showed that they did not differ (Caldwell & Peplau, 1982). Thus, the quantitative variable of friendship established as consistent for both sexes (Caldwell & Peplau, 1982), research on the intimacy of social relationships continued when Reis, Senchak, and Solomon (1985) studied diary-like reports of naturalistic interaction to gain deeper insight into the issue. The reports also indicated that males’ same-sex interaction was substantially less intimate than that of females (Reis et al., 1985). Williams (1985) then brought the variables of masculinity and femininity into the investigation by testing the degree to which they contributed to the relationship between gender and emotional intimacy. Through Spence, Helmreich, and Stapp’s (1973) Personal Attributes Questionnaire (as cited in Williams, 1985), Williams first confirmed that males report significantly lower levels of intimacy in same-sex friendship than do females. He then found that masculinity, defined in terms of instrumental qualities, has little effect on the degree of reported intimacy. Femininity, defined in terms of expressive qualities, is positively associated with intimate friendship (Williams, 1985). Sex-role expectations also appear to prohibit displays of emotional vulnerability among males (Williams, 1985). GENDER AND FRIENDSHIP IN SOCIAL BEHAVIOR 4 Reisman (1990) investigated further into the subject of intimacy in same-sex friendships through his specific evaluation of whether females are more disclosing in same-sex friendships than are males. The three studies he conducted differed from previous ones in that they each involved different ages: adolescents, undergraduates, and adults. Men did not seem to feel as close to same-sex friends as females, as previous findings had already established (Bank & Hansford, 2000), and both sexes believe that females are more disclosing of feelings and problems than are males (Reisman, 1990). Males seemed to wish to be more disclosing, however, but expected to be more open mainly in other-sex friendships (Reisman, 1990). During adulthood this concern about disclosure in same-sex friendships decreased, yet women reported feeling more at ease in relating to other women than to men throughout all stages of their lives (Reisman, 1990). Adolescents reported an expectancy to become more disclosing in other-sex friendships in adulthood and the spousal relationship to be the most intimate (Reisman, 1990). Levant, et al. (1992) narrowed the investigation to the specific study of the male role in light of contemporary norms. Levant and colleagues (1992) developed the Male Role Norms Inventory (MRNI), which consists of 58 items grouped into 7 subscales that measure theoretically derived norms of traditional masculinity ideology: avoidance of femininity, homophobia, self-reliance, aggression, achievement/status, attitudes toward sex, and restrictive emotionality. Avoidance of femininity seemed to be a homogenous and constantly changing factor of male role norms, whereas self-reliance and aggression seemed to tap aspects of male role norms that remain stable. Yet the MRNI is by no means a set inventory, but rather an ongoing development with a consistently altered structure based on current psychological research (Levant, Hall, & Rankin, 2013). The developments have led Levant and colleagues (as cited in Levant et al., 2013) to change the inventory’s name to Male Role Norms Inventory – GENDER AND FRIENDSHIP IN SOCIAL BEHAVIOR 5 Revised, or MRNI-R, and a 21-item MRNI-Short Form, or MRNI-SF, also exists. The most recent study that reports the evolution of the MRNI-R and MRNI-SF was conducted just this month: April, 2013 (Levant et al., 2013). From the initial development of the MRNI to the multitudinous published studies and articles, the hypothesis that men’s same-sex friendships are less intimate and supportive than women’s was confirmed by the 21st century (Bank & Hansford, 2000). Bank and Hansford (2000) therefore came up with six possible explanations as to why such gender differences are so prevalent. The explanations were based on the results from the preceding studies, specifically that they seemed likely to be related to gender, on the one hand, and intimacy and supportiveness of best same-sex friendships, on the other (Bank & Hansford, 2000). Each also seemed to explain how and why gender affects same-sex friendships in the manner suggested in the literature (Bank & Hansford, 2000). The six possible explanations were lack of parental models for friendship, emotional restraint, homophobia, masculine self-identity, competitive strivings, and role conflicts (Bank & Hansford, 2000). A variety of questionnaires that used 16 total measures to operationalize the six variables were distributed to 324 female students and 241 male students, ranging from freshmen to graduates, at a large Midwestern state university. Best friendships described by men and women were the same kinds of relationships, and not surprisingly, fewer than one-fifth of the men in the study scored above the mean or median of the women’s scores on intimate friendship. Yet men do enjoy friendships that are intimate and supportive more than those that are not (Bank & Hansford, 2000). Lack of same-sex parental model for friendship had an independent, significant effect on supportive, but not intimate, friendship (Bank & Hansford, 2000). Men were also more likely GENDER AND FRIENDSHIP IN SOCIAL BEHAVIOR 6 than women to lack same-sex parental models and to favor men’s career commitment (Bank & Hansford, 2000). Both of these tendencies decreased the supportiveness of respondents’ best same-sex friendship. Emotional restraint and homophobia toward gay men provided the most explanatory power for gender effects on both intimacy and support in best friendships, most likely because both of these variables tap a tendency to be cautious or reserved in personal relationships (Bank & Hansford, 2000). Masculine self-identity and the norm for men’s career commitment did not have significant effects on supportive friendship, but did mediate the relationship between intimate friendship (Bank & Hansford, 2000). Status orientation, one of the supposed measures of competitive strivings, was lower for men than for women and was positively related to intimate friendship (Bank & Hansford, 2000). The findings related to status orientation overall signify that status-seeking in one’s friendship actually advances levels of intimacy and support, and that women find it more important than men that their friendships provide them with statutes involving respect, influence, and prominence (Bank & Hansford, 2000). Bank and Hansford (2000) were still unable to explain all of the effects of gender on intimate and supportive friendships through their study. However, Bank and Hansford (2000) suggest more explanations could be found if future research takes a different approach by abandoning the male deficit model of friendship in favor of studying the reasons why some men and women seek close same-sex friendships. Bank and Hansford’s (2000) published article disclosing their study’s results guided the research approaches and methods of 29 subsequent studies to further the in-depth examination of gender differences in friendship. Wright (2006) explored sex differences within a broader conceptual and empirical context because he believed previous research, like Bank and GENDER AND FRIENDSHIP IN SOCIAL BEHAVIOR 7 Hansford’s (2000), clearly favors a single model of friendship rather than separate models for men and women. Wright (2006) therefore reexamined this previous research to provide an overall empirical picture, and utilized its implications to propose an expanded orientation of the study of women’s and men’s friendships. Borsari and Carey (2006) applied the previous findings to study how the quality of peer relationships enhance the influence of social reinforcement, modeling, and cognitive processes on personal alcohol use in college. The quality of peer relationships influences drinking by means of three ways: The lack or breakdown of quality peer relationships, alcohol use being an integral part of peer interactions, and if peers disapprove of alcohol use or do not drink (Borsari & Carey, 2006). This conceptualization of peer influence supports the finding of gender differences in college drinking (Borsari & Carey, 2006). Bank and Hansford’s (2000) findings were further applied to the study of understanding the social and emotional lives of gifted students, which sets forth an inclusive treatment of social and emotional development in high-ability learners (Hébert, 2011). Hébert (2011) examined the social and emotional characteristics and behaviors displayed by gifted learners, the friendships and family relationships that support them, situational effects that determine their social and emotional lives, and identity development. Based on the study’s results, Hébert (2011) proposes a framework design of the ideal gifted-friendly classroom environment for social and emotional development, as well as a thorough compilation of resources to support professionals in gifted education research and practice. Recent studies have also focused on gender differences pertaining to friendship ideals and gossip. Demir and Orthel (2011) investigated if men and women have different ideals for their friendships, and the resulting feelings if their actual friendship experiences fell short of their GENDER AND FRIENDSHIP IN SOCIAL BEHAVIOR 8 ideals. The real and ideal best friendships of women were higher in quality and lower in conflict when compared to those of men (Demir & Orthel, 2011). Friendship quality and gossip tendencies are also related to gender differences because gossip can increase the bond between people and sense of belonging to a group (Watson, 2012). Watson (2012) found that friendship quality affirmatively corresponds with gossip inclination in males, but not with females. This finding may be due to the greater importance males place on status, and that possession of knowledge and control of information is a method of gaining status (Watson, 2012). Gossip associated with achievement was also related to male friendship quality, which reflects the greater emphasis on individuation in male friendships (Watson, 2012). As for females, physical appearance gossip is more prevalent, but not related to friendship quality because it is more of a competitive threat to the relationship (Watson, 2012). From ideals (Demir & Orthel, 2011) and alcohol use (Borsari & Carey, 2006), to gifted students (Hébert, 2011) and gossip (Watson, 2012), Bank and Hansford (2000) impacted numerous studies across a variety of issues within the realm of social behavior research through their study on why men’s best same-sex friendships are less intimate and supportive than female’s best same-sex friendships. Previous studies that consistently confirmed the finding of such gender differences in friendship degree and intimacy led Bank and Hansford (2011) to conduct their study regarding why this was so. I argue the published article that documents their study’s findings holds its own historical significance because of the influence it has had in the past decade on a variety of areas of psychological research, inquiry, and application. Thus, I believe their study is one of the most important papers to come from psychological research in the area of social behavior. GENDER AND FRIENDSHIP IN SOCIAL BEHAVIOR References Bank, B.J., & Hansford, S.L. (2000). Gender and friendship: Why are men’s best samesex friendships less intimate and supportive? Personal Relationships, 7, 6378. Borsari, B., & Carey, K.B. (2006). How the quality of peer relationships influences college alcohol use. Drug and Alcohol Review, 25, 361-370. Caldwell, M.A., & Peplau, L.A. (1982). Sex differences in same-sex friendship. Sex Roles, 8, 721-732. Demir, M., & Orthel, H. (2011). Friendship, real-ideal discrepancies, and well-being: gender differences in college students. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 145, 173-193. Hébert, T.P. (2011). Understanding the social and emotional lives of gifted students. Texas: Prufrock Press. Levant, R.F., Hall, R.J., & Rankin, T.J. (2013). Male Role Norms Inventory-Short Form (MRNI-SF): Development, confirmatory factor analytic investigation of structure, and measurement invariance across gender. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 60, 228-238. Levant, R.F., Hirsch, L.S., Celentano, E., Cozza, T.M., et al. (1992). The male role: An investigation of contemporary norms. Journal of Mental Health Counselor Association, 14, 325-337. Lewis, R.A. (1978). Emotional intimacy among men. Journal of Social Issues, 34, 108121. Reis, H.T., Senchak, M., Solomon, B. (1985). Sex differences in the intimacy of social 9 GENDER AND FRIENDSHIP IN SOCIAL BEHAVIOR interaction: Further examination of potential explanations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1204-1217. Reisman, J.M. (1990). Intimacy in same-sex friendships. Sex Roles, 23, 65-82. Stokes, J., Fuehrer, A., & Childs, L. (1980). Gender differences in self-disclosure to various target persons. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 27, 192-198. Watson, D.C. (2012). Gender differences in gossip and friendship. Sex Roles, 67, 494502. Williams, D.G. (1985). Gender, masculinity-femininity, and emotional intimacy in same-sex friendship. Sex Roles, 12, 587-600. Wright, P.H. (2006). Toward an expanded orientation to the comparative study of women’s and men’s same-sex friendships. In K. Dindia and D. Canary (Eds.), Sex differences and similarities in communication, 2nd ed. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. 10