September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Implementing the NCHIP Online Drinking Survey to Keele University and Researching Risk Factors for Alcohol Use Disorder By Kara Holloway Word count: 15,306 Submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of MSc Psychology September 2014 1 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Abstract Rationale: Research assessing the links between the alcohol hangover and its links to alcohol use disorder (AUD) are sporadic and still in their infancy. This research will provide a body of evidence from the UK, of which there is currently none. Furthermore, Keele University has a recognised issue with alcohol on campus with the need to implement a drugs and alcohol policy for students and staff; the data collection aspect of the survey will provide information on student’s drinking patterns and behaviours. Methods: An online survey consisting of the NCHIP, HSS and SMAST was emailed to every student member of Keele University. From which participants drinking patterns and behaviours were calculated, participants were categorised into those with a positive family history of AUD (FHP) and those with a negative family history of AUD (FHN) and hangover severity was derived. The survey was run in March and July in order to gain information about drinking behaviours at differing times of the academic year. Aims: Use the NCHIP to collect relevant statistics for the benefit of Keele University to address issues of harmful and hazardous drinking amongst students. Use the NCHIP, HSS and SMAST to assess links between the familial risk for AUD, the alcohol hangover and the development of AUD. Results: Students at Keele University are showing high levels of high risk drinking episodes (29% of students engaging in high risk drinking at least once in the previous fortnight). Furthermore, 13% of males and 21.5% of females displayed estimated blood alcohol levels above a safe level. There was no difference in drinking patterns between FHP and FHN participants however hangover severity was significantly higher in FHP participants (p=0.012). We can predict that these participants will be more likely to engage in ‘hair of the dog’ drinking in order to overcome the symptoms of the hangover which could lead to AUD. 2 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Acknowledgements I would like to thank all the people who helped and supported me in the writing of this dissertation. Firstly, I would like to thank Dr Richard Stephens, my supervisor, for guiding me through every aspect of this project. I would also like to thank you for all the support you have provided me throughout my entire academic year at Keele University. Secondly, I would like to thank Ian Munton and his team at Student Support for supporting the project and distributing and encouraging participation of the survey. I would also like to thank you for the Amazon vouchers you provided as an incentive to students. Thirdly, I would like to thank my family for proof reading many stages of the written project, and again for the support you have provided me whilst at Keele University. 3 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table of Contents Title page 1 Abstract 2 Acknowledgements 3 Table of contents 4 Introduction 5 Methods 16 Results 22 Discussion 37 References 44 Appendices Attached disc Tables 1. Demographics and descriptives of participants 2. Drinking patterns of participants 3. Rates (frequency and %) of drinking related harm 4. Social impacts of drinking: experiences as a result of someone else’s drinking 5. Drinking behaviours: respondents who provided help to others 6. Rates (frequency and %) of drinking related benefits 7. Participants above safe limits for eBAC 8. Descriptives for FHN and FHP participants 9. Drinking patterns and behaviours of FHN and FHP participants 10. Mean & SD of hangover severity according to eBAC 11. Frequencies for positive consequences of drinking Figures 1. Regression graph of hangover severity against drinks consumed for FHP participants 2. Regression graph of hangover severity against drinks consumed for FHN participants 4 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Introduction Alcohol Use Alcohol is Europe’s largest risk factor for disease burden and is associated with many serious social and developmental issues (Global Strategy to Reduce Harmful Use of Alcohol, 2011). The NHS website states that women should not regularly drink more than 2-3 units a day and men should not regularly drink more than 3-4 units a day (NHS, 2014). Here, they define ‘regularly’ as drinking this amount every day or most days. Keeping within these guidelines will limit the harm caused by alcohol consumption. Binge drinking for men is defined as 8 or more units of alcohol which is the equivalent of three pints of strong beer. For women, a binge drink is defined as 6 or more units which is the equivalent of two large glasses of wine (Drink Aware, 2014). Binge drinking can lead to accidents, injuries and antisocial and violent behaviour (Drink Aware, 2014) yet despite this it is a common occurrence amongst young adults, in particular, students (Johnson, 2014). The Drink Aware website (Drink Aware, 2014) states that alcohol is a factor in 30% of sexual offences, 33% of burglaries and 50% of street crimes. Furthermore, binge drinking is most common among 16-24 year olds, and emphasises the dangers of binge drinking when young; those who binge drink in their teens are up to twice as likely to be binge drinkers 25 years later (The BMJ, 2005). The Institute of Alcohol Studies reported that 31% of 16-24 year olds drink above the recommended low-risk guidelines. Furthermore, a higher proportion of 16 to 24 year-old women reported drinking more than the guidelines on at least 1 day in the previous week compared with the national average for all female age groups [31% compared to 28% total] in 2011 (IAS, 2013). Stark (2007) interviewed 16-24 year olds and reported that 33% classify themselves as binge drinkers, 22% have passed out five or more times and 63% were more likely to have a one-night stand while drunk. These statistics emphasis the danger of adolescent and young adult drinking, and highlights this age group as at risk. Webb, Ashton, Kelly & Kamali (1996) issued 3075 second year students at university with a questionnaire assessing drinking, use of illicit drugs, lifestyle variables and ratings of depression and anxiety. It was reported that 61% of the males and 48% of the females exceeded safe limits of alcohol consumption; emphasising the student age range to be at high risk of negative alcohol consequences in the future. Johnson (2014) as part of the learning collaborative on high risk drinking suggest several reasons as to why this age group may be of risk; student living areas that provide and encourage high risk drinking such as fraternities and sororities, the density of bars and clubs that surround campuses, the events and rituals that are engrained in university life such as hazing, and inconsistent communication and messaging about high risk drinking and consequential harm. 5 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology The demand for alcohol research has been emphasised in the European Commission Strategy released in 2006 (EU Alcohol Strategy, 2006). Section 5.4 and 5.5 state the need to inform, educate and raise awareness on the impact of harmful and hazardous alcohol consumption and to develop, support and maintain a common evidence base. The report highlighted young people as a target audience for health education and awareness, raising interventions and for the gaining of comparable information on alcohol consumption in young people; achieved through the use of students in the current study. Section 5.5 also details the demand for more defined effects of alcohol, both on health and socially and that gaps in the research are filled, which has promoted recent research in the field. Since the implementation of this strategy, there have been promising beginnings, for example, the link between alcohol use and wider socio-economic issues has been brought more sharply into focus. However, the First Progress Report on the Implementation of the EU Alcohol Strategy (2009) stated that alcohol consumption still contributes to a substantial loss of productivity and thus more research needs to be conducted to establish more definite effects of alcohol consumption which will become more recognised by the public, and in particular, young people. This strategy has been recognised in several recent studies that support the idea of education through research. Zeigler et al. (2005) stated that adolescent alcohol consumption is associated with implications in learning and intellectual development; and that they fully support the view that action is justified to protect adolescents from this through marketing and changing the normalisation of adolescent alcohol consumption. The consequences of alcohol abuse are evident not just on a personal level but also on a societal level. Stockwell (1998) stated that the greatest cost incurred in the workplace is decreased productivity as a direct effect of alcohol-related absence and poor job performance. According to a study carried out by Jarvilehto, Laakso and Virsu (1975), 1 million workdays are lost in Finland each year due to hangover-related issues. The NICE guidelines are accredited, internationally recognised guidelines followed by clinicians. The NICE guidelines (2013) identify alcohol use as a major health problem, and they advise the government on various policies that may be implemented with the aim of reducing this problem; specifically in regards to pricing, the way alcohol is marketed and availability. The implications and dangers listed above are a few of a long list of drinking-related issues, yet despite this, research around the alcohol hangover and its links to alcohol use disorder have been sporadic and are in it’s infancy. Recently, attention has been paid to the link between alcohol use and the hangover, and its potential links with familial risk and the development of Alcohol Use Disorder (AUD). The Alcohol Hangover 6 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology The alcohol hangover has been known since Biblical times: “Woe unto them that rise up early in the morning, that they may follow strong drink” (Isaiah, 5:11). The alcohol hangover occurs as the blood alcohol concentration (BAC) approaches zero, a “puzzling post-intoxication phenomenon” (Penning, Nuland, Fliervoet, Olivier & Verster; 2010). Although a clear consensus on a definition of the alcohol hangover is lacking, previous literature identified a common cluster of symptoms such as headache, sickness, dizziness, nausea and diarrhoea (Wiese, Shlipak & Browner, 2000). More objective indications reported are decreased occupational, cognitive, or visual–spatial skill performance (Yesavage & Leirer, 1986; Kupari, 1983). Just as a clear definition is lacking so is an elicit mechanism of the alcohol hangover. It is suggested that Acetaldehyde, the dehydrogenated product of alcohol metabolism, might be responsible for hangover symptoms (Bogin, Nostrant, Young, 1987; Tsukamoto, Kanegae, Saito, Nagoya, Shimamura & Tainaka, 1991). It is also suggested that Congeners, the by-products of individual alcohol preparations found in brandy and wine increase the frequency and severity of hangover (Damrau & Goldberg, 1971; Pawan, 1973; Damrau & Liddy, 1960). The alcohol hangover has also been associated with a dysregulated cytokine pathway which would produce symptoms such as nausea, headache and diarrhoea. Studies involving experimentally induced alcohol hangover have reported that alcohol increases levels of thromboxane B2 which in turn alters cytokine production (Kanasaho, Hillbom, Kaste & Vapaatalo, 1982). Furthermore, changes in antidiuretic hormone levels have been observed in people with a hangover; the more alcohol consumed the more inhibited the function of the antidiuretic hormone becomes from which dehydration persists; and this is what gives rise to so many of the symptoms of the alcohol hangover that get reported (Linkola, Ylikahri, Fyhquist & Wallenius, 1978; Linkola, Fyhrquist & Ylikahri, 1979; Linkola, Fyhrquist, Nieminen, Weber & Tontti, 1976; Hangover Cures, 2012). Verster, Herwijnen, Olivier and Kahler (2009) developed the Dutch version of the brief young adult alcohol consequences questionnaire and found that most commonly reported alcoholrelated consequence was the alcohol hangover (74.3%). As previously mentioned, 16-24 year olds are at particularly high risk of binge drinking behaviours and thus more susceptible to hangovers. A recent study showed how college students often overestimated the number of alcoholic drinks it would take to cause a hangover the following day showing a failure to learn from past drinking experiences. Mallett et al. (2006) used self-report techniques to assess drinking behaviour associated consequences of drinking in 303 college students. They also asked students to estimate the number of drinks associated with the risk of 7 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology experiencing future similar consequences. T tests revealed that students significantly overestimated the number of drinks it would take to vomit, have unwanted sexual experiences, experience hangover and black out (Mallett, Lee, Neighbors, Clarimer & Turrisi; 2006). As a result of publications exploring the effects of the alcohol hangover on functioning, several hangover scales have been produced; with the aim of identifying hangover severity in various populations, and to improve the quality and reliability of research in the field (Penning, McKinney, Bus, Olivier, Slot & Verster; 2013). One prominent scale, produced by Slutske, Piasecki and Hunt-Carter in 2003, is the thirteen item Hangover Symptom Scale (HSS) where subjects report hangover symptoms they have experienced in the past twelve months. Each item focuses on one of thirteen hangover symptoms; participants are asked to report in the past 12 months when they drank alcohol, how often they experienced the symptom the next morning. Participants are then asked to state more specifically the number of times they experienced that symptom the next morning after drinking alcohol as a sub section to the question. Following the development of the HSS, Slutske, Piasecki and HuntCarter (2003) reported that the HSS captures a valid set of adjectives describing common hangover effects. More recently, Robertson, Piasecki, Slutske, Wood, Sher, Shiffman & Heath (2012) corroborated the HSS as a valid tool for alcohol hangover research. They reported that “higher HSS scores identify individuals who complain of real world hangovers and who may be especially likely to display particular symptoms after a night of drinking.” An interesting calculation that can help clinicians and researchers to make an approximation on the blood alcohol level (BAC) of participants in order to obtain information about ethanol exposure is the eBAC (estimated blood alcohol concentration). The formula to obtain the eBAC takes into account the number of drinks consumed and the hours over which they were consumed, as well as height, weight and sex of the participant. There are many other extraneous variables to be considered when calculating the eBAC such as body type, food consumed and medication however it provides a rough estimate of the blood concentration in order to gain a higher level of understanding about a participant’s alcohol consumption. Recent research has suggested that the alcohol hangover is experienced when the individual’s eBAC reaches 0.1; this value can be used in research now to assess the likelihood of a hangover occurring (Kruisselbrink, 2014). The Alcohol Hangover & Familial Risk 8 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology The National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (2012), states the seriousness of Alcohol Use Disorder (AUD) and highlights symptoms including craving (a need or urge to drink), loss of control (being unable to stop drinking once started), physical dependence (withdrawal symptoms such as nausea and dizziness) and tolerance (drinking more alcohol but feeling the same effect as a lower dose would have previously). According to the DSM-5, Alcohol Use Disorder is defined as the presence of at least two of the following symptoms; alcohol is often taken in larger amounts or over a longer period than intended, there is a persistent desire or unsuccessful efforts to cut down or control alcohol use, a great deal of time is spent in activities necessary to obtain alcohol, use alcohol or recover from its effects, craving or a strong urge to use alcohol, recurrent alcohol use resulting in a failure to fulfil major role obligations, continued alcohol use despite having persistent problems caused by alcohol, important activities given up because of alcohol use, recurrent alcohol use in situations in which it is physically hazardous, alcohol use is continued despite knowledge of physical or psychological problem, tolerance and withdrawal (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The severity of AUD is defined as either mild (2 or 3 symptoms), moderate (4 or 5 symptoms) or severe (6 or more symptoms). The alcohol hangover can alter the development of alcohol use disorders, yet this factor has received little attention in terms of current research; by 2005 only five studies had explored the links between hangover and familial risk for AUD (Piasecki, Sher, Slutske & Jackson, 2005). Piasecki et al. (2005) exposed that family history positive (FHP) participants tended to report more hangover than family history negative (FHN) participants; even when drinking frequency and sex was controlled in multilevel models. Furthermore, FHP participants tending to be more impulsive and less sensitive to punishment cues, suggesting a longer trial and error period to detect relationships between alcohol consumption and the negative effects of the alcohol hangover. Similarly, in 1999, Span and Earlywine conducted a study which sought to replicate work done on the relationship between risk for AUD and hangover. Newlin and Pretorious (1990) and McCaul, Turkkan, Svilis and Bigelow (1991) had published work previously suggesting that sons of alcoholics (SOA) report greater hangover symptoms than sons of non-alcoholics (SONA). Span and Earlywine (1999) used the McAndrew scale as an index of personality risk for alcoholism to compare the sons of alcoholics (SOA) and sons of non-alcoholics (SONA). Participants also completed the McCaul, Turkkan, Svilis and Bigelow (1991) and Newlin and Pretorious (1990) assessments of hangover. Subjects attended two consecutive testing sessions where they consumed a placebo drink in one 9 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology condition and alcohol in the other. It was concluded that the 20 SOAs reported significantly greater hangover symptoms than SONAs. Furthermore, those with an increased risk for alcoholism reported that they experienced more acute withdrawal and hangover which might contribute to the development of problem drinking. Over the past 25 years, researchers have used familial risk to enrich studies and encourage future investigation into the risk factors for the development of AUD. Piasecki, Sher, Slutske & Jackson (2005) conducted a longitudinal study where data was drawn from 3,156 college freshman all of whom were screened for family history of AUD using the Short Michigan Alcohol Screening Test (SMAST); this assesses paternal and maternal drinking problems. The 26 item SMAST was developed in 1992 by Crews and Sher and it is commonly used to identify familial risk of AUD; with questions relating to both the subjects mother’s (M-SMAST) and father’s (F-SMAST) drinking patterns and behaviours. The questions are designed to reveal problem behaviours exhibited by participants’ parents. Crew and Sher (1992) reported that the overall reliability and validity of the F-SMAST and M-SMAST was good and appropriate for researchers and clinicians interested in a parental history of AUD. They compared those with a positive family history of alcohol use disorders (FHP), and those with a negative family history (FHN) of Alcohol Use Disorder. Hangover was measured using the Young Adult Alcohol Problem Screening Test (YAAPST) which is a 27-item questionnaire designed to assess the frequency of a variety of negative consequences of drinking; responses were between 0 to 9 where 0 indicated no hangover in the past year and 9 indicated hangover experienced 40 or more times in the past year. The Alcohol Hangover as a Risk Factor for AUD In 2004, Park assessed whether the alcohol hangover acted as a deterrent to further consumption, as previously assumed, or if it in fact caused further consumption of alcohol. They found that college students reviewed drinking more alcohol, once hung-over, as a positive way to outweigh the negative consequences and that these positive feelings would influence their future drinking decisions. Several studies have explored this further and again found that experience of the alcohol hangover is not associated with reduced consumption; furthermore, it has been suggested that not only will it fail to deter drinkers; it may even increase their consumption the next morning (Mallett, Lee, Neighbors, Clarimer & Turrisi, 2006; Earlywine, 1993a). The idea of drinking alcohol in order to relieve symptoms of previous alcohol consumption is commonly called ‘hair of the dog’ drinking. As previously mentioned the alcohol hangover is associated with congeners in alcohol. Ethanol and methanol are metabolised by the alcohol dehydrogenase and so drinking ethanol prevents or delays the metabolism of methanol causing a temporary relief of symptoms (Ylikahri, 10 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Huttunen, Eriksson & Nikkila, 1974; Bentdsen, Wayne Jones & Helander, 1998). Robertson (2012) stated that past hangovers predicted future hangovers, suggesting hangovers do not necessarily discourage or inhibit future drinking. As alcohol relieves its own symptoms the following day, it is a case of negative reinforcement which causes it’s consumers to engage in ‘hair of the dog’ drinking. Negative reinforcement is when behaviour is strengthened by avoiding an aversive stimulus; in this case, drinking alcohol is enforced by the lack of hangover symptoms experienced. One mechanism that could underlie the relationship between the hangover and alcohol consumption is the physiological effects of alcohol on FHP participants when the limb of BAC is rising compared to when the limb is falling. Rising limb effects suggest that as your BAC increases, so does your sensitivity to drinking consequences. In 1999, Leonard and Blane wrote that sons of alcoholics show hypersensitivity to alcohol’s effects during the rising limb of the BAC curve when the pleasurable and activating consequences of the drinking dominate. When the BAC curve displays a falling limb, the negative and depressing effects of alcohol dominate. Furthermore, McKinney and Coyle (2006) reported that during the rising limb of the blood alcohol curve, alcohol has a stimulating effect by affecting neurotransmitters that control the sleep/wake cycle. Sher (1991) reported that FHP participants may exhibit stronger physiological reactions to a variety of stimuli when compared to FHN participants. Furthermore, Fin et al. (1990) reported that children of alcoholics display greater responses to stimuli regardless of the nature of the stimulus. There is a wealth of evidence suggesting that the rising limb of the BAC curve correlates with a hypersensitive response in FHP participants for a range of stimuli including the physiological consequences of drinking, however, it is suggested that the exact mechanism underlying the rising limb effects are replicated and clarified (Sayette, 1999). Alcohol use disorders are genetic, suggesting they run in families (Dawson, Harford & Grant, 1992) Hussong, Bauer and Chassin (2008) reported that children of alcoholics progressed more quickly from adolescent drinking to the onset of an AUD when compared to children of non-alcoholics which was a direct result of their parent’s externalising symptoms and drinking patterns. Therefore, it is imperative to examine familial links in terms of severity of hangovers and the various alleviating behaviours for this negative consequence. Earlywine (1993b) investigated hangover as a potential mediator of the relation between personality and drinking problems. The subjects were 178 undergraduates who were asked to complete a questionnaire assessing drinking patterns, familial risk for AUD, personality risk for AUD and hangover. Earlywine found that drinking problems only correlate with those who experience severe hangovers, not moderate hangovers, thus indicating that those who suffer from an AUD suffer from greater hangovers than those who do not suffer from an AUD. It is 11 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology then suggested that as those who are at risk for AUD experience the worst hangovers might then consume alcohol again in order to relieve the symptoms. One factor to be considered in terms of the hangover being a risk factor for AUD is the withdrawal effects of alcohol. Newlin & Pretorious (1990) assessed alcohol-induced hangovers among college men; using both sons of alcoholics and sons of non-alcoholics. The sons of alcoholics reported significantly worse hangovers to the extent where the hangover represented an acute withdrawal from alcohol. From this, Newlin and Pretorious have suggested that patients with frequent hangovers are at the greatest risk for further alcohol dependence. McCaul, Turkkan, Svikis and Bigelow (1991) extended previous research investigating the breadth of addiction risk conferred by a positive family history. Previous findings reported that FHP participants showed greater sensitivity across a variety of subjective measures than FHN participants. Data in the present study examined hangover effects from a controlled dose of alcohol, up to 3 to 18 hours after the administration of the drink. Withdrawal was assessed, amongst other factors, through a combination of mood analogue scales, drug effect self-reports and the Subjective High Assessment Scale. They reported that positive familial-risk participants reported extended intoxication and greater withdrawal effects following alcohol. Furthermore, these withdrawal effects persisted longer than those reported by those at negative familial-risk. Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie and Fiore (2004) presented the reformulated negative reinforcement model of addiction. This explores how early in a participant’s drug use, instances of drug-contingent relief of withdrawal are demonstrated to be influences towards the development of drug dependence. Theoretically, this explains how using the drug as a response to its own effects can easily lead that consumer to dependence i.e. drinking alcohol to alleviate symptoms of a hangover. The withdrawal relief model proposed by Span and Earlywine (1999) states that high risk participants of alcoholism will report a more severe hangover; showing withdrawal effects similar to those presented by Newlin and Pretorious (1990), and suggesting that individuals will then drink alcohol again to alleviate the severe symptoms; potentially leading to AUD (Span & Earlywine, 1999). Both expectancies of hangover and personality types were assessed and found no differences between sons of alcoholics and sons of non-alcoholics; reducing the effect of external influences. Verster (2009) argued that finding a cure for the alcohol hangover can result in more excessive drinking episodes, supporting the notion of ‘hair of the dog’ drinking. Earlywine (1993a) supported this by stating that those who experience greater hangover may drink more alcohol to alleviate adverse effects. Furthermore, in 2010, a survey revealed that 11% of participants had engaged in hair of the dog drinking; highlighting the percentage of that population at risk of AUD from this 12 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology mechanism (Verster, Keurten, Olivier & Laar; 2010). However, it is important to identify the difference between the alcohol hangover and withdrawal to prevent conflicting ideas. The hangover occurs after a single session of alcohol consumption; no matter how many times these sessions have occurred. Withdrawal, on the other hand, depends on a prolonged duration of alcohol abuse (Penning, Nuland, Fliervoet, Olivier & Verster; 2010). Relevance to Keele University Keele University, Staffordshire, has a regularly-updated alcohol policy currently in place (Human Resources Department, 2011) illustrating that it has been a recognised problem at the university to the extent where a policy needed to be implemented. Keele University would benefit greatly from a better understanding of students’ drinking patterns and consequent behaviours; creating a clearer focus for action. Once various analyses have been conducted, Keele University can then use the results to compare levels of drinking against those at other institutions, and also compare Keele student’s drinking levels to safe regulations and guidelines, provided by the government, to assess the extent to which students are engaging in binge drinking. The Student Support and Development Services at Keele University provide students with any support they require during their time at Keele University from advice and support to dealing with a physical or mental health disability. The head of Student Support, Ian Munton, has been extremely involved in the process of the survey as he has recognised the need for increased research and findings on student drinking. Ian Munton and his team are responsible for the distribution of the survey as they have access to email addresses of the entire student body. Furthermore, it was Student Support who were generous enough to provide the two £25 Amazon vouchers offered as mild incentives for students to complete the survey. Alongside the release of the survey in March, Ian Munton and his team ran a month long alcohol campaign at the Student’s Union where topics such as hangover, drink driving and Police Community Support were focused upon to raise awareness amongst students. This alcohol campaign not only raised awareness of critical issues which relate to the survey but also raised the validity of the survey; by emphasising that Student Support encourage participation and takes the topic seriously. Further analysis would reveal gender differences; allowing Student Support to run a more gender directed approach to tackling binge drinking. Finally, with the survey being run twice in the year, it will be possible to identify changes in drinking levels throughout the year. No university in the UK has a survey like this, therefore once the data has been collected it is possible that other institutions would be interested in Keele’s example and wish to be 13 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology involved in the project; representatives at Edge Hill University have already shown interest in using the survey to assess drinking patterns and behaviours at their university. Present Study Research surrounding links between familial risk, the alcohol hangover and the development of Alcohol Use Disorder (AUD) is still in its infancy and has been explored little and sporadically. It appears the consensus to be that those at highest familial risk for AUD will display significantly greater hangover than those who are not (Piasecki, Sher, Slutske & Jackson, 2005; Span & Earlywine, 1999). Furthermore, there is growing evidence that those who suffer from severe hangovers will then engage in hair of the dog drinking; and this is what can lead consumers to the onset of AUD; alleviating symptoms of alcohol use with alcohol (Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie and Fiore, 2004; Newlin & Pretorious, 1990). It is imperative that more research is conducted assessing the links between the alcohol hangover and the development of AUD as there is a gap in the literature for more in-depth analysis, especially using at risk samples such as students (Stark, 2007). As emphasised in the European Commission Strategy released in 2006, there is an increased need to educate, raise awareness and support and maintain the evidence base on alcohol research findings. This supports the need for a piece of research such as this one due to the sporadic nature of the existing literature on the specific topic. This survey will provide a deeper understanding of the topic, and not only this, but it will be the first study of its kind to be implemented in the UK. Furthermore, the NCHIP has not been applied outside of the USA before and thus this study presents an opportunity to gather detailed and thorough information about UK students drinking behaviours, which has never been done before to this extent. Combined with on-going research in the department, it will provide a body of UK evidence, as currently there is none. This suggests that studies conducted elsewhere, for example in the United States, are potentially not generalisable to UK samples; for example, drinking culture in the UK is much more embedded in an undergraduates university experience compared to the USA, also, the legal drinking age in the UK is much lower than in the USA. This research aims to provide a solution to this flaw. Keele University has found the need to implement an alcohol and drugs policy suggesting that is it a recognised problem with students. Student Support also plan to run a corresponding alcohol campaign to increase the awareness of such topics and encourage participation in the survey, showing the lengths the University are going to address the issue; illustrating the demand for an intervention. 14 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology The aims of the research are: 1) use the NCHIP (NCHIP, 2011), the HSS (Slutske, Piasecki & Hunt-Carter; 2003) and the F-SMAST and M-SMAST (Crew & Sher, 1992) to collect relevant statistics for the benefit of Keele University to address student drinking patterns and behaviours and 2) use the NCHIP (NCHIP, 2011), the HSS (Slutske, Piasecki & Hunt-Carter; 2003) and the F-SMAST and M-SMAST (Crew & Sher, 1992) to assess links between the familial risk for the alcohol hangover and the development of AUD. We hypothesise that the participants at highest familial risk for AUD as identified by the NCHIP, HSS and SMAST will report the greatest hangover symptoms, and thus present the highest risk for development of AUD. The survey consisting of the NCHIP, the HSS and the SMAST will be distributed online to every student member at Keele, once in March and once in July. From this, participants can be categorised into those who have a positive family history of AUD (FHP) and those who do not (FHN), from which hangover severity can be compared. Furthermore, information about student’s drinking patterns and behaviours can inform Keele University and Student Support on levels of hazardous and harmful drinking amongst its students. The two waves of the survey means that data can be collected from two opposing times of the academic year; mid-semester and during the examination period, from which levels of drinking amongst students can be compared and contrasted in order to create much more focused interventions. The National College Health Improvement Program (NCHIP) The National College Health Improvement Program was developed in 2011 at Dartmouth University and addressed a new way of looking at the problem of harmful and hazardous drinking amongst students. The survey uses comprehensive evaluation and measurement techniques to identify and implement the most effective ways to tackle the problem (NCHIP, 2011). The program relies on the Breakthrough Series Model which encourages face-to-face learning sessions to share outcomes of the survey; this helps to address high-risk drinking during ‘action periods’ where various campus teams engage in ‘plan-do-study-act’ cycles (a novel way to address a problem is tested on a small scale, from which rapid changes can be made based on the results). All members share a commitment to gathering and sharing evidence retrieved from their respective institutes, measuring their outcomes and feeding back to a collaborative system. The NCHIP thus allows rapid information sharing so that effective and practical strategies can be gained which suit each institutes circumstances this has shown significant success. Two years since the implementation, Dartmouth has collaborated with 32 colleges in America and has demonstrated great success; with the number of students treated for 15 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology extreme alcohol intoxication being reduced by more than half (Platt, 2013). The collaborative members of the program identify various cultural and geographic factors which shape alcohol consumption; with those on urban campuses drinking mainly in off-campus bars, but those in residential campuses drinking mainly in residences. Annual events, celebrations, sports and fraternities are also identified as environments where alcohol is strongly associated (NCHIP, 2011). With appreciation to contacts at Acadia University, Canada, there is an opportunity to extend the NCHIP for the first time into a British University, Keele. At Acadia University, the NCHIP revealed that peer to peer influence is one of the most effective ways to change behaviour; and thus have developed appropriate messaging in regards to responsible behaviour which has been proven to be very influential and effective (Platt, 2013). Working in collaboration with the head of Student Support and Developmental Services, Ian Munton, the survey can be distributed through the Keele student body and analysed to discover ways that binge drinking can be addressed in a university setting. Methods Participants The participants for this study will be recruited using opportunistic sampling; with every student member of Keele University being invited to take part. The majority of participants will be undergraduate degree members of the university, aging typically 18-21, however, mature students will also be invited to participate. Approximately 10,000 students will be emailed by Student Support requesting their participation in the online survey, with a roughly even split between males and females. Recruitment will be aided by an alcohol campaign run by Ian Munton, head of Student Support and Development Services at Keele University, as this will increase face validity in the eyes of the students and awareness of the survey. Other recruitment methods will include posters, social media campaigns and other various forms of advertising. The survey has no exclusion criteria; even students who don’t drink will be encouraged to participate as a control measure. The inclusion criterion is broad; any students with a Keele University e-mail address will be invited to take part in the study. As a mild incentive, participants will be offered to enter into a prize draw to win an Amazon voucher; Student Support is providing two £25 Amazon vouchers for two participants (if an e-mail address is provided). As the survey is being used as a data collection tool for Keele University specifically it will be a very representative sample, however, student survey fatigue may impact negatively on the response rate (Porter, Whitcomb & Weitzer, 2004). Student Support reported that at Keele, the average response rate for a survey of this nature is 2.5-5%. In terms of a research project, 16-21 year olds have been highlighted as a risk 16 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology population for binge drinking (IAS, 2013) and thus will be generalisable to the research hypothesis. In order to assess risk factors for AUD in the general UK student population however, it would require other academic institutions such as Universities and Colleges to partake in the survey in order to obtain more generalisable data. Materials All students will be required to have access to their university e-mail account in order to receive their invitation and thus the link to participate. Three questionnaires make up the overall survey. The NCHIP was developed by the Dartmouth Institute for Health Policy and Clinical Practice in 2011. Colleges and universities from around the USA use the NCHIP to address the issue of high risk drinking through the use of comprehensive evaluation and measurement techniques. The survey consists of 25 items. Questions 1-5 focus on gathering information on the participants normal drinking behaviour, questions asked include ‘how many times did you drink in the past month?’ and ‘on the days that you drank, how many drinks did you normally have?’. These items can be used to calculate a normal drinking rate of participants and assess number of high risk drinking episodes in the previous two weeks. Questions 6-9 then proceed to ask the participant about their highest volume drinking episode, questions asked include ‘what is the largest number of drinks you can recall having on one occasion in the past month?’ and ‘on this occasion, over how many hours did you drink?’. These items allow the researcher to calculate a high volume drinking rate and assess perceived hangover severity following such a session. Questions 10-13 ask participants to state which of the positive and negative consequences of drinking they have experienced in the last month and also the social impacts experiences as a direct impact of someone else’s drinking. At this point, participants select options from a list and can select as many as they feel is required. Question 14-15 assess participants helping behaviours towards others who have drank such as getting them some water or taking away their car keys. The options here are again presented in a list format. Question 16 onwards collects various participant demographics such as height, weight, age and sex in order to gain an understanding of who is responding to the survey and to calculate participant related statistics such as the estimated blood alcohol level. The NCHIP has been tried and tested at Dartmouth College and directly from this a combination of strategies has been implemented at a number of levels; individual, interpersonal, institutional and community. Dartmouth College has now experienced a significant reduction in the number of students medically transported with BACs greater than 0.25g/dL (a near total loss of motor function) between 2011 and 2013 (NCHIP, 2011). 17 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Furthermore, Cornell University reported an 8% relative reduction in high risk drinking from a baseline wave at the start of the academic year to the end of the academic year. These statistics show the reliability and validity of the survey in University settings for the promotion of alternative and more appropriate interventions than those previously in place at academic institutions. The Short Michigan Alcohol Screening Test (SMAST) was developed by Crew & Sher in 1992. This test is employed to gather and assess information about an individual’s father’s (F-SMAST) and mother’s (M-SMAST) alcohol abuse. There are 26 items in total, 13 questions about the participant’s father’s alcohol consumption and 13 about the participant’s mother’s. Questions include “Do you feel your mother/father has been a normal drinker?”, “Was your mother/father able to stop drinking when they wanted to?” and “Has your mother/father ever neglected his obligations, family, or work for two or more days in a row because he was drinking?” For the majority of the questions, if the participant answers yes this indicates evidence of some problematic behaviour. For these questions, ‘yes’ will scored as 1 and ‘no’ scored as 0, this is reversed for the questions where a ‘yes’ response does not indicate any problematic behaviour (‘yes’=0 and ‘no’=1). A total from the F-SMAST and MSMAST is calculated separately. If participants score 5 or above in either of the tests then they are categorised as family history positive (FHP), those who score 0 or 1 in both tests are categorised as family history negative (FHN) (Piasecki et al. 2005). Crews and Sher (1992) report that the overall reliability and validity of the F-SMAST and M-SMAST is quite good; in particular, the M-SMAST demonstrated high reliability as well as validity showing good internal consistency, stability and convergence with parallel instruments. More recently, Crews & Sher (2006) emphasized the validity and reliability again, stating that it is appropriate for use by clinicians and researchers who are interested in assessing parental history of AUD. Barry and Fleming (1993) reported a moderate Cronbach’s alpha of 0.85 suggesting good internal reliability of the questionnaire. The Hangover Symptom Scale (HSS) was developed by Slutske, Piasecki and Hunt-Carter in 2003. This scale is used to assess the psychological and subjective effects commonly experienced the morning after drinking. This scale is a 13 item scale; each question represents a different hangover symptom and participants are required to state how many times in the past year they have experienced these symptoms. Items take the format of “Within the last 12 months when you drank alcohol, how often did you feel more tired than usual the next morning?” with each question stating a different symptom such as headache, nausea and sensitivity to light. Each question has two parts; part 1 requires participants to select their answer from a list of ‘never’, ‘occasionally’, ‘about half the time’, ‘most of the time’ and ‘every time’. The second part of the question then requires participants to select 18 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology more specifically how many times in the past 12 months this occurred, for example ‘2 times or less’, ‘3-11 times’, ’12-51 times’ or ’52 times or more’. For each item, participants were given a score of 1 if they had experienced that particular hangover symptom in the past 12 months. Scores were then totalled to give the total hangover score. For the present study, only the first part of the question was used; participant could score 0-13. Once the scale was developed, Slutske, Piasecki and Hunt-Carter (2003) reported that the HSS captures a valid set of adjectives describing common hangover effects and that the scale is appropriate for laboratory and survey investigations into hangover. In 2012, Robertson, Piasecki, Slutske, Wood, Sher, Shiffman & Heath reaffirmed that the HSS is a valid tool for hangover research by representing ‘real world’ hangovers. Participants who endorsed the HSS headache and nausea items were especially likely to report the elevations of corresponding symptoms in diary records. Penning et al. (2013) reported that the Cronbach’s alpha score for the HSS was acceptable (0.8) with a relatively high predictive validity (R2=081.5%) suggesting that the HSS has “sufficient efficacy in in measuring overall hangover severity” (Penning, McKinney, Bus, Olivier, Slot & Verster, 2013; 808). The study was conducted via an online questionnaire hosted by Google Docs – the same platform that has been used to deliver the NCHIP survey in colleges and universities across the US. The European Commission’s Directive on Data Protection prohibits the transfer of personal data to non-European Union countries that do not meet the European Union (EU) “adequacy” standard for privacy protection. The Safe Harbor Framework was designed to bridge the gap between EU data protection law and the different way that data protection is handled in the US (EU Safe Harbor, 2012), and provide a means for US organisations to comply with EU data protection directive. Google participates in the Safe Harbor Framework of the European Union and the United States (Certification and Data Privacy, 2011). Other materials included posters for advertisement and the survey was made more salient to students with access to various forms of social media such as Facebook or Twitter. Once the survey was distributed, SPSS software was necessary to collate descriptive statistics and perform a series of ANOVAs, regression, chi square, general linear model and calculations of other variables such as estimated blood alcohol level. Procedure The first stage of the process was the initial meetings with Dr Stephens to address initial links between familial risks for the alcohol hangover, these occurred in October 2013; at this point the three questionnaires were decided upon. Once the survey was complied, meetings 19 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology with the head of Student Support and Development to discuss distribution of the survey took place in November 2013; here a corresponding alcohol campaign was developed and incentives for the participants offered. The final stage before the survey was circulated was the ethics submission; general principles of the BPS Ethical Guidelines will be adhered to and the process included application to both the School of Psychology Ethics Committee (SPEC) and the University Ethical Review Panel. Once the ethics was approved, recruitment began, utilising posters, social media and emails so that the survey was distributed to the entire student body at Keele University. In order to maintain confidentiality, participants were asked to volunteer an e-mail address only if they wish to be entered into the prize draw for the £25 Amazon vouchers. All students received an e-mail requesting their participation in the survey. This e-mail contained an invite letter; stating the scales being utilised, the aims of the research and the benefits of taking part. If they chose to take part in the study, they had the option to click on a web link which directed them to Google Drive, upon which is the survey. The first page of the survey is composed of a consent form where participants are provided with the aims of the research once more, why they have been chosen, what will happen if they take part and various benefits and information about the risks (participants are introduced to the fact that there are no risks but the survey may include some sensitive topics). Information about the participant’s data is then displayed and the participant is informed of who they should contact should there be any problems. The final part of the consent form is the consent agreement; a tick box list for participants to show that they confirm that they have read and understood the information, that they are 18 years or above, they understand their participation is voluntary and can withdraw, they agree to take part in the study, they understand that the only identifying information that will be asked of them is their e-mail address (should they wish to be entered into the prize draw) and that they allow the use of their data to be used for future research projects. Once consent has been provided, the next pages of the survey will contain the three questionnaires; the NCHIP, the HSS and the SMASTs. The three questionnaires took approximately 20 minutes to complete. Once the participants have finished the survey a final screen will show appropriate debrief information including the details of the principle investigator (Dr Richard Stephens) should the participant have any further questions or wish to withdraw. Also relevant services provided by the university will be listed should any concerning issues be raised through completion of the questionnaire for an individual. Furthermore, the final page will provide participants with a space to insert their e-mail address if they wish to be entered into the prize draw for the Amazon vouchers. Once the survey is complete, the participants can exit out of Google Docs. 20 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology The next stage of the procedure was the statistical analysis of the data collected. To analyse the survey various descriptive statistics were implemented including the drinking frequency of students, percentage of light to heavy drinkers and drinking patterns compared to UK drinking guidelines. These were then be presented to Student Support in a clear and easy to understand format utilising diagrams and charts; this then allows Ian Munton and team to identify risk behaviours and take action to prevent them occurring. ANOVAs were utilised to compare differences on a range of variables between FHP and FHN participants (variables include number of high risk drinking episodes, normal drinking rate, peak drinking rate and hangover severity). A regression was ran to observe if there is a link between hangover symptom score and amount of alcohol consumed to assess units consumed affects hangover symptoms or if there is another variable to consider such as familial risk. The eBAC was calculated to compare student’s eBACs against safe limits, and also to assess the relationship between eBAC and hangover symptoms. Finally, a chi squared test was utilised to assess the frequency of rising limb effects occurring during and immediately following alcohol consumption such as noticing a release in tension and having lots of energy. The survey ran twice in the academic year, once in March and once in July. The second wave of the survey in July only consisted of the NCHIP. This added to the benefit for Keele University by assessing whether there are times of the year when students are most likely to binge drink, and thus be at highest risk for AUD. This then enabled Student Support to plan alcohol awareness weeks for those high risk stretches of the semester. Design The design of the study is an online survey design where patterns of consumption are addressed, and risk factors of AUD are explored: particularly the alcohol hangover. It is understood that employing an online questionnaire reduces the level of control when compared to laboratory studies, including issues around verification. The benefit however of using an online survey is that a large sample can be accumulated efficiently, and subsequent controlled research can occur if need be. Dissemination Dissemination will include a written dissertation, a short presentation to various students and staff at Keele University and also a booklet of relevant statistics, graphs and charts for the use of Student Support and Development. 21 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Results As the project is two-fold, I present the findings in two sections; results from the survey for the benefit of Keele University and the results for the research question regarding risk factors for AUD. An alpha level of 0.05 was used for all statistical tests. The Survey 1. Demographics Table 1 shows the various participant demographics including year of study, age, gender, whether they live on campus, if they are a halls of resident support assistant and average module % last term for wave 1 and wave 2. 22 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table 1. Demographics and Descriptives of Participants MARCH Wave 1 JULY Wave 2 Frequency % Frequency % Size of sample 10,000 10,000 surveyed Survey response 273 310 totals Survey response rate 2.73% 3.1% Year of Study 1st 2nd 3rd 4th/5th Post-graduate 103 50 61 14 45 37.7 18.3 22.4 5.1 16.5 92 68 79 18 53 29.7 21.9 25.5 5.8 17.1 Age 18 19 20 21 22 23+ 26 46 62 53 22 64 9.5 16.9 22.7 19.4 8.1 23.4 15 57 65 63 31 79 4.8 18.4 21 20.3 10 25.6 Sex Male Female 101 172 37 63 123 187 39.7 60.3 Live on Campus Yes No 108 165 39.6 60.4 122 188 39.4 60.6 Halls of Resident Support Assistant Yes No 15 258 5.5 94.5 7 303 2.3 97.7 Average Module % last term Less than 40 40-49 50-59 60-69 70+ 6 63 118 76 10 2.2 23.1 43.2 27.8 3.7 1 8 75 151 75 0.3 2.6 24.2 48.7 24.2 23 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology A series of chi square tests revealed there were no significant differences between age, sex, living on/off campus and year of study between participants in wave 1 and 2 (p>0.05). There was, however, a significant difference between whether participants were a halls of resident assistant and the wave of the survey X2(1)=4.26, p=0.039 with there being more halls of resident assistants in wave 1. Similarly, there was a significant difference between average module grade and wave of the survey also X2(4)=10.44, p=0.034 in that module grade was much higher in wave 2. 2. Drinking patterns in students Table 2 show various drinking behaviours reported by students including high risk drinking episodes, drinking rates and energy drink consumption for wave 1 and wave 2. A high risk drinking episode was defined as 5 or more drinks for a male and 4 or more drinks for a female in one occasion. A series of t-tests revealed no significant differences between mean number of drinks perceived to be moderate, mean usual drinking rate, mean highest volume drinking rate, mean hangover or mean eBAC for participants in wave 1 to participants in wave 2 (p>0.05). Chi square also revealed no significant differences between number of high risk drinking episodes or whether participants mixed alcohol with energy drinks between the wave of the survey (p>0.05). 3. Consequences of Drinking Table 3 shows the frequency of participants who reported experiencing various drinking related harms for wave 1 and wave 2. Harm caused by alcohol consumption was the same for both waves with participants reporting feeling nauseated/vomiting and forgot where they were or what they did. Wave 1 participants also selected having a severe hangover whereas wave 2 participants selected doing something they later regretted. 24 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table 2. Drinking Patterns of Participants Number of drinks perceived to be moderate when partying Drinking Rates & Hangover Severity Usual Drinking Rate (drinks/hour) Highest Volume Drinking Rate (drinks/hour) Hangover severity rating eBAC High Risk Drinking Episodes* Dry this month Didn’t HRD 1-2 times 3-5 times 6+ times MARCH – Wave 1 Mean SD 4.48 1.789 JULY – Wave 2 Mean SD 4.47 1.905 1.24 0.578 1.15 0.731 1.673 1.028 1.52 0.981 2.02 1.686 1.50 1.649 0.04 0.771 0.06 0.559 Frequency % Frequency % 23 78 81 49 17 8.4 28.6 29.7 17.9 6.2 36 86 93 40 7 11.6 27.7 29.9 12.9 2.3 Mixed alcohol with 80 35.6 68 51 energy drink Didn’t mix alcohol 145 64.4 158 21.9 with energy drink * defined as 5 or more drinks for a male and 4 or more drinks for a female in one occasion 25 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table 3. Rates (Frequency and %) of Drinking Related Harm Reference Sample: Drinkers Been nauseated or vomited Got into an argument or fight Got in trouble with security/police Had unprotected sex Physically injured yourself Physically injured another person Required medical attention Did something you later regretted Had a mild hangover Had a severe hangover Forgot where you were or what you did Fell behind in University work Missed a class Performed poorly on a test or important project Seriously considered suicide MARCH – Wave 1 Frequency % 225 100 JULY – Wave 2 Frequency % 262 100 101 44.9 123 46.2 50 22.2 55 21 14 6.2 19 7.3 29 68 12.9 30.2 36 67 13.7 25.6 12 5.3 11 4.2 10 4.4 11 4.2 75 33.3 103 39.3 133 84 77 59.1 37.3 34.2 177 86 94 67.6 32.8 35.9 39 17.3 15 5.7 66 10 29.3 4.4 65 14 24.8 5.3 10 4.4 9 3.4 Chi square analyses revealed a significant difference between frequency of experiencing of a mild hangover and the wave of the survey X2(1)=3.913, p=0.048 with higher frequency of participants reporting mild hangover in wave 2. Furthermore, there was a significant difference in frequency between falling behind in University work and the wave of the survey X2(1)=15.532, p=0.00 with many more participants reporting falling behind in University work in wave 1. There were no other significant interactions between harm caused by alcohol consumption and the wave of the survey (p>0.05). 26 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table 4 shows the social impacts experienced as a direct result of someone else’s drinking for wave 1 and wave 2. The same three most prevalent social impacts of others drinking for both waves were; having a drink spilt on you, having your sleep interrupted and having to baby-sit another student. Chi square revealed a significant difference between whether participants had a drink spilled on them and the wave of the survey X2(1)=5.117, p=0.024 with more participants reporting this in wave 1. Furthermore, there was a significant difference between whether participants had had their property damaged and the wave of the study X2(1)=4.225, p=0.04 with more participants reporting this in wave 1. Similarly, there was a significant difference between whether participants had experienced none of the above social impacts and the wave of the study X2(1)=6.158, p=0.013 with more of the participants reporting having experienced none of the social impacts in wave 2. There were no significant differences between the remaining social impacts and the wave of the study (p>0.05). Table 5 shows behaviours participants have had to provide to help others who had consumed alcohol for both wave 1 and wave 2 data. 27 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table 4. Social Impacts of drinking: Experiences as a result of someone else’s drinking Full sample Was prevented from enjoying an event Was made to feel unsafe in your surroundings Had a drink spilled on you Experienced an unwanted sexual advance Been insulted or humiliated Got into an argument or fight Were physically injured by someone who had been drinking Got into trouble with security/police Had to “baby-sit” or take care of another student who drank too much Had unprotected sex Had your property damaged Had your sleep interrupted Had your studying interrupted None of these applied to me Frequency 273 % 100 Frequency 310 % 100 78 28.6 82 26.5 57 20.9 57 18.4 158 57.9 151 48.7 49 17.9 47 15.2 32 11.7 36 11.6 28 10.3 32 10.3 14 5.1 14 4.5 3 0.7 7 2.3 103 37.7 109 35.2 13 15 4.8 5.5 13 7 4.2 2.3 127 46.5 127 41.0 62 22.7 57 18.4 61 22.3 98 31.6 28 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table 5. Drinking Behaviours: Respondents who provided help to other drinkers Full sample MARCH – Wave 1 Frequency % 273 100 JULY – Wave 2 Frequency % 310 100 Was around people who were drinking Wasn’t around people who were drinking 243 89 269 86.8 30 11 41 13.2 Sample: Was around people who were drinking Was around people who were drinking, but didn’t need to assist anyone Suggested they drink more slowly Suggested that they stopped drinking Took away their car keys Got additional assistance (e.g. security/police) Stayed with them until they got home safely Kept them awake until you knew they were OK Gave them water / other non-alcoholic drink Gave them something to eat Put them to bed on their side Kept them warm Monitored their breathing to make sure it wasn’t abnormally shallow or slow 243 100 269 100 129 53 157 58.4 30 12.3 33 12.3 64 26.3 69 25.7 9 3.7 12 4.5 18 7.4 12 4.5 83 34.2 71 26.4 24 9.9 28 10.4 92 37.9 90 33.5 48 19.8 52 19.3 47 19.3 51 19 32 16 13.2 6.6 25 20 9.3 7.4 29 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology In addition, the same three helping behaviours were deemed to be most prevalent to students in both wave 1 and wave 2 of the survey were giving someone water or another non-alcoholic drink, staying with them until they got home safely and suggested that they stopped drinking. Chi square analyses revealed a significant difference between participants staying with someone until they got home safely and the wave of the study X2(1)=0.4313, p=0.038 with more participants engaging in this helping behaviour in wave 1. There were no other significant interactions between the remaining helping behaviours and the wave of the study (p>0.05). Table 6 examines the various drinking related benefits reported by wave 1 and wave 2 participants. The three most common benefits of drinking to students in both waves 1 and 2 were enjoying the taste of the drinks, enjoying the feeling you got from drinking and talking to someone they probably wouldn’t have otherwise. Chi squared analyses revealed no significant differences between the benefits of drinking and the wave of the survey (p>0.05). 4. eBAC for students A ‘safe’ limit for eBAC was calculated for females based on a weight of 67.5kg, a height of 162.5cm and 4 drinks over a period of 2.5 hours. The value of a safe limit for eBAC for females is 0.079 or below. Similarly, a ‘safe’ limit for eBAC was calculated for males based on a weight of 79.8kg, a height of 175cm and 5 drinks over a period of 2.5 hours. The value of a safe limit for eBAC for males is 0.068. Table 7 shows the frequency and percentage of participants who had an eBAC of above the safe calculated limits. 30 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table 6. Rates (frequency and %) of Drinking Related Benefits Sample: Drinkers Enjoyed the taste of what you drank Enjoyed the feeling you got from drinking Talked to someone you probably wouldn’t have spoken to otherwise Was able to tell a funny story or joke that made others laugh Was able to express thoughts and feelings you normally keep to yourself Noticed a release of tension in your muscles and nerves Felt like you had lots of energy Forgot your worries Had a good time that wouldn’t have been as much fun sober Felt more attractive Facilitated a romantic encounter MARCH – Wave 1 Frequency % 225 100 JULY – Wave 2 Frequency % 262 100 194 86.2 240 91.6 170 75.6 201 76.7 141 62.7 161 61.5 110 48.9 129 49.2 98 43.6 137 52.3 101 44.9 126 48.1 89 39.6 106 40.5 113 126 50.2 56 139 135 53.1 51.5 62 63 27.6 28 90 78 34.4 29.8 31 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table 7. Participants Above Safe Limits for eBAC MARCH – Wave 1 Frequency % JULY – Wave 2 Frequency % Above safe Males 34 12.45 43 13.87 limit for Females 71 26.01 53 17.10 eBAC Chi square analyses revealed there was a significant difference between the number of females above the safe limit for eBAC and the wave of the study X2(1)=6.883, p=0.009 which was significantly higher in wave 1 than it was in wave 2. This was not observed for the male participants (p>0.05). Risk Factors for AUD 1. FHN vs. FHN Demographics Participants were categorised as family history negative (FHN) participants if they had an FSMAST and M-SMAST score of 1 or below (n=162). Participants were categorised as family history positive (FHP) participants if they had an F-SMAST or M-SMAST score of 5 or above (n=33). Table 8 shows the descriptives for FHN and FHP participants. A t-test revealed no significant difference between the ages or the age of first intoxication of FHP and FHN participants. A chi square analysis revealed a significant difference between failing last year of FHP and FHN participants X2(1)=6.773, p=0.009 with more FHP participants failing the year than FHN participants. No other variables significantly differed from the participant group (p>0.05). 2. FHN vs. FHP drinking patterns Table 9 shows drinking patterns and behaviours of FHN and FHN participants. 32 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table 8. Descriptives for FHN and FHP Participants FHN FHP Mean Age Age first intoxication SD Mean 23.18 14.91 SD 3.935 1.57 % 21.41 13.97 3.343 5.354 Sample Frequency 162 100 Frequency 34 100 Male Female 58 104 35.8 64.2 11 23 34.3 65.7 Live on Campus Live off Campus 71 91 43.8 56.2 10 24 28.6 71.4 Undergraduates Postgraduates 136 26 84 16 25 9 71.4 28.6 2.5 25.9 40.1 30.2 1.2 1 4 14 12 3 2.8 11.3 42.8 34.7 8.5 Average Module Mark Last Year 3:3 4 2:2 42 2:1 65 1:1 49 Fail 2 % An ANOVA revealed a significant difference for hangover severity between FHP and FHN participants F(1,160)=6.446, p=0.012 with FHP participants displaying significantly higher hangover severity (mean=2.38) compared to FHN participants (mean=1.48). No other variables’ means were significantly different (p>0.05). 3. Hangover & Drinks Consumed - Regression A regression is used to predict an outcome variable from a single predictor variable. Here, we use a regression model to predict hangover severity from the number of drinks consumed for both FHP and FHN participants. 33 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table 9. Drinking Patterns and Behaviours of FHN and FHP participants FHN Mean FHP SD Mean No. times drank last 4 weeks No. times high risk drinking last 2 weeks No. drinks normal night No. of hours drinking normal night No. drinks heavy night No. of hours drinking heavy night Hangover severity Drinks perceived moderate eBAC 6.11 4.913 8.58 2.939 1.629 P value 0.204 1.38 1.908 2.03 7.934 0.739 0.391 4.86 3.052 4.54 3.036 2.102 0.226 4 1.903 3.79 1.414 0.092 0.762 7.78 4.532 8.96 4.648 1.366 0.244 5.07 2.519 5.67 2.220 1.996 0.160 1.48 1.572 2.38 1.740 6.446 0.012 4.44 1.788 4.67 1.926 0.420 0.518 0.001 0.991 0.062 0.069 0.125 0.724 HSS Score 5.531 3.745 6.941 4.249 3.799 0.053 34 SD F September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Figure 1 shows a regression between the hangover severity and number of drinks consumed for FHP participants. The regression proved to be insignificant b=0.051,f(1,22)=0.166,p=0.688. Figure 2 shows a regression between the hangover severity and the number of drinks consumed for FHN participants. The regression proved to be significant b=0.126, f(1, 37)=8.692, p=0.004. 35 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology 4. eBAC and Hangover Severity – ANOVA An ANOVA was run to assess hangover severity of those with an eBAC of below 0.1 (group 1) and those above 0.1 (group 2) as referred to by Kruisselbrink (2014). Table 10. Mean & SD of Hangover Severity according to eBAC Mean SD Group 1 1.85 1.700 Group 2 2.25 1.616 The ANOVA revealed no significant difference between hangover severity between group 1 and group 2 F(1,187)=2.610, p=0.108. 5. eBAC and Hangover Severity - General Linear Model A univariate general linear model was utilised to assess the relationship between the eBAC and hangover severity for FHN and FHP participants. The dependent variable for the model was hangover severity with the fixed factor being the group participants belonged to (FHP or FHN). eBAC was included into the model as a covariate. The general linear model revealed no significant interaction between hangover severity and group with eBAC as a covariate F(1, 158)=2.137, p=0.146. 6. Rising Limb Effects – Chi Square A chi square was utilised to assess frequencies of participants reporting various sensitivities to drinking consequences for FHP participants and FHN participants. The four consequences were: enjoying the taste of what you drank, enjoying the feeling you got from drinking, noticing a release of tension in your muscles or nerves and felt like you had lots of energy. 36 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Table 11. Frequencies for Positive Consequences of Drinking FHN Enjoyed the taste of what you drank Enjoyed the feeling you got from drinking Noticed a release of tension in muscles and nerves Felt like you had a lot of energy FHP No 40 Yes 122 No 20 Yes 13 X2 16.601 P value 0.000 61 101 14 19 0.264 0.608 99 63 24 9 1.588 0.208 104 58 26 7 2.626 0.105 Chi square analyses revealed no significant differences in frequency of responses between FHP and FHN participants for three effects; enjoyed the feeling you got from drinking, noticing a release of tension in your muscles or nerves and felt like you had lots of energy (p>0.05). However, there was a significant difference between frequency of FHN participants and FHP participants who reported enjoying the taste of what they drank X2(1, 1)=16.601, p=0.000. 122 out of 162 FHN participants reported enjoying the taste; however, only 13 of 33 FHP participants reported enjoying the taste. Discussion The Survey The survey was used to obtain data on Keele students drinking patterns and behaviours which it was extremely successful in doing. A full report was produced in order to provide Ian Munton and his team at Student Support with graphical and tabular information from which interventions to reduce harmful and hazardous drinking on campus could be planned and implemented. Participants were fairly similar over the two waves of the survey, with slight differences in the average module grade. Average module grade was much higher in the second wave of the 37 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology study; however this could be due to the fact that for the first wave of the study participants had not sat January exams or received grades back and so used a predicted grade, however by July they would have received final grades and thus could explain the difference. In terms of the drinking patterns and behaviours of participants there were no significant differences between participants in wave 1 and wave 2 suggesting that people are still drinking similar amounts throughout the progress of the academic year. Having contacted the bar manager at the Student’s Union I can report the numbers attending nights out at the Union decreases by a substantial amount during exam time and slowly grows towards the end of exam time as more students finish their exams. The same is for January when the first semester exams are set. Webb, Ashton, Kelly & Kamali (1996) reported that 61% of males and 48% of women from their second year university students exceeded safe limits of alcohol consumption. One piece of information that can be extracted from the report in order to compare Keele to these levels of consumption is the frequency of high risk drinking amongst students in the past fortnight. In the first wave 53.8% of participants engaged in high risk drinking at least once in the past fortnight, and 45.1% of wave two participants engaged in high risk drinking at least once in the past fortnight. Furthermore, 6.2% of first wave participants and 2.3% of second wave participants reported high risk drinking episodes 6+ times in the past two weeks which shows extreme levels of harmful and hazardous drinking. This data also mirrors high levels of drinking seen in other institutions suggesting that student drinking is a common issue (Webb, Aston, Kelly & Kamali, 1996; Stark, 2007; The IAS, 2013). Student Support can now use this information to compare to other levels of drinking in other institutions, and to also gage the level of action required with the aim of reducing these numbers. Dartmouth University and other institutions which partook in the survey have been able to reduce the number of students treated for extreme alcohol intoxication being reduced by more than half (Platt, 2013); emphasising the power of the survey. This data is supported by the eBAC calculations which show that a large percentage of participants are displaying an eBAC which is unsafe. This data supports research from the Institute of Alcohol Studies, who reported that 31% of 16-24 year olds drink above the recommended low-risk guidelines. The survey requires participants to state various negative and positive consequences of, as well as the social impacts of other’s drinking, which again can be used by Student Support to mould and focus interventions on the consequences of drinking that most affects its students. In both waves, participants placed a huge emphasis on feeling nauseated and forgetting what they did the night before which could be focused upon in intervention messages sent by the University as a deterrent for drinking large amounts. Furthermore, the 38 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology most prevalent benefits of drinking were enjoying the taste, enjoying the feeling you got from drinking and talking to someone you probably wouldn’t have otherwise. This information can aid Student Support in providing alternative activities for students that produce the same benefits e.g. producing non-alcoholic drinks which also taste nice and provide an atmosphere where people have the opportunity to speak to new people such as speed dating. Keele University has a regularly updated alcohol policy which shows that it has been a recognised problem on campus with, on occasion, fatal consequences. This information collected will be invaluable for Student Support who has been struggling to combat harmful drinking on campus. The responses from the survey will provide a renewed push towards tackling the issue, from which Ian Munton and his team can create and implement novel approaches to reducing harmful drinking; from providing alternative activities with the same associated benefits of drinking to focused messaging to students on days when drinking is most common. Hangover & AUD Aside from being a tool for data collection, the survey was also utilised to assess risk factors for Alcohol Use Disorders. This survey was an important first step in gathering data which explores the relationship between familial risk, AUD and the alcohol hangover. From this, there is now a reasonable and substantial base for future research to build upon and replicate in order to confirm or challenge findings. The SMAST (Crew & Sher, 1992) scored enabled me to categorise participants into either those who were FHP, FHN or neither. The FHN and FHP participants characteristics revealed no significant differences in the age sex and level of study, indicating that the demographically the participants were all similar. In terms of the drinking patterns and behaviours of the FHP and FHN participants they were also very similar, with no significant difference between drinking rates, high risk drinking episodes and drinks perceived to be moderate on a night out. I found evidence that FHP participants tended to report more severe hangovers than FHN participants, even though there was no difference in drinking levels and patterns. The present findings partially replicate and support previous research stating that family history positive (FHP) participants report more severe hangover than family history negative (FHN) participants (Piasecki, Sher, Slutske & Jackson, 2005; Newlin & Pretorious, 1990; McCaul, Turkkam, Svilis & Bigelow, 1991). 39 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology The replication being partial is due to the fact that the significant difference was only observed from the hangover severity item on the NCHIP; HSS score, although very close (p=0.053) was not significantly different for FHP and FHN participants as predicted. A regression was conducted to assess whether as the number of drinks consumed increased so did hangover severity the following morning. For FHP participants there was no significant relationship between the two variables, however, there was one for FHN participants. This would suggest that for those FHP participants, there are other factors influencing their perceived hangover severity other than it being purely down to the alcohol consumed. However, with the FHN participants, there is a positive regression; the more drinks that are consumed, the greater the hangover severity, suggesting that their experienced hangover is reflects on the amount of alcohol consumed. Similarly, Piasecki et al. (2005) found that when drinking frequency and various demographics were controlled, FHP participants tended to report more hangover than FHN participants. This research reflects similar findings as even though FHN and FHP participant’s drinking frequencies were no different, FHP participant’s hangover severity was significantly larger. This could suggest that FHP participants may have an underlying genetic mechanism or alternatively a learned effect which causes them to experience hangovers regardless of the number of alcoholic drinks consumed; proposing they are at the highest risk of consuming further alcohol to relieve the symptoms of the hangover. This supports existing research which suggests that hangovers do not always deter drinking but can actually facilitate it due to its ability to relief its own symptoms the following day (Mallett, Lee, Neighbors, Clarimer & Turrisi, 2006; Earlywine, 1993a; Robertson, 2012). According to a paper written by Kruisselbrink (2014), hangovers are experienced when you reach a BAC of 0.1 or greater. An ANOVA was utilised to check mean hangover scores between those who had an eBAC below 0.1 and those with an eBAC 0.1 and above. We found that there was no significant difference between hangover scores for those with low eBACs compared to those with higher eBACs, and so Kruisselbrink’s research is not replicated. Further analysis using a General Linear Model to assess the relationship between eBAC and hangover severity for FHP and FHN participants also revealed no significant interaction. The eBAC calculation is a fairly new development in alcohol research and so further study will reveal the occurrence of this relationship between eBAC and hangover severity. One aspect of alcohol research which requires more investigation is the rising limb effects of the BAC and the increased sensitivity to the consequences of drinking. Leonard and Blane (1999) reported that sons of alcoholics display a hypersensitivity to drinking consequences 40 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology when the BAC curve rises, and this is the time pleasurable consequences of drinking dominate. We selected four physiological positive consequences of drinking reported by students; enjoyed the taste of what you drank, enjoyed the feeling you got from drinking, noticed a release of tension in muscles and nerves and felt like you had lots of energy in order to obtain information about participants sensitivities to these consequences and assess differences between FHN and FHP participants. We found there was no difference in the frequency of participants reporting three of the four consequences between FHP and FHN individuals (enjoyed the feeling, noticed a release in tension, felt like you had lots of energy) however, there was a significant difference in the frequency of FHN and FHP participants who reported enjoying the taste of what they drank. The data reveals that FHP participants reported enjoying the taste of what they were drinking significantly fewer times than FHN participants. This shows that the FHP participants in the present study showed a decreased sensitivity to the taste of alcohol which does not support Leonard and Blane’s (1999) work. This could suggest that FHP participants have different motivations for drinking as they are displaying harmful levels of alcohol consumption yet very little report to enjoy the taste. One possibility for a motivation for drinking in FHP participants is to relieve symptoms of a hangover. Earlywine (1993a) stated that those who experience the greatest hangovers may drink alcohol to alleviate the adverse effects of previous alcohol consumption and thus this could provide a potential motivation for drinking in FHP participants. Furthermore, in 2010, a survey revealed that 11% of participants had engaged in hair of the dog drinking (Verster, Keurten, Olivier & Laar; 2010) which shows that this behaviour does occur and is a possibility in explaining the differences in frequencies of experiencing various consequences of drinking. The survey was very effective at collecting a variety of data from participants which was used to explore the idea that the alcohol hangover is a risk factor for AUD. I was able to obtain various participant demographics in order to obtain an idea of who responded to the survey and if this reflects a naturalistic sample of the university, which it did. Furthermore, using the SMAST I was able to categorise participants into the two main groups; FHP and FHN from which responses could be compared and contrasted in order to address the research question. The HSS meant I could calculate a hangover score in a reliable and valid way, which mean that numeric score could be involved in the analysis in order to assess hangover severity. The NCHIP data allowed me to calculate the eBAC for participants which proved to be very valuable in understanding the relationship between ethanol exposure and the alcohol hangover. The three questionnaires worked together complimentary to provide a uniform set of data from which the research question of assessing risk factors for alcohol use disorder could be explored. 41 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology Utilising an online survey design meant that the survey was extremely easy to administer to students and meant that a large number of participants could be contacted simultaneously; this was particularly useful in the present study as it meant the entire student body at Keele University could be sent the survey and asked to complete. Furthermore, the online data set meant that a variety of statistical tests could be run easily on the data using the SPSS software including ANOVAs, chi square and regression. However, one disadvantage of survey design is that participants may feel discouraged to provide answers that paint themselves or others in an unfavourable light, for example, the SMAST items which refer to participant’s parent’s drinking behaviours are of a sensitive nature and thus may make the participant feel uncomfortable, causing a bias in responses. One way I attempted to overcome this is to emphasise the confidential nature of the responses and that it is only members of the research team who have access to the data. Furthermore, the details of the principal investigator were provided should anyone have any issues and wish to withdraw their data set. One limitation to the study was the lack of option for students to express drinks consumed in terms of units or simply type of drink. The majority of information available to people in regards to drinking alcohol uses units to demonstrate safe levels and calculate individual drinking behaviours and thus an option for individuals to include this would have not only aided statistical analyses in the current study, but could also have simultaneously raised awareness on the number of units in drinks and how individuals may compare to the safe limits encouraged by professionals. Future research could utilise a similar survey but simply alter the question format to ask for drinks in terms of units, providing guidance for participants as to how many units are in common drinks consumed. A further limitation of the project is the low response rate. Although I was assured by Ian Munton that this level of response is normal for a survey of its kind at Keele University it still didn’t capture a large proportion of students. A response rate this low lowers the generalizability of the data to all students at Keele as there is only a small percentage of the target sample who participated in the study; for example, my sample could consist purely of people who were more likely to take the time to respond which biases my results. However, the data I collected provides others with a good base to build upon in future research. One was to potentially improve the response rate is to take advantage of the benefits of social media; the survey could be mentioned by Student Support on their Facebook or Twitter account to build up awareness and gain people’s interest in the survey and the findings. Another potential way to increase the response rate is to personalise the study and make the survey as meaningful to participants as possible. Ways to achieve this is to ensure to emphasise how the findings will benefit students and the University and to reference 42 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology people’s names who are familiar to the participants e.g. utilising Student Support to send the email rather than myself, as a student. Finally, once the survey is sent out, gentle reminder emails could have been utilised to prompt participants to complete the survey as they might not have had time when the email was sent out but may find time now reminded. It would appear that the alcohol hangover is a risk factor for AUD, as those who report the greatest hangovers tend to have a positive family history of alcohol use issues and thus are more likely to engage in hair of the dog drinking in order to overcome the effects of the hangover. However, it is important to remember that is cannot be explicitly stated from this research that the hangover leads certain individuals to engage in ‘hair of the dog’ drinking; other factors should be considered such as personality factors. For example, Piasecki et al. (2005) reported that FHP participants were more impulsive and less sensitive to punishment cues, suggesting that FHP participants may have an inability to effectively learn from their actions. In the future, research could focus on more explicitly dividing the alcohol hangover and withdrawal symptoms into two clear constructs, and also further exploring the idea of hair of the dog drinking to see how often this occurs in participants who are of high familial risk for AUD. 43 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology References 1. American Psychiatric Association. (2003). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM–5). Arlington VA: American Psychiatric Association. 2. Baker, T.B., Piper, M.E., McCarthy, D.E., Majeskie, M.R., & Fiore, M.C. (2004). Addiction motivation reformulated: an affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111(4), 33-51. 3. Barry, K.L., & Fleming, M.F. (1993). The alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT) and the SMAST-13: predictive validity in a rural primary care sample. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 8, 33-42. 4. Bentdsen, P., Wayne Jones, A., & Helander, A. (1998). Urinary excretion of methanol and 5-hydroxytryptophol as biochemical markers of recent drinking in the hangover state. Alcohol, 33(4), 431–8. 5. BMJ. (2005). Young people who binge drink are likely to continue into adulthood. Retrieved on August 11, 2014, from http://www.bmj.com/content/330/7495/809.2.extract 6. Bogin, R. M., Nostrant, T.T., & Young, M.J. (1987) Propranolol for the treatment of the alcoholic hangover. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse, 13,175-80. 7. Certification and Data Privacy. (2011). Retrieved on January 20, 2014, from http://www.google.com/apps/intl/en-GB/trust/data_protection.html 8. Crews, T.M. & Sher, K.J. (1992). Using adapted short MASTs for assessing parental alcoholism: Reliability and validity. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 16, 576-584. 9. Damrau, F., & Liddy, E. (1960). Hangovers and whisky congeners: comparison of whisky with vodka. Journal of the National Medical Association, 52, 262-4. 10. Damrau, F., & Goldberg, A.H. (1971). Adsorption of whisky congeners by activated charcoal: chemical and clinical studies related to hangover. Southwest Med., 52,17982. 11. Dawson, D.A., Harford, T.C., & Grant, B.F. (1992). Family history as a predictor of alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clinical and Experimental Research, 16, 572-575. 12. Drink Aware. (2014). Binge drinking. Retrieved on January 14, 2014, from https://www.drinkaware.co.uk/understand-your-drinking/is-your-drinking-aproblem/binge-drinking 13. Earlywine, M. (1993a). Personality risk for alcoholism covaries with hangover symptoms. Addictive Behaviours, 18(4), 415-420. 44 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology 14. Earlywine, M. (1993b). Hangover moderates the association between personality and drinking problems. Addictive Behaviours, 18(3), 291-297. 15. EU Safe Harbour. (2012). Retrieved on January 20, 2014, from http://export.gov/safeharbor/eu/eg_main_018365.asp 16. Finn, P.R., Zeitouni, N.C., & Pihl, R.O. (1990). Effects of alcohol on psychophysiological hyper reactivity to nonaversive and aversive stimuli in men at high risk for alcoholism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99(19), 79-85. 17. Global strategy to reduce harmful use of alcohol. (2014). Retrieved on January 14, 2014, from http://www.who.int/substance_abuse/activities/gsrhua/en/ on 18. Hangover cures. (2012). Retrieved on January 15, 2014, from http://www.nhs.uk/Livewell/alcohol/Pages/Hangovers.aspx 19. Human Resources Department. (2011). Alcohol and drugs policy. Retrieved January 20, 2014, from http://www.keele.ac.uk/media/keeleuniversity/hr/healthsafetyandwellbeing/Drugsand AlcoholPolicy.pdf 20. Hussong, A., Bauer, D., & Chassin, L. (2008). Telescoped trajectories from alcohol initiation to disorder in children of alcoholic parents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 63-78. 21. IAS. (2013). Retrieved on August 11, 2014, from http://www.ias.org.uk/Alcoholknowledge-centre/Young-people-and-alcohol/Factsheets/Changing-trends-in-youngpeoples-drinking.aspx 22. Jarvilehto, T., Laakso, M.L., & Virsu, V. (1975). Human auditory evoked responses during hangover. Psychopharmacology, 42(1), 173-177. 23. Johnson, L.C. (2014). Using a public health and quality improvement approach to address high-risk drinking with 32 colleges and universities. Retrieved on August 12, 2014, from http://webapp.dartmouth.edu/nchip/uploads/NCHIP_whitepaper_5.8.14_FINAL.pdf 24. Kangasaho, M., Hillbom, M., Kaste, M., & Vapaatalo, H. (1982). Effects of ethanol intoxication and hangover on plasma levels of thromboxane B2 and 6-ketoprostaglandin F1 α and on thromboxane B2 formation by platelets in man. Thromb Haemost, 48, 232-4. 25. Kruisselbrink, D. Exploring hangover immunity. Oral presentation at the 6th Alcohol Hangover Research Group meeting, Hyatt Regency Bellevue, 21st June, 2014. 26. Kupari, M. (1983). Drunkenness, hangover, and the heart. Acta Med Scand, 213, 8490 45 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology 27. Leonard, K.E., & Blane, H.T. (1999). Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. The Guilford Press: New York. 28. Linkola, J., Fyhrquist, F., & Ylikahri, R. (1979). Renin, aldosterone and cortisol during ethanol intoxication and hangover. Acta Physiol Scand,106, 75-82. 29. Linkola, J., Fyhrquist,. F., Nieminen, M.M., Weber,T.H., & Tontti, K. (1976). Reninaldosterone axis in ethanol intoxication and hangover. Eur J Clin Invest, 6, 191-4. 30. Linkola, J., Ylikahri, R., Fyhquist, F., & Wallenius, M. (1978). Plasma vasopression in ethanol intoxication and hangover. Acta Physiol Scand, 104, 180-7. 31. Mallett, K.A., Lee, C.M., Neighbors, Clarimer, M.E., & Turrisi, R. (2006). Do we learn from our mistakes? An examination of the impact of negative alcohol-related consequences on college students’ drinking patterns and perceptions. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 67(47), 269-276. 32. McCaul, M.E., Turkkan, J.S., Svikis, D.S., & Bigelow, G.E. (1991). Alcohol and secobarbital effects as a function of familial alcoholism: extended intoxication and increased withdrawal effects. Alcohol Clinical and Experimental Research, 15(1), 94101. 33. McKinney, A., & Coyle, K. (2006). Alcohol hangover effects on measures of affect the morning after a normal night’s drinking. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 14(1), 54-60. 34. McKinney, A., Coyle, K. & Verster, J. (2012). Direct comparison of the cognitive effects of acute alcohol with the morning after a normal night’s drinking. Human Psychopharmacological Clinical Experiments, 10(02), 295-304. 35. NCHIP. (2011). Retrieved on January 14, 2014, from www.nchip.org 36. Newlin, D.B., & Pretorious, M.B. (1990). Sons of alcoholics report greater hangover symptoms than sons of non-alcoholics: a pilot study. Alcohol Clinical and Experimental Research, 14(5), 713-716. 37. NHS. (2014). Alcohol units and guidelines. Retrieved August 30, 2014, from http://www.nhs.uk/change4life/Pages/alcohol-lower-risk-guidelines-units.aspx 38. NICE. (2013). Alcohol use disorders – preventing harmful drinking. Retrieved on January 16, 2014, from http://guidance.nice.org.uk/PH24 39. Park, C.L. (2004). Positive and negative consequences of alcohol consumption in college students. Addictive Behaviours, 29(2), 311-321. 40. Pawan, G.L. (1973). Alcoholic drinks and hangover effects. Proc Nutr Soc, 32, 15. 41. Penning, R., McKinney, A., Bus, L.D., Olivier, B., Slot, K., & Verster, J.C. (2013). Measurement of alcohol hangover severity: development of the alcohol hangover severity scale (AHSS). Psychopharmacology, 225(8), 803-810. 46 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology 42. Penning, R., Nuland, M., Fliervoet, L.A.L., Olivier, B., & Verster, J.C. (2010). The pathology of the alcohol hangover. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 3(3), 68-75. 43. Piasecki, T.M., Sher, K.J., Slutske, W.S., & Jackson, K.M. (2005). Hangover frequency and risk for alcohol use disorders: evidence from a longitudinal high-risk study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(2), 223-234. 44. Platt, B. (2013). Dartmouth-led high-risk drinking collaborative notes progress. Retrieved on January 14, 2014, from http://now.dartmouth.edu/2013/06/dartmouthled-high-risk-drinking-collaborative-notes-progress/ 45. Porter, S. R., Whitcomb, M. E., and Weitzer, W. H. (2004). Overcoming survey research problems, new directions for institutional research. San Francisco: JosseyBass. 46. Robertson, B.M., Piasecki, T.M., Slutske, W.S., Wood, P.K., Sher, K.J., Shiffman, S. & Heath, A.C. (2012). Validity of the hangover symptoms scale: evidence from an electronic diary study. Alcohol Clinical Experimental Research, 36(1), 171-177. 47. Sayette, M.A. (1999). Does drinking reduce stress? Alcohol Research and Health, 23(4), 250-255. 48. Sher, K.J. (1991). Children of alcoholics: a critical appraisal of theory and research. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 49. Slutske, W.S., Piasecki, T.M., & Hunt-Carter, E.E. (2003). Development and initial validation of the hangover symptoms scale: prevalence and correlates of hangover symptoms in college students. Alcoholism Clinical Experimental Research, 27(8), 1442-1450. 50. Span, S.A., & Earlywine, M. (1999). Familial risk for alcoholism and hangover symptoms. Addictive Behaviours, 24(1), 121-125. 51. Stark, J. (2007). Alarm over our binge-drinking 'epidemic’. Retrieved on November 14, 2013, from: http://www.theage.com.au/news/national/alarm-over-ourbingedrinking-epidemic/2007/02/13/1171128980481.html 52. Stockwell, T. (1998). Towards guidelines for low risk drinking: quantifying the short and long term costs of hazardous alcohol consumption. Alcohol Clinical and Experimental Research, 22(2), 635-695. 53. The Bible; King James Version. Isaiah, 5:11. 54. The National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. (2012). Alcohol use disorders. Retrieved on January 16, 2014, from http://www.niaaa.nih.gov/ 55. Tsukamoto, S., Kanegae, T., Saito, M., Nagoya, T., Shimamura, M., & Tainaka, H. (1991). Concentrations of blood and urine ethanol, acetaldehyde, acetate and acetone during experimental hangover in volunteers. Arukoru Kenkyuto Yakubutsu Izon, 26, 500-10. 47 September 2014 Kara Holloway MSc Psychology 56. Verster, J.C., Herwijnen, J., Olivier, B., & Kahler, C.W. (2009). Validation of the Dutch version of the young adult alcohol consequences questionnaire (B-YAACQ). Addictive Behaviours, 34(5), 411-414. 57. Verster, J.C., Kuerten, Y., Olivier, B., & Laar, M.W. (2010). The ACID survey: methodology and design of an online survey to access alcohol and recreational cocaine users and its consequences for traffic safety. Open Addiction Journal, 3, 2431. 58. Webb, E., Ashton, C.H., Kelly, P., Kamali, F. (1996). Alcohol and drug use in UK university students. The Lancet, 348(9032), 922-925. 59. Wiese, J.G., Shlipak, M.D., Browner, W.S. (2000). The alcohol hangover. Annual of International Medicine, 132(11), 897-902. 60. Yesavage, J.A., & Leirer, V.O. (1986). Hangover effects on aircraft pilots 14 hours after alcohol ingestion: a preliminary report. Am J Psychiatry, 143,1546-50. 61. Ylikahri, R.H., Huttunen, M., Eriksson, C.J., & Nikkila, E.A. (1974). Metabolic studies on the pathogenesis of hangover. Eur J Clin Invest, 4, 93–100. 48