Mott, T. & Frederick, C. 2013 Text medium and critical

advertisement
The Relation Between Text Medium and Critical
Reading Scores (2012)
Troy Mott
Christina Frederick*
Sierra Nevada College
Abstract
Post-secondary educational institutions have incorporated tablets in the educational curriculum (Woodford,
2001). To investigate how reading medium impacts critical reading ability, I performed two studies. In the first
study, participants read an SAT practice test passage (Mathur, 2012) from either paper or an iPad 2 tablet. The
identical passages were 949 words. Once the reading was complete, participants responded to 12 critical
reading assessment questions about the passage. 116 participants completed the critical reading assessment
study. A second study, a self-response survey, which examined the reading preferences and demographics
(gender and age) of 115 participants was also conducted. A two-way ANOVA was used to analyze the results
of the critical reading comprehension test. No significant difference was found between critical reading
comprehension scores between mediums and academic standing (p = .911). The self-response survey was
assessed using a chi-square analysis. There was no significant difference in preference between upper and lower
division undergraduates (p = .157). Females showed a stronger preference for reading from paper than males (p
= .045), and a significant preference was found among the total sample population for reading from paper over
other surveyed forms of media (p < 0.001). The implications of this study are relevant to the future of education
and sustainability efforts in the classroom.
Introduction
Does reading medium affect critical reading ability when an individual reads from screen vs. from paper? iPads,
iPad 2, and other tablet computers are popular. Apple released the iPad April 3, 2010. Within 80 days of the
release 300,000 units had been sold. Apple sold 32 million iPads in the 2011 fiscal year (Lowensohn, 2011).
There has also been a push to integrate tablet technology into the classroom (Woodford, 2011). Tablet
computers moving into the classroom setting is a natural progression, especially considering the widespread use
of laptop computers and cell phones in today's society. Personal computers are already prevalent in Universities
with 97.5 percent of university students reporting they own a personal laptop (Kaplan, 2011). Class technology,
such as Classroom Presenter, has been shown to increase classroom participation and focus (Koile & Singer,
2006). In August 2011, Yale school of medicine gave 520 students an iPad 2 in an effort to save money by
cutting down on paper, books, and issues transporting information (Woodford, 2011). Because they are a
relatively new technology, little research has been conducted into critical reading ability on these devices and
the movement of the tablet into the classroom.
There are conflicting results regarding reading comprehension and reading speed in relation to the text medium
presented. Oborne and Holton (1988) tested reading comprehension, comparing distance from screen, angle of
the paper or screen, and light text on a dark background versus dark text on a light background. Participant's
reading comprehension was evaluated along with time taken to read the four passages from short stories written
by H.H. Munro. Oborne and Holton (1988) found no significant difference in reading comprehension when
comparing screen versus paper. However, considering the change in technology and quality of screen and text
presentation available today, it is reasonable to suggest results may differ if the study were replicated with
modern technology.
Kruk and Muter (1984) found university students read slower from a video screen than from books. Researchers
placed participants at varying distances from the screen and altered line spacing. Neither of these conditions
accounted for the significant difference in reading speed between video screen and from paper (Kruk & Muter,
1984). These conflicting results beg the question: does an actual difference exist? If so, is the difference enough
to hinder or encourage the integration of tablet computer technology into the modern classroom?
In a study by Holzinger, Baernthaler, Pammer, Kratz, Bjelic-Radisc, and Ziefle (2011), 111 Austrian clinicians
read two separate reports, one from paper and one from of a computer screen. Clinicians performed equally well
on a chunked reading evaluation of the two separate reports in the two separate mediums. The Austrian doctors
stated a 90 percent preference for reports read on paper versus on a high-resolution screen, however there was
no significant difference in reading comprehension (Holzinger et al., 2011). Participants reported preferred
paper because of it's flexibility, durability, portability and their ability to take notes on the paper itself simply
and efficiently (Holzinger et al. 2011).
Ball and Horcade (2012) found that reading speed and comprehension increase with age, regardless of reading
medium. Two groups were involved in testing critical reading ability. One group read a text passage from paper,
while the other group read the same text passage from an iPad 2. A 12-question critical reading evaluation was
given to assess critical reading differences as influenced by text medium, academic standing, and review for
interactive effects.
The general lack of consistency in study results, as well as the amount of time that has passed between studies
(Kruk & Muter, 1984 & Holzinger et al. 2011) begs the question: Is there a difference in critical reading scores
between mediums. If so, should there be an integration of tablet computers into the classroom?
The purpose of the current study is to establish whether there is a difference in critical reading ability between
mediums; and if this does exist, does the comprehension gap differ by academic standing (e.g., freshman)? My
prediction, based on previous research, is that there will be a statistically significant difference in between
mediums in favor of paper and that difference expands as the age of the participant increases. Further, I predict
the preference for paper rather than computer screen will increase with the age of the participant.
Method
Because of the lack of consistency concerning reading comprehension studies and the effects of medium as
related to both comprehension and preference, two studies were performed. The first study was a critical
reading assessment based on a 949 word reading passage. The second was a self-assessment of preference and
reading habits.
Study 1
The goal of this study was to assess differences in critical reading scores comparing scores produced from
reading from iPad 2 tablet screens and paper. Participant academic standing was collected to assess whether
familiarity with computer screens and exposure to literature impact critical reading ability.
Participants
116 Sierra Nevada College undergraduate students were randomly assigned to one of four groups categorized
by text medium and academic standing: (a) lower classmen reading from paper, (b) lower classmen reading
from a tablet, (c) upper classmen reading from paper, (d) upper classmen reading from a tablet.
Materials
Participants were presented a portion of a text titled The Mayor of Casterbridge by Thomas Hardy (Mathur,
2010). In the paper condition, the passage was presented on OfficeMax 50% recycled multipurpose paper (92
bright, 75 gsm, 216 mm x 279 mm) with 2.54 cm margins. In the iPad condition, the participants were
presented with the text on a 64 gb, Apple iPad 2. The document was presented in the "Pages" application.
"Pages" is a word processing application used for reading and editing documents. The iPad screen was 197 x
148 mm.
Procedure
Participants were presented a passage from The Mayor of Casterbridge by Thomas Hardy (Mathur, 2010) (see
Appendix A for reading Material). The passage consisted of 949 words. For the screen condition, the font used
was Helvetica (13 pt font). The paper version of the reading was an identical, printed version of the computer
screen reading condition to be sure then line breaks and words on each line are the same.
Participants were randomly assigned a condition, either paper or screen, using a random number generator.
Upon entering the testing room, the participant was greeted and given a description of the informed consent
form outlining the confidentiality and protection of information, procedures and responsibilities of the
experimenter. It was clarified that participants were not obligated to participate.
After completing the consent document, participants were provided their randomly assigned reading materials.
They were asked to read the passage with no time restriction. After participants indicated they were finished
reading the passage, they were presented a critical reading ability test (see Appendix C for questionnaire) in the
same medium their text passage was presented. If the participant read the passage on paper, their
comprehension test was also presented on paper and answered with a pen. Likewise, if the participant read the
passage on the iPad 2 tablet computer, the comprehension test was also presented and completed on the iPad 2
tablet computer.
Study 2
Participants
The second study assessed whether there was a preference among 112 Sierra Nevada College undergraduate
students for reading from paper vs. from iPad 2 screens. It also assessed the academic standing, gender, and
reading habits of the participants.
Procedure
The second study, a self-response survey, was administered once the comprehension test was complete.
Participants were handed a brief questionnaire about their age and academic standing. Questionnaires for
participants in both conditions were presented on paper and answered with a pencil (See Appendix D for
questionnaire).
Upon completion of the questionnaire, participants were thanked for their participation and debriefed. During
the debriefing session, participants were informed that they could request an email outlining the details of the
study.
Results
Study 1
The critical reading ability differences between tablet screens and paper were scored based on the number of
correct responses provided by participants. The scoring scale ranged from 0 – 12 using the SAT critical reading
evaluation (Mathur, 2010). The two factors (text medium and academic standing) were assessed using a twoway ANOVA test (α = .05); this compared the interactive effects between book and tablet critical reading
ability as well as the academic standing of the participants. There were no significant differences when
comparing critical reading scores between the iPad 2 and paper (p = .841) or when comparing upper and lower
division undergraduates and their critical reading ability (p = .375). The analysis of any interactive effects
using the general linear model showed no significant differences (p = .911).
251658240
Figure 1. Two-way ANOVA produced no significant difference among participant academic standing and
critical reading scores (p = .375), reading medium and critical reading scores (p = .841) or interactive effects (p
= .911).
Study 2
Evaluation of the self-response surveys concerning academic standing and reading preference were evaluated
using a chi-square test of independence (α = .05) as well as a goodness of fit test to assess statistically
significant differences among groups and preferences. A statistically significant difference was found when
assessing the preferences of the entire sample population. There was strong statistical evidence to support the
alternative hypothesis that there was a preference for reading from paper relative to a computer screen (p <
.001)*. There was no significant difference found when analyzing the preferences of upper and lower division
undergraduates (p = .157). A significant difference was found when comparing the reading preferences of males
and females. It was found that females have a stronger preference for reading from paper than males (p =
.045)*.
251658240
Figure 2. Chi-square test of independence showed no significance when comparing participant academic
standing and reading preference (p = .157)
251658240
Figure 3. Chi-square test of independence showed a significant difference for females preferring paper over
males (p = .045)*.
251658240
Figure 4. Chi-square goodness of fit test showed a significant preference for paper across the test population (n
= 112) (p < .001)*.
Discussion
The findings of the two-way ANOVA failed to reject the null hypothesis that there is a difference in critical
reading scores between mediums and support the null hypothesis that there is no difference in critical reading
ability when comparing reading from an iPad 2 tablet computer and paper. Additionally this study's findings
indicated a strong preference for reading from paper rather than computer screen. Although there is no
difference in reading comprehension scores between mediums, preference plays a large role in willingness to
attend to a particular task. Post secondary educational institutions have already begun integrating iPads into
their curriculum in an effort to reduce textbook costs and create a more sustainable educational system
(Woodford, 2011).
Because there was such strong evidence shown that medium does not influence critical reading ability, there is a
strong case made for the implementation of iPads into the college curriculum, if for no other reason than a longterm attempt to save book costs for both the students and the institution itself.
This finding falls in line with those of Holzinger et al. (2011) who found reading comprehension was unaffected
by computer screens, but there was an overwhelming preference for reading from paper, rather than computer
screen. Ball and Hourcade, (2011) found a difference in reading ability by age. The current study did not find
that to be the case, especially across mediums.
In an attempt to control for outliers and confounding variables, participants were randomly assigned the reading
medium (tablet vs. paper). The findings of this study could, alternatively, be due to familiarity with iPads and
other handheld electronic devices. This exposure was not taken into account. There were also no precautions
taken to rule out the influence of learning disabilities such as dyslexia that could have altered the outcome.
Further research could assess the potential for newer technology in the field and ink based screens that may
affect critical reading ability. The passage and questions used may not be the ideal material for assessing critical
reading ability and future research could address the relevance of the length, content, and analysis of critical
reading scores when assessing technology and its relation to written language comprehension.
Critical reading analysis is an essential component at any level of education. There is strong evidence to support
the claim there is no difference when comparing critical reading scores from an iPad 2 tablet and paper. This
supports the movement toward a more technologically based curriculum.
Books are still an essential part of both education and daily life. A preference for reading from paper, rather
than iPads or other forms of media is present within the education community. This means that, although there
is no difference in reading performance between mediums, there will likely always be a place for paper within
the curriculum. Some stated preferences for paper because they felt it was easier on the eyes, more convenient,
and easier to work with in general. As technology improves, these problems may become less relevant.
Currently, paper is still an important part of the education system.
As tablet technology progresses, the production process for iPads and tablet computers is likely to become
more sustainable and the processes involved in recycling iPads more practical. By switching the course
curriculum and reading lists to tablet computers, we are could offset the cost of a post-secondary education and
support sustainable practices at the same time, thereby making education more affordable to a population who
otherwise might not have access.
References
Ball, R., & Hourcade, J. P. (2011). Rethinking reading for age from paper and computers. International
Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 27(11), 1066-1082.
Holzinger, A., Baernthaler, M., Pammer, W., Kratz, H., Bjelic-Radisic, V., & Ziefle, M. (2011). Investigating
paper vs. screen in real-life hospital workflows: Performance contradicts perceived superiority of paper in the
user experience. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 69(9), 563-570.
Kaplan, A. (2011, November 29). Lets' get digital: Nearly all undergrads own laptops. Brown Daily Herald.
Retrieved from http://www.browndailyherald.com/let-s-get-digital-nearly-all-undergrads-own-laptops1.2677510#.T1gxHpdWq8o
Koile, K. & Singer, D. (2006). Improving learning in cs1 via tablet-pc-based in-class assessment. ICER '06:
Proceedings of the 2006 international workshop on Computing education research, 119–126.
Kruk, R. S., & Muter, P. (1984). Reading of continuous text on video screens. Human Factors, 26, 339-345.
Lowensohn, J. (2011, October 18). Apple's fiscal 2011 by the numbers. CNet News. Retrieved from
http://news.cnet.com/8301-27076_3-20122276-248/apples-fiscal-2011-by-the-numbers/
Mathur, H. (2012). SAT reading comprehension test 8. [Online Test]. Retrieved from
http://www.majortests.com/sat/reading-comprehension-test08
Oborne, D., & Holton, D. (1988). Reading from screen versus paper: There is no difference. International
Journal of Machine Studies, 28(1), 1-9.
Woodford, A. (2011, August 26). Med school gives students iPads. Yale Daily News. Retrieved from
http://www.yaledailynews.com/news/2011/aug/26/med-school-gives-students-ipads/
Appendix A
The Ring at Casterbridge was merely the local name of one of the finest Roman Amphitheatres, if not the very
finest, remaining in Britain.
Casterbridge announced old Rome in every street, alley, and precinct. It looked Roman, bespoke the art of
Rome, concealed dead men of Rome. It was impossible to dig more than a foot or two deep about the town
fields and gardens without coming upon some tall soldier or other of the Empire, who had lain there in his silent
unobtrusive rest for a space of fifteen hundred years. He was mostly found lying on his side, in an oval scoop in
the chalk, like a chicken in its shell; his knees drawn up to his chest; sometimes with the remains of his spear
against his arm, a fibula or brooch of bronze on his breast or forehead, an urn at his knees, a jar at his throat, a
bottle at his mouth; and mystified conjecture pouring down upon him from the eyes of Casterbridge street boys
and men, who had turned a moment to gaze at the familiar spectacle as they passed by.
Imaginative inhabitants, who would have felt an unpleasantness at the discovery of a comparatively modern
skeleton in their gardens, were quite unmoved by these hoary shapes. They had lived so long ago, their time was
so unlike the present, their hopes and motives were so widely removed from ours, that between them and the
living there seemed to stretch a gulf too wide for even a spirit to pass.
The Amphitheatre was a huge circular enclosure, with a notch at opposite extremities of its diameter north and
south. From its sloping internal form it might have been called the spittoon of the Jotuns. It was to Casterbridge
what the ruined Coliseum is to modern Rome, and was nearly of the same magnitude. The dusk of evening was
the proper hour at which a true impression of this suggestive place could be received. Standing in the middle of
the arena at that time there by degrees became apparent its real vastness, which a cursory view from the summit
at noon-day was apt to obscure. Melancholy, impressive, lonely, yet accessible from every part of the town, the
historic circle was the frequent spot for appointments of a furtive kind. Intrigues were arranged there; tentative
meetings were there experimented after divisions and feuds. But one kind of appointment—in itself the most
common of any—seldom had place in the Amphitheatre: that of happy lovers.
Why, seeing that it was pre-eminently an airy, accessible, and sequestered spot for interviews, the cheerfullest
form of those occurrences never took kindly to the soil of the ruin, would be a curious inquiry. Perhaps it was
because its associations had about them something sinister. Its history proved that. Apart from the sanguinary
nature of the games originally played therein, such incidents attached to its past as these: that for scores of years
the town-gallows had stood at one corner; that in 1705 a woman who had murdered her husband was halfstrangled and then burnt there in the presence of ten thousand spectators. Tradition reports that at a certain stage
of the burning her heart burst and leapt out of her body, to the terror of them all, and that not one of those ten
thousand people ever cared particularly for hot roast after that. In addition to these old tragedies, pugilistic
encounters almost to the death had come off down to recent dates in that secluded arena, entirely invisible to the
outside world save by climbing to the top of the enclosure, which few towns-people in the daily round of their
lives ever took the trouble to do. So that, though close to the turnpike-road, crimes might be perpetrated there
unseen at mid-day.
Some boys had latterly tried to impart gaiety to the ruin by using the central arena as a cricket-ground. But the
game usually languished for the aforesaid reason—the dismal privacy which the earthen circle enforced,
shutting out every appreciative passer's vision, every commendatory remark from outsiders—everything, except
the sky; and to play at games in such circumstances was like acting to an empty house. Possibly, too, the boys
were timid, for some old people said that at certain moments in the summer time, in broad daylight, persons
sitting with a book or dozing in the arena had, on lifting their eyes, beheld the slopes lined with a gazing legion
of Hadrian's soldiery as if watching the gladiatorial combat; and had heard the roar of their excited voices, that
the scene would remain but a moment, like a lightning flash, and then disappear.
It was related that there still remained under the south entrance excavated cells for the reception of the wild
animals and athletes who took part in the games. The arena was still smooth and circular, as if used for its
original purpose not so very long ago. The sloping pathways by which spectators had ascended to their seats
were pathways yet. But the whole was grown over with grass, which now, at the end of summer, was bearded
with withered bents that formed waves under the brush of the wind, returning to the attentive ear aeolian
modulations, and detaining for moments the flying globes of thistledown.
Henchard had chosen this spot as being the safest from observation which he could think of for meeting his
long-lost wife, and at the same time as one easily to be found by a stranger after nightfall. As Mayor of the
town, with a reputation to keep up, he could not invite her to come to his house till some definite course had
been decided on.
Appendix B
Brief script the experimenter is to follow when administering the experiment.
Experimenter: Hello, thanks for coming.
Experimenter invites participant to sit down and explains the IRB consent form and makes sure the participant
understands that he or she is allowed to leave at any time and is in no way obligated to participate in the study.
Experimenter: Please read this passage and let me know when you've finished (hand out both media types).
*Show participant how to scroll on iPad
Experimenter: Begin.
When participants indicate they have finished the reading, they are given the questions.
Experimenter: please answer these questions to the best of your abilities and let me know when you are
finished.
*iPad participants are shown how to indicate their answer on the iPad screen.
When participant indicates they have finished answering the Critical Reading Ability questions, they are handed
the Self-Response survey and asked to complete it.
Experimenter: Thank you very much for your participation. If you leave me your email, I can let you know the
importance of your participation in the study.
Appendix C
1. The amphitheatre is described as a 'suggestive' place because
A. its real size could not be appreciated at a glance.
B. it was full of historical associations
C. mysterious meetings took place there
D. it was lonely yet accessible
E. it was best appreciated in the evening.
2. The word 'hoary' is closest in meaning to
A. unimaginative
B. buried
C. curled up
D. mummified
E. ancient
3. The 'curious enquiry' refers to finding out
A. why happy lovers never met there
B. why interviews never took place there
C. what historical events took place there
D. how the amphitheatre came to have sinister associations
E. why the amphitheatre lay in ruins
4. The word 'round' most nearly means
A. route
B. routine
C. meanderings
D. circle
E. journey
5. The boys had given up cricket in the Amphitheatre in part because
A. it was too dark
B. crimes commonly took place there
C. there were no spectators or passers-by to applaud their efforts
D. they were afraid of being caught
E. it was too exposed to the weather
6. The author's primary purpose is to
A. justify his opinion of the Ring
B. attempt to account for the atmosphere of a place
C. chronicle the development of the Amphitheatre
D. describe the location of a Roman relic
E. explain the uses to which historical sites are put
7. The attitude of the local residents to the unearthed remains of dead Romans was one of
A. total apathy
B. confusion and unease
C. trepidation
D. momentary interest
E. revulsion
8. The incident of the woman who was burnt is mentioned in order to
A. horrify the reader
B. illustrate one reason for the unsavoury reputation of the place
C. show the bloodthirsty nature of former occupants
D. add realistic details to an imaginary plot
E. show the magnitude of the gulf between the past and the present
9. All of the following are said to have taken place at the Ring except
A. ghostly apparitions
B. boxing matches
C. hangings
D. secret assignations
E. theatrical performances
10. It can be inferred from the last paragraph that Henchard
A. is afraid of his wife
B. has something to hide from the townspeople
C. is a stranger to the Ring
D. is about to commit a crime
E. is an infamous resident of Casterbridge
11. The ring was 'safest from observation' because
A. no one inside could be seen from outside the arena
B. it was far from the main road
C. people found it a pleasant place only in Summer
D. no one except lovers ever went there after dark
E. it was too inaccessible
12. It appears that in general the attitude of Casterbridge residents to the Roman past suggests that they
A. appreciated the art of the Romans
B. feared the ghosts of the buried Roman soldiers
C. felt far removed from the concerns of the Romans
D. were awe-struck by their civilization
E. were proud of their heritage
What is your Academic Standing? :
Appendix D
Reading preference questionnaire given to participants.
Age:
Gender:
Academic Standing
1. What is your preferred media form to use when reading?
a) Computer Screen
b) Paper
c) I have no preference
d) Other
If other please state here:
2. How often do you read for pleasure?
a) Never
b) Less than 5 times a year
c) Once a month
d) Once a week
e) Every day
3. If you indicated you have a preferred media form to read, why do you prefer that form of media?
4. In an average day what form of media do you use to read most often?
a) Computer Screen
b) Paper
c) Television
d) Other
If other please state here:
Download