SLEEP Defining and describing sleep Neural mechanisms Sleep disorders Functions SLEEP-WAKE CYCLE A circadian rhythm (about 1 day in length). ultradian rhythm (>1 per day) e.g. REM sleep. infradian rhythm (<1 per day) e.g. hibernation. Rhythms provide temporal organisation Anticipating environmental changes E.g. change from day to night, preparation to feed etc. © N. E. Wilson 2 Sleep is associated with vertebrates A behavioural state of warm-blooded vertebrates (mammals and birds) Regarded as evolutionarily recent Emergent and ‘higher’ brain function. A behaviour and state of consciousness Characteristics of sleep Recumbent postures (typically closed eyes) Raised sensory thresholds Reduced motor activity Electrographic signs © N. E. Wilson 4 Measuring sleep Electroencephalogram (EEG) Electrooculogram (EOG) - electrical activity of eye movements. Electromyogram (EMG) - muscle activity. © N. E. Wilson 5 Major EEG patterns Synchronised (neurons firing at same time) Desynchronised (neurons firing at different times) Stages of sleep 5 stages identified by Kleitman and Dement in the 1950s Continuous and variable changes in EEG EEG patterns Alpha waves, regular medium frequency of 8-12 Hz during quiet rest Beta waves, irregular, low amplitude, at 13-30 Hz seen during alert wakefulness and REM sleep Theta activity (3.5-7.5 Hz) in stage 1 sleep (transiting from awake to sleep) Delta waves – high amplitude, low frequency (<3.5 Hz) pattern seen in stage 3 and 4 sleep Sleep spindles – short burst of 12-14 Hz activity during sleep stages 1 – 4 K complexes – sudden sharp waveform only seen in stage 2 sleep NREM (non rapid eye movement) sleep Stages 1, 2, 3, and 4 A.k.a. synchronised, S or quiet sleep. EEG waves grow progressively slower and larger moving from alpha to delta waves. Waking threshold increases. Heart rate and temperature fall. Muscle activity decreases passively. Dreams infrequent and of ‘thinking’ type Slow wave sleep (SWS) used as term for stages 3 and 4 REM (or emergent stage 1) sleep Aserinsky and Kleitman (1953) EEG irregular, low voltage fast waves (beta and theta activity), heart rate and breathing variable, intermittent rapid movement of eyes, Higher frequency of dreaming than in NREM sleep (usually involves imagery) 95% of awakenings from REM report dreams But REM and dreaming logically distinct (only correlation as evidence) Start 60 - 90 mins into sleep. Periods become more frequent towards morning. Cycle of sleep All mammalian sleep is cyclical with NREM punctuated by REM sleep. Humans have 4 to 5 cycles per night each 90 - 100 mins long and including 20-30 minutes of REM sleep. Broader basic rest-activity cycle (Kleitman 1982) E.g. eating, drinking, heart rate changes, play behaviour in kids, day dreaming (Cohen 1979) etc. Dreaming Primarily visual and motor elements Lack of coherence consistent with low frontal lobe activity (reduced executive control) In lucid dreaming cortex shows more activity PGO (Pons-geniculate-occipital) waves in REM Classic theory: Activation-synthesis hypothesis (Hobson and McCarley 1977, McCarley and Hoffman 1981) Do dreams have meaning? For Greeks and Romans had symbolic meanings But Greek diviner Artemidorus (AD 120) recognised that interpreters needed to know about each dreamer, his/her customs and where he/she lived ‘the rules of dreaming are not general, and therefore cannot satisfy all persons, but often, according to times and persons, they admit of varied interpretations.’ Do dreams have meaning? For some, portents of the future Compensatory function for Freud and Jung Substitute for repressed emotions or unconscious wishes (Freud) An expression of a collective unconscious (Jung) Do dreams have meaning? For biopsychologist Hobson ‘Meaning’ in creation of story, not expression of repressed wishes Analysis of form rather than content E.g. hallucinatory images linked to activation of visual cortex, emotional salience linked to amygdala etc. Do dreams have meaning? Activation-synthesis theory: Order within dreams is a function of one’s personal view of the world, current preoccupations, remote memories, feelings and beliefs (Hobson 1986) not Freudian disguise and censorship (similar to Artemidorus?) REM sleep as a ‘protoconscious’ state (Hobson 2009) Dreaming has features of ‘primary’ consciousness – simple awareness including perception and emotion But lacks ‘secondary’ consciousness – self-awareness, abstract thinking, volition A ‘protoconsciousness’ – primordial state of brain organisation that has the building blocks of consciousness Sleep Disorders Narcolepsy (disordered sleep-wake boundary) Peptide hypocretin (orexin) deficiency produces narcolepsy but mechanism unclear as low activity of hypocretin is normal during waking and NREM 1 in 2000 Repeated brief (2-30 mins) day time sleep attacks Sleep Disorders Narcoleptic symptoms (not all may be present) Sleep attack (intrusion of REM sleep into wakefulness?) Daytime sleepiness Microsleeps (continue automatic actions but asleep) Cataplexy (like REM atonia?) Sleep paralysis (like REM inhibition of voluntary movement?) inability to move when falling asleep or waking up Hypnagogic or hypnapompic hallucinations (like REM dreaming?) sudden loss of muscle tone that can lead to collapse triggered by sudden intense emotional stimuli (laughter, anger etc.) associated with sleep paralysis dreamlike experiences occurring just as falling asleep (-gogic) or waking (-pompic) (not unique to narcolepsy) Treated with stimulants modafinil (Provigil) dexamphetamine (Dexedrine) Sleep Disorders - parasomnias REM sleep behaviour disorder or REM without atonia person acts out dreams may be associated with impaired inhibition of motor neurons Sleep Disorders - parasomnias Following all commonest in children and associated with SWS Sleepwalking (somnambulism) Night terrors (pavor nocturnus) Not acting out of dreams Can be very complex actions Experiences of intense anxiety from which person may awaken screaming Bedwetting (nocturnal enuresis) may be cured with training Brain mechanisms and sleep Consider neural mechanisms in 1. Timing 2. Induction and Inhibition 1. Timing of sleep Endogenous circadian rhythms controlled by internal clocks entrained by external cues (‘Zeitgebers’) Free-running rhythms demonstrate intrinsic activity of clocks Isolated cave studies (e.g. caver Michel Siffre in 1970s ) 25 hr free running period in humans without external cues (Wever 1979) Learning unnecessary (Richter 1971) Endogenous clocks in birds & reptiles The pineal gland. Light sensitive, releases melatonin. Melatonin regulates circadian rhythms and seasonal reproductive behaviour In humans, no clear evidence that melatonin promotes sleep, but may affect circadian cycle (used in jet-lag to promote small phase shifts) Endogenous clocks in mammals The supra-chiasmatic nucleus (SCN) of the hypothalamus. 10 000 neurons with intrinsic circadian rhythmic firing. Connects to eye (retinohypothalamic pathway independent of vision) and pineal gland. Evidence that SCN is a clock Lesions disrupt rhythms of drinking, movement and adrenal steroid release (Stephan and Zucker 1972, Moore and Eichler 1972)) The SCN controls the length of the sleepwake cycle (Ralph and Menaker 1988, Ralph et al 1990) Transplanted foetal SCN tissue from hamsters with a 20 hour cycle (tau mutation) into SCN lesioned normal foetuses (and vice versa). Donor tissue determined length of cycle Multiple clocks Desynchronisation can occur between free running sleep and temperature rhythms (Harrington et al 1994) Separate clocks for circadian and ultradian rhythms Ultradian rhythms occur in animals with no free running circadian rhythm (Takahashi 1995) Destruction of SCN affects circadian rhythms but not ultradian (these are affected by lesions to other parts of hypothalamus) 2. Interacting neural mechanisms (i) The raphé system Thin strip of serotonin producing nuclei from medulla to midbrain. Originally suggested that system induces sleep Destruction in cats produces complete insomnia for 3 4 days with partial recovery afterwards (<2.5 hours sleep per day, all SWS) (Jouvet and Renault 1966) PCPA injections reduce sleep but recovery occurs even though serotonin levels stay low (Dement et al 1972) 2. Interacting neural mechanisms However: Serotonin injections don't promote sleep PCPA antagonism only disrupts sleep in cats Stimulation causes cortical arousal Activity of serotonin nuclei is highest during waking and falls through sleep stages (AstonJones and Bloom 1981) Now thought that serotonergic activity promotes alertness and suppresses REM sleep ‘REM-off’ cells (ii) Basal forebrain region and SWS In front of hypothalamus Lesions abolish SWS (Sterman and Clemente 1962) Stimulation produces drowsiness and EEG changes (Sterman and Clemente 1962) Suppresses histamine mediated alertness (iii) Locus Coeruleus Noradrenergic system in the pons Active in SWS and inhibits REM Active during waking when attention to unusual stimulus is required – role in vigilance? (Aston-Jones and Bloom 1981) REM only begins when activity of serotonergic (raphé system) and adrenergic systems (LC) reduces ‘REM-off’cells (iv) Caudal reticular formation REM circuits Interacting regions producing atonia, EEG desynchronisation, REM etc. Pons crucial Lesions abolish REM sleep (Friedman and Jones 1984) Injections of cholinergic agonists promote REM in humans, antagonists decrease it (Sitaram et al 1978) ‘REM-on’ cells (v) The ascending reticular activating system (ARAS) Moruzzi and Magoun 1949 System of neurons running from the medulla to the forebrain (near the raphé system) (‘rete’ means net) ‘Active’ vs. passive theory of sleep The cerveau isolé (‘isolated forebrain’) lesion produced prolonged SWS EEG. But only if reticular activating system isolated. The Encéphale isolé (‘isolated brain’) lesion doesn’t disrupt sleep EEG. So difference is in ARAS being isolated Chemical ‘switches’? Awake – all brainstem neurons involved, ACh, dopamine, histamine, noradrenaline, serotonin Asleep – balance changes REM-off cells are aminergic – serotonin (raphe nuclei) and noradrenaline (LC) REM-on cells are cholinergic – ACh Shift from mainly external input to internal input in dreaming The ‘off-line’ brain is activated by ACh and dopamine – the ‘psychosis’ of dreams So why sleep at all? Very strong motivation Almost universal - all mammals and birds sleep (Durie, 1981), reptiles sleep, insects sleep (Horne, 2006), other organisms ‘rest’, a few vertebrates never sleep (Kavenau 1998) Retained where it would seem maladaptive – one eyed ducks and dolphins Bottlenose dolphin (Mukhametov 1984) Blind Indus dolphin (Pilleri 1979) Sleep deprivation Affects cerebral not physical functioning (meta analysis by Horne 1978) Prolonged boring tasks become difficult (e.g. vigilance task, Gillberg et al 1996) Demanding tasks unaffected ( e.g abstract reasoning, Percival, Horne & Tilley 1983) Vision blurs, speech becomes incoherent, mild perceptual hallucinations, irritability, disorientation Most post deprivation recovery is of stage 4 and REM sleep (common finding). Horne (1998) suggests difference between ‘core’ and optional sleep. Adaptive or circadian theories A behaviour developed through evolution which keeps animal safe during inactive periods and conserves energy (Meddis 1977) REM sleep deprivation leads to loss of homeothermic control Size of an animal and danger of being attacked account for 58% of the differences in length of sleep between species (Allison and Chichetti 1976). But bigger animals lose less energy than smaller ones (e.g. humans vs. mouse) and more advanced animals can adopt ‘relaxed wakefulness’. Sleep adds little to energy conservation compared to resting. Relaxed wakefulness seems to require a more advanced brain (Horne, 2006). A shift of function? Restoration theories Sleep enables ‘repair’ (but of what?) Fail to account for between species differences in length of sleep Effect of exercise Sleep deprivation doesn’t affect physical performance e.g. Takeuchi et al (1985) Exercise increases SWS but only if brain temperature increases (Horne 1988) Suggests restoration is ‘cerebral’ Deprivation affects behaviour and cognitive ability Cerebral effects Horne – with increasing brain complexity and overall size, sleep becomes less important for energy conservation through immobilisation and more important for cerebral recovery. Increasing mental activity increases SWS (Horne and Minard, 1985) suggesting recovery Correspondence between task activity and subsequent activity of related brain areas in sleep (e.g. bird song Dave & Margoliash 2000) Ps reducing sleep time lose it from stages 1&2 not 3&4 (Mullaney et al 1977) – increasing sleep efficiency REM sleep Sleep deprived subjects recover mainly REM and stage 4 sleep. Dement (1960) - ‘Pressure’ for REM sleep increases during REM sleep deprivation. ‘Rebound’ into REM seen in recovery. But not essential Lavie et al, 1984, report on 33 yr old man injured who engaged in almost no REM sleep Bottlenose dolphins have no REM sleep Fur seals have no REM sleep while at sea but regain it on land (but without recovery of lost periods) REM sleep and learning Various theories involving memory restructuring REM sleep increases in animals and humans learning a task until ‘mastered’ (e.g. Hennevin et al 1995) REM deprivation impairs learning REM sleep and learning “Habitual reactions, which are closely linked with survival, are REM independent; but activities involving assimilation of unusual information require REM sleep for optimal consolidation” (Greenberg and Pearlman 1974 p.516) Perhaps simpler tasks don’t need REM sleep but complex ones do or new knowledge do (Pearlman, 1979; Stickgold, 2001) The developing brain – the ontogenetic hypothesis Infant humans have more REM sleep than precocial animal infants (Roffwarg, Muzio and Dement 1966). 50-70% of newborns sleep is REM, 15% in adult. (1 month premature – 67%, 2 months 80%) Perhaps active role in brain development is succeeded by later role in learning. REM provides necessary stimulation for development Integrating SWS and REM SWS deprivation affects explicit memories (consciously recollected) while REM deprivation affects implicit memories (performance improvement without conscious recall) (Plihal and Born 1999) Distinction may be too simplistic – some studies (such as the following) suggest optimal learning needs both types of sleep Integrating SWS and REM Complementary role in learning procedural task First nights sleep after implicit learning of visual discrimination task is required for improvement (Stickgold et al 2000) But optimal performance occurs after both SWS and REM are experienced (Gais et al 2000). Dual processes? SWS initiates and is required for consolidation, REM adds to this process but isn’t needed. Is sleep needed for learning? Effects are statistically significant but small Semantic memory not strongly enhanced by sleep Selective REM or NREM sleep does not always affect memory consolidation REM suppressants like anti-depressant drugs can enhance learning So useful but not essential for learning Integrating theories of sleep Adaptive theories better in some key areas Relationship between sleep time and vulnerability Effects of sleep deprivation minor But adaptive and restorative theories are complementary May also be a shifting of function with more complexity (in evolution) Waking and REM sleep - dreaming are distinct states but with relationships affecting optimal functioning of both Sleep in Humans From Hobson, J.A. (2005) ‘Sleep is of the brain, by the brain and for the brain’ Nature 437, 1254 - 1256