Budi Ilhami 2003512081 Rombel 1 No. 22 BEHAVIORISM INTRODUCTION Question: What Is Behaviorism? Answer: Give me a dozen healthy infants, well-formed, and my own specified world to bring them up in and I'll guarantee to take any one at random and train him to become any type of specialist I might select -- doctor, lawyer, artist, merchantchief and, yes, even beggar-man and thief, regardless of his talents, penchants, tendencies, abilities, vocations, and race of his ancestors. --John Watson, Behaviorism, 1930 The term behaviorism was first publicly appeared in Watson’s Behaviorist Views (1913), a classic paper psychology. The establishment of a school which implemented behaviorism was also committed. The perception of learning theory of behaviorism is conditioning influences behavior. The interaction with the surrounding is the moment in which conditioning takes place. Responses to the conditioning as stimulus create our behaviors. It is very possible to reveal behavior systematically through a study by not taking internal mental condition into account. As believed by behaviorists that behavior is observable and cognitions, emotions and moods are not objective as a field of study. The behaviorist school of thought insists that behaviors is possibly described scientifically without the presence of thoughts and beliefs (Baum: 1994). One of the most celebrated behaviorists was Ivan Pavlov, who examined classical conditioning although he did not constantly shared the same ideas with behaviorism or behaviorists, Edward Lee Thorndike, John B. Watson who rejected introspective methods and sought to restrict psychology to experimental methods, and B.F. Skinner who conducted research on operant conditioning (Fraley: 2001). There are two types of conditioning namely classical conditioning and operant conditioning. According to Papalia, Feldman, & Olds (2007) “classical conditioning is a type of learning which based on the association of a stimulus that does not ordinarily elicit a particular response with another stimulus that does elicit the response”. In other words, classical conditioning is technique in which a naturally stimulus and a response are exposed simultaneously. Next, a previously neutral stimulus is paired with the natural stimulus. In the end, even though the naturally occurring stimulus is absent, the neutral stimulus appears to remind the response. The earlier stimulus is called as the conditioned stimulus and the latter one is called as conditioned response. For instance, when a student constantly experiences unfortunate situations as stimuli in Mathematics class, he eventually concludes that Mathematics is hard and he hates it. The latter conditioning is operant conditioning. Morris and Maisto (2001) defines it as “Behavior that brings about a satisfying effect (reinforcement) is apt to be performed again, whereas behavior that brings about negative effect (punishment) is apt to suppressed”. From the definition above, we understand that operant conditioning (also known as instrumental conditioning) is a method of learning through two kinds of consequence. They are rewards and punishments. Through operant conditioning, those two possible consequences emerge as reaction to a behavior. HISTORY OF BEHAVIORISM The presence of behaviorism in the beginning of 20th century answered the problems of measurement when one dealt with “mentalistic psychology”. Here we can use experimental methods accurately. One of the principles of behaviorism is one needs to focus on observable behavior of people and animals, not event in one’s mind which is not visible as mentioned by John B. Watson, B.F. Skinner and others (Skinner:1984). Some prominent events followed the presence of behaviorism. In 1863, Ivan Sechenov published Reflexes of the Brain. He introduced the concept of inhibitory responses in the central nervous system. In 1900, Ivan Pavlov began studying the salivary response and other reflexes. He accidentally initiated an investigation of classical conditioning. It gave great influence toward behaviorism in the early of 19th century until 20th century. Even though at first he did not have the same opinion with other behaviorist such as Thorndike, Watson and Skinner. In 1913, John Watson's Psychology as a Behaviorist Views It was published. The article presented the many of the main points of behaviorism. In 1920, Watson and assistant Rosalie Rayner conducted the famous "Little Albert" experiment. 1943 was the first year of publication of Clark Hull's Principles of Behavior. B.F. Skinner published Walden II which described a utopian society founded upon behaviorist principles in 1948. Noam Chomsky in 1959 published his criticism of Skinner's behaviorism, "Review of Verbal Behavior." B.F. Skinner published his book Beyond Freedom and Dignity, in which he argued that free will is an illusion in 1971. Behaviorism possessed the same points with the psychoanalytic and Gestalt movements in psychology for 2 centuries. On the contrary, it had some differences from the Gestalt psychologist’s mental philosophy (Gazzaniga: 2010). It lost popularity in the second half of the 20th century due to the presence of cognitivism (Friesen: 2005, Waldrop: 2002). PRINCIPLES OF LEARNING ACCORDING TO BEHAVIORISM Key Features Stimulus – Response Classical Conditioning & Operant Conditioning Methodology Lab Experiments Little Albert Edward Thorndike (the cat in a puzzle box) Reinforcement & Punishment (Skinner) Objective Measurement Social Learning Theory (Bandura) Nomothetic Reductionism Basic Assumptions Psychology should be seen as a science, to be studied in a scientific manner. Behaviorism is primarily concerned with observable behavior, as opposed to internal events like thinking. Behavior is the result of stimulus – response (i.e. all behavior, no matter how complex, can be reduced to a simple stimulus – response features). Behavior is determined by the environment (e.g. conditioning). Strengths Scientific Highly applicable (e.g. therapy) Emphasizes objective measurement Many experiments to support theories Identified comparisons between animals (Pavlov) and humans (Watson & Rayner Little Albert) Skinner box (rats & pigeons) Pavlov’s Dogs Bandura Bobo Doll Experiment Ethical Considerations Areas of Application Gender Role Development Behavioral Therapy (e.g. Flooding) Phobias Behavior-Modification Aversion Therapy Scientific Methods Relationships Language Moral Development Aggression Addiction Limitations Ignores mediational processes Ignores biology (e.g. testosterone) Too deterministic (little free-will) Experiments – low ecological validity Humanism – can’t compare animals to humans Reductionist APPLICATION FOR LANGUAGE TEACHING AND LEARNING Verbal behavior had been the attention of Skinner at the moment he turned his focus from experimental to philosophical supports of behavior (Skinner: 1957) and works related to language (Skinner: 1969). To analyze Verbal Behavior functionally, vocabulary and theory were arranged which received strong criticism of Chomsky (Chomsky & Skinner: 1959). Contingencies of Reinforcement (Skinner: 1969) is an essay in which Skinner stated that one of human abilities regarding linguistics is to give stimuli and manage their behavior as external stimuli do. The reinforcement contingencies result differently on human behavior as they posses what we call “instructional control”. Therefore the study of interaction between the instructional control and contingency control has been the main field among behaviorist. In addition, the study aims at achieving instructions need constructing and control over behavior managed by behavioral processes. Behaviorism declares that language is a product of habitual action. Its acquisition is the result of conditioning. Behaviorists reckon practice as habit formation by exposing “correct” response reinforcement. Language is studied through practice/drill as the main means without the presence of helping to rationalistic explanation. Hadley (2001) mentioned that they considered language acquisition a matter of practice or "operant conditioning". Skinner (1957) as cited in Hadley (2001) stated that "Behaviors that are reinforced will be learned". Below are some examples on how operant conditioning can be applied in the classrooms: recognize and reinforce positive behaviors and genuine task accomplishments,use various types of reinforcement such as teacher approval (praise, smiles, attention, and pats on the shoulder),concrete reinforcement (cookies, candies, and stationeries) and privileges (longer recess tim eand more time with friends), reinforce good behaviors and punish bad ones consistently, use schedule of reinforcement, such as surprise rewards, to encourage persistance, use positive punishment as the last option, use negative punishment, such as detention class, instead, punish students’ behavior, not their personal qualities, tell the students which behavior is being punished. We are able to apply classical conditioning to give positive and pleasant events with learning and classroom activities, such as use attractive learning aids, decorate the classrooms, encourage students to work in small groups for difficult learning tasks, greet the students and smile at them when they come to the classroom, inform the students clearly and specifically the format of quizzes, tests, and examinations, make students understands the rules of the classrooms, give adequate time for students to prepare for and complete the learning tasks. Behaviorism states that error fossilization must be avoided, so clear correction must be done. Cook (2008) mentions that parents do not normally perform corrective action towards utterances of their children and when correction is given, it focuses more on meaning rather than grammar. On the contrary, teachers give feedback to correct students’ mistakes grammatically. Success in language learning according to behaviorist theory is a result of extremely large number of drills, the closeness between L1 and L2 and formation of language habit. In the beginner course behaviorist theory is helpful, especially if students learn how to pronounce sounds. REFERENCES ______________. (2009). Learning Theories: Introduction to Behaviorism - in Learning Theories e-Book - Asia e University. Retrieved Nov 11, 2012, from http://www.pedagogy.ir/index.php/behaviorism/94-learning-theoriesintroduction-to-behaviorism Baum, W. M. (1994). Understanding behaviorism: science, behavior, and culture. New York: HarperCollins College Publishers. Cherry, K. (2012). What is Behaviorism? Retrieved Nov 11, 2012, from http://psychology.about.com/od/behavioralpsychology/f/behavioris m.htm. Chomsky, N., & Skinner, B.F. (1959). A Review of B.F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Language 35 (35): 26– 58.doi:10.2307/411334. JSTOR 411334. Cook, V. (2008). Second Language Learning and Language Teaching (forth edition ed.). Fraley, L.F. (2001). Strategic interdisciplinary relations between a natural science community and a psychology community (pdf). The Behavior Analyst Today 2(4): 209–324. (retrieved Nov 11, 2012) Friesen, N. (2005). Mind and Machine: Ethical and Epistemological Implications for Research. Thompson Rivers University, B.C., Canada. Gazzaniga, M. (2010). Psychological Science. New York: W.W. Norton & Company. pp. 19. Hadley, A. O. (2001). Teaching Language in Context (3d ed.). Mcleod, S. A. (2007). Behaviorism. Retrieved Nov 11, 2012, from www.simplypsycology.org/behaviorism.html. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. Massachusetts: Copley Publishing Group. Skinner, B.F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: a theoretical analysis. Appleton-Century-Crofts. p. 283. ISBN 0-13-171728-6. Skinner, B.F. (1984). The operational analysis of psychological terms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7(4): 547–81. Retrieved 2008-01-10. Skinner, B.F. (1969). An operant analysis of problem-solving. pp. 133– 57.; chapter in Skinner, B.F. 1969. Contingencies of reinforcement: a theoretical analysis. Appleton-Century-Crofts. Stemmer, N. (1990). Skinner's verbal behavior, Chomsky's review, and mentalism. J Exp Anal Behav 54(3doi10.1901/jeab.1990.54307PMC1323000PMID2103585 Waldrop, M.M. (2002). The Dream Machine: JCR Licklider and the revolution that made computing personal. New York: Penguin Books