The Psychology of Human Development David F. Bjorklund Why Study Development? “The child is the father of man (and the mother of woman)” - to understand adults, including ourselves To understand human nature To foster development and well-being (optimization) Goals of Studying Life-Span Development Description Normal development, individual differences Explanation Typical and individually different development Optimization Positive development, enhancing human capacities Prevention and overcoming difficulties What is Development? Systematic changes and continuities In the individual Between conception and death “Womb to Tomb” Three broad domains Physical, Cognitive, Psychosocial Types of “change over time” Ontogeny: changes in the individual over the course of his or her lifetime Phylogeny: development of the species (i.e., evolution) Maturation: a biological unfolding of the individual according to a genetic plan. Learning: relatively permanent changes in thoughts, feelings, or behaviors as a result of experience, that usually occur over brief periods Growth: Physical changes that occur from birth to maturity Aging: Positive and negative changes in the mature organism Development: Changes in structure or function over time Structure refers to some substrate of the organism, such as nervous tissue, muscle, or limbs, or—in cognitive psychology—the mental knowledge that underlies intelligence. Function denotes actions related to a structure and can include actions external to the structure being studied, such as neurochemical or hormonal secretions, and other exogenous factors that can best be described as “experience”—that is, external sources of stimulation. Development is characteristic of the species and has its basis in biology. Its general course, therefore, is relatively predictable. Development progresses as a result of bidirectional, or reciprocal, relationship between structure and function, and can be expressed as: structure function. A Brief History of Childhood Childhood as a sociological concept Infanticide legal in much of ancient world and common in Europe through middle ages (abandonment outlawed in Europe in 1600s) Oblation: leaving infants with religious organizations Children incorporated into daily lives of adults 50% of children died before age 5 until 1800s Enlightenment: Jean Jacques Rousseau: children are important in their own right, and are not merely a means to an end (that is, adulthood). Literacy and Education Postman: invention of moveable type changed definition of adulthood (being literate); necessitated schooling and “the invention” of childhood Increases in Life Expectancy over the 20th Century in the U.S. 2000 1990 1980 1970 1960 1950 1940 1930 1920 1910 1900 77 75.5 74 71 69 68 63.5 59 56.5 51.5 49 0 20 40 60 Life Expectancy 80 100 Life Expectancy at Birth for Different Countries: 2000 Andorra: 83.5 San Marino: 81.1 Japan: 80.7 Sweden: 79.6 Italy: 79.0 United States: 77.1 China: 71.4 Vietnam: 69.3 Iraq: 66.5 Egypt: 63.3 India: 62.5 Brazil: Cambodia: South Africa: Haiti: Somalia: Afghanistan: Uganda: Botswana: Angola: Malawi: Zambia: 62.9 56.5 51.1 49.2 46.2 45.9 42.9 39.3 38.3 37.6 37.2 Issues in Lifespan Development The Stability and Plasticity of Human Behavior: The Effects of Early Experiences and the Changeability of Behavior Continuity vs. Discontinuity: The “Stages Debate,” or Understanding How Development Progresses Normative vs. Idiographic Approaches: Developmental Function and Individual Differences Nature vs. Nurture: About the Role of Genetics and Environment in Human Development The Stability and Plasticity of Human Behavior Stability refers to the degree to which people maintain their same rank order in comparison to other children with respect to some characteristic Plasticity refers to the ability to change as a result of experience. Continuity vs. Discontinuity Stages of Development Qualitative versus Quantitative Differences Continuity versus Discontinuity Homogeneity of Cognitive Function Discontinuous (stage-like) versus Continuous changes Some abilities, such as language, are more easily acquired during a critical, or sensitive, time in development Framing the Nature/Nurture Issue Nature: heredity(nativism) Maturational processes guided by genes Biologically based predispositions Biological unfolding of genes Genetic determinism Nurture: environment (empiricism) Learning: experiences cause changes is thoughts, feelings, and behaviors Environmental determinism Interactionist view: nature & nurture interact Caspi et al., 2002 monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) associated with antisocial behavior in rats and humans Gene on X chromosome controls MAOA (high versus low levels) Examined antisocial behavior in boys with high and low levels of MAOA as function of childhood maltreatment Relationship between childhood maltreatment (none, probable, severe) and MAOA activity (low versus high) on antisocial behavior. Composite Index of Antisocial Behavior 1.25 1 Low MAOA Activity High MAOA Activity 0.75 0.5 0.25 0 -0.25 -0.5 None Probable Severe Childhood Maltreatment Methods of Studying Life-Span Development Historical Baby Biographies: Charles Darwin Questionnaires: G. Stanley Hall Key Assumptions of Modern Life-Span Perspectives Lifelong, multidirectional process Gain and loss and lifelong plasticity Historical/cultural contexts, multiple influences Multi-disciplinary studies Conducting Developmental Research Self-reports: interview, questionnaires, tests Behavioral Observations Naturalistic Advantage: natural setting Disadvantage: conditions not controlled Structured (Lab) Disadvantage: cannot generalize to natural settings Advantage: conditions controlled Figure 1.2 The Experimental Method Three Critical Features 1. Manipulation of independent variable 2. Random assignment of individuals to treatment conditions 3. Experimental control Quasi-Experiment: No random assignment The Correlational Method Determine if 2 or more variables are related Correlation: A measure of the relationship Can range from +1.0 to –1.0 Positive: variables move in same direction Negative: variables move in opposite dir. No relationship if correlation is 0 Cannot establish a causal relationship Figure 1.3 Developmental Research Designs Cross-Sectional Designs +1 cohorts or age-groups studied 1 time of testing Studying age differences at any one time Longitudinal Designs 1 cohort +1 time of testing Study changes across time in one cohort Figure 1.4 Age, Cohort, and Time of Measurement Effects Age effects: Changes which occur due to age Cohort Effects: Born in one historical context Changes due to differences in society Disadvantage of cross-sectional design Time of measurement effects: Historical Takes place at time of data collection Disadvantage of longitudinal design Sequential Designs A combination of cross-sectional and longitudinal designs Advantages of both designs Gives information about Which age-related trends are age effects? Which age-related trends are truly cohort effects? Which age-related trends are a result of historical events? Figure 1.6 “He should have published” Protecting the Rights of Participants Risk to benefit balance of the research Researcher responsibilities Informed consent Debriefing Protection from harm Confidentiality Ethical Standards for Research with Children Doing research with children involves all the same ethical considerations when one does research with adults, plus some additional ones. The principles listed below were published in the 1990-91 Directory of the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD), except for Principles 15 and 16, first published in the Fall 1991 Newsletter. Principle 1. Non-harmful procedures: The investigator should use no research operation that may harm the child either physically or psychologically. The investigator is also obligated at all times to use the least stressful research operation whenever possible. . . When the investigator is in doubt about the possible harmful effects of the research operations, consultation should be sought from others. . Principle 2. Informed Consent: Before seeking consent or assent from the child, the investigator should inform the child of all features of the research that may affect his or her willingness to participate and should answer the child’s questions in terms appropriate to the child’s comprehension. The investigator should respect the child’s freedom to choose to participate in the research or not by giving the child the opportunity to give or not give assent to participation as well as to choose to discontinue participation at any time. . . Investigators working with infants should take special effort to explain the research procedures to the parents and be especially sensitive to any indicators of discomfort in the infant. . . Principle 3. Parental consent: The informed consent of parents, legal guardians or those who act in loco parentis (e.g., teachers, superintendents of institutions) similarly should be obtained, preferably in writing. Informed consent requires that parents or other responsible adults be informed of all the features of the research that may affect their willingness to allow the child to participate. This information should include the profession and institution affiliation of the investigator. Not only should the right of the responsible adults to refuse consent be respected, but they should be informed that they may refuse to participate without incurring any penalty to them or to the child. Principle 4. Additional consent: The informed consent of any persons, such as school teachers for example, whose interaction with the child is the subject of the study should also be obtained. . Principle 5. Incentives: Incentives to participate in a research project must be fair and must not unduly exceed the range of incentives that the child normally experiences. . . . Principle 6. Deception: Although full disclosure of information during the procedure of obtaining consent is the ethical ideal, a particular study may necessitate withholding certain information or deception. Whenever withholding information or deception is judged to be essential to the conduct of the study, the investigator should satisfy research colleagues that such judgment is correct. . . Principle 7. Anonymity: To gain access to institutional records, the investigator should obtain permission from responsible authorities in charge of records. Anonymity of the information should be preserved and no information used other than that for which permission was obtained. Principle 8. Mutual responsibilities: From the beginning of each research investigation, there should be clear agreement between the investigator and the parents, guardians or those who act in loco parentis, and the child, when appropriate, that defines the responsibilities of each. The investigator has the obligation to honor all promises and commitments of the agreement. Principle 9. Jeopardy: When, in the course of research, information comes to the investigator’s attention that may jeopardize the child’s well-being, the investigator has a responsibility to discuss the information with the parents or guardians and with those expert in the field in order that they may arrange the necessary assistance for the child. Principle 10. Unforeseen consequences: When research procedures result in undesirable consequences for the participant that were previously unforeseen, the investigator should immediately employ appropriate measures to correct these consequences, and should redesign the procedures if they are to be included in subsequent studies. Principle 11. Confidentiality: The investigator should keep in confidence all information obtained about research participants. The participants’ identity should be concealed in written and verbal reports of the results, as well as in informal discussion with students and colleagues. . Principle 12. Informing participants: Immediately after the data are collected, the investigator should clarify for the research participant any misconceptions that may have arisen. . . Principle 13. Reporting results: Because the investigator’s words may carry unintended weight with parents and children, caution should be exercised in reporting results, making evaluative statements, or giving advice. Principle 14. Implications of findings: Investigators should be mindful of the social, political and human implications of their research and should be especially careful in the presentation of findings from the research. This principle, however, in no way denies investigators the right to pursue any area of research or the right to observe proper standards of scientific reporting. Principle 15. Scientific misconduct: Misconduct is defined as the fabrication or falsification of data, plagiarism, misrepresentation, or other practices that seriously deviate from those that are commonly accepted within the scientific community for proposing, conducting, analyzing, or reporting research. It does not include unintentional errors or honest differences in interpretation of data. The Society . . . will not tolerate the presence of scientific misconduct among its members. It shall be the responsibility of the voting members of Governing Council to reach a decision about the possible expulsion of members found guilty of scientific misconduct. Principle 16. Personal misconduct: Personal misconduct that results in a criminal conviction of a felony may be sufficient grounds for a member’s expulsion from the Society. . . Source: As followed by members of the Society for Research in Child Development. Adapted from Winter 1990 SRCD Newsletter. Reprinted by permission. Theories of Development Mechanistic theories liken people to machines, such as the mind-as-a-computer model of information-processing approaches. Learning theories (operant & classical conditioning; social learning theory) Information-processing theory Organismic theories take a more “biologic” (nature) view of development, seeing people as whole beings who cannot be understood by decomposing them into their constituent parts. Piaget’s theory of cognitive development Psychoanalytic theories of Freud and Erikson Learning Theories: Classical Conditioning Behaviorism: Conclusions should be based on observable behavior Tabula Rasa: Environmental view Association learning UCS: built-in, unlearned stimulus UCR: automatic, unlearned response CS: causes learned response CR: learned response Figure 2.2 Learning Theories: Operant Conditioning Probability of behavior based on environmental consequences Reinforcement Pleasant consequence Increases probability Punishment Decreases probability Unpleasant, aversive Bandura: Social Cognitive Theory Formerly called social learning theory Humans think, anticipate, believe, etc. Cognitive emphasis: observational learning BoBo doll studies Model praised or punished Child learned to imitate rewarded model Vicarious reinforcement Reciprocal determinism Learning Theory: Strengths & Weaknesses Strengths Precise and testable theory Carefully controlled experiments Practical applications across lifespan Weaknesses Inadequate account of lifespan changes Ignored genetic and maturational processes Piaget: Cognitive Developmental Theory Intelligence: Ability to adapt to environment Constructivism: Understanding based on experience Interactionist Both biological maturation and experience required for progress At each new stage, children think in a qualitatively different way Cognitive Developmental Theory Strengths Well-accepted by developmentalists Well-researched, mostly supported Influenced education and parenting Weaknesses Ignores motivation and emotion Stages not universal – esp. last one Freud: Psychoanalytic Theory Instincts and unconscious motivation Id, Ego, and Superego formed from psychic energy (Libido) Id: Instinctual nature of humans Ego: rational and objective Superego: internalized moral standards A dynamic personality system Regular conflicts between the three parts Freud’s Psychosexual Development Child moves through 5 stages Stages result from conflict between Id & Superego Conflict creates anxiety Ego defends against anxiety with defense mechanisms Early experiences have long-term effects on personality Erik Erikson Most influential neo-Freudian Some differences with Freud Less emphasis on sexual urges More emphasis on rational ego More positive and adaptive view of human nature Believed development continues through adulthood Contextual/Systems Theories Lev Vygotsky: Sociocultural perspective Cognitive development is a social process Problem solving aided by dialogues Gottlieb: Evolutionary/Epigenetic Systems Genes, neural activity, behavior, and environment mutually influential Normal genes and normal early experiences most helpful Sociocultural Perspectives on Cognitive Development “Cognitive development is an active constructive process that involves beings who are evolutionarily predisposed to live and learn in social context with other ‘like-minded’ beings. They are like-minded in terms of both the neurological system available and the social requirements that are in place” Mary Gauvain, 2001 Lev Vygotsky Development should be evaluated from the perspective of four inter-related levels: Phylogenetic Ontogenetic Microgenetic Sociohistorical Tools of Intellectual adaptation Methods of thinking and problem-solving strategies that children internalize from their interactions with more competent members of society that permit them to use basic mental functions more adaptively Chinese and English number words from 1 to 20 Number Chinese word English word 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 yee uhr sahn suh woo lyo chee bah jyo shi shi yee shi uhr shi shan shi suh shi woo shi lyo shi chee shi bah shi jyo ershi one two three four five six seven eight nine ten eleven twelve thirteen fourteen fifteen sixteen seventeen eighteen nineteen twenty Median level of counting (highest number reached) by age for Chinese and U.S. preschoolers (Miller et al., 1995 Median Counting Level 100 80 60 China US 40 20 0 3 4 Age in Years 5 Zone of Proximal Development ZPD: the difference between a child’s actual developmental level as determined by independent problem solving and his or her level of potential development determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers. Scaffolding: When experts are sensitive to abilities of a novice and respond contingently to the novice’s responses in a learnikng situation, so that the novice gradually increases his or her understanding of a problem Apprenticeship in Thinking Guided participation refers to adult-child interactions, not only during explicit instruction, but also during the more routine activities and communication of everyday life. Guided participation is “the process and system of involvement of individuals with others, as they communicate and engage in shared activities” (Rogoff et al., 1993, p.6) Some functions of shared remembering in children’s memory development from Gauvain, 2001) Children learn about memory process, for example, strategies Children learn ways of remembering and communicating memories with others, for example, narrative structure Children learn about themselves, which contributes to the development of the self-concept Children learn about their own social and cultural history Children learn values important to the family and the community, that is, what is worth remembering Promotes social solidarity Developmental Systems Approach Development proceeds as a result of the bidirectional interaction between structure and function over time at all levels of organization, from the genetic through the cultural. Epigenesis: “any gene-regulating activity that doesn’t involve changes to the DNA code and that can persist through one or more generations” (Pennisi, 2001) Genetic activity (DNA <-> RNA <-> proteins) <-> structural maturation <-> function, activity Preformationist view of development QuickTime™ and a TIFF (U ncompres sed) decompressor are needed to see this picture. Genetic activity (DNA <-> RNA <-> proteins) <-> structural maturation <-> function, activity A simplified schematic of the developmental systems approach, showing a hierarchy of four mutually interacting components (from Gottlieb) Newly hatched ducklings approach maternal call. Any role of auditory experience? (Gottlieb) Prior to hatching: Remove mother: Approach maternal call Remove clutch mates: Approach maternal call Prevent from vocalizing in egg: No preference Transactional Model of Development Development is seen as the continuous and bidirectional interaction between an active organism and with a unique biological constitution and a changing environment. Zeskind & Ramey (1981) High-risk rural population Experimental (daycare) group vs. Control group Biologically normal vs. Fetally malnourished Stanford-Binet IQ scores at 36 months (from Zeskind & Ramey, 1981) IQ Scores at 36 months Biologically normal Fetally malnourished Exper’tal group 98.1 96.4 Control group 84.7 70.6