Unit 1c: Essential Elements of Research PSYC 4310 COGS 6310 MGMT 6969 Michael J. Kalsher Department of Cognitive Science © 2015, Michael Kalsher 1 Internal vs. External Validity Internal Validity Extent to which causal/independent variable(s) and no other extraneous factors caused the change being measured. External Validity (generalizability) Degree to which the results and conclusions of your study would hold for other persons, in other places, and at other times. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 2 Threats to Internal Validity: Factors that reduce our ability to draw valid conclusions Selection History Maturation Repeated Testing Instrumentation Regression to the mean Subject mortality Selection-interactions Experimenter bias © 2015, Michael Kalsher 3 Reducing Threats to Internal Validity The role of Control Behavior is influenced by many factors termed—confounding variables—that tend to distort the results of a study, thereby making it impossible for the researcher to draw meaningful conclusions. Some of these may be unknown to the researcher. Control refers to the systematic methods (e.g., research designs) employed to reduce threats to the validity of the study posed by extraneous influences on both the participants and the observer (researcher). © 2015, Michael Kalsher 4 Group/Selection threat Occurs when nonrandom procedures are used to assign subjects to conditions or when random assignment fails to balance out differences among subjects across the different conditions of the experiment. Example: A researcher is interested in determining the factors most likely to elicit aggressive behavior in male college students. He exposes subjects in the experimental group to stimuli thought to provoke aggression and subjects in the control group to stimuli thought to reduce aggression and then measures aggressive behaviors of the students. How would the selection threat operate in this instance? © 2015, Michael Kalsher 5 History threat Events that happen to participants during the research which affect results but are not linked to the independent variable. Example: The reported effects of a program designed to improve medical residents’ prescription writing practices by the medical school may have been confounded by a self-directed continuing education series on medication errors provided to the residents by a pharmaceutical firm's medical education liaison. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 6 Maturation threat Can operate when naturally occurring biological or psychological changes occur within subjects and these changes may account in part or in total for effects discerned in the study. Example: A reported decrease in emergency room visits in a long-term study of pediatric patients with asthma may be due to subjects outgrowing childhood asthma rather than to any treatment regimen introduced to treat the asthma. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 7 Repeated testing threat May occur when changes in test scores occur not because of the intervention but rather because of repeated testing. This is of particular concern when researchers administer identical pretests and posttests. Example: A reported improvement in medical resident prescribing behaviors and order-writing practices in the study previously described may have been due to repeated administration of the same short quiz. That is, the residents simply learned to provide the right answers rather than truly achieving improved prescribing habits. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 8 Instrumentation threat When study results are due to changes in instrument calibration or observer changes rather than to a true treatment effect, the instrumentation threat is in operation. Example: In Kalsher’s Experimental Methods and Statistics course, he evaluates students progress in understanding principles of research design at week 3 of the semester. A graduate T.A. evaluates the students at the conclusion of the course. If the evaluators are dissimilar enough in their approach, perhaps because of lack of training, this difference may contribute to measurement error in trying to determine how much learning occurred over the semester. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 9 Statistical Regression threat The regression threat can occur when subjects have been selected on the basis of extreme scores, because extreme (low and high) scores in a distribution tend to move closer to the mean (i.e., regress) in repeated testing. Example: if a group of subjects is recruited on the basis of extremely high stress scores and an educational intervention is then implemented, any improvement seen could be due partly, if not entirely, to regression to the mean rather than to the coping techniques presented in the educational program. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 10 Experimental Mortality threat Experimental mortality—also known as attrition, withdrawals, or dropouts—is problematic when there is a differential loss of subjects from comparison groups subsequent to randomization, resulting in unequal groups at the end of a study. Example: Suppose a researcher conducts a study to compare the effects of a corticosteroid nasal spray with a saline nasal spray in alleviating symptoms of allergic rhinitis (irritation and inflammation of the nasal passages). If subjects with the most severe symptoms preferentially drop out of the active treatment group, the treatment may appear more effective than it really is. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 11 Selection Interaction threats A family of threats to internal validity produced when a selection threat combines with one or more of the other threats to internal validity. When a selection threat is already present, other threats can affect some experimental groups, but not others. Example: If one group is dominated by members of one fraternity (selection threat), and that fraternity has a party the night before the experiment (history threat), the results may be altered for that group. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 12 Threats to External Validity: Ways you might be wrong in making generalizations People, Places, and Times Demand Characteristics Hawthorne Effects Order Effects (or carryover effects) © 2015, Michael Kalsher 13 People threat: Are the results due to the unusual type of people in the study? Example: You learn that the grant you submitted to assess average drinking rates among college students in the U.S. has been funded. In late November, you post an announcement about the study on campus to get subjects for the study. 100 students sign up for the study. Of these, 78 are members of campus fraternities; the other 22 are members of the school’s football team. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 14 Places threat: Did the study work because of the unusual place you did the study in? Example: Suppose that you conduct an “educational” study in a college town with lots of high-achieving educationallyoriented kids. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 15 Time threat: Was the study conducted at a peculiar time? Example: Suppose that you conducted a smoking cessation study the week after the U.S. Surgeon General issued the well publicized results of the latest smoking and cancer studies. In this instance, you might get different results than if you had conducted the study the week before. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 16 Demand Characteristics Participants are often provided with cues to the anticipated results of a study. Example: When asked a series of questions about depression, participants may become wise to the hypothesis that certain treatments may work better in treating mental illness than others. When participants become wise to anticipated results (termed a placebo effect), they may begin to exhibit performance that they believe is expected of them. Making sure that subjects are not aware of anticipated outcomes (termed a blind study) reduces the possibility of this threat. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 17 Hawthorne Effects Similar to a placebo, research has found that the mere presence of others watching a person’s performance causes a change in their performance. If this change is significant, can we be reasonably sure that it will also occur when no one is watching? Addressing this issue can be tricky but employing a control group to measure the Hawthorne effect of those not receiving any treatment can be very helpful. In this sense, the control group is also being observed and will exhibit similar changes in their behavior as the experimental group therefore negating the Hawthorne effect. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 18 Order Effects (carryover effects) Order effects refer to the order in which treatment is administered in a within-subjects/repeatedmeasures design and can be a major threat to validity if multiple treatments are used, and no attempt to counterbalance the order is made. Example: If subjects are given medication for two months, therapy for another two months, and no treatment for another two months, it would be possible, and even likely, that the level of depression would be least after the final no treatment phase. Does this mean that no treatment is better than the other two treatments? It could mean that the benefits of the first two treatments have carried over to the last phase, artificially elevating the no treatment success rates. © 2015, Michael Kalsher 19