Iara Cury Week #2 Trinity Term Inge Daniels Anthropology and the Senses Anthropologists struggle with Descartes’ legacy. There is no way around it: the Cartesian mind/body dualism has permeated our analytical thinking since the foundation of the discipline. Yet through an emerging anthropology of the senses we have developed an ambiguous relationship with philosophy. Making extensive use of phenomenology, anthropologists proceed to berate philosophers for not daring to ask the kinds of questions that lead to a better understanding of lived experience. This critical stance originates in a postmodern realization of and struggle against the primacy of the “representationalist theory of knowledge” in the Western tradition. Beyond the hegemonic limitations of such visualism, some anthropologists have argued, lies a world of rich multi-sensorial, experiential knowledge. Independent of mental representations—unbounded by them— this world must be explored if we are ever to make sense of the “sensuous interrelationship of body-mind-environment” (Howe, 2005, p.7) at the root of human existence. In the introduction to the book, Empire of the Senses, D. Howe explains how some anthropologists have embarked on an “ideological revolution” to “recover a full-bodied understanding of culture and experience” (2005, p.1). In particular, overcoming the analytical framework of language and graphic-spatial conceptualization has allowed the discipline a return to basics—to how people derive meaning and enact life based on sensory experience. The notion of embodiment and emplacement has gained currency in a variety of fields, including the study of memory, identity and power. Yet, interestingly enough, this rediscovery of the ground beneath our feet has challenged the traditional naturalization of the senses, an attitude ideologically significant for an exclusive focus on ideas inside the mind. Instead of taking the senses to be “the raw material of bodily sensations” (Ingold, 2000, p.282), that is, objective data upon which we build our understanding of the world and out of which we create sociality and culture, Howe asserts that the senses are themselves culturally shaped. According to him, “just as human nature itself is a product of culture, so is the human sensorium”; furthermore, the senses are “carefully hierarchized and regulated so as to express and enforce the social and cosmic order (2005, p.3). The present effort to reconstitute our conceptualization of the senses touches anthropology in its foundations. It brings about the question of whether different cosmologies among different cultures arise because of differences in the way people perceive the world around them. Ingold states, “it is supposed that where vision predominates, people will apprehend the world in one way, and where hearing predominates they will apprehend it in another” (2000, p.281). But focusing on the example of Western culture, widely believe to privilege vision above all senses, he further argues that in reality things are more complicated. In the book chapter entitled, “Stop, Look and Listen! Vision, Hearing and Human Movement”, Ingold outlines a strong critique of the anthropology of the senses. For him, people around the world have access to a similar range of sensory information; different cultural emphases on specific senses originate in “the ideal realm of mental representations” and not in divergent sensorial or cognitive abilities (2000, p.284). Likewise, the West’s obsession with vision, termed visualism, is inherently linked to a Cartesian “representationalist theory of knowledge”, where vision is equated with visualization—not an over reliance on visual input per say (p.282). Based on this philosophical framework, vision has been co-opted “in the service of a peculiarly modern project of objectification” (p.253). Within the Western tradition, it is strongly associated with rationality and objectivity, a perspective where reality is “there to be seized by the eyes, analysed by science, exploited by technology and dominated by power” (p.246). Ingold’s extensive analysis of vision and hearing drives home the point that vision as normal Westerners experience it is absolutely interdependent on the other senses, particularly that of hearing. “[I]t is the incorporation of audition into the process of visual perception that converts passive spectating into active looking” (p.277), he affirms, citing Merleau-Ponty’s belief that the body is a synergic system (p.268). Not only that, Ingold points out that scholars too often neglect the fundamental reciprocal and intimate aspects of vision, for example the significance of eye to eye contact for human sociality. His conclusion is that “any attempt to separate out the discourse surrounding vision from the actual practices of looking, watching and seeing is unsustainable” (p.286). Lived experience should be probed independently of the predominant discourse and set of values associated with the senses. An appropriate case study for illustrating the incompatibility of anthropology’s “visualism” or single-minded search for “social structures” is C. Helliwell’s book chapter, “Space and sociality in a Dayak longhouse”. Eschewing the usual structuralist agenda, Helliwell communicates that much understanding resides in the admission of the complex fluidity of human social relations within longhouses in Borneo. She details how daily life inside of private apartments is quite open to the scrutiny and support of the longhouse community. Mainly through the sense of hearing, but also using vision and the movement of goods across permeable partitions, people create an atmosphere of sharing and conviviality. In Helliwell’s words, “within the longhouse, voices create a powerful sense of community” (1996, p.138). Interestingly enough, this diffuse kind of sociality is also a method of social regulation: “the openness of the lawang section of an apartment to the gaze and audit of neighbors places those inhabitants under much greater pressure to conform to recognized community standards of behavior” (p.142). Thus “private” apartments quite open to the scrutiny of the internal community, something that previous ethnographic studies of the Dayak have failed to determine through visual observation alone. The point that Helliwell makes is that the variety of forms of sociality across cultures goes much beyond what “visualism” can identify and what researchers focused on “‘structure’ as opposed to ‘process’” can analyze (p.129). Perhaps the key realization in Helliwell’s account is the fact that people employ all of their senses in the transformation of a space into a place—into a familiar and safe physical context where life can take root. K. Hetherington expresses a similar interpretation, stating that our experiences depend on our “ability to make us of such proximal and performative forms of knowledge”, particularly of haptic familiarity, in the making of place (2002, p.1936). Touch, for this author, is one of the more powerful means to achieve proximal knowledge—“embodied, sensory and unsightly” (p.1935)—where boundaries between bodies and objects are blurred and meaning emerges without the mediation of conscious thought. Hetherington’s emphasis on the significance of nonrepresentational practice and knowledge (p.1934) accords well with Ingold’s discussion of the evident difference between representationalist versus experience-based accounts of the earth. According to Ingold, “creatures live in the land and not on it” (2007, p.S33). In experiencing the world as an environment replete with sentient beings and dynamic natural processes, our capacity for feeling wind, weather and movement leads us to an completely different understanding of what “in reality” is a sphere suspended in space. Yet which is the more valid reality? Ingold, Helliwell and Hetherington’s accounts all stand in agreement that hearing and the “contact” senses of touch, smell and taste are essential inasmuch as they constitute a large part of the intricate sensorial fabric of sociality and affect our physical, psychological and social experience of place. The consensus appears to be that the Western penchant for the visual representation of information has, until recently, stood in the way of a holistic approach to sensory experience that literally disfigures our understanding of living culture. Overall, anthropological work attuned to the new current of phenomenology suggests that a narrow focus on the visual is non-anthropological for the basic reason that human life deeply surpasses the experience of sight. Yet as much as scholars attribute this intellectual lens to ‘western’ practices and theories, things may not be so simple. Through technologies related to literacy and the recording of images we are bombarded by amounts of visual information that exceed all other kinds of sensory input. Does this state of affairs originate from a Western, Cartesian visualism or a representationalist theory of knowledge? Are people intuitively drawn towards the visual? Or is this an artifact of the material practicality of the inscription of visual information as opposed to auditory, tactile, gustatory and olfactory information? Perhaps Ingold is correct in noting that for people that live in a dense, closed forest environment, vision is less important than the other senses for practical reasons (p.253). What anthropologists would do well not to forget is that today a majority of people live in an urban environment where vision is paramount for physical orientation as much as for social engagement. How can anthropologists’ interpret the demonstrable quantitative difference between visual data and information transmitted through the other senses in modern, urban life? Could quantity be marker of hierarchy—the more visual information, the more important vision? With regards to Ingold’s critique of the theory of visualism, is it possible that through the power of discourse people’s cognitive make-up can be modified so that they indeed experience the world in discourse-oriented ways? It is important to remember that sensory perception has both “a history and a politics”. D. Howe finds this all the more reason for “analyzing social ideologies conveyed through sensory values and practices” (2005, p.4), keeping in mind that “intersensoriality” does not imply order or equality, either in the sensory or social realm. He gives the example of how the sense of smell is used to associate the upper classes with pleasant smells and the lower classes with repulsive odors, stating that “this perception of malodor had less to do with practices of cleanliness than it had to do with social status” (p.10). The point is that sensations of disgust go much beyond natural physiological reactions, molded as they are by social and cultural ideology. Ingold’s opinion is that an analysis of discourse is meaningless when divorced from an accurate understanding of lived experience. Anthropology is acutely aware that discourse is not simply superimposed on people’s experiences—it is continually crafted and modified; yet its existence does affect these very experiences in a feedback loop. The question that Ingold tackled remains on the table: to what extent is cognition malleable and in which cases do cosmologies convey cognitively different experiences rather than diverse cultural or social discourses? If visualism is comprised of a reduction of the richness of the senses to a narrow range of visual practices, is this reduction discursive rather than phenomenological, as Ingold maintains? Human intersensoriality is a reality to be acknowledged and incorporated into anthropological analysis. However, in itself intersensoriality is not incompatible with a hierarchy of the senses rooted in cognition, whether this hierarchy arises “naturally” or from social influence. Ascribing such a hierarchy to the discursive level Ingold neglects to probe not only the possibility but also the mechanisms that might produce a hierarchization out of interdependent or synergetic sensory organs. At the end of the day, ethnographers may be justified in focusing their limited datacollection resources on the visual because of its substantial role in human life. Meanwhile, anthropology must continue to probe how the visual is inextricably integrated with the other senses and what is its role in the practice of sociality and reciprocity. On the whole, one thing is clear—vision is an extraordinary ability and as such deserves extraordinary attention. Bibliography Howes, D. 2005. Introduction. In D. Howes (ed.). Empire of the Senses: The sensual culture reader. Oxford: Berg Publishers. Ingold, T. 2000. Stop, look, Listen! Vision, Hearing and Human Movement. The Perception of the Environment. Oxford: Routledge. Ingold, T. 2007. Earth, Sky, Wind and Weather. JRAI Special Issue: 19-38 Helliwell, C. 1996. Space and sociality in a Dayak Longhouse. In M. Jackson (ed.) Things as they are: New directions in Phenomenological Anthropology. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Hetherington,K. 2003. Spatial Textures: place, touch, and praesentia. Environment and Planning A35: 1933-44.