The Relationship between First Imprisonment and Criminal Career Development: A Matched Samples Comparison Presentation at the 2nd Annual Workshop on Criminology and the Economics of Crime June 5-6, Wye Maryland Paul Nieuwbeerta & Arjan Blokland NSCR Daniel Nagin Carnegie-Mellon University Main Question • To what extent is there an effect of imprisonment on subsequent criminal career development (here: in the three years after imprisonment)? T1 T2 Criminal behavior Criminal behavior Imprisonment Imprisonment Criminal propensity = Incapacitation effect = Deterrence effect Hypotheses on effect of imprisonment DLC and Deterrence literature: • No effect: – Life circumstances (incl. imprisonment) have no effect • Decrease: – Imprisonment causes the punished individual to revise upward his/her estimate of severity and/of likelihood of punishment for future lawbreaking – Rehabilitation, for example by education and vocational training • Increase: – – – – ‘Imprisonment was not as adverse as anticipated’ Imprisonment reduces estimate of punishment certainty Prison is ‘school for crime’ Labeling: stigmatization socially and economically • Different effects for different (groups of) persons: – E.g. for ‘life course persisters’ no effect of imprisonment, for adolescent limited negative effect of imprisonment (imprisonment = ‘snare’) How to test for effects of imprisonment? • In a perfect world for science: randomized treatment assignment in an experimental setting – Then by design all differences between people in treatment group and in the non-treatment group are cancelled out • However, randomly imposing prison sentences is somewhat difficult and debatable • So, we (have to) use: – Data from observational longitudinal studies – A ‘quasi-experimental design’ and – Statistical approaches to control for differences between the treatment and non-treatment group Criminal Career and Life Course Study CCLS Data Sample: • 5.164 persons convicted in 1977 in the Netherlands – – – – – 4% random sample of all persons convicted in 1977 500 women (10%) 20% non-Dutch (Surinam, Indonesia) Mean age in 1977: 27 years; youngest: 12; oldest 79 Data from year of birth until 2003: for most over 50 years. CCLS Data • Full criminal conviction histories (Rap sheets) – Timing, type of offense, type of sentence, imprisonment. • Life course events (N=4,615): – Various types: marriage, divorce, children, moving, death (GBA & Central Bureau Heraldry) – incl. Exac timing. – Cause of death (CBS) Challenges when examining effects of imprisonment I • Challenges: – – – – Crime is age-graded Men and women differ in criminal behavior People die Earlier imprisonment experiences may also influence criminal behavior • Solutions used in this paper: – We only examine effects of imprisonment at a certain age: i.e. at age 26, 27 or 28 and examine the number of convictions in next 3 years. – We only examine a selection of persons (N = 3,008): • Men • Persons that did not die before age 31 • Persons who pre age 26 had not been imprisoned excluding 424 women excluding 20 men excluding 1163 men earlier imprisoned Outcome variable • Number of convictions in three year period after imprisonment • Imprisonment at age 26 (N = 66) 27 (N=55) 28 (N=63) Non-imprisoned age 26-28 Dep. Var.: convictions at age: 27, 28, 29 age: 28, 29, 30 age: 29, 30, 31 age: 28, 29, 30 • Correction for exposure-time / incarceration First time imprisonment between age 26-28 • 184 (6%) of the 3,008 persons who pre age 26 had not been imprisoned, are imprisoned for the first time at age 26, 27 or 28 • Length of imprisonment: 30 25 Percentage 20 15 10 5 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 Imposed sentence (in weeks) Naïve / Baseline comparison Challenges when examining effects of imprisonment II • Selection effect: prison sentences are consequence of: – Offender’s prior criminal record – Other characteristics Differences between imprisoned and non-imprisoned 4.00 Num of Conv. 3.00 2.00 1.00 0.00 Num. of conv. age 12-25 Num. of conv. age: 20-25 Num. of conv. age 25 non-imprisoned (n=2,824) Imprisoned age 26-28 (N = 184) Differences between imprisoned and non-imprisoned 0.50 Proportion 0.40 0.30 0.20 0.10 0.00 Non-Dutch Married Children non-imprisoned (n=2,824) Unemployed Alcohol dep. Imprisoned age 26-28 (N = 184) Drugs dep. Methods • Four statistical approaches to account for systematic differences between imprisoned and non-imprisoned: – – – – Regression Propensity scores matching Trajectory group matching Combination of Trajectory group and Propensity score matching Trajectory group matching • For more information: See Haviland & Nagin 2005 • Semi-Parametric group-based trajectories of lagged outcome variable estimated for non-treated up to age t (here: age 12-25) • Outcome variable measured between age t and age t+x (here: age 26-28) • Within-groups: compare outcomes from age t forward (here: age 26-28) to assess treatment effect Age–crime curve Four Trajectories Group 0: Effect of imprisonment Group 1: Effect of imprisonment Group 2: Effect of imprisonment Group 3: Effect of imprisonment • Conclusion: – Imprisonment increases the number of convictions significantly, i.e. with about 0.6 convictions per year. • However: – Although substantial improvement compared to ‘uncontrolled situation’ – Within Trajectory groups no perfect balance between imprisoned and non-imprisoned on criminal history characteristics and personal characteristics was achieved Propensity Score Matching • Logistic regression: Dependent variable = imprisonment (0=no, 1=yes), Independent variables = all available (here: – Criminal history characteristics: • Num. of convictions age 12-25, 20-25 and at 25, • Age of first registration, age of first conviction, • Trajectory group membership probabilities. – Personal Characteristics: • • • • Age in 1977, non-Dutch, Unemployed around age 25, Number of years married at age 25, Married at age 25, Number of years children at age 25, children at age 25, Alcohol and/or drugs dependent around age 25 • Calculate propensity scores: i.e. predicted probabilities to be imprisoned. • Match imprisoned persons to non-imprisoned persons with same/similar propensity scores – This creates ‘balance’ on all available characteristics between imprisoned and non-imprisoned (See: Rosenbaum & Rubin1983, 1984, 1985) Combination Trajectory Group Matching & Propensity Score Matching • Within each trajectory group the imprisoned are matched to a non-imprisoned person with the same/similar propensity score Group 0: Effect of imprisonment Group 1: Effect of imprisonment Group 2: Effect of imprisonment Group 3: Effect of imprisonment Summary of Estimated Treatment Effects of Imprisonment (in number of convictions per year) Trajectory Group Gr. 0 Uncontrolled Trajectory Group Matching Combination Traj. Group & Prop. Matching 0.60 0.47 Gr. 1 0.57 0.53 Gr. 2 0.33 0.25 Gr. 3 0.83 0.90 0.62 0.62 All (PATE) 0.62 Note: All effects are statistically significant p<0.05 Q: What if you look at …..? • Participation (i.e. 0 = no conviction, 1 = one or more conviction(s) in a year) [instead of ‘number of crimes’]: – Same conclusions • Convictions of specific types of crimes, e.g. property crimes, violent crimes and other crimes [instead of ‘all convictions’] - Same conclusions - Imprisonment at other ages, e.g. 20-22 [instead of at age 26-28]: – Same conclusions Conclusions • Conclusion: – In the three years after imprisonment those who have been imprisoned have on average .6 extra convictions per year, compared to the non-imprisoned – Effects of imprisonment are similar across trajectory groups – Conclusions are very similar regardless of method used • Theoretical implications: – Results in line with dynamic DLC theories • Life circumstance “imprisonment” has effect - even for ‘persistent’ group • Policy implications: – Incapacitation effect of imprisonment may partly be nullified by imprisoned offenders subsequently offending at higher rates