Voor Werner: Changes in Aspect Elly van Gelderen Munich, February 2016 Outline Werner’s contributions on outer and inner aspect have been foundational. This paper builds on that by looking at: a. Some examples of changes in outer aspect, b. Changes in inner aspect/Aktionsart: unaccusative and unergative causative and transitive copula, experiencer verbs ditransitives Werner on grammatical/outer aspect: -1996. Introduction to Aspect and Aktionsart. Folia Linguistica. 30.1/2, 1-3. W. Abraham & W. Klimonow (eds). Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter. -1997. The interdependence of case, aspect, and referentiality in the history of German: the case of the genitive. In van Kemenade & Vincent (eds.), Parameters of morphosyntactic change, 29-61. Cambridge: CUP. -1999. How descending is ascending German? On the deep interrelations between tense, aspect, pronominality, and ergativity.” In: Abraham & Kulikov (eds) Tense-aspect, transitivity, and causativity.Amsterdam : John Benjamins, 253-292. -2004. The European demise of the simple past and the emergence of the periphrastic perfect: Areal diffusion or natural, autonomous evolution under parsing facilitation?“. In: Abraham (ed.), Focus on Germanic typology, 241-272. Berlin: Akademieverlag. Werner on Aktionsart/inner aspect: 1986. Unaccusatives in German. Groninger Arbeiten zur germanistischen Linguistik 28: 1-72. 1990. A note on the aspect-syntax interface.” In: J. Mascaró en M. Nespor (red.), Grammar in Progress. GLOW Essays for Henk van Riemsdijk, Dordrecht , 1990, 112. 1999. Introduction. Werner Abraham & Leonid Kulikov (eds), Tense-aspect, transitivity, and causativity. Amsterdam : John Benjamins, xi-xxxiii 2002. (At least) Two types of unaccusativity – or non at all. In: A. Wedel & H.-J. Busch (eds) Verba et litterae: explorations in Germanic languages and German literature. Essays in honor of Albert L. Lloyd. Newark , Delaware : Linguatext, 1-12. Broader relevance Argument structure forms the basis of our propositions and, without it, there is no meaning. It is likely that AS is part of our larger cognitive system and not restricted to the language faculty. Bickerton (1990: 185) suggests that the “universality of thematic structure suggests a deep-rooted ancestry, perhaps one lying outside language altogether.” If argument/thematic structure predates the emergence of language, an understanding of causation, intentionality, volition - all relevant to determining theta-structure - may be part of our larger cognitive system and not restricted to the language faculty. It then fits that argument structure is relevant to other parts of our cognitive make-up, e.g. the moral grammar. Gray et al. (2007), for instance, argue that moral judgment depends on mind perception, ascribing agency and experience to other entities. De Waal (e.g. 2006) has shown that chimps and bonobos show empathy, planning, and attribute minds to others. outer vs inner outer can change inner, e.g. perfective over durative: (1) I built the house But not always, e.g. imperfective over state: (2) *I am seeing the blue sky (for hours) Terminative/biphasic vs monophasic Abraham (1989; 1990) uses these terms to distinguish two crucial classes, also known as telic vs durative. And prefixes (ein, zu) can add terminativity. It is safe to say that most diachronic research has been on outer aspect. I’ll mention a few and then go on to inner aspect, showing how the two are related through biphasic vs monophasic. Sources of outer aspect Imperfective < durative Positional verbs, e.g. staan, liggen, and zitten `stand, lay, and sit’: (1) Tegen heug en meug ga je lopen fietsen. Without pleasure go you walk cycle `Without pleasure you will be cycling.’ (2) Dan zit ie te zorgen dat ie een goede opvolger krijgt. Then sits he to make sure ... Prepositions > imperfective, e.g. on. Perfective < telic P and V The Chinese perfective marker derives from a verb (liao meaning `to complete') and the Nupe perfective á from a light verb (lá meaning `take’, see Kandybowicz 2008). Smyth (1920: 366):"[t]he addition of a preposition ... to a verbal form may mark the completion of the action of the verbal idea (perfective action)". (1) eis-elthen eis ton oikon NT Greek in-came in the house `He entered the house.’ (Luke 1.40, Goetting 2007: 317) Bulgarian (2) (3) Ivan skoči prez ogradata Ivan jumped over fence-the `Ivan jumped over the fence.’ Ivan pres-koči ogradata Ivan over-jumped fence.the `Ivan jumped the fence.' (Mariana Bahtchevanova p.c.) Structural renewal a. ASPP ASP VP (ge-) V > > AP up c. ASPP ASP VP up V b. ASPP ASP VP up V ... AP up Adding telic adverbs > ASP evaporate out dissipate away spend down receive in copy out present out compact down boost up issue out order up offer up distribute out include in calculate out Sea Island Creole, spoken, and online: (1) If I was somebody could turn up that hospital up, I’d do it. `If I were somebody who could tear up that hospital, I’d do it.’ (Cunningham 1992: 95) (2) To evaluate whether there is air mixed with water in the radiator, turn on it on for a little while. (http://www.wisegeek.com/what-is-radiator-bleeding.htm) (3) Can we just point out something out very quickly. (COCA, Sp Fox 5 2011) (4) Makeup is used to cover up things up, right? (COCA, Sp CBS 2002) (5) and help the congregation tear down it down. (http://bayridgebrooklyn.blogspot.com/2008/02/rally-to-save-bayridge-methodist.html) Adverb > affix (1) You can’t open up it because it is glued on. (Abe, 2.11.21, Kuczaj corpus) (2) to pick up it (Abe 3.4.08, Kuczaj corpus) There are 40 or more in the Spoken COCA, just with up and it: (2) somebodys going to go to that phone and pick up it. (COCA, sp MSNBC 2005) have loss and renewal (1) We're assuming that the headache would have not have gone away if you hadn't taken the placebo. (COCA TalkNation 2010) (2) This may have not have turned out the way we wanted, but it was quite a ride (COCA WashPost 2009) (3) It turns out that there have not have been, had not been nearly as much progress (COCA MeetPress 2008) with other adverbs: (4) And we 've actually have gathered a lot of evidence today (COCA Dateline 2002) (5) We 've actually have had some surprises (COCA NPR Science 1997) (6) I 've never have seen the specifics of how they want to do that (COCA ABC 2008) (7) some television stations have power generators and we 've never have lost ours (COCA TalkNation 2007) Have renewal PerfP Perf have u-asp vP v’ v seen i-pf ... Many changes to the outer aspect: basically renewal of eroding material a. b. c. d. e. f. change ge-/for- --> Ø modal --> ASP A/P --> ASP have --> -a and renewed to/do/ing in ASP I eat now --> -ing century C12-C14 C15 continual C15 C14-C17 C19 Now onto inner aspect Three types of inner aspect: durative, telic, and stative. Arguments depend on this: durative has agent; telic has theme. Change is slower; mostly keeps its inner aspect. Changes Unaccusative verbs > adding light verbs + labile and unergatives > transitive + particle Unaccusatives > copulas Unaccusatives ̸> unergatives; Unergatives ̸> unaccusatives Psych-verbs: ObjExp > SuExp; but not the other way round. Ditransitives: little changes Basic insight: biphasic and monophasic character remains From OE>ME: Loss of Intransitives a) a complete loss of the verb, e.g. bifian `to shake’, b) the loss of prefixes and addition of resultative particles, e.g. aberstan `burst out, escape’, c) the replacement by light verbs and adjective or noun, e.g. emtian `become empty’, d) a change to labile verbs, e.g. dropian `drop’, i.e. alternating between causative and unaccusative, and e) increase in (manner of) motion (Fanego 2012) 80 intransitives from Visser aberstan `burst out, escape’ Th ablican `shine’ Th ablinan `cease, desist’ Th æfnian `become evening’ 0 æmtian/emtian `become empty’ Th ærnan `run’ A ætfellan `fall away’ Th ætglidan `disappear, glide away’ Th ætslidan `slip, slide’ Th ætspringan `rush forth’ Th aferscan `become fresh’, Th afulian `become fowl, rot’ Th alatian `to grow sluggish’ Th aleoran `to depart/flee’ Th ascortian `become short/pass away’ aslapan `slumber, fall asleep’ Th Th particle verb obsolete obsolete light v light v (and labile) labile (caus, unerg, unacc) particle verb particle verb labile obsolete light v light v obsolete obsolete light v obsolete berstan `burst’ Th bifian `tremble/shake’ A blinnan `cease’ Th brogdian, brogdettan `tremble’ A bugan `bow down/bend’ Th cidan `quarrel, complain’ A cirman `cry (out)’ A climban (upp) `climb’ A cloccian `cluck, make noise’ A clum(m)ian `mumble, mutter’ A clymmian `climb’ A cneatian `argue’ A cneowian `kneel down’ A cnitian `dispute’ A creopan `crawl’ A cuman `come, approach, arrive’ Th burst labile (causative rare) obsolete obsolete obsolete obsolete transitive obsolete (same and) transitive transitive (archaic) obsolete (particle verb and) transitive obsolete obsolete obsolete same: creep same: come (to) Results Obsolete Unchanged Light v Particle Labile Transitive Total 44 11 8 6 6 5 80 80 verbs in OE The verbs that are replaced by light verbs are deadjectival and denominal verbs, namely æfnian, æmtian, aferscan, afulian, ascortian, dimmian, fordragan, and gegyltan: all unaccusative verbs in Old English but the new light verb determines whether it is unaccusative or causative. The change to labile verb affects ærnan, ætslidan, berstan, droppian, droppetan, and growan. Apart from ærnan, these are all unaccusative and end up with an optional causative. The case of ærnan is complex; it is an unergative in Old English but acquires causative and unaccusative meanings. The new particle verbs replace a prefix, as in aberstan, ætfellan, ætglidan, forscrincan, forþgangangan, and forþræsan. Like the prefixes, the new particles indicate a path or result and imply perfective aspect. The five unergative verbs that become transitive are cidan, climban, cloccian, clymmian, and felan. Cloccian is archaic but the others acquire a regular Theme. Obsolete? A possible pattern may be that many, among the 44 that become obsolete, are `uncontrolled process’: bifian `tremble/shake’, brogdian, brogdettan `tremble’, cirman `cry (out)’, clum(m)ian `mumble, mutter’, flicorian `flutter’, giccan `itch or hiccup’ , ginan/ginian `yawn, gape, utter a sound’, giscian `sob’, glisian `glitter’, and glit(e)nian `glitter, shine’. These verbs are durative but non-agentive. Acquisition Bloom et al (1980) show that children are conscious of aspectual verb classes very early on. Thus, –ed morphemes go with non-durative events, -ing with durative non-completive activities, and infinitives with stative verbs. Various researchers agree on this, e.g. Broman Olsen & Weinberg (1999) likewise show that a telic verb correlates with the presence of –ed and that – ing is frequent with dynamic and durative verbs. The next slide lists all the adjectives and verbs for Eve at the time of her first recording. All types of verbs are there and a few of the activity verbs are marked with –ing (swimming and writing) and a past is marked on an unaccusative (broke). Eve at 1;6 unaccusative block broke (Neil) sit down, busy, gone Mommy down, open come down, sit down, fall down (finger) stuck lie down stool unergative transitive other (fish are) swimming Eve pencil that radio wait, play, cook I did it look Eve/you find it Eve writing see ya stand dance doll eat celery Mommy step read the puzzle Mommy swing? change her man (no) taste it get her/it fix (it)/ Mommy fix bring it want Mommy letter write a paper man/papa have it (you) find it play (step) Intransitives Few mistakes in acquisition Bickerton (1990: 185) suggests that the “universality of thematic structure suggests a deep-rooted ancestry, perhaps one lying outside language altogether.” Diachronic instability in the morphological expression of v/ASP but very predictable change: unaccusative > causative unergative > transitive Aspect is stable Next: copulas and psych-verbs Change to copulas etc... English: duration (remain and stay), change of state (become and fall), and mood (seem and appear). Curme (1935: 66-8): 60 copulas in English; “no other language shows such a vigorous growth of copulas” (67). Visser (1963: 213-9) lists over a 100 for the various stages. Unaccusative > copula appear, become, fall, go, grow, turn, wane, break, last, remain, rest, stay, continue Copulas in Modern English Sorace’s Hierarchy: Theme over Agent Change of Location Change of State Continuation come, fall, befall appear, break, blush, become remain, stay, persist, persevere, loom, stand, lie, rest Existence of State seem Uncontrolled process shine Controlled motion --Controlled non-motion ObjExp færan/fear lician/like loathe marvel relish OE-1480 OE-1800 OE-1600 1380-1500 1567-1794 SuExp 1400-now 1200-now 1200-now 1380-now 1580-now Loss of causative –i-: many Exp verbs are causative and therefore reanalyzed fǽran < *fæ̂rjan `frighten’ Other productive causatives: a-hwænan `vex, afflict’, gremman `enrage’, abylgan `anger’, swencan `harrass’, a-þrytan `weary’, wægan `vex’, and wyrdan `annoy’. So, does the loss of the causative in ferian cause reanalysis? Possibly with ferian but not with marvel and relish. Object Exp are unstable, e.g. please `Last’ ObjExp with `fear’ (1) (2) Þe fend moveþ þes debletis to fere Cristene [men] fro treuþe. `The enemy moves these devils to frighten Christian men from the truth.’ (MED, a1425 Wycl.Serm. Bod 788 2.328) Thus he shal yow with his wordes fere. `Thus, he’ll frighten you with his words.’ (MED, Chaucer TC 4.1483) The addition of result/instrument in ObjExp emphasizes Change of State in the later stages. First SuExp with `fear’ (1) Fele ferde for þe freke(z), lest felle hym þe worre. `Many feared for the man lest the worst happened to him.’ (MED, c1390 Gawain Nero A.10 1588) (2) I fere me þat I shuld stond in drede. `I fear that I shall stand in dread.’ (MED, a1500 Play Sacr. Dub 652 218) The ambiguity depends on whether the postverbal pronoun is seen as a reflexive or not. Thus, it is not clear whether (2) means `I frighten myself that ...’ or `I fear that ...’ Renewal of Object Experiencers anger, scare astonish grieve please irritate stun worry 1200 Old Norse 1375 unclear 1330 French 1350 Anglo-Norman 1531 Latin 1700 internal change 1807 internal change New ObjExp: new v-Cause (1) Suche daunsis, whiche‥dyd with vnclene motions or countinances irritate the myndes of the dauncers to venereall lustes. (1531 Elyot Bk. named Gouernouri. xix. sig. Kijv) (2) Impiety‥doth embitter all the conveniencies and comforts of life. (a1677 I. Barrow Serm. Several Occasions 1678: 52) (3) Which at first did frighten people more than anything. (1666 S. Pepys Diary 4 Sept VII 275) Agent and Th > Th/Cause and Exp: reintroduction of cause-v (1) a. They kill it [a fish] by first stunning it with a knock with a mallet. (OED 1662 J. Davies tr. A. Olearius Voy & Trav. Ambassadors 165) b. The ball, which had been nearly spent before it struck him, had stunned instead of killing him. (OED, 1837 Irving Capt. Bonneville I. 271) (2) Why doe Witches and old women, fascinate and bewitch children? (OED 1621 R. Burton Anat Melancholy i. ii. iii. ii. 127) Current changes: ExpSu>Agent? (1) I am liking/loving/hating it. E.g. in COCA: (2) how I got guard duty and how I'm going to be hating that and totally tired. (3) and I am liking what I see in the classrooms (4) lately we've been loving broccoli rabe, which (5) And so everybody in town was knowing that this was happening (6) I've been fearing the answers. Cyclical change in psych-verbs ObjExp stun SuAg fear `frighten’ SubExp seeing/liking it Acquisition Eve (Brown 1973) has SuExp like, love, want but not ObjExp anger, scare; her hurt is SuExp initially. Eve love crayon (1;9), want mommy letter (1;6), want watch (1;6), want mommy out (1;6), want lunch, want down, want mommy read (1;6) ... but: hurt xxx self (1;7), hurt knee (1;9), I hurt my finger (1;11) Sarah has early want (2;3), love (2;5), and hurt as in: I hurt again (2;9.6). Her scare is late at 3;7: to scare me on the dark (3;7.16) Linguistic conclusions Changes: Unaccusative > causative (labile and light verbs) Unergative > transitive Unaccusative > copula Increase in light verbs and particles Increase in lability: 80 > 800 Psych-verb and copula: Theme is crucial and stable Conceptual Structure? Aspectual +/-telic, +/- durative is pervasive Verbs always have a Theme argument but they don’t always have an Agent or Causer. The latter are introduced by optional light verbs which may be overt or not. The vP shell is stable and may show the conceptual structure with an emphasis on aspect and theta-roles. Non-Werner References Allen, Cynthia. 1995. Case marking and reanalysis. OUP Borer, Hagit 2005. In Name Only. OUP. Brinton, Laurel. 1988. The Development of English Aspectual Systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Gelderen, Elly van 2011. Valency Changes. JHL 1.1: 106-143. Gelderen, Elly van 2014. Changes in Psych-Verbs. CJL 13: 99-122. Hale, Ken & Keyser, Samuel Jay. 2002. Prolegomenon to a Theory of Argument Structure. MIT Press. Jackendoff, Ray 1987. Consciousness and the Computational Mind. MIT Press. Lavidas, Nikolaos 2013. Null and cognate objects and changes in (in)transitivity: Evidence from the history of English. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 60.1: 69-106. Leiss, Elisabeth. 2000. Artikel und Aspekt. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. Levin, Beth & Malka Rappaport Hovav. 1995. Unaccusativity. MIT Press. Lohndal, Terje 2014. Phrase structure and argument structure. OUP. McMillion, Allan. 2006. Labile Verbs in English. Stockholm PhD. Pinker, Steven 1989. Learnability and Cognition. MIT Press. Ryan, John 2012. The Genesis of Argument Structure. Lambert AP.