ΠΑΝΤΕΙΟΝ ΠΑΝΕΠΙΣΤΗΜΙΟ ΚΟΙΝΩΝΙΚΩΝ ΚΑΙ ΠΟΛΙΤΙΚΩΝ ΕΠΙΣΤΗΜΩΝ PANTEION UNIVERSITY OF SOCIAL AND POLITICAL SCIENCES Embedding Cognizance in Intellectual Development Smaragda Kazi Panteion University of Social and Political Sciences • This presentation is based on: Spanoudis, G., Demetriou, A., Kazi, S., Giorgala, K., & Zenonos, V. (accepted). Embedding Cognizance in Intellectual Development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. Intoduction • The model proposed by Demetriou and colleagues posits that, during development: • (i) Intellectual development advances in four cycles, with two phases in each Fluid cognition Counterintuitive logical relations WM Logically-based principles Speed 14 years 11 years Inference-based concepts Speed WM Symbol-based mental representations Actionbased episodic representations WM Speed WM Speed 2 years 8 years 6 years 4 years Pragmatic inference Systematic logical inference • (ii) Cognizance (i.e., awareness of the perceptual and the inferential origins of knowledge, awareness of mental processes and mental states, metarepresentation) advances through the same cycles Cognizance Awareness of the formal relations between rules and principles Awareness of underlying mental processes connecting representations Children do not differentiate between mental functions Awareness of own and others’ representation Awareness of perception as a source of knowledge 2 years 4-5 years 6 years 13-14 years 8-9 years • (iii) Cognizance mediates between basic processing efficiency functions (i.e., processing speed, inhibition control, and working memory) and fluid cognition. • Cognizance (i.e., self-evaluation and selfawareness of mental operations) is one of the causal factors for cognitive change because it makes metarepresentation possible. • That is, cognizance produces cognitive experiences about mental functioning (including self evaluations and external feedback about its efficiency vis-à-vis a goal). • In this process, various aspects of executive control, such as attention control and mental attention, may be instrumental (Wiebe, Espy, & Charak, 2010; Wiebe et al., 2011), because they necessitate choices which, in turn, may cause awareness of the processes currently involved (Demetriou et al., 2013b, 2014b) • Also, it seems that working memory activates voluntary control processes which may eventually generate awareness of the processes involved. • Thus, it is important to specify the flow of effects between awareness, mental control, and working memory in the successive phases of intellectual development. Aims of the present study • (i) To examine the mediating role of cognizance in development and, • (ii) whether this mediation is phase-specific, based on the representational and inferential possibilities of each phase. Hypotheses • 1. Cognizance of mental states and mental processes mediates between fundamental mental processing functions, such as processing efficiency and working memory, and inferential processes underlying fluid cognition. • 2. This mediation is phase-specific: at preschool it is based on representational awareness, which is expressed through awareness of the perceptual origin of knowledge and ToM. At the next phase, in primary school, it is based on procedural awareness, which is expressed through awareness of the inferential origin of knowledge. • 3. Control of mental processing emerges gradually from mental functioning. At the beginning, it emerges from processes which require attention to particular stimuli, either in the environment (perceptual awareness) or in mental representations (working memory). Later, once mastered, it may become instrumental in steering mental functioning according to goals. Sample • 181 children (N=89 male) were examined: • preschool (N=53, Mage = 66.31 months; SD = 6.71 months), • first (N=40, Mage = 78.36 months; SD = 3.70 months), • second (N=48, Mage = 90.94 months; SD = 3.71 months), • and third primary school grade (N=44, Mage = 103.85 months; SD = 3.61 months). Tasks (1) Processing efficiency (2) Working memory (3) Awareness of cognitive processes and ToM (4) Fluid cognition (1) Processing Efficiency (i) Speed of processing (ii) Speed of perceptual discrimination (iii) Perceptual discrimination and conceptual control (i) Speed of processing • Simple, computerized choice reaction time task • Target stimuli (geometrical figures) appeared on the right or the left half of the screen and children were instructed to press the corresponding button (60 trials in each condition). • Total correct responses and mean reaction times were recorded (i) Speed of processing (ii) Speed of perceptual discrimination • Children were presented two sets of dots side by side on the two halves of the screen, split by a black line. Their task was to specify as fast as possible if the right-side set was equal (or not) to the left-side set (4 conditions x 8 trials). (ii) Speed of perceptual discrimination Same number – Same arrangement Same number – Different arrangement Different number – Same arrangement Different number – Different arrangement (iii) Perceptual discrimination and conceptual control • This task included two conditions: Perceptual discrimination and conceptual control (48 trials). • Reaction times to correct responses were used in the analyses (iii) Perceptual discrimination and conceptual control Perceptual discrimination (choose the bigger of the two) Conceptual control (choose the object which is actually bigger in reality) (2) Working Memory (i) Phonological memory (ii) Spatial memory (iii) Numerical information (i) Phonological memory • This battery included two separate tasks: (a) words and (b) pseudowords. • These tests involved 16 familiar words and 16 pseudowords. Children were verbally presented with series of words or pseudowords which they recalled in presentation order. (ii) Spatial memory (Corsi block) (iii) Numerical working memory • Children saw stimulus arrays of colored dots, each including different number of dots. One to seven dot arrays appeared in the center of the computer screen and the child’s task was to remember the number and color of dots of each array. • It was followed by a dot array inside a frame (probe stimulus). The children’s task was to indicate if the dots in the probe set were more than the same color dots seen before (max 28 trials). (3) Awareness of cognitive processes and ToM (i) Differentiating vision, hearing and inference (ii) Hearing-inference differentiation (iii) Theory of mind (i) Differentiating vision, hearing and inference (ii) Hearing-inference differentiation • Two short videos were presented showing a child watching a teacher hiding three different color toy cars (red, green, and blue) into their corresponding color boxes (red, green, and blue). What the child saw and heard in each video was systematically manipulated, so that the source of the child’s knowledge was vision, hearing, or inference. Participants were asked several “how did he/she know” questions after watching each video. (i) Differentiating vision, hearing and inference (ii) Hearing-inference differentiation A Phase: John sat beside the teacher. •John saw and heard the teacher placing the red car in the red box. •He saw, but did not hear, that the green car was placed in the green box •He neither saw nor heard where the blue car was placed. A Phase: Ann sat across the table. The teacher raised a wooden separation between them. Ann could not see what the teacher was doing. •Ann heard that the teacher placed the red car in the red box •She neither saw nor heard where the green and blue car were placed. B Phase: Ann (John) were later asked to find each of the cars. The participant was asked : •“How did Ann (John) know where the red (green/blue) car is?” After looking for the blue car, the participant was asked about the teacher’s reason for placing the blue car in the blue box: •“Why did the teacher place the blue car in the blue box?” (i) Differentiating vision, hearing and inference (ii) Hearing-inference differentiation (iii) Theory of mind • The classical Sally task was used. In this task, Sally places a ball in a basket and she goes away. In her absence, Anna moves the ball in a box. Sally returns and the participant is asked to indicate where Sally will look for the ball. Performance is scored on a pass-fail basis. (4) Fluid cognition • A Raven-like test involving ten matrices was used which addressed three levels of complexity (single dimension, intersection between the two dimensions, intersection between three dimensions. • Scoring was effected on a pass-fail basis (0 for wrong and 1 for correct choices) and level scores were obtained by adding across level-specific items. Fluid cognition Results • Several types of models were pitted against each other. In accordance with our first prediction, a first set of models assumed that awareness of mental processes mediates between fundamental efficiency processes and Gf. Gf was regressed on both awareness factors - awareness of mental processes contributes to the formation of inferential processes involved in Gf. Both awareness factors were regressed on WM - the executive and information handling processes in WM generate awareness of mental processes WM was regressed on speed and attention control - WM is a time dependent process mediating between processes reflecting efficiency and representational and inferential processes Speed and attention control were regressed on age - changes in these parameters directly reflect age-related changes in processing efficiency The model applied on the whole sample (first value in each column, χ2 (145) = 168.69, p > .09, CFI = .97, RMSEA = .03, Model AIC =-121.31) and the 5-6 (the second value) and the 7-8 years old children (the third value, χ2 (300) = 334.25, p = .08, CFI = .93, RMSEA = .04, Model AIC= -265.50). The direction of effects between awareness and Gf was inverted so that Gf mediated between efficiency and working memory and awareness. The fit of this model was significantly weaker. In line with our first prediction, awareness appeared to mediate between the processing efficiency factors and Gf rather than to emerge from it. The model inverting the gf-awareness (compared to model 3A). Note 1: The first value in each column refer to the whole sample (χ2 (146) = 182.63, p> .02, CFI= .95, RMSEA= .04 (.00-.05, AIC= -107.37) and the second and third value to 56, 7-8 years old children, (χ2 (300) = 350.84, p = .02, CFI = .90, RMSEA = .04, Model AIC= -249.16), respectively. • The third model focused on the place of attention control in the system. In both previous models, the role of attention control was weak. It was only related to age, suggesting that, in the studied age span, it is not an effective agent in mental processing. It may be the case that attention control emerges gradually from the experience of mental processing and ensuing awareness of the perceptual or representational content of processing. As previously, Gf was regressed on both awareness factors - awareness of mental processes contributes to the formation of inferential processes involved in Gf. Inferential awareness was regressed on WM and attention control to implement the assumption that focusing on, selecting, and organizing representations generates awareness about them Attention control was regressed on WM - mental processing involved in effortful storage and recall of information sharpens the ability to focus on and select representations according to goal We regressed working on speed and perceptual awareness - both of these factors contribute to working memory performance We relegated perceptual awareness to the same level of speed from early in the age span studied here perceptual awareness is already a strong index of mental efficiency. The relations between attention control and its adjacent factors were indeed stronger than in the model where attention control resided on the same level with speed. The model interpolating attention control between awareness and working memory, at the level of the whole sample, χ2 (143) = 167.74, CFI=.97, p < .08, RMSEA = .031, AIC= -118.26, and the two age groups, χ2 (296) = 327.83, CFI=.94, p < .10, RMSEA = .030, AIC= -258.19 Note 1: The first, second, and third value in each column refer to the whole sample, 5-6, 7-8 years old children, respectively. Age groups’ similarities and differences Age is similarly related to speed and perceptual awareness in the two age groups. However, in the younger age group, WM is more closely related to perceptual awareness (.54) than in the older age group (.39). Inversely, WM was weakly related to speed in the younger group (-.22) and more strongly in the older group (-.38). Age groups’ similarities and differences Attention control (-.14) and inferential awareness (.17) were weakly related to WM in the younger age group; they were much higher in the older age group (-.35 and .69, respectively). The relation between attention control and inferential awareness was very low in both groups (-.12 and -.12 in both). In the younger age group, Gf was highly related to perceptual awareness (.67) and moderately to inferential awareness (.29). This pattern was inverted in the older age group where the perceptual awareness-Gf relation dropped to non-significance (.18) and the inferential awareness-Gf was much higher (.41). Age groups’ similarities and differences This pattern is generally in line with our second prediction that the mediation of cognizance between processing efficiency and Gf is phasespecific and the third prediction that attention control emerges gradually from more fundamental processes but its embedding in mental functioning is slow, exceeding the age span studied here. Discussion • First, the dominance of models assuming that cognizance mediates between Gf and processes indexing processing and representational efficiency suggested that cognizance is an important factor of intellectual development and individual differences in mental functioning. Discussion • Second, the relative advantage of the third model, relative to all other models, suggested that its role differentiates with development. Specifically, in the 4-6 years phase cognizance of the perceptual origins of knowledge was as basic as speed, being the primary predictor of both working memory (.54) and Gf (.67). In the next phase, from 6-8 years, cognizance of the inferential aspects of knowledge took over as a mediator between all other processes and Gf (.41) Discussion • A basic level of awareness is critical for handling mental representations (in the 4-6 years phase) but attention control emerges gradually, after a certain level of this handling is attained. Thus, this executive core is shaped gradually and it becomes transparent to awareness later (in the 6-8 years phase). Even then, these relations are still under formation. They are rather well established in the 8-10 years. Discussion • These findings bear important implications for theories of cognitive development and intelligence. • In concern to developmental theory, this study suggests that cognizance is an important developmental force that contributes to the transformation of processing and representational possibilities into actual problem solving capabilities. • Thus, their interactions with other aspects of cognitive development need to be mapped and understood more precisely. Discussion • Various aspects of cognizance, such as theory-ofmind, which were accorded a very special status in intellectual and social development, are just some of the stepping-stones in a long course of development. • Executive control, which is very popular in developmental research, emerges from cognizance. • Understanding its development requires to examine the possible role of several important factors not examined here, such as language. Discussion • In concern to theory of intelligence, this study suggested that cognizance deserves a stronger position than it was accorded to it by traditional differential theories of intelligence • This assumption has a practical implication as well. Developmental and intelligence testing need to broaden its scope to include measures of cognizance together with other more traditional measures. Thank you!