Emily Jeffries PEP 291 Andrew Eberline 18 November 2014 Journal #1 Retention of motor skills is one of the fourteen principles of motor development that I have noticed through my pre-school age student in the Muncie Head Start program. This principle implies that grasping the knowledge of various motor skills is related to over learning the skills to retain the cues and perform them properly. Continuous skills are more likely to be retained better than discrete skills by children. My student, who is three years old, picked up the continuous skill of running quite easily from the very first trial. She was able to follow every cue without fail. Although, the discrete skill of throwing she did struggle with a first, because she would leave out vital cues such as stepping with her opposite foot and rotating her hips. As we continued to practice that skill, she performed all of the skills properly and actually became very accurate at throwing to a target. Some implications that follow this principle are concentration and focusing. My student would sometimes have to be reminded on a specific cue she was leaving out, but didn’t always take the time to focus on the cue she missed. She was more concentrated on what activities she wanted to do. Demonstration is another principle of motor development that was very vital to my student’s learning process. This principle implies that performing the skill before a child attempts the skill will result in a better outcome. Children often rely on visual information and are more likely to repeat what they see and not what we say. This principle was often used as a motivator in my experience at Head Start. I would demonstrate the specific skill to her and she would perform it, but then she would often say, “Now it’s your turn.” I used this technique to motivate her by saying, “ If I go one more time you have to go twice.” An implication that could occur from this principle is during a testing day. An implication with this principle is that at the end of an assessment period, the student is supposed to be knowledgeable of the skills and should not rely on an educator to demonstrate the skill. Without demonstration I don’t believe that my student would have performed as much as she had, because she is very impatient and can only stick to an activity for a short amount of time. This principle for me was more often used as a motivator than as a visual. The preoperational stage of development has a sub stage known as the preconceptual stage and includes children ages 2-4 years old. During this particular stage of development children begin pretend play, but also start to cooperate with others and move into parallel play and interactive play. Children in this category also tend to react to similar objects as if they were identical. The main characteristic of the preconceptual stage that I have noticed in my student is egocentrism. Egocentrism refers to the child's inability to see a situation from another person's point of view. According to Piaget, an egocentric child assumes that other people see, hear, and feel exactly the same as the child does. For example, at Head Start my student only wanted to do the activities that looked visually appealing to her. I would try to explain to her that if we did all of the activities we would make it around eventually to the ones she was looking forward to, but from her point of view all she was worried about was completing the activities she liked. Most children at this age have mild meltdowns and pout in order to get their way. She thought that if she talked to her classroom teachers they would allow her to do want she wanted, not knowing that her teachers would tell her to follow the instructions I gave her. Egocentrism in children ages 2-4 leads them to believe that everyone thinks the same way they do. Egocentrism is definitely a huge implication when it comes to teaching students successfully. Piaget and others have suggested that children learn how to take the perspectives of others better through interacting with their peers than with adults. So therefore, activities that emphasize cooperative learning, peer group discussion, and crossage teaching are well-suited to introduce instances of cognitive conflict that require better appreciation of the perspective of others. Working with others doesn’t appear to be a problem during this stage as long as there is enough equipment for every child. Parallel and interactive play is popular with this age group, but may not work well when sharing is involved. This aspect might bring up issues, because sharing relates back to egocentrism.