Running head: REPRESSED MEMORIES Repressed Memories and The Debate Over Recovered Memory Therapy Amanda L Magoon Drury University PSYC 334 1 REPRESSED MEMORIES 2 Abstract Repressed memories have been on debate for many years among therapists. Repressed memories involve the use of a defense mechanism called dissociation; through the use of therapy these repressed memories can be brought back to conscious memory. Repressed memories have affected law practice and healthcare coverage of therapy. There are many cases of therapist and client accounts that will provide support for both sides of the long standing debate. REPRESSED MEMORIES 3 Repressed Memories and The Debate Over Recovered Memory Therapy Repression is one of the most haunting concepts in psychology. Something shocking happens and the mind pushes it into some inaccessible corner of the unconscious. Later, the memory may emerge into consciousness. Repression is one of the foundation stones on which the structure of psychoanalysis rests. Recently there has been a rise in reported memories of childhood sexual abuse that were allegedly repressed for many years. With recent changes in legislation, people are suing alleged perpetrators for events that happened 20, 30 even 40 or more years earlier. Elizabeth F. Loftus 1993 Dissociation Dissociation is a defense mechanism that is used to push aside overwhelming feelings that are too difficult to cope with. Repression is memories and painful thoughts that are forgotten. Dissociation and repression sound similar, but have key differences. Dissociation usually involves “forgetting large amounts of personal information and, accordingly disrupts personal identity” (Hansell & Damour 2008, p 264). REPRESSED MEMORIES 4 Dissociative amnesia takes place usually after a traumatic or stressful event and is characterized by not being able to remember personal information that cannot be explained by “normal forgetfulness” (DSM-IV-TR 520). Accuracy of Memories The controversy over recovered memories has been going on over a century now. The accuracy of memories is essentially what is being debated. Sigmund Freud, the pioneer of psychotherapy, struggled with recovered memories as psychotherapists do today (Hansell & Damour 2008, p 262). In the beginning Freud believed what his clients were revealing to him about sexual abuse was true. Freud even wrote a report titled Aetiology of Hysteria (1896) that insisted “sexual experiences were ‘at the bottom of every case of hysteria’” (Hansell & Damour 2008, p 262). Eventually, Freud’s opinion of recovered memories would change. The shift was due to “memories of childhood sexual abuse emerged in nearly every one of his clinical cases” (Hansell & Damour 2008, p 262). When Freud explained his reason for the shift in opinion he said, “I was at last obliged to recognize that these scenes of seduction had never taken place, and that they were only fantasies which my patients had made up” (Hansell & Damour 2008, p 262). The controversy over how reliable these “recovered memories” are lives on today as well. On one side of the debate the clinicians believe that highly traumatic events can be repressed for long periods of time and subsequently recovered. On the other hand there is Elizabeth F Loftus, Department of Psychology, University of Washington. REPRESSED MEMORIES 5 Loftus argues that memories are fallible, easily distorted, and can even be implanted. A child’s imagination can even play a role in how events are remembered. Loftus and colleagues have proven that when a child is told or asked repeated questions about sexual abuse, the child can “remember” an incident that truly never occurred. Case Example In 1990 George Franklin Sr. (51 Years old) was convicted of murder of and 8 year old girl, Susan Kay Nelson, in 1969 (Brahams 2000). Franklin’s daughter Eileen was only 8 years old herself when the murder happened and claims the memory of her witnessing the crime was repressed. Eileen does not only accuse Franklin, her father, of murder but also of sexually assaulting Nelson. The memories are claimed to not have come back all at once, Eileen begins to remember fragments of the event more than 20 years later while playing with her own children (Loftus 1993). Eileen’s memory affected her father tremendously. Eileen’s therapist, family and San Mateo County District Attorney’s office believed her report and the San Mateo County District Attorney’s office chose to prosecute. The jury went into deliberations on November 29, 1990 and came back the next day with a verdict of guilty (Loftus 1993). “The jury’s finding apparently relied heavily on his [Franklin’s] daughter’s unreliable “repressed memory” (Brahams 2000). After spending 6 years in prison, Franklin’s conviction was overturned due to several factors. First, the jurors were told that Franklin remaining silent while being accused he was admitting to guilt. REPRESSED MEMORIES 6 Second, the details of what happened to Nelson were published prior to her recovered memory. Finally Eileen had falsely accused her father of a second murder (Brahams 2000). Therapy Recovered memories can be damaging to families and oneself, not to mention the views on repressed and recovering memories is still a divided issue amongst therapists. According to Brahams in 1992 there were 18,000 families in the False Memory Syndrome Foundation of Philadelphia Pennsylvania. Regardless of the warnings issued by the American Psychiatric Association, therapists are still moving forward with the treatment. A major question in the debate over repressed memories is how reliable are these said memories? In the course of therapy some therapists use hypnosis, drug therapy, and repetitive interviews to recover lost memories (Brahams 2000). Memories are not reliable on their own let alone combined with hypnosis and drugs. Credible instances of repressed memory do occur, sometimes. A major problem with repressed memories if the therapists themselves. Therapists are intentionally and unintentionally suggesting to clients that they are victims of abuse. Loftus writes that “clients have been reporting that a therapist has suggested that childhood abuse was the cause of their current distress”. Loftus also writes about a woman from Oregon who started therapy for depression and anxiety. Only a few months into therapy her therapist suggests her depression and anxiety are due to childhood sexual abuse (Loftus 1993). The client has no memories of any sexual abuse and has undergone hypnosis and trance work trying to remember and cannot. Hypnosis is not a reliable means of gathering memories. Through hypnosis and age regression a REPRESSED MEMORIES 7 client was able to remember “horrifying memories of abuse by her father” (Loftus 1993), from as young as 4 years old till she was 18. Hypnosis is not reversible; if these memories are not true the client has to live with her new reality. Through hypnotic regression people “have been known to recall being abducted by aliens a board exotic space craft and other forgotten events” (Loftus 1993). Conclusion The evidence shows there are two very real sides to this debate. On one side there are professionals against the use of therapy to recover memories of childhood sexual abuse that may not have occurred at all. Loftus has a valid point saying memories are fallible. Memories can be remembered distorted and be manipulated. On the other hand, the therapists using this technique in therapy have a long and difficult battle ahead of them validating its use. There are numerous reports and accounts of therapists and clients on repressed memories, and based on the research, recovered memory therapy is not reliable and there will undoubtedly be a continuous controversy over the issue. REPRESSED MEMORIES 8 References Brahams, D. (2000). 'Repressed memories' and the law. Lancet, 356(9227), 358. Retrieved from EBSCOhost. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. (4th ed., p. 520). (2000). Dissociative Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Hansell, J., & Damour, L. (2008). Abnormal psychology (2nd ed.). Hoboken, N.J.: Wiley. Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48(5), 518. Retrieved from EBSCOhost. Additional Sources Goldstein, E. (1997). FALSE MEMORY SYNDROME: WHY WOULD THEY BELIEVE SUCH TERRIBLE THING IS THEY WEREN'T TRUE?. American Journal of Family Therapy, 25(4), 307-317. Retrieved from EBSCOhost. Ofshe, R., & Watters, E. (1996). Making monsters: false memories, psychotherapy, and sexual hysteria. Berkeley: University of California Press.