TOWARDS A SAFER AND MORE CONSISTENT APPROACH TO ALLEGATIONS OF CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE IN FAMILY LAW PROCEEDINGS – EXPERT PANELS AND GUIDELINES. BY: MATTHEW MYERS Disclaimer: The views expressed in this paper are those of the writer and do not in any way represent the views of the Federal Magistrates Court of Australia. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 1 INDEX 1 Overview 2 Low prevalence, but high degree of difficulty 3 Not all allegations are true 4 Children’s reluctance to disclose 5 Reservations about children’s evidence 6 Children’s capacity to remember and report events accurately 7 Children’s Capacity to Lie 8 Distinguishing fact from fantasy 9 Suggestibility 10 Comprehension of Questions. 11 Reliance on expert evidence 12 Family Reports 13 Expert witnesses 14 Interview techniques 15 Use of memory aids 16 The science of expert opinion 17 Recognition of a specialist area of skill 18 Guidelines 19 Training , credentials and implementation of panels 20 References Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 2 1 Overview While allegations of child sexual abuse in proceedings before the Family and Federal Magistrates Courts are infrequent they are highly problematic given the issue is very serious and such allegations are hard to prove or disprove. Often there is no physical evidence of sexual abuse and the only witnesses to the alleged abuse are the alleged perpetrator and the child. Courts must be careful when examining allegations of child sexual abuse as such abuse can cause lasting psychological damage while remaining mindful that often the party alleging abuse is a parent who may obtain a strategic advantage in the proceedings and if false allegations are believed. Where such allegations are believed the consequences for the accused parent are severe. Cases involving child sexual abuse before the Family Court tend to be more difficult to resolve than those in criminal proceedings because the children in family law cases are typically much younger. Young victims of sexual abuse are vulnerable in that their evidence can easily be misunderstood or contaminated through the use of inappropriate interview techniques and repeated questioning. As a means of dealing with the complex nature of sexual abuse allegations in family law cases the Federal Legislature, High Court, Family and Federal Magistrates Courts have developed tests of “unacceptable risk” in order to deal with those allegations and promote the best interest of the child, but children are not permitted to provide evidence directly as witnesses in Family or Federal Magistrate Court proceedings. Federal Magistrates and Judges are therefore heavily reliant on experts to interview both the child who is the subject of the allegation of abuse as well as the alleged perpetrator, in order to determine whether the perpetrator presents as an unacceptable risk of abuse to the child or whether a finding of sexual abuse should be made. This paper will address both the competence of child testimony and the competence of expert evidence and then suggest a strategy for ensuring that evidence in these matters is reliable and of a consistently high standard to protect the interests of all parties including children. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 3 2 Low prevalence, but high degree of difficulty Allegations of child sexual abuse are not common in Family Court matters (Thoennes and Tjaden, 1989; Brown, Fredrico, Hewitt and Sheehan, 1998; Bordow cited in Neoh and Mellor, 2009), but these cases tend to be the most difficult to resolve, more often go to trial and more often require involvement of specialist practitioners (Neoh and Mellor, 2009). Brown et al (1998) found in Canberra and Melbourne that only five per cent of cases over a 30 months period involved allegations of abuse (including physical and emotional abuse and neglect as well as sexual abuse), but these cases made up 50 per cent of the workload halfway through proceedings and 30 per cent at trial stage. Cases involving child sexual abuse before the Family Court tend to be more difficult to resolve than those in criminal proceedings, because the children in Family Court cases are typically much younger (Strand, 1994; Neoh and Mellor, 2009). While child sexual abuse most commonly begins between the ages of six and 10 years (Finkelhor, Baron, Browne, Doyle, Peters and Wyatt, 1986; Suwa, Tversky, Nicoll, Radin, Santos and Tuan, 1997), Family Court cases more often involve children aged between two and four years (Macfarlane, 1986). A study of allegations in children’s proceedings in Victoria and South Australia by the Australian Institute of Family Studies (Smyth, Moloney, Weston, Richardson, Qu and Gray, 2007) found that allegations of parental abuse of children made up 22-24% of the general litigants sample, but 11-46% of cases requiring judicial determination, illustrating that these cases are more often formally litigated, rather than dealt with at a precourt negotiation stage. Cases that progressed to a defended hearing were not only more likely than others to include allegations of child abuse, but also more likely to involve twosided allegations. While cases involving allegations of sexual abuse were few in the general litigants sample (3% of both Family Court and Federal Magistrates Court cases), they made up a larger proportion of judicially determined cases (11% of those in the Family Court). Most cases involving allegations of parental child abuse (63% of those in Family Court, 86% of Federal Magistrates Court cases) contained no supporting information. Criminal prosecution and conviction rates for child sexual abuse in Australia are believed to be very low (Fitzgerald, 2006). However, the low number of allegations made in criminal and family law contexts should not be read as indicators that child sexual abuse occurs infrequently in Australian society. This is more likely a consequence of the high standard of Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 4 proof that is required to obtain a criminal conviction (a Judge or Juror must weigh the evidence against the standard of being “beyond reasonable doubt”) and parents alleging child sexual abuse in Family or Federal Magistrates Court risk a mandatory cost order against the alleging party if the Court is satisfied that the allegation is false. Research suggests the low prosecution and conviction rates in the criminal justice system may also be due to jurors tending to perceive child witnesses to be unreliable (Sumner-Armstrong and Newcombe, 2007), particularly if there has been a delay in reporting the abuse (Lewis, 2006); due to parents choosing not to report these matters because contact with the court system can be highly traumatic for children, particularly the experience of cross-examination (Davies, Devere and Verbitsky, 2004; Powell, 2005; Coussins, 2006); due to children having difficulty understanding legal language and answering questions within the constraints imposed during cross-examination (Hoyano, 2007) and under the pressure exerted during cross-examination, which has been described as “an opportunity to confuse” (Coussins, 2009). While low in absolute terms, the number of child sexual abuse allegations made in Federal Magistrates and Family Court proceedings is relatively high in comparison with allegations made in non-family dispute matters. The Family and Federal Magistrates Court process is different from that of the Criminal Justice System in that children are not required to appear as witnesses in the court to give evidence and the testing of a disclosure of sexual abuse in order to determine whether a person presents an unacceptable risk of abuse to a child is usually left to a single expert appointed by the court. The evidence of young children in cases involving allegations of abuse is both controversial and problematic in terms of its reliability. In order that the best interest of a child be promoted and protected, the issues involved in obtaining accurate testimonies from young children about sexual abuse must be carefully examined to establish a means of reliable fact finding. Because physical evidence of molestation is rarely present, disclosure by the alleged child victims in these cases is often the only substantial evidence against an accused parent. Quite often, Family Court Judges and Federal Magistrates are required to choose between two competing and largely uncorroborated accounts of events. In the absence of definitive material evidence, the plausibility of the respective accounts is heavily dependent on the perceived reliability of a disclosing child. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 5 3 Not all allegations are true There has long been concern that, in the context of bitter divorces and child-custody disputes, children may be manipulated into making false accusations of sexual abuse (Wilson, 1986; Jones and McGraw, 1987; Byrne, 1991, 1992). Rayner (1989) concluded that there is a real risk of wrongful conviction, though few documented occasions where children have told convincing stories which were not true. In the late 1980s a movement developed that questioned the credibility of child abuse evidence, attributing many disclosures by children to “brainwashing” (Crewdson, 1988; Hechler, 1988). Given the serious nature of the child abuse allegation and the consequences to a child’s relationship with a parent when a finding of unacceptable risk is upheld by the Court, it is crucial that the Court be able to distinguish between cases where false disclosure is instigated by one parent as a means of obtaining a strategic advantage over the other and cases where there is a genuine unacceptable risk of abuse to the child. Those investigating such allegations must take care to not contaminate evidence so it can be deemed reliable in Court, while at the same time ensuring that accusations that may be false are not accepted without question. Complaints of this nature addressed in the criminal justice system have sometimes been shown to be unfounded (Wilson, 1986; Scott and Dickens, 1989) and the Australian Sea Beach Kindergarten case (better known as “Mr Bubbles”) serves as a warning about some common problems in the investigation of child abuse allegations and the giving of evidence by children. The true facts of this matter remain unknown, because both the forensic tests applied by medical professionals and the interviewing techniques used to collect evidence from children were so flawed that the evidence emerging was worthless. While medical examinations did not provide any credible evidence of sexual abuse, they did show that the children's stories of stabbing and cutting had no basis in fact because no physical evidence of injury was found. This case also exemplifies failure of the system to protect and balance the rights of the alleged victim with those of the accused (Hatty, 1991). While it remains unclear whether there was any truth in the sexual abuse allegations, it is certain that these children grew up with appalling “memories” of sadistic sexual abuse, whether based on real experiences or implanted ideas. If they were sexually abused, then the system failed them. If they were not abused, then the system’s processes may have damaged them emotionally. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 6 Several US studies have addressed the probability of allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Court contexts being false. Thoennes and Tjaden (1989) found that of 105 divorce cases involving allegations of child sexual abuse, half were confirmed, one third were found to be not true and the rest remained unclear. A smaller study of 20 Family Court cases (Jones and Seig, 1988) found that most allegations of child sexual abuse were found to be reliable (14 of 20 or 70%), a minority (4 or 20%) were found to be untrue and two (10%) remained unclear. However, most of the unsubstantiated allegations were perceived to have been made in good faith, rather than through malice. Faller (1991) examined 136 cases and found that most allegations were true, but between 15 and 25 per cent were untrue. In Australia, Brown et al (2003) re-examined the 100 cases of the Magellan Project (a quarter of them substantiated by a Child Protection Agency report) and found that allegations of abuse had been found to be false in only 11 cases. Mothers had made allegations twice as often as fathers (48% vs 21%). While two thirds of allegations by mothers (32 cases of 48) were substantiated, only one third (7 cases of 21) of allegations by fathers were substantiated. In only one case was the allegation made by the child and this case was substantiated. In most of the substantiated cases (32 cases or 63%), the father was the perpetrator. Most of the other substantiated cases (16 or 31%) were other family members, but these did not include any mothers. The 11 cases where allegations were found to be false were fairly evenly divided between allegations by mothers (5) and allegations by fathers (6), but it was also noted that in most of these cases, the alleging parent was receiving treatment for serious mental illness and/or had a history of being sexual abused as children themselves. After reviewing the literature, Neoh and Mellor (2009) concluded that most allegations of child sexual abuse made in Family Court cases are true, but that a small proportion do appear to be false and, since the stakes for all concerned are exceedingly high, assessment of families in these circumstances must be conducted with great care. As Neoh and Mellor noted, concern in the Family Court is with the specific case, not with the majority of other cases. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 7 4 Children’s reluctance to disclose In cases before the Family Court, allegations of child sexual abuse are very rarely initiated by the children themselves. There is general agreement that children have more difficulty reporting abuse within the family than abuse from outside (DiPietro, Runyan and Fredrickson, 1997; Sjöberg and Lindblad, 2002b; Goodman-Brown, Edelstein, Goodman, Jones and Gordon, 2003). Children may feel loyalty to the perpetrator and fear family reactions to the disclosure or possible consequences for family members. Yet even when the perpetrator is a stranger, children are unlikely to report the event spontaneously and they may be reluctant to disclose details of the sexual abuse when questioned about it. Occasionally researchers have been able to address children’s reporting in cases where the abuse had already been established as fact through independent photographic or video evidence, medical evidence and/or confession by the perpetrator (Bidrose and Goodman, 2000; Leander, Granhag and Christianson, 2005; Orbach and Lamb, 1999; Sjöberg and Lindblad, 2002a; Svedin and Back, 2003; Leander, Christianson and Granhag, 2007). These studies have generally found children’s accounts to be incomplete and fragmentary, tending to omit sexual detail and sometimes denying it directly, but that what children did tell about sexual abuse was generally accurate. The chance discovery by Swedish police of photographic evidence of child sexual abuse created the opportunity for a recent study (Leander, Christianson and Granhag, 2007), particularly when the perpetrator confessed to sexually abusing on separate occasions 17 children and provided detailed information about each of these offences. Only six of these incidents had previously come to police attention and a further two children were interviewed a few years after the relevant events. The remaining 9 children either remained unidentified or their parents refused permission for the children to be questioned by police. While almost all of the eight children who were interviewed provided details of events leading up to and following the sexual acts, only two gave detailed reports of the sexual acts themselves and one other gave a few sexual details. As well as omitting all sexual details from their accounts, most children showed reluctance to discuss the sexual acts, even in response to leading questions. There was no simple relationship evident between age and reporting of the sexual assaults. The youngest (aged 3) had difficulty providing both sexual and non-sexual details, but the next (aged almost 4) was Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 8 able to provide substantial non-sexual detail and some sexual detail, while three children who were aged 5 at the time of the offences, but were older when interviewed (8-10 years), gave extensive details of events before and after the sexual assault, but no sexual detail at all. The two who provided the most sexual detail were aged 6 and 9 respectively, both at the time of assault and at interview, and they also provided extensive information about events before and after. The eldest at the time of interview (aged 10) provided a lot of non-sexual detail, but no sexual detail. Some research has found that younger children have more difficulty reporting sexual abuse (Smith et al, 2003; Sjöberg and Lindblad, 2002b), while other studies have found the reverse (Goodman-Brown et al, 2003). This apparent inconsistency may arise because the constraints on reporting sexual abuse change as children mature. In younger children, omission of sexual details has been attributed to factors such as limited memory capacity (Howe and Courage, 1993; Kail, 1988; Schneider and Pressley, 1989), limited language capacity (Fivush, 1993; Howe and Courage, 1993; Ornstein, Larus and Clubb, 1991) and limited knowledge about the acts (Bussey and Grimbeck, 1995). Older children know more and have better verbal skills, but are also more aware of the taboo nature of the sexual acts and so may omit sexual details because they feel shame or guilt (Goodman-Brown et al, 2003). Fear of negative consequences of disclosure may also contribute, as well as fear of being blamed or not believed (Sas and Cunningham, 1995; Sauzier, 1989). In the Family Court, most allegations of child sexual abuse are made by mothers, most involve female children and the father is far more often the accused than the mother, though other relatives and new partners of mothers make up a substantial minority (Brown et al, 2003; Bordow, cited in Neoh and Mellor, 2009). This is in keeping with other research findings that perpetrators of sexual abuse within the family are usually male and victims are usually female. Solomon (1992) reviewed 3,864 substantiated cases of sexual abuse within the family to find that 87 per cent of perpetrators were male and 85 per cent of victims were female and Smallbone and Wortly (2001) interviewed 169 convicted sex offenders to find that incest victims were 10 times more likely to be girls than boys. Given that, in Family Court cases, young children are rarely the initiators of allegations of child sexual abuse and most are at an age where limited cognitive and verbal skills restrict their accounts of the relevant events, most allegations are based upon interpretations of their behaviour by mothers and others. The problems associated with collecting reliable evidence Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 9 from very young children, together with the preponderance of mothers making allegations, has led to a lengthy debate about the proportion of these allegations that are either misguided or maliciously false. A “parental alienation syndrome”, whereby false allegations of sexual abuse are maliciously made by mothers to undermine the father-child relationship and deny access (Gardner, 1999, 2001; Turkat, 1994; Freckleton and Selby, 2008), has retained some currency in the Family Court (Berns, 2001; Achimovich, 2003), despite being widely criticised in academic literature (Bruch, 2001; Faller, 1991; O’Leary and Moerk, 1999). Historically, due to the difficulties involved in eliciting reliable verbal evidence from children, there has been heavy reliance upon behavioural evidence to support allegations of child sexual abuse and a few writers in the US child custody evaluation field persist in recommending that pre-adolescent children not be interviewed at all. “Typically, the evaluator does not interview the pre-adolecent child... (This) remains the best practice approach in light of the validity and reliability concerns adequately documented in the literature.” (Benjamin and Gollan, 2003, p86). However, this is no longer the predominant view because interpretation of behavioural indicators is highly problematic, because there is no clear-cut syndrome or definitive set of behavioural symptoms that can identify that a child has been sexually abused (Gratz and Orsillo, 2003; Hagen, 2003; Friedrich, 2005; Faust, 2009; Kuehnle and Connell, 2009; Gould and Martindale, 2009). “One of the most important findings from the past several decades of research in this area is that no single sign or symptom, including aberrant sexualized behaviour, characterizes the majority of sexually abused children.” Kuehnle and Connell, (2009, p378) While the short term and long term effects of child sexual abuse are well documented (Jumper, 1995; Neuman, Houskamp, Pollock and Briere, 1996; Fleming, Mullen, Sibthorpe and Bammer, 1999; Paolucci, Genuis and Violato, 2001), there is no established set of behavioural indicators that can be used as a diagnostic tool for identifying cases of child sexual abuse (Kornberg, 1992; Neoh and Mellor, 2009). Children traumatised by sexual abuse can exhibit a wide range of behaviours (Finkelhor et al 1986; Everstein and Evertstein, 1989; Udwin, 1993; Bernet et al, 1997) or there may be no noticeable change in their outward behaviour. There are many psychological variables, such as aberrant sexual behaviour, postMatthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 10 traumatic stress disorder, eating disorders and loss of bladder or bowel control that have demonstrated validity as indicators of child sexual abuse (that is there is an association), but that are not differentiating (that is they may occur in children who have not been sexually abused for other reasons) (Faust, Bridges and Ahearn, 2009; Kuehnle and Connell, 2009; Gould and Martindale, 2009). The behavioural symptoms exhibited by sexually abused children vary significantly and are believed to be influenced by the child’s personality and interpretation of the abuse event/s, the identity of the perpetrator, characteristics of the sexual acts, concurrent family problems such as violence or instability and the other care-giver/s’ responses. Some sexually abused children show no behavioural symptoms. Sexually aberrant behaviour, considered to be the most explicit and treatment-resistant symptom of sexual abuse (Friedrich, 1993; 2005), does occur in some children with no known history of sexual abuse. However, most of these children have experienced other forms of abuse such as family violence and/or exposure to adults engaged in sexual activity or to pornographic materials (Bonner, Walker and Berliner, 1999; Pithers, Gray Busconi and Houchens, 1998; Friedrich, Grambsch, Damon, et al 1992). There are many sources of stress for children involved in acrimonious separations which may produce changes in behaviour. A plausible explanation for misguided allegations of sexual abuse is that the trauma of parental separation, involving new living arrangements and often new relationships for the children, as well as increased conflict between the parents, may itself produce a range of symptoms including sexualised, regressive or aggressive behaviours that may also be associated with sexual abuse (Thoennes and Pearson, 1988; Ehrenberg and Elterman, 1995; Mapes, 1995). Kuehnle and Connell (2009) in their recent overview also urged caution in relying on the recollections and interpretations of parents and other family members and carers, explaining in some detail the many potential sources of error in these accounts. Despite the existing safeguards within the Family Law Act, innocent people can appear to present an unacceptable risk, while the guilty may be allowed to spend time with young children with harmful consequences. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 11 Reservations about children’s evidence 5 Judges and Magistrates have acknowledged misgivings regarding the capacity of children to give accurate evidence at trial; not because they believe children are more prone to lying than adults, but because children may make ‘unconscious errors’, influenced by fantasy or by other people (Cashmore and Bussey, 1996). Issues of concern in relation to the testimony of children in abuse cases have included: the accuracy of children’s memories the ability of children to distinguish between truth and lies, fact and fantasy the suggestibility of children’s minds the capacity of children to comprehend what is being asked the capacity of the questioners to ask appropriate questions in an appropriate way and to correctly interpret children’s responses. 6 Children’s capacity to remember and report events accurately It has been alleged that the limits of children's attention and memory capacity make their testimony, not merely quantitatively, but qualitatively inferior to that of adults (Heydon, 1984). However, recent research suggests that children as young as three can provide reliable and useful testimony (Bussey and Grimbeek, 1993; Leander, Christianson and Granhag, 2007; Kuehnle and Cowan, 2009). Davis (1991) found that the first spontaneous accounts of staged events by children were as accurate as those by adults, though the quantity of information recalled was generally less. Errors in children’s memories are often errors of omission rather than commission. While children often recall less than adults do, what they do recall in response to open-ended questions like “What happened?” is not less accurate (Goodman and Hegelson, 1985; Taylor, Geddis and Henaghan, 1990). The quantity of detail freely recalled appears to increase with age, probably due to development of memory storage and retrieval skills: limits on young children’s attention may reduce the amount of information encoded and limits on retrieval strategies related to verbal development may reduce the information that is retrievable (Turtle and Wells, 1987; Raskin and Yuille, 1989). When asked to recall as much as they can about an event without prompting, younger children omit more details than adults do, but children Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 12 as young as three years have been found to be capable of supplying details about a complex event (Bussey, 1992) and some studies found no significant variation in accuracy related to age (King and Yuille, 1987). Because children remember less than adults in unprompted recall, retrieval of their evidence is usually more reliant upon verbal and nonverbal probing, but this provides opportunities for intentional or unintentional suggestions to influence their memory. While it is generally agreed that by the age of ten or eleven, children are no more vulnerable to misleading or incorrect information than adults (Cole and Loftus, 1987), younger children, particularly those under seven years, may be particularly vulnerable to misinformation about peripheral facts, such as a person’s clothing, though more reliable about central events. Goodman, Bottoms and Aman (1990) found that children's accounts of the central features of staged events remained reasonably accurate even up to a year later. Wakefield and Underwager (1988) suggested that memory is never absolute, but an interpretation of reality and that children may implant or create memories from information given to them, but that this stems from problems in interviewing techniques, rather than the limited ability of children to recall events from memory without error. These research findings support the current criminal justice system position that child sexual assault witnesses should not be considered any less reliable than adult witnesses, but also underline the importance of using methods to extract information from the memories of children that minimise the risk of contamination. 7 Children’s Capacity to Lie While historically the “children never lie” proposition has had a strong following, there is abundant evidence that they do not always tell the truth, but the age at which children become capable of intentionally misleading others and the cognitive abilities underlying this capacity have long been debated (Asington, Harris and Olson, 1988). An Australian study of deceptive behaviour in young children using mothers as observers, found that from three or four until seven years of age, most children use strategies of denying, pretending and falsely claiming credit, in order to avoid negative consequences or gain reward (Hoogenraad and McKenzie, 1995). These are consistent with earlier findings by Ekman (1985) and by Perner and Wimmer (1985). Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 13 Children’s propensity to lie is generally thought to be related to their age and cognitive development. Cashmore and Horsky (1987) and later Bussey (1992) suggested that older child witnesses in sexual assault cases may be seen as less credible by juries, more likely to lay false complaints or possibly as more blameworthy than younger complainants. They argued that lying and truthfulness become increasingly subject to self-regulatory control. As children are exposed to more lies from adults and peers, they learn to generate plausible lies themselves; they become increasingly skilled at lying and their statements become more credible with the result that observers are less likely to detect deception (Bussey, 1992). While it can be accepted that children do not have a greater propensity than adults to lie, the extent to which children tell the truth about a particular event may depend on what the child perceives will happen as a consequence of the information they offer. A study by Lewis, Stanger and Sullivan (1989) suggested that children can deliberately lie at age three in relation to their own misdeeds. Bussey (1990) found that five-year olds were more likely to lie than three-year olds. Hoogenraad and McKenzie (1995) found that children in the age range of three to seven years will engage in deception for the purpose of self-interest. They note that children up to the age of seven employ strategies of denying, pretending and falsely claiming credit in order to avoid negative consequences or to gain reward. Ceci (1990) and later Bussey (1993) found that three-and-a-half to four-year old preschool children may lie to protect someone they like from punishment. In keeping with Ekman (1985) noted earlier, Ceci identified motives of personal aggrandisement, protecting a loved one, avoiding embarrassment, sustaining a game and conforming to a stereotype underpinning the lies of very young children. While it is clear that deception is a feature of children’s interactions, the central question remains untested: would young children make false allegations of sexual abuse? Some writers believe that children rarely lie about sexual abuse of their own volition, but they may be pressured to lie to avoid negative consequences. Wakefield and Underwager (1989) suggested that children do not intentionally lie, but they can be influenced by adults to make statements about events that have not happened. Wakefield and Underwager (1989) argued that, through a process of adult social influence, the adult interviewer may create stories of abuse that become incorporated into the child’s memory. Taylor (1990) commented that a Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 14 child could be forced to lie when a parent makes an allegation on behalf of the child for their own reasons and Bussy (1992) argued that contamination of a child’s story is possible in divorce proceedings, when a mother might have had the time and motivation to coach the child to give misleading evidence. Bussey (1992) found that "demonstrable" lying by children in sexual abuse cases is very rare, and almost certainly lower than for adults testifying from the witness box. After a review of various findings, Spencer and Flin (1990) estimated that about two per cent of allegations by children of six years or less were fictitious, rising to 10 per cent for those over ten years. Bussey (1993) also suggested that children may falsely deny or later retract information related to an assault previously disclosed, due to fear of punishment, anticipated rewards for lying, embarrassment or not wanting the perpetrator to get into trouble. False denial may be as common as false allegation. These studies suggest that children lie for many of the same reasons that adults lie. In this sense an alleged child victim is no more but also no less a reliable witness than any other with respect to events of personal significance. In response to beliefs that some children make up stories out of malice or to get attention, methods for identifying false reports have been developed. Statement Validity Assessment (SVA), a diagnostic technique developed in Germany, is sometimes used to assess the validity of a child's allegation (Steller, 1989; Steller and Koehnken, 1989). SVA includes a process known as criteria-based content analysis, which allows a statement to be systematically assessed by means of defined criteria. This analysis assumes that descriptions of events that actually happened differ in content, quality and expression from those that are invented. The absence of any particular criterion does not necessarily mean that the statement is false, but the presence of most would indicate that the statement is true. In recent decades children’s evidence has been deemed inadmissible in several high profile criminal proceedings partly because it could not be proven that they understood the difference between truth and lies (Hatty, 1991; Wittaker, 1991). However, several researchers have noted that there is no evidence that the present competency test is a valid measure of the likelihood that the child will tell the truth (Taylor, Geddis and Henaghan, 1990; Bussey, 1992). While research has shown that children as young as four years can Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 15 differentiate a lie from a truthful statement (Haugaard and Crosby, 1989), it does not necessarily follow that they will tell the truth. Bussey (1992) concluded that, where truthfulness is no more favourably rewarded than lies, pre-schoolers are less likely to be truthful. Under the present test of competency, children can show they are capable of telling the truth, but then choose to not do so. A caution that children may not have an adult understanding of the truth or may not adhere to it if they do would be more useful than a test of competency that may mislead. 8 Distinguishing fact from fantasy Children have highly imaginative minds: they may have imaginary friends, imbue dolls with personalities or take on the roles of superheroes in play and over many years there has been debate about whether children can actually distinguish fact from fantasy. In the 1692 Salem Massachusetts witch trials, the testimony of girls aged between five and twelve was assumed to be so reliable that many people were hanged as witches on the basis of their evidence (Dale, 1993). Although later investigation concluded that some of the witchcraft allegations were actually due to erogitism-poisoning by a fungus contaminating rye causing hallucinations that were interpreted as bewitchment, rather than children fantasising (Goodman, 1984), in reaction to this, children were long held to be unreliable as witnesses, incapable of distinguishing their fantasies from actual events. However, there is now realisation that children have been undervalued as witnesses (Davis and Stevenson, 1986), particularly in relation to allegations of sexual abuse (Dale, 1993). Laboratory studies have found that children can perform as well as adults in separating actual memories from other perceptual sources such as imagined images (Johnson and Foley, 1994). Adults have sometimes mistaken a child’s genuine allegation of sexual abuse for a product of imagination, possibly due to an adult’s unwillingness to face the reality that their child has been sexually abused (Taylor, 1990). Other researchers have concluded that, when a child describes a sexual experience in vivid detail, it is unlikely that the child has created a fantasy. “When a five year old provides a clear-cut verbal description, dramatic enactment, or pictorial representation of an erect discharging penis, or of vaginal or anal penetration, it is unlikely that such a memory has been invented.” (Nucrome, 1986, p 474) Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 16 Similarly, Greene (1990) noted that while children may have numerous fantasies about sex, those fantasies tend to be vague and not concrete in their descriptions. Greene concluded that when children play activities they consider to be sex, their concept of sex is revealed to be quite different from the types of things that adult child abusers actually do. 9 Suggestibility Consistent with Wakefield and Underwager (1988) and Loftus and Ketcham (1991), Kuehnle and Cowan (2009) asserted that, at all ages, memory of experience is reconstructed, with autobiographical recall filtered through current beliefs, knowledge, expectations and motivations. Memories are always subject to influences, the extent of influence depending on the suggestibility of the individual. Children’s vulnerability to contamination of their episodic memory (memory for specific events, as opposed to accumulated knowledge) is increased with repetitive and delayed interviews containing requests for speculation or misinformation and very young children are the most vulnerable to suggestive interviewing. Nevertheless, some children have been found to be remarkably resistant to false suggestions (Harris, Goodman, Augusti, Chae and Alley, 2009). Taylor (1990) reported that, while it is generally held that children are influenced by questions of a leading nature such as “He took your pants off, didn’t he?”, recent research has demonstrated that children as young as four years of age are far more resistant to such suggestive questioning than formerly believed. Taylor concluded that, while some children will provide the answers that adults seem to want in relation to peripheral events that could have occurred but did not, when the question refers to central events and actions that lie within the child’s understanding, then suggestive questioning is likely to be rejected. Vulnerability/resistance to suggestion has been found to be related to many specific personality variables, including self-confidence (Vrij and Bush, 2000), creativity (Shapiro and Purdy, 2005), intelligence (Chae and Ceci, 2005; Eisen, Goodman, Qin, Davis and Crayton, 2007), cultural beliefs and expectations (Bottoms, Shaver and Goodman, 1996; Goodman, Quas, Batterman-Faunce, Riddlesberger and Kuhn, 1997). However, the predictive power of these variables is too weak to be useful for Court purposes and interview factors are more likely to be determinative. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 17 Research has shown that both adults and children are susceptible to altering their accounts of an event in response to cues from the questioner. Some researchers have concluded that suggestive material can overwrite the previous memory, creating a false new memory (Loftus and Palmer, 1974). Zaragoza (1987) found that people tend to incorporate information about an event from different sources into memory and, to the extent that new information is inaccurate, the testimony will also become inaccurate. However, rather than actually impairing memory, misleading information may be incorporated into testimony because the witness is capitulating to demand or other pressures. There are several possible explanations for children's tendency to accept misleading information. In keeping with Melton (1981), both Lees-Haley (1988) and Misener (1991) found that smiles, warmth and positive remarks in response to some of a child's answers and frowns, coldness and negative reactions to others, can shape the child's account. Misener (1991) suggested there are a number of explanations for children’s tendency to accept misleading information: children may wish to please the interviewer the adult interviewer’s non-verbal cues might suggest the type of response which will be rewarded or punished, particularly if the interviewer is an authority figure children may believe that the adult interviewer’s suggestions as to what occurred are more reliable than their own recollections. Kuehnle and Cowan (2009) noted that children are very familiar with being tested on their knowledge by adults who know all the answers. They are much less familiar with adults questioning them to collect information unknown to the adult. Ceci, Ross and Toglia (1987) found that children may believe that the adult interviewer's suggestions as to what occurred are more reliable than their own recollections. They demonstrated that young children's susceptibility was due, at least in part, to the fact that they are likely to conform to whatever they believe to be the expectation of the adult. Zaragoza (1987) similarly found that children may wish to please the interviewer and King and Yuille (1987) reported that children volunteered that they had "gone along" with misleading suggestions. The adult interviewer's cues might suggest the type of responses that would be rewarded or punished, which would be particularly influential if the interviewer is an authority figure (Melton, 1981). Green and Schetky (1988) suggested that this problem may Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 18 be particularly likely to occur in family dispute cases, where adult influence might lead a child to make statements about sexual abuse which may not be true. In family dispute cases where an adult parent has complained of abuse, a child may incorporate into their answers during interviews misleading information that has been put to them in a leading question about something that the child observed, even though that information conflicts with what the child has remembered in free recall. Wakefield and Underwager (1994 ) commented that, while studies have shown that both adults and children are influenced by leading questions, their own study found children to be more suggestible than adults and younger children to be more suggestible than older children. In keeping with this conclusion, Dale (1993) found that, although both adults and children are suggestible during questioning, children are more susceptible to suggestion under certain circumstances: when they are younger when they are questioned after long delay when an intimidating person interviews them when the questioner makes strong suggestions and repeats them often when more than one interviewer makes the suggestion. Wakefield and Underwager (1994) noted that the suggestions made in a typical suggestibility experiment are much weaker than those that are made in interviews related to real cases of alleged sexual abuse. In experiments, children are presented with misleading information once and asked two or three misleading questions; while in real life interviews, young children often face many leading questions and significant pressure to confirm the allegation of abuse. The possibility that a child might come to believe that they have been sexually abused when they have not is of serious concern, because in that case the interrogation has distorted the child’s inner reality. Gardner (1991) suggested that children who come to believe a suggestion that they have been sexually abused may develop psychosis as a consequence. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 19 10 Comprehension of Questions. Children’s ability to fully understand questions is fundamentally important in assessing the credibility of disclosures of sexual abuse. In some situations, children seem able to comprehend questions and give answers, while in other situations children find it difficult to understand and answer questions. Taylor (1990) commented that the system of cross examination that children are exposed to during a criminal trial is problematic, because young children do not always respond well to direct questioning and cannot readily produce accurate answers to questions out of context. Taylor further commented that questions that require abstract inferences about other people’s motivation cause difficulty for young children. Sometimes the answers children give to questions suggest that either the child did not understand the question or the questioner did not fully understand the child’s answer. In the latter case, this can result in perception that the child’s responses are self-contradictory when in fact the child is just being literal. Berliner and Barbieri (1984) demonstrated the problem of adult-child misunderstanding with the case of a five-year-old girl being cross-examined by her father’s lawyer: “Lawyer: And then you said you put your mouth on his penis. Child: No Lawyer: You didn’t say that? Child: No Lawyer: Did you ever put your mouth on his penis? Child: No Lawyer: Well, why did you tell your mother that your dad put his penis in your mouth? Child: My brother told me to.” (Berliner and Barbieri, 1984, p 125) Such testimony might lead a jury to believe that the child had rescinded her earlier story, a fabrication initiated by her brother. However, counsel for the prosecution recognised the misunderstanding and clarified the child’s responses. “Lawyer: [Name], you said that you didn’t put your mouth on daddy’s penis. Is that right? Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 20 Child: Yes. Lawyer: Did daddy put his penis in your mouth? Child: Yes. Lawyer: Did you tell your mom? Child: Yes. Lawyer: What made you decide to tell? Child: My brother and I talked about it, and he said I better tell or dad would just keep doing it” (Berliner and Barbieri, 1984, p 125) Taylor (1990) argued that limitations on children’s understanding of questions should not be used as an excuse to lessen their involvement in the justice system. Rather, the onus should be on lawyers and Judges to use methods that allow for these limitations so that complete and accurate information can be obtained from the child. In an extensive discussion of this issue, Gould and Martindale (2009) provided many examples of the ways and reasons why children may misunderstand questions from adults and many examples of the ways and reasons why adults may misunderstand children’s answers, although the failure to understand may not be apparent. These include: Very young children use language in highly idiosyncratic ways and may not understand the usual meaning of words they appear to be using competently, so the intended meaning of a sentence may be quite different from the received meaning and questions may be answered without being understood. While it is obvious that sophisticated legal language may not be understood, very young children may not understand the meaning of simpler words like “any”, “big”, “promise”, “move”, “touch” or “at the bottom of”; or understand the differences between “yesterday/today/tomorrow”, “under/over”, “above/below”, “in front of /behind”, “before/after”, “always/never”, “let/make”, “ask/tell”, “know/think”, “guess/be sure”, “neither/either/another/each”, “more/less”, “different/same”, “forget/remember”, “first/last”, “inside/outside”, “some/all”, “come/go”, “bring/take”, “here/there”. Very young children often confuse pronouns, so they may say “my house” when they mean “your house” or “his” when they mean “her”. Children may only be able to attend to one part of a compound question and will therefore only answer that part, though their answer may appear to refer to all parts. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 21 Children may not realise it is acceptable to say they do not understand a question and will therefore attempt an answer anyway. Sentences that contain negatives (“Did you not see...” and particularly double negatives “Is it not true that you did not see...” are often not understood, so the question answered may not be the question asked. Passive voice may not be understood. Tag questions (“It’s sunny, isn’t it?”) are highly suggestive to children. Children may not understand that they can give other answers to forced choice questions when none of the options offered are true. Children may have a very concrete understanding of words based on their own experience, so they may deny “visiting a doctor”, because that is not their understanding of what “visiting” means. 11 Reliance on expert evidence Children are not permitted to provide evidence directly as witnesses in Family or Federal Magistrate Court proceedings, so Federal Magistrates and Judges are heavily reliant upon experts to interview both the child who is the subject of the allegation of abuse as well as the alleged perpetrator, in order to determine whether the alleged perpetrator presents an unacceptable risk of abuse to the child or whether a finding of sexual abuse should be made. In order to avoid parties engaging advocate experts, employed to pursue a particular outcome, to avoid lengthy trials and to prevent any alleged child victim being interviewed by multiple experts, the Family and Federal Magistrates Court have turned to the use of single experts to provide evidence in proceedings where there are allegations of child sexual abuse. Because victims of sexual abuse are often very young children, experts must have particular skills and knowledge in order to provide credible, accurate, and reliable opinion based on the uncontaminated evidence of children and the expert’s clinical assessment of the alleged perpetrator. Given the problems associated with disclosures of sexual abuse of young children, there is a need for experts in this area to have a high level of specialist professional training within forensic psychology. Internationally, there has been a paradigm shift within the field of child custody evaluations, from a clinically based model that emphasises clinical Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 22 judgement, to a forensic model that emphasises the gathering of reliable data using tested and approved methods and procedures. Forensic psychology skills are significantly different from therapeutic skills or social work skills. Currently, no formal Australian guidelines exist to define how experts should conduct such interviews, the types of tests and questions that should be or should not be used or the age limits within which they are meaningful and those delivering "expert evidence" in Australian Family and Federal Magistrates Courts rarely have the training, knowledge and skills needed to do this type of work adequately. Currently experts are often sourced by solicitors for the parties or by independent lawyers for the children. They may hold various qualifications and prepare their reports using the skills and practices of their own professional training, rather than a set of forensic psychology guidelines for interviewing children of various ages. This article, Part 2, follows up the exploration of problems associated with children providing disclosures about sexual abuse with an exploration of common problems associated with experts providing opinion about whether there is an unacceptable risk of abuse related to that disclosure, including the advantages and disadvantages of single experts, multiple experts and the practice of hearing evidence from more than one expert concurrently and the general failure of these methods to deliver expert evidence of uniformly high quality. This paper concludes that, given the crucial role of expert opinion evidence concerning unacceptable risk in child sexual abuse cases and the clear need for highly specialised skills to do this work well: There is need for a well-founded and clearly defined set of best practice guidelines for interviewing children and giving expert evidence in the Family Court of Australia or the Federal Magistrates Court, including a requirement for audio-visual recording of all interviews. All experts providing evidence in cases involving allegations of child sexual abuse must be appropriately qualified in forensic psychology. Training towards an appropriate qualification to provide evidence in child sexual abuse cases, probably as a postgraduate degree (Masters in Forensic Psychology), must be available in one or more Australian universities, with the requisite competencies approved by the office of the Federal Attorney General. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 23 All consultants employed by the Child Dispute Service to prepare Family Reports should be required to undertake study towards this qualification and comply with the guidelines. In cases before the Federal Magistrates or Family Court involving allegations of child sexual abuse, single experts should be independently appointed from a panel of appropriately qualified experts, with accreditation requirements administered by the office of the Federal Attorney General. 12 Family Reports Brown Fredrico, Hewitt and Sheehan (1998) found that cases proceeded much more quickly when child abuse allegations had been investigated and substantiated in a report by a child protection agency and a Family Report was available, which reduced the stress of proceedings on the children involved. In response to these findings, pilot project Magellan was established, in which specific procedures intended to reduce stress on children were introduced to 100 cases before the Family Court that involved allegations of serious child abuse, including child sexual abuse. Procedures included assessment through a Family Report and appointment by the Court of an Independent Children’s Lawyer. Evaluation showed success in reducing trauma to children by reducing delays and avoiding duplication of bureaucratic procedures (Brown et al, 2001). The Australian Family Court now provides a counselling and mediation service, known as the Family Court Child Dispute Service (CDS), for clients of the Family Court or Federal Magistrates Service. While the CDS may be involved at many stages of proceedings, one of the main services provided is preparation of Family Reports. A Family Report is a “professional appraisal of the family from a non-legal and non-partisan perspective... contributing to informed and child-centred judicial decisions” (Family Court of Australia, 2000). Family Reports are now routinely ordered when there is family conflict and concern that the behaviour of a family member may pose a threat to the welfare of the child, because children are rarely interviewed directly by the Court (Family Court of Australia, 2000; Neoh and Mellor, 2009). The CDS consultants who prepare Family Reports are mainly social workers or psychologists by training and the Reports are based on structured and unstructured Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 24 interviews with family members, as well as observation of interactions between them, conducted over one or two days. Family Reports cannot comment on facts, such as whether or not the alleged abuse occurred, but are required to be balanced representations of all sides and focused on the best interests of the child (Melton, Petrila, Poythress and Slobogin, 1997). They may provide information about family dynamics, which may suggest whether or not sexual abuse is likely to have occurred (Dessau, 1999; Chisolm, 2002; Neoh and Mellor, 2009), but judgement is left to the Court, which may disregard opinions expressed in Family Reports (Nikou, 2002). 13 Expert witnesses In cases where serious maltreatment such as sexual abuse of a child has been alleged, additional experts may be nominated by the Court or agreed to by all parties and employed privately by the family as a single expert witness. The Family Law Rules 2004 define expert witness as “an independent person who has relevant specialised knowledge, based on the person's training study, or experience” and an expert report is a report prepared by such a person. An expert witness must be independent and has a duty to the Court that transcends any duty to a party. The position of an expert witness is distinguished from a person who may have similar specialised knowledge, but is called to give evidence primarily because of their involvement with a party or a child. A teacher, medical practitioner or other person, who may otherwise be regarded as an expert, may give factual evidence of their involvement with a party or a child, but may not to give opinion evidence. Expert evidence is privileged in that it is opinion based on specialist knowledge, as opposed to evidence of facts (Ingram, 1995), but many writers have argued it is nevertheless open to bias. In an adversarial system, where expert witnesses are called by the parties and not by the Court, parties are free to “shop around” for an expert who will give the opinion they want the Court to hear and to not present any report that does not supports the commissioning party’s case (Freckelton, 1986; Spencer, 1987). Expert witnesses have been described as “hired guns” (McDougall, 2004), who “do battle like gladiators or fighting cocks with their lawyers acting as ringmasters” (Selby, 1984, p11). Walsh (1991, p70) argued that in cases involving child sexual abuse “the expert often identifies with the party who is calling them and quite Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 25 often is cause driven as distinct from issue driven” and that experts with predetermined views should not be permitted to investigate and testify in cases of child sexual abuse. Since the late 1980s, writers including Spencer (1987) have argued that these problems could be alleviated, if expert witnesses were called by the Court rather than by the defence or the prosecution. In 2004 the Federal legislature addressed this issue by sanctioning the use of single experts, despite opposition from professional groups and interested parties including the Family Law Section of the Law Council of Australia. Rule 15.44 and 15.45 of the Family Law Rules provides that parties or the Court are able to appoint a single expert to provide expert evidence in proceedings before the Family Court and in rule 15.49 the legislature discourages the use of experts that are not single experts. Additional expert witnesses may only be called with the permission of the Court in instances where “there is a substantial body of opinion contrary to any opinion given by the single expert witness and …the contrary opinion is or may be necessary for determining the issue; another expert witness knows of matters, not known to the single expert witness, that may be necessary for determining the issue; or there is another special reason for adducing evidence from another expert witness.” A single witness may be appointed by agreement between the parties or, when parties do not agree, by order of the Court. The Court may select from a list prepared by the parties or may require the parties to agree on a single expert and the instructions to be provided to the expert. The Hon Justice Peter Rose declared the change to single experts a success and noted: “A criticism ...of the opportunity for an expert witness to be called by each of the parties in relation to the same issues is that there has been has been tendency for such witnesses to become advocates in the cause of the party who has called that witness.... It is recognised that the pressure that is placed on the expert witness in those circumstances is exacerbated by the... team environment in which the witness becomes an integral part of the battle or campaign against the other ‘team’, in which the prize is a ‘win’, rather than an outcome of a dispassionate resolution of the issues which are the subject of the expertise of the relevant witnesses.” (Rose, 2009, p2) Justice Rose noted that benefits include: “Prospects of settlement have increased; Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 26 Legal costs and witness expenses have dramatically reduced; Delays in awaiting trials have also reduced; and The length of trials have shortened.” (Rose, 2009, p3) Rose also noted the United Kingdom experience that: “Single experts are more impartial Single experts see their duty as being to the court The process saves time and money Single experts assist in levelling the playing fields between parties with unequal resources Single experts increase the prospect of settlement.” (Rose, 2009, p4) When issues are relatively straightforward, a single expert opinion should suffice, but matters before the Family Court are sometimes not straightforward, so single witnesses may not be appropriate in every case. Downs (2006) urged caution in relation to single expert reports because there is usually no way of testing whether their conclusions are correct. He noted that the ultimate decision-maker must always be the Judge, with expert opinion playing a subservient role, preferring to determine a matter by hearing evidence presented by both sides, when two sustainable perspectives are available. On occasion multiple experts provide concurrent evidence in a process known as “hottubbing”, where two or more experts are sworn in at the same time and placed in the witness box together, to give evidence and answer questions at the same time (McClellan, 2008). They may also be required to undertake a “conferencing of the experts” to prepare a Joint Experts’ Report, setting out the areas of agreement and disagreement between the experts. This practice allows the Judge, Magistrate or Commissioner to focus attention on the issues that need to be resolved. While this process is more commonly used in the New South Wales Land and Environment Court and in the Administrative Appeals Tribunal, Justice Rose has used concurrent evidence in the Family Court. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 27 Benefits of concurrent evidence over the traditional process include: All the evidence on each topic is given at the same time. Refining of issues into areas of agreement and disagreement before cross examination saves time and quickly focuses the Court’s attention on issues that are disputed. Cross examination with all experts present allows them to immediately be asked to comment on answers of colleagues. Experts are less inclined to act in an adversarial manner in this environment. A 2002 study by the Administrative Appeals Tribunal of the use of concurrent evidence in the New South Wales Registry concluded that concurrent evidence is generally effective and broadly liked. 14 Interview techniques As noted earlier, serious errors in interview practices have led to spectacularly flawed evidence in several high profile criminal cases involving allegations of child sexual abuse. One of the first US multi-victim multi-offender (MVMO) child abuse criminal cases involved the McMartin Preschool in Manhattan Beach, California. It was the longest US criminal trial in history and the most expensive, taking six years and costing the state US$15 million, yet it resulted in no convictions. When the District Attorney decided to not prosecute in relation to a single complaint of molestation, due to lack of physical evidence or confirmation from other children, the local Chief of Police circulated a "strictly confidential" letter to 200 parents suggesting that a McMartin staff member may have forced the children to engage in oral sex, fondling of genitals, buttocks or chest area and sodomy, urging parents to question their children, seeking confirmation. A local television station escalated the story by speculating that the preschool might be linked to child pornography rings and various sex industries in nearby Los Angeles. Arguably this created a degree of mass hysteria in the community. Soon 360 children were diagnosed as having been abused, following interviews by police and by the Children's Institute International. Medical examinations of 150 children concluded that about 120 of these had been sexually abused, despite there being no physical evidence. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 28 The specific complaints of abuse emerging from these interviews included that the children: were forced to submit to millions of "kiddy-porn" photographs being taken and to act in pornographic movies saw the mutilation and killing of animals including stabbing of a sea turtle were forced to engage in Satanic rituals including ritual murder of infants and drinking of a baby's blood saw dead and burned babies, flying witches, movie stars and local politicians were forced into a coffin and buried were molested in a market and in a car wash were taken by aeroplane or by hot air balloon or were flushed down toilets and travelled through sewers to a place where they were abused were taken through trapdoors in the floor of the centre, through underground tunnels to rooms both above and below ground, where they were abused. Despite a lack of confirmatory physical evidence such as actual trapdoors, tunnels or airline records (physical evidence amounted to a pair of rabbit ears, a black cape and some black candles, produced to substantiate allegations of satanic ritual abuse), 208 counts of child abuse involving 40 children were laid against seven adults. The state offered the defendants immunity from prosecution or leniency if they were willing to testify against the other defendants, but none took up the offer. After a change of District Attorney, all charges against 5 of the adults were dropped. After three years of testimony and nine weeks of deliberation, one of the remaining two defendants was cleared of all charges and the other was cleared of most, with the jury divided about the remainder. After a second trial, the second jury has also divided and the prosecution decided not to proceed with a third trial. Commentary on this case has generally concluded that this colourful list of unsustainable allegations was the inevitable result of very young children being pressured by parents, CII interviewers using leading and suggestive questions and rewards being given for providing the "right" answer, that is confirming that the children had been sexually abused (Eberle and Eberle, 1993; Nathan and Snendeker, 1995). In this case and some subsequent cases, critics have pointed to the presence of leading and suggestive questions in interviews and the use of repetitive interviews serving to contaminate the children's evidence. It was suggested both that the children had been coached and that repeated interviews and questioning had encouraged children to create new answers, hoping to satisfy the adult questioner. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 29 Wilson (1986), noting that some prosecutors “grilled” the alleged child victims for hours and pressed them to confirm the alleged incidents of molestation, suggested that desire to end such stressful questioning might provide sufficient motive for children to give any answer that seemed required. In one Minnesota case discussed by Wilson, children were questioned up to 29 times by five different investigators, often using highly suggestive questions. Commenting on the Australian MVMO case involving the Seabeach Kindergarten at Mona Vale, Sydney (known as the “Mr Bubbles” case), the Woods Royal Commission 1997 found that police investigations into the allegations made by the children were inadequate for reasons which included that investigators lacked necessary training, that the children were interviewed on multiple occasions and that evidence was contaminated by inappropriate questioning techniques, noting it was consequently no longer possible to determine whether there was any truth in the underlying allegations or not. As noted earlier, various factors have been shown by research to adversely affect the witness capacities of children from toddlers to elementary school. Kuehnle and Connell (2009) listed five: 1. Children’s tendency to be reticent and generally uncommunicative with unfamiliar adults 2. Children’s familiarity with being tested by adults (where adults know the correct answers), and lack of familiarity with adults asking questions to gain information not known by the adults 3. Children’s poorer linguistic skills 4. Very young children’s poorer memory for events 5. Children’s tendency to forget information more quickly. Kuehnle and Connell (2009, p390) noted: “Interviews of children are challenging in general because of the limitations of children’s verbal development and imperfect capacity to narrate events to adults in a logical chronological sequence. The interviewer is typically a stranger to the child and must maintain the child’s co-operation, attention and effort while attending to critical distinctions between well-phrased and poorly phrased questions. Even well-trained and experienced interviewers are vulnerable to errors that can significantly affect the accuracy of children’s reports.” Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 30 Studies have shown that both mental health and law enforcement professionals may encourage false accusations of sexual abuse through inappropriate interviewing methods. Often police and caseworkers have received little or no training in how to conduct interviews to assess the validity of a child's allegation of sexual abuse. However, the issue is not only the lack of appropriate training but that they may inappropriately apply interviewing skills learned for a different purpose: therapeutic interview skills are inappropriate from an investigative perspective, while standard interrogation skills are inappropriate for interviewing small children. Due to origins as a therapeutic technique for children known to have been abused, the assessment interview has traditionally been predicated on an assumption that abuse has taken place. Its use as a diagnostic technique has therefore led to criticism. Dent (1988) found that when interviewers did not know the facts beforehand, they tended to draw a conclusion at an early stage and then attempt to obtain confirmatory information from their young witness. In the United Kingdom, the Report of the Advisory Committee on the Investigation, Detection and Prosecution of Offences Against Children (1986, p 20) concluded: "This may lead to the phrasing of highly suggestive questions and lack of receptiveness to relevant information that does not fit into the interviewer's preconceived version." Similarly, Byrne (1991) commented that interviewers can lose objectivity by becoming emotionally or professionally aligned with one side of the dispute and that there is a grave danger that over-zealous police and other professionals will readily assume that accused persons are guilty when they investigate allegations, thinking of one party as “the offender” and another as “the victim”. Research by Ceci, Bruck and their associates has been particularly valuable in demonstrating how poor interviewing techniques have a high risk of eliciting false reports from children. Ceci, Loftus, Leichtman and Bruck (1996) found that in some cases, not only could preschoolers be convinced that what is false is actually true, but they could fool adults into professing confidence in their accuracy. They raised concern that false memories cannot be detected because children who have been repeatedly encouraged to imagine events that have not occurred, have come to believe that they are accurately recalling real events. Given their limitations with free recall, it was commonly thought that children perform better in response to questioning, but research has made clear that any questioning of children should be done with great care. Dent (1988) for example, found that children of 8-12 years Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 31 were as accurate as adults in recalling a lived event by free recall or in response to general, open-ended questions, but that the children were less accurate, that is had a greater proportion of incorrect recall, when asked specific, more clearly directed questions. This suggests that people involved in questioning children should try to encourage free recall, only using specific questions as a last resort. Recognising the complex interaction of variables operating when dealing with child witnesses, from the late 1980s onward, many writers have suggested guidelines for interviewing allegedly abused children, usually including: The child should be interviewed alone, at a neutral location, by a professional. Neither parent nor any other authority figure should be allowed to be present. The interviewer must establish rapport with the child and explain the purpose of the interview. The interview should be recorded so that the interaction can later be analysed for any contaminating influence. Raskin and Yuille (1989) identified several considerations for analysing each interview: What was the quality and type of evidence obtained from the child? What are the child's developmental level and specific language abilities? How deep is the child's sexual knowledge and what are the sources? How many prior interviews have there been and in what context? What are the roles and relationships of the interviewer with the child? Studies by Goodwin and Saywitz (1990) indicated that a supportive environment may be helpful to accuracy, especially for younger children. Initial discussion should be used to establish the child's verbal ability and then the interviewer should begin by encouraging a free recall narrative. After the child has finished, open-ended questions should be asked. Only if more detail is needed, should be more specific questions be asked with great caution to prevent leading. McIver, Wakefield and Underwager (1989) asserted that verbal reinforcement such as "good", "that's right" or close attention from the adult can increases the chance that the child will give answers they believe the adult wants to hear. If an interviewer feels the child is showing signs of suggestibility, this can be assessed at the end of the interview by the use of Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 32 suggestive questions unrelated to the incident (Raskin & Yuille, 1989; Steller & Koehnken, 1989). Taylor (1990) argued that, because some children say so little in response to open questioning, untrained investigators may be tempted to ask suggestive questions in order to gain more information and Oates (1990) suggested that questions asked of children must be simple, brief and reasonably concrete. Wakefield and Underwager (1994) argued that the child should be told that the interviewer is only interested in what the child remembers and that memory failure or memory gaps are expected. They proposed that interviewers should begin any interview with children by saying: “I don’t want you to say something that didn’t happen, but if something did happen, I want you to tell me about it.” They argued that questions should not be repeated, because this suggests to the child that their previous answers were not acceptable, They also said that discussions of “good touch” and “bad touch” cause confusion and therefore should be avoided. 15 Use of memory aids A variety of “memory aids” including anatomically detailed dolls (dolls with genitalia), props like puppets, dollhouses or telephones and play materials like paints, crayons and play dough have been widely used to help children describe experiences of sexual abuse during interviews. These aids are intended to assist the processes of memory retrieval and reduce self-consciousness and embarrassment by allowing the child to tell their story indirectly and have often been specifically recommended for use when interviewing very young children. Conte, Collins and Fogarty (1991) found that 92 per cent of surveyed US professionals conducting child abuse evaluations used anatomically detailed dolls. However, the use of memory aids, particularly anatomically detailed dolls, has also been strongly criticised, particularly when used with very young children (Freeman and EstadaMullaney, 1988; Underwager, 1988; Levy, 1989; McIver, Wakefield and Underwager, 1989). Levy (1989) argued that many problems associated with doll use stemmed from the diverse interviewing methods that professionals employed in conjunction with dolls, because there was no standard practice and training for doll use was minimal or non-existent. Underwager Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 33 (1988) found no difference between non-abused and abused children in their comments about the dolls or their behaviours in play with them. Boat and Everson (1987), White (1988) and McIver, Wakefield and Underwager (1989) all found that use of dolls could stimulate demonstrations of sexual abuse by non-abused children. Children were particularly likely to demonstrate aggressive and sexual acts that had not occurred when the interviewer asked leading questions, cued and encouraged the child. McIver et al concluded that, when an interviewer uses leading questions, cues, modelling and reinforcement, there is no difference in behaviour or responses to the dolls between abused and non-abused children and argued that the observed sexual play with anatomically detailed dolls by non-abused children could lead inexperienced interviewers to believe that the children had been sexually abused. Taylor, Geddis and Henaghan (1990) found that anatomically detailed dolls could be effective in facilitating a meaningful exchange with children about their sexual knowledge and experience, but argued that the effectiveness of this tool depends upon the skill of the user. They also noted the need to document exploratory and play behaviours with such dolls by young children who have not been sexually abused for comparison, so practitioners can more readily distinguish signs of abuse from normal exploratory play. Goodman and Aman (1990) concluded that, while use of anatomically detailed dolls with very young children is controversial, they can be useful for eliciting information from older children. They found that both anatomically detailed and regular dolls, as well as other props, aided five year-olds more than three year-olds in recounting a staged event and that three year-olds were generally more suggestible through misleading questions than five year-olds. Prop use increased rather than decreased age differences in the accuracy of recall. Based on these findings, they argued that anatomically detailed dolls do not foster false reports of abuse in five year olds. As noted earlier, cases involving allegations of child sexual abuse in the Family and Federal Magistrates Courts usually involve children younger than five years. Wakefield and Underwager (1994) argued that there was no basis for the expectation that a child who is unable to talk about what happened will be able to accurately demonstrate an event using a doll and that children may actually learn about sexual practices from untrained interviewers placing the dolls in sexually explicit positions when asking children what happened to them. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 34 Bruck, Ceci, Francouer and Renick (1995) similarly warned that the use of anatomically detailed dolls with very young children can allow the creation of multiple memory sources, allowing real memories to co-mingle with fictional suggestions. They asked three year old children to show what happened in a routine medical examination using anatomically detailed dolls. While the majority of demonstrations were accurate, some children inaccurately reported genital touching including insertion of a finger into the anal or genital cavity, regardless of how they were questioned and regardless of whether they had received a genital examination at all. Similarly, De Loache and Marzolf (1995) found that two and a half, three and four year old children had difficulty describing what had happened in a staged game, using the doll as self-representation. They provided more correct information in direct verbal and nonverbal responses to the interviewer's questions than they demonstrated on the doll. These studies suggest that anatomically detailed dolls may serve as an aid to uncovering the truth, rather than acting as a source of evidence contamination, only when children are old enough to understand this form of representation and only when used by suitably trained and experienced interviewers. 16 The science of expert opinion Many critics of expert evidence in this field have suggested that the science underpinning opinions is dubious and or that the experts may be tempted to do more than provide a factual basis for the Court’s decision making. Stephen and Newman (1994) warned that legal professionals must avoid the belief that what the expert offers is purely scientific. They argued that the form of expert opinion heard in child sexual abuse cases is often a blend of “expert knowledge, theoretical preferences, beliefs derived from the expert’s unique set of past clinical cases, inferences and interpretations drawn from interviews with and observations of the principle actors in the case, predictions of future behaviour, and individual character, values, intuition and judgment.” (Stephen and Newman, 1994, p181). Walsh (1991) argued that because experts base their evidence about what they have learned from theoretical and research literature, it is important that the Court be placed in a position where it knows precisely upon what materials the expert based his or her opinion. Experts have sometimes been accused of misinterpreting or misrepresenting the work of others and certainly it is appropriate that expert witnesses provide information to the Court about their Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 35 methods, their assumptions and the research underpinning these and the degree of certainty or doubt remaining in their conclusions. However, the forensic science of assessing allegations of child sexual abuse has advanced a great deal in the past 20 years. The issue is now less whether it is possible to elicit reliable evidence from children and more whether methods known to be effective are being used in practice. “Remarkable contributions have been made by social science researchers and theoreticians to an understanding of the conditions under which children make errors in reporting and the ways to enhance accurate reporting. This is a heavily researched area with increasingly refined research paradigms so that generalizability to clinical practice is supportable.” (Kuehnle and Connell, 2009, p389) According to Kuehnle and Connell (2009) in their overview, research in the past two decades has consistently shown that when interviews are conducted in keeping with best practice recommendations, that is a neutral interviewer, open-ended questioning, no repeated, suggestive questioning and no induction of a motive to make a false report, even very young children’s recall will be very accurate, though limited in detail. This view is particularly supported by the work of Baker-Ward, Gordon, Ornstein, Larus and Clubb (1993) and Parker, Bahrick, Lundy, Fivush and Levitt (1999). Conversely, research has consistently shown that children’s suggestibility errors increase when contra-recommended interview characteristics are present, such as repetitive questioning (Fivush and Schwartzmueller, 1995; Poole and White, 1995), biased questioning and inappropriate reinforcement of responses (Goodman, Bottoms, Schwartz-Kenney and Rudy, 1991; Tobey and Goodman, 1992; Lepore and Sesco, 1994; Ceci and Bruck, 1995; Carter, Bottoms and Levine, 1996) and yes-no question pairs (Petersen and Bell, 1996; Petersen and Briggs, 1997). The combination of suggestive questions with social pressure has been shown to produce the highest number of errors (Garven, Wood, Malpass and Shaw, 1998; Garven, Wood, Malpass and Shaw, 2000). Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 36 The issue is that there is no specific credentialing of forensic psychologists and no standard set of practice guidelines to ensure that expert evidence provided in the Family Court and Federal Magistrates Court will always be reliable. 17 Recognition of a specialist area of skill Research, particularly that emerging in the past two decades, has revealed the very high level of knowledge, skill and specific focus that is required of practitioners conducting forensic interviews with young children if they are to be effective in eliciting reliable evidence and the need for specialised training. “Mental health practitioners entering the child custody field are stepping into a very complex and formidable area of work... Some are skilled in forensic thinking and the application of forensic methodology and procedures to psycholegal questions. Others step into the forensic arena with a poor understanding of the differences between clinical and forensic roles and responsibilities... as well as of standard forensic methods and procedures... Many lack basic forensic training, while others do not know about admissibility standards for expert witness testimony... Mental health professionals who enter the forensic arena as it is currently structured need to be prepared for participation in an adversarial process.” (Gould and Martindale, 2007, p 6-7) “Interviews of children to determine sexual abuse should follow a research-based protocol and should only be conducted by those with adequate specialized training. They should be video-recorded to allow for analysis of the child’s statements and how they were elicited.” (Kuehnle and Connell, 2009, p392) While in the Family Court of Australia it is not the role of the expert witness to determine the matter, but only to provide information, opinion and recommendations that will assist the Court in determining the best interests of the child, this advice appears to be no less pertinent. In the United States there have been increasing efforts to regularise expert evidence in many areas of the law, including those related to allegations of child abuse. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 37 “In many contexts courts have increasingly demanded that expert testimony live up to some reasonable standards. Confronted with ever increasing amounts of ‘junk science’ and studies of very dubious merit generated for the purpose of litigation, courts have tried to better address issues of how information provided by experts is to be assessed.” (Galazter-Levy, 2009, pxvii) 18 Guidelines There are now a number of explicit guidelines available to forensic mental health professionals involved in child custody evaluations, including some detailed protocols for interviewing children in relation to allegations of child sexual abuse in a manner that will facilitate recollection and construction of a reliable narrative of important facts, without risk of contamination by the interviewer’s expectations. These include The Cognitive Interview (Fisher, Geiselman, Raymond, Jurkevich and Warhaftig (1987), The National Institute of Child Development (NICHD) Investigative Interview Protocol (Orbach, Hershkowitz, Lamb, Sternberg, Esplin and Horowitz, 2000), The Narrative Elaboration Technique (Saywitz and Snyder, 1993,1996) and the RATAC Method (Walters, Holmes, Bauer and Vieth (2003). All of these methods include a generally accepted structure and sequence of interview steps and all emphasise the superiority of recall testing questions over recognition testing questions (Kuehnle and Connell, 2009). Some earlier handbooks, while more clinical in orientation (eg Ackerman, 2001, revised 2006), remain useful as complementary reading in that they provide useful background concerning cognitive development, the psychological dynamics of families in crisis and guidelines for administration of psychological tests and do acknowledge the need for forensic skills when conducting an evaluation related to child custody. “To begin with, the evaluator should have formal training in forensic assessments, as the sensitive nature of the evaluation consists of a specific style and a set of skills unique to this experience... Mental health professionals are trained to perform the role of clinician; however, one must clearly avoid this when performing a custody evaluation. The nature of the evaluation is neither confidential nor clinical. There is no role for treatment during this process... Trying to fulfil both roles of clinician and Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 38 evaluator is wrong and will likely have negative impacts on both the evaluation and the therapy.” (Ackerman, 2006, p226) The NICHD Investigative Interview Protocol is currently the option best supported by research (Lamb, Orbach, Hershkowitz, Esplin and Horowitz, 2002; Brown and Lamb, 2009). The NICHD Investigative Interview Protocol uses a “funnel” approach that begins with broad, open-ended questions that are then cautiously followed up with more direct questions. Research has demonstrated that, in comparison with those who improvise, child protection interviewers who follow this protocol conduct better organised interviews, elicit more information and are better able to avoid interview practices that risk contamination (Lamb, Sternberg and Esplin, 1998). Given the degrees of uncertainty operating, it is also important that professionals providing expert evidence in this area be up to date in their awareness of relevant research and the implications of new research findings for their practice, but also they must have the skills to separate credible research findings from “junk science”. Galazter-Levy, Gould and Martindale (2009) provided a discussion of how information from the behavioural sciences can usefully be brought to bear on custody issues by assessing its credibility and relevance to each particular case. They outlined potential strengths and weaknesses of this type of information and suggested tools for assessing the quality of scientific information used in relation to custody matters. Earlier Gould and Martindale (2007) specified an extensive literature they believed all practitioners conducting assessments of child sexual abuse allegations in the context of custody disputes should be familiar with. However, while the US has some very clear guidelines, adherence to these guidelines has been another matter. Bow, Quinnell, Zaroff and Assemany (2002) surveyed the procedures used by forensic psychologists investigating allegations of child sexual abuse in the context of child custody disputes in the US. They found considerable variation in practice including that one quarter interviewed the alleged victim and alleged perpetrator together. Two thirds tested the alleged victims in some way, but some relied upon projective tests not supported by peer-reviewed literature (Lilienfield, Wood and Garb, 2000) and/or found to be ineffective as diagnostic tools (Kuehnle, 1998). Only one third used a sexual abuse protocol, model or professional practice guideline in their evaluations of allegedly sexually abused children. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 39 Bow et al (2002) found that many evaluators were not guided by published research in choosing their techniques and instruments. They asserted that an evaluator assessing allegations of child sexual abuse in the context of a custody dispute must have knowledge of three forensic areas: practices and procedures related to child custody sexual abuse evaluation techniques assessment of alleged sexual offenders. They further asserted that in a comprehensive model for forensic assessment of child sexual abuse: the forensic evaluator should be appointed by the court and have no prior involvement with the case; consideration should be given to whether a sole evaluator or team approach would be better; all relevant records should be reviewed; the nature, sequence and circumstances of the allegation/s should be assessed; multiple data sources consistent with current literature on forensic methods and procedures should be used the evaluator should interview the alleged victim using forensic interview protocols and be cautious about the use of projective tools such as anatomically detailed dolls; the evaluator should also use several (specified) psychological testing tools. An influential and well respected US model is Kuehnle’s (1996) multiple hypothesis approach, which postulates that, for each case, nine possibilities must be considered: 1. The child is a victim of sexual abuse and the allegation is credible and accurate. 2. The child is a victim of sexual abuse but, due to age or cognitive deficits, does not have the verbal skills to provide a cr4edible description of his or her abuse. 3. The child is a victim of sexual abuse but, due to fear, will not disclose his or her abuse. 4. The child is a victim of sexual abuse but, due to misguided loyalty, will not disclose his or her abuse. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 40 5. The child is not a victim of sexual abuse and is credible but has misperceived an innocent interaction. [Gould and Martindale, (2009) add to this truthful, but has misperceived an ambiguous situation or misidentified an alleged suspect]. 6. The child is not a victim of sexual abuse but has been unintentionally contaminated by a concerned or hypervigilant caretaker or authority figure. 7. The child is not a victim of sexual abuse but has been intentionally manipulated by a caretaker or authority figure into believing that he or she has been abused. 8. The child is not a victim of sexual abuse but knowingly falsely accuses someone of sexual abuse because of pressure from caretakers or authority figures who believe the child has been abused. 9. The child is not a victim of sexual abuse but knowingly falsely accuses someone of sexual abuse for reasons of self-aggrandisement or revenge. Lamb, Orbach, Sternberg, Esplin and Hershkowitz (2002) defined five types of utterance by interviewers, three of them useful, two of them potentially contaminating: Facilitators (eg “OK” or restatement of child’s utterance) Invitations (eg “And then what happened?”) Directive utterances (eg “What colour was the couch?”) Option-posing utterances, which are considered leading (eg “Were his clothes on or off?” “Did he tell you not to tell?”) Suggestive utterances, which strongly indicated a desired response (“He told you not to tell, didn’t he?”). In New South Wales, agencies including the Police, Department of Community Services and The Joint Investigative Response Team (JIRT) have attempted to meet demands for greater accountability by developing methods and protocols such as interview techniques that add rigour to the legal truth-finding process, in order to ensure that there are sufficient safeguards in the system for dealing with potential false allegations. However, these guidelines have limited value if they are not kept up to date and consistent with a fast moving area of research they are not adhered to by practitioners acting as expert witnesses few practitioners have the training needed to fully understand and conscientiously follow such guidelines. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 41 19 Training, credentials and implementation of panels. The consequences of child sexual abuse can be very serious for the child and the consequences of a false finding of child sexual abuse can be very serious for the alleged perpetrator. Since often there are no witnesses to the alleged abuse other than the alleged perpetrator and the alleged victim and no material evidence, it is essential that the information that can be provided by children is elicited in a manner that ensures its reliability as evidence in the Family Court or the Federal Magistrates Court when determining cases that involve allegations of child sexual abuse. Currently there are very few practitioners in this field, so the waiting time involved in obtaining an expert report can be many months. Although the cost to the family of obtaining an expert evidence report is usually several thousand dollars, the professional skills of these practitioners are diverse and generally not specific to the forensic purpose, so the needs of children who may be at risk and the needs of parents who may lose access to their children may not be well served. There is therefore urgent need for both a suitable training program in forensic psychology and a clear set of guidelines to ensure that those who provide expert evidence in these cases meet a high standard. It is clearly highly desirable that a set of guidelines to be observed by those providing expert evidence in relation to alleged child sexual abuse in the Family and Federal Magistrates Court be developed by a group including representation of all relevant fields of expertise. a coursework-based Masters in Forensic Psychology degree be developed to be provided by one or more Australian universities to equip those who provide expert evidence in relation to alleged child sexual abuse with the specialist skills they need to satisfactorily undertake this work that Masters in Forensic Psychology graduates be able to apply for accreditation and registration in a panel of experts qualified to be appointed to provide evidence in child sexual abuse proceedings before the Family or Federal Magistrates Court. The Federal Legislature is able to pass regulations that would allow the Federal Attorney Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 42 General’s Department to approve education providers to administer a coursework masters and to then administer the accreditation and registration of experts to a panel. This proposal is similar to the current system administered by the Federal Attorney General’s Department for the accreditation and registration of Accredited Dispute Resolution Practitioners. To provide the core competencies needed to provide reliable expert opinion in relation to allegations of child sexual abuse, a Masters in Forensic Psychology would need to address in some detail the following areas: 1. Legal foundation and ethics, including the framework, processes and protections for children within the Family Law Act, disclosures and mandatory reporting obligations and the duty of the expert witness to the Court 2. Child abuse: causes, correlates, definitions and diagnosis, with specific focus on child sexual abuse 3. Children’s cognitive development, including development of language skills and common sources of misunderstanding 4. Forensic interview techniques for use with adults, including both theoretical and practical components 5. Forensic interview techniques for use with children, including both theoretical and practical components 6. Administration of age-appropriate tests and memory aids 7. Requirements of expert evidence reports, including their approach, structure, content, supporting material and role in court processes 8. A written assignment requiring a substantial literature search. It is anticipated that many of those admitted as candidates to the coursework Masters in Forensic Psychology, will already have postgraduate qualifications as a clinical psychologist or psychiatrist, but whether this should be the formal prerequisite may need to be reviewed. The introduction of a coursework Masters in Forensic Psychology and establishment of an Expert Panel would give the Family and Federal Magistrates Courts a larger pool of experts to draw upon, resulting in shorter waiting times, a reduction in costs and a more consistent standard of expert evidence. In turn this would facilitate safer and fairer outcomes for both children and accused parents. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 43 20 References Achimovich L, Parent alienation syndrome, paper delivered at Child Sexual Abuse – Justice Response or Alternative Resolution Conference, Australian Institute of Criminology, Adelaide, May, 2003. Ackerman, MJ Clinician’s Guide to Child Custody Evaluations (3rd Edition) New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons, 2006 Aronson, M and Hunter, J Litigation, Evidence and Procedure (1995) (5th Ed), Butterworths, Sydney. Asington, J , Harris, P and Olson, D, Developing Theories of Mind 1988, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Baker, J, ‘Assessing and managing allegations of child sexual abuse: an Australian perspective’ Family and Conciliation Courts Review, 1997, 35, 293-299. Baker-Ward, L, Gordon, B, Ornstein, PA, Larus, D and Clubb, P Youn children’s long term retention of a pediatric examination, Child Development, 1993, 64, 15191533. Benjamin, GAH and Gollan, JK Family Evaluation in Custody Litigation: Reducing Risks of Ethical Infractions and Malpractice Washington DC: American Psychological Association, 2003. Berliner, L, Should Investigative Interviews of Children be Videotaped? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, June 1992, 277 - 288. Berliner, L and Barbieri, K, The testimony of Child Sexual Assault, Journal of Social Issues, 1984, 40, 125-137. Bernet, W, Ayres W, Dunne JE, Benedek E, Bernstein GA, Bryant E et al ‘Practice parameters for the forensic evaluation of children and adolescents who may have been physically or sexually abused’ Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 1997, 36, 37S-57S. Berns, SS, Parents behaving badly: parental alienation syndrome in the Family Court – magic bullet or poisoned chalice Australian Journal of Familt Law, 2001, 15, 191214. Bidrose, S and Goodman, GS, Testimony and evidence: a scientific case study of the memory for child sexual abuse Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2000, 14, 197-213. Boat, B and Everson, M, Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting, 1987. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 44 Bonner, BL, Walker, CG and Berliner, L Final Report on Children with Sexual Behaviour Problems: Assessment and treatment Washington DC: US Department of Health and Human Services Office of Child Abuse and Neglect, 1999. Bottoms, B,L, Shaver P,R, and Goodman, G,S, An analysis of ritualistic child abuse allegations, Law and Human Behavior, 20, 1996, 1- 34. Bow, JN, Quinnell, FA, Zaroff M and Assemany A Assessment of sexual abuse allegations in child custody cases Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 2002, 33(6), 566-575. Brown, T Knowing When, Where and How to Remember: A Problem of Metacognition, in Reese, H,W, (Ed), Advances in Child Development and Behaviour, Vol 10, 1978. Brown, D and Lamb, M “Interview Protocols: adherence to best practice” In K Kuehnle and M Connell (Eds) The Evaluation of Child Sexual Abuse Allegations: A comprehensive Guide to assessment and Testimony Hoboken NJ: Wiley, 2009 Brown, T, Fredrico, M, Hewitt, L and Sheehan, R ‘Problems and solutions in the management of child abuse allegations in custody and access disputes before the Family Court of Australia’ Family and Conciliation Courts Review, 1998, 36, 431-443. Brown, T, Fredrico, M, Hewitt, L and Sheehan, R, ‘Resolving family violence to children: The evaluation of Magellan Project: a pilot project for managing residence and contact disputes when allegations of child abuse have been made’ Family Court Papers, 2001, http://www.familycourt.gov.au/papers/html/magellan.html Brown, T ‘Fathers and child abuse allegations in the context of parental separation and divorce’ Family Court Review, 2003, 41, 367-380. Bruch, CS ‘Getting it wrong in child custody cases’ Family Law Quarterly, 2001, 35, 527545. Bruck, M, Ceci, S, Francouer, E & Renick, Anatomically Detailed Dolls Do Not Facilitate Preschooler's Reports of a Pediatric Examination Involving Genital Touching Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 1995, Vol. 1, No. 2 95-109 Budai, P, Rehabilitation of Children's evidence in Child Sexual Assault Cases, Current Issues in Criminal Justice, 7, (2), November 1995, 223 - 230. Bussey, K The Competence of Child Witnesses in Calvert, G, Ford, A & Parkinson, P, (Eds), The Practice of Child Protection: Australian Approaches, Sydney: Hale & Iremonger, 1992. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 45 Bussey, K Chidren's Lying and Truthfulness: Implications for Children's Testimony in Ceci, S,J, Leichtman, M.D. & Putnick, M, (Eds) Cognitive and Social Factors in Early Deception, Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, 1992, Chapter 7. Bussey, K, Steward, M,S, Goodman, G.S & Saywitz, K,J, Implications of Developmental Research for Interviewing Children, Child Abuse & Neglect, 17, 1993, 25-37. Bussey, K, Kerry L and Grimbeek, EJ, Lies and Secrets: Implications for Children's Reporting of Sexual Abuse, from Goodman, G.S. & Bottoms, B,L, (Eds), Child Victims, Child Witnesses: Understanding and Improving Testimony, Guilford Publications, 1993, 147 -167. Bussey, K, and Grimbeek, EJ, Disclosure processes: issues for child sexual abuse victims, in Rotenberg, K,J, (Ed) Disclosure processes in children and adolescents, Cambridge University Press, 1995, 166 -203. Bussey, K, Allegations of Child Sexual Abuse: Accurate and Truthful Disclosures, False Allegations and False Denials, Current Issues in Criminal Justice, 7, 2, November 1995. Byrne, P, Child sexual assault, Australian Law Journal, 62, July 1988, 553 -556 Cashmore, J, and Horsky, M, The Prosecution of Child Sexual Assault, Australian and New Journal of Criminology, 21, December 1988, 241 - 252. Cashmore, J, and Cahill, R, Achieving its objective?, Legal Service Bulletin, 15, 4, August, 1990, 155 -157. Cashmore, J, and Bussey, K, Judicial Views of Child Witness Competence, unpublished manuscript, Macquarie University, 1996. Carter, C, Bottoms, B and Levine, M Linguistic and socio-emotional influences on the accuracy of children’s reports, Law and Human Behaviour, 1996, 20, 335-358. Ceci, SJ, Ross, DF, and Toglia, MP, Children's eyewitness memory, New York, SpringerVerlag, 1987. Ceci, SJ, DeSimone, M,B, Putnick and Lee, M,L, Motives to Lie, Paper presented at the Psychology and Law Conference, Williamsburg, Vancouver, 1990. Ceci, SJ, and Bruck, M, Suggestibility of the Child Witness: A Historical Review and Synthesis, Psychological Bulletin, 113, 3, 1993, 403-439. Ceci SJ and Bruck, M, Jeopardy in the Courtroom: a scientific analysis of children’s testimony Washington DC: American Psychological Association, 1995. Ceci, SJ The Role of Source Misattributions in the Creation of False Beliefs Among Preschoolers, Unpublished paper. Chaaya, M, Children’s Evidence in Sexual Abuse Cases: The Need for a Radical Reappraisal, Current Issues in Criminal Justice 1998, 9, 3, p.262. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 46 Chae, Y and Ceci, SJ, The effect of intelligence, temperament and self-perceptions, Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2005, 19, 383-407. Chadwick, D, Child Abuse - Book Review JAMA 1989, 261, 20, p.3035. Chisolm, R ‘The role of experts in assisting courts in children’s cases – a judicial view’ Family Court of Australia , 2002. Clipson, CR ‘Practical considerations in the interview and evaluation of sexual offenders’ in R Geffner, K Crumpton Franey, T Geffner Arnold and R Falconer (Eds) Family and Conciliation Courts Review, 2003, 36, 431-433. Cole, Loftus, The Memory of Children, in Ceci, S.J, Taglia, MP, and Ross, DF, (Eds) Children's Eyewitness Memory, 1987. Conte, JR, Collins, M and Fogarty, L National survey of professional practice in child sexual abuse Journal of Family Violence, 1991, 6(2), 149-166. Coussins, A Prosecuting child sexual assault cases – are vulnerable witness protections enough? Current Issues in Criminal Justice, 2006, 18 (2), 299-317. Coussins, A Cross-examination in child sexual assault trials: evidentiary safeguard or an opportunity to confuse? Melbourne University Law Review, 2009, 33bn, 68104. Crewdson, R, and Martin, C, The Great Ormand Street Policy on Video Recordings, Family Law 1989,19, 161-162. Crewdson, J, By Silence Betrayed: The Sexual Abuse Of Children In America, Little Brown & Co, 1988. Crisp, L, When a child's word is the only evidence, Bulletin, December 12, 1989, 140-144. Davies, E, Devere, H and Verbitsky, “Court education for young witnesses: Evaluation of the pilot service in Aotearoa, New Zealand” Psychiatry, Psychology and the Law, 2004, 11(2), 226-235. Dale, J,A, Ensuring Reliable Testimony From Child Witnesses in Sexual Abuse Cases: Applying Social-science evidence to a new fact-finding method, Albany Law Review, 57, 1993, 187 -212. Davis, G, Stevenson, Y, and Flin, R, The Reliability of Children Testimony, International Legal Practitioner, December 1986, 95-103. Davis, G, Davies, G, Research on Children's Testimony: Implications for Interviewing Practice in Hollin, CR, and Howells, K, Clinical Approaches to Sex Offenders and Their Victims, 1991. Children in the Witness Box: Bridging the Credibility Gap, Sydney Law Review, 15, 3, 1993, 283 - 291. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 47 DeLoache, J and Marzolf, D, The Use of Dolls to Interview Young Children: Issues of Symbolic Representation, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 155-173, (1995) Dent, H, and Stephenson, G, Identification Evidence: Experimental Investigations of Factors Affecting the Reliability of Juvenile and Adult Witnesses, in Farrington, D.P; Hawkins, K & Lloyd-Bostock, S.M.(Eds) Psychology, Law and Legal Processes, 1979, London, Macmillan Press, 195-206. Dent, H, Children as Witnesses, unpublished paper presented at the Leo Cussen Institute, Melbourne, 1988. Dessau, L, ‘Children and the court system’ Paper presented at The Australian Institute of Criminology Conference, Brisbane, 1999. DiPietro, EK, Runyan, DK and Fredrickson, DD Predictors of disclosure during medical evaluation for suspected sexual abuse Journal of Sexual Abuse, 1997, 6, 133142. Downes The Hon. Justice Garry AM, The Use of Expert Witnesses in Court and International Arbitration Processes, Speech delivered to the 16th InterPacific Bar Association Conference 2006, Sydney 3 May 2006. Eberle,P and Eberle, S, The Abuse of Innocence : The McMartin Preschool Trial, Prometheus Books (1993). Ehrenberg, MF and Elterman, MF ‘Evaluating allegations of child sexual abuse in the context of divorce, child custody and access disputes’ in T Ney (Ed) True and False Allegations of Child Sexual Abuse: Assessment and case management NY: Brunner/Mazel, 1995. Eisen, ML, Goodman, GS, Qin, J, Davis, S and Crayton, J Maltreated children’s memory: accuracy, suggestibility and psycho-pathology Developmental Psychology, 2007, 43, 1275-1294. Ekman, P, Telling Lies 1985 NewYork, W.W Norton Publishers. Enright, S, Refuting Allegations of Child Sexual Abuse, Criminal Law Journal, 1987, 1, 633- 635. Ergas, H, Reflections on Expert Economic Evidence (2007) 45 Law Society Journal 68 at 73. Everstine, DS and Everstine, P Sexual trauma in children and adolescents: dynamics and treatment, NY: Brunner/Mazel, 1989. Faller, KC, Possible explanations for child sexual abuse allegations in divorce, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1991, 61, 86-91. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 48 Family Court of Australia, Guidelines for the ordering and preparation of family reports in contested children’s matters Canberra, 2000. Faust, D, Bridges, AJ and Ahearn, D, Overview of Issues and Needed Features for Abuse Indicators, In K Kuehnle and M Connell (Eds) The Evaluation of Child Sexual Abuse Allegations: A comprehensive Guide to assessment and Testimony Hoboken NJ: Wiley, 2009 Finkelhor, D, Araji, S, Baron, L, Browne, A, Doyle Peters, S and Wyatt, GE A Sourcebook on Child Sexual Abuse London: Sage, 1986. Fisher, RP, Geiselman, RE, Raymond, DS, Jurkevich LM and Warhaftig, ML Enhancing Rnhanced Eye Witness Memory: Refining the Cognitive Interview, Journal of Police Science and Administration,1987, 15, 291-297. Fitzgerald, J “Attrition of sexual offences from the NSW criminal justice system” Contemporary Issues in crime and Justice 92. Sydney: Bureua of Crime Statistics and Research, 2006. Fivush, R “Developmental perspectives on autobiographical recall” in GS Goodman and BL Bottoms (eds) Child victims, child witnesses: understanding and improving testimony pp1-24 NY: Guildford Press, 1993. Fivush, R and Schwartzmueller, A Children remember childhood: implications for childhood amnesia Applied Cognitive Psychology, 1995, 12, 455-473. Fleming, J, Mullen, PE, Sibthorpe, B and Bammer, G, ‘The long term impact of childhood sexual abuse in Australian women’ Child Abuse and Neglect, 1999, 23, 145159. Flens, JR and Drozd, L (Eds) Psychological Testing in Child Custody Evaluations New York and London: Psychology Press, 2005 Freckleton, I and Selby, H Expert evidence, law practice, procedure and advocacy 4th edition Sydney: Thomson, 2008. Freckelton, I, The Trial Of The Expert, Oxford University Press, Melbourne, 1986. Friedman, N and Jones, M, Children giving evidence of sexual offences in criminal proceedings: special measures in Australian states and territories, Journal of Judicial Administration, 2005, 14, 157-170. Friedrich, WN Sexual victimisation and sexual behaviour in children: a review of recent literature Child Abuse and Neglect, 1993, 17, 59-66. Friedrich, B Correlates of sexual behaviour in young children Journal of Child Custody, 2005, 2(3), 41-55. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 49 Friedrich, WN, Grambsch, P, Damon, L, Hewitt, SK, Koverola, C, Lang, RA, et al Child Sexual Behaviour Inventory: Normative and Clinical Comparisons Psychological Assessment, 1992, 4, 303-311. Freeman, and Estada-Mullaney, Using Dolls to Interview Child Victims: Leal Concerns and Interview Procedures, N.I.J Reports 1988, 207,2. Galazter-Levy, RM Introduction to Galatzer-Levy, RM, Kraus, L and Galatzer-Levy, J (Eds) The Scientific Basis of Child Custody Decisions New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons, 2009. Galazter-Levy, RM, Gould, J and Martindale, D, From empirical findings to custody evaluations, in Galatzer-Levy, RM, Kraus, L and Galatzer-Levy, J (Eds) The Scientific Basis of Child Custody Decisions New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons, 2009. Galatzer-Levy, RM, Kraus, L and Galatzer-Levy, J (Eds) The Scientific Basis of Child Custody Decisions New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons, 2009. Gardner, RA, Differentiating between parental alienation syndrome and bona fide abuseneglect, American Journal of Family Therapy, 1999, 27, 97-121. Gardner, RA, Should courts order PAS children to visit/reside with alienated parents? A follow up study, American journal of Forensic Psychology, 2001, 19, 61106. Gardner, R, Child Sexual Abuse: Hysteria and the Salem Witch Trials, Creative Therapeutics, New Jersey, 1991. Garven, S, Wood, JM, Malpass RS and Shaw, JS More than suggestion: the effect of interviewing techniques from the McMartin Preschool case Journal of Applied Psychology, 1998, 83, 347-356. Garven, S, Wood, JM, Malpass RS and Shaw, JS Allegations of wrong-doing: the effects of reinforcement on children’s mundane and fantastic claims Journal of Applied Psychology, 2000, 85, 38-49. Gee, D, Training the Expert Witness Medical Science Law 1988, 28, 2, 93-97. Gibson, J,A,L, Child Sexual Abuse, New Zealand Law Journal, August 1989, 269 - 273. Glass, Justice, Expert Evidence, Australian Bar Review 1987, 3, 43-51. Goodman, GS, Bottoms, BL, Schwartz-Kenney, B and Rudy, L Children’s memory for a stressful event: improving children’s reports Journal of Narrative and Life History, 1991, 1, 69-99. Goodman, GS, Quas, JA, Batterman-Faunce, JM, Riddlesberger, MM and Kuhn, G Children’s reaction to and memory for a stressful event: influences of age, Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 50 anatomical dolls, knowledge and parental attachment Applied Developmental Science, 1997, 1, 54-75. Goodman, GS, and Hegelson V, S, Child Sexual Assault: Childrens Memory and the Law, University of Miami Law Review 1985, 40, 181. Goodman, GS, Children’s Testimony in Historical Perspective, (1984) 40 Journal of Social Issues 9, p.10. Goodman, GS, The Child Witness: Conclusion and Future Directions for Research and Legal Practice, Journal of Social Issues, 1984, 40: 157. Goodman, GS, Rudy, L, Bottoms, B,L, & Aman, C, Children's concerns and memory: Ecological issues in the study of children's eyewitness testimony, in Fivush, R. & Hudson, J, (Eds) Knowing and Remembering in Young Children, N.Y; Cambridge University Press, 1990. Goodman, GS, Aman, C, and Hirschman, J, Child Sexual and Physical Abuse: Child Testimony, in Ceci, S.J et al (Eds), Children’s Eyewitness Memory, 1987, New York, Springer, Verlay. Goodman, GS, and Aman, C, Children's Use of Anatomically Detailed Dolls to Recount an Event, Child Development, 1990, 61, 1859-1871. Goodman, GS, and Clarke-Stewart, A, Suggestibility in Children's testimony: Implications for Sexual Abuse Investigations, in Doris, J. The Suggestibility of Children's recollections: Implications for Eyewitness Testimony, American Psychological Association, Washington, 1991, Chapter 6. Gould JW and Martindale DA The Art and Science of Child Custody Evaluations NY: The Guildford Press, 2007. Goodman, GS, Hirschman, JE, Hepps, DH and Rudy, L, Children's memory for stressful events, Merril Palmer Quarterly, 1991, 37, 109–158. Goodman-Brown, TB, Edelstein, RS, Goodman, GS, Jones, DPH and Gordon, DS, Child Abuse and Neglect, 2003, 27, 525-540. Gould JW and Martindale DA, A second call for clinical humility and judicial vigilance: comments on Tippins and Wittman Family Court Review, 2005, 43(2), 253259. Gould, JW and Martindale, D The Art and Science of Child Custody Evaluations New York: The Guildford Press, 2007. Gratz, KL and Orsillo, SM, Scientific expert testimony in child sexual abuse cases: legal, ethical and scientific considerations Clinical Psychology: Research and Practice, 2003, 10, 358-363. Green, A, and Schetky, D, True and False Allegations of Child Sexual Abuse, in Brunner and Mazel (Eds) Child SexualAbuse, Chapter 6, N.Y; 1988. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 51 Greene, D, Westchester Q & A: Dr Jan Tucker; Can Children Be Believed About Abuse? New York Times Feb 25, 1990 in Dale, J, Ensuring Reliable Testimony from Child Witness in Sexual Abuse : Applying Social Science Evidence to a New Fact Finding Method, Albany Law Review 1993, 57, 187-212. Hagen, MA Faith in the model and resistance to research Clinical Psychology: Research and Practice, 2003, 10, 344-348. Hampton, N, Recovered Memory Syndrome V False Memory Syndrome or in repression and revenge, where resides justice?, New Zealand Law Journal, May 1995, 154 -157. Harris, L, Goodman, G, Augusti, EM, Chae, Y and Alley, D “Children’s resistance to suggestion” In Kuehnle and M Connell (Eds) The Evaluation of Child Sexual Abuse Allegations: A comprehensive Guide to assessment and Testimony Hoboken NJ, Wiley, 2009. Hatty, S, Of Nightmares and Sexual Monsters: Struggles Around Child Abuse in Australia, International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, (1991), Vol. 14, P. 255-267 Haugaard, JJ The challenge of defining child sexual abuse American Psychologist, 2000, 55(9), 1036-1039. Haugaard, JJ, and Crosby, C, Children's definitions of the truth and their competency as witnesses in legal proceedings. Paper presented at the Southeastern psychological Association Confereence, Washington, DC, 1989. Hechler, D, The Battle and the backlash, Lexington Books, 1988. Hoogenraad, K, and McKenzie, B,E, Maternal Reports of Children's Deceptive Behaviour, Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 1, 1995, 42 - 46. Hooper, C and Humphreys, C, Women whose children have been sexually abused: reflections on a debate, British Journal of Social Work, 1998, 28, 565-580. Horsky, M, Powell, A, and Swinfield, P, Child sexual Assault: Responding to the issue of false complaints, Legal Service Bulletin, 11, 4, August 1986, 169-172. Howe, ML and Courage, ML, On resolving the enigma of infantile amnesia, Psychological Bulletin, 1993, 113, 305-326. Hoyano, L, Child witnesses and much ado about too little, Criminal Law Review, Nov 2007, 849-865. Ingram, R, Being An Expert Witness In Court - What To Expect Paper delivered at the 1995 Medico-Legal Congress 13 & 14 Feb 1995, The Hyatt Kingsgate Hotel Sydney. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 52 Johnson, M, and Foley, M, Differentiating Fact From Fiction: The Reliability of Children’s Memory, Journal of Social Issues, 1994, 2, 33, 37-38. Jones, DPH and Seig, A, Child sexual abuse allegations in custody or visitation disputes – a report of 20 cases, in EB Nicholson and J Bulkley (Eds) Sexual Abuse Allegations in Custody and Visitation Cases, Washington DC: American bar Association, 1988. Jones, DPM, and McGraw, JM, Reliable and fictitious accounts of sexual abuse in children, Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 1987, 2, 27-45. Jones, E and Parkinson, P, Child sexual abuse, access and the wishes of children, International Journal of Law and the Family, 1995, 9, 54-85. Jumper, SA, A meta-analysis of the relationship of child sexual abuse to adult psychological adjustment, Child Abuse and Neglect, 1995, 19, 715-728. Kail, R, Developmental changes in speed of processing: Central limiting mechanism or skill transfer: Reply to Stigler, Nusbaum and Chalip, Child Development, 1988, 59, 1154-1157. King, MA, and Yuille, JC, Suggestibility and the child witness, in Ceci, SJ, Toglia, MP and Ross, DF, (Eds) Children's eyewitness memory, New York, Springer-Verlag, 1987, 24-35. Kleinman, TG, Child protection and child custody: domestic violence, abuse, and other issues of child protection, Journal of Child Custody, 2004, 1, 115-126. Kornberg, AE, Recognising and reporting child abuse, in S Ludwig and AE Kornberg (Eds) Child Abuse: A medical reference (2nd Ed) NY: Churchill Livingston,1992. Kovera, M, and Levy, R, & Borgida, E, & Penrod, S, Expert Testimony in Child Sexual Abuse Cases, Law And Human Behaviour, 1994, 18, 6, 653-674. Kuehnle, K and Connell, M, Evaluation child sexual abuse allegations, in RM Galatzer-Levy, L Kraus and J Galatzer-Levy (Eds) The Scientific Basis of Child Custody Decisions New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons, 2009. Kuehnle, K and Connell, M, Evaluating child sexual abuse allegations, in Galatzer-Levy, RM, Kraus, L and Galatzer-Levy, J (Eds) The Scientific Basis of Child Custody Decisions New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons, 2009. Kuehnle, K, Child Sexual Abuse: Treatment Issues, In GP Koocher, JC Norcross, SS Hill III The Psychologist’s Desk Reference (Eds) NY: Oxford University Press, 1998. Lamb, ME, Orbach, Y, Sternberg, K, Esplin, PW and Hershkowitz, D I, The Effects of Forensic Interview Practices on the Quality of Information Provided by Alleged Victims of Child Abuse, In HL Westcott, GM Davies and HC Bull (Eds) Children’s Testimony: A Handbook of Psychological Research and Forensic Practice Hoboken NJ: Wiley, 2002. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 53 Lamb, ME, Sternberg, K and Esplin, P, Conducting investigative interviews of alleged sexual abuse victims, Child Abuse and Neglect, 1998, 22, 813-823. Lawson, L, and Chaffin, M, False Negatives in Sexual Abuse Disclosure Interviews, Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7, 4, December 1992, 532-542. Leander, L, Christianson, S-Å and Granhag, PA, A sexual abuse case study: Children’s memories and reports, Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, 2007, 14(1), 120129. Leander, L, Granhag, PA and Christianson S-Å, Children exposed to obscene phone calls: what the remember and tell, Child Abuse and Neglect, 2005, 29, 871-888. Lees-Haley, PR, Innocent Lies, Tragic Consequences, Trial, 24, 4, April, 1988, 39-40. Lepore, S and Sesco, B Didtorting children’s reports and interpretations of events through suggestion Journal of Applied Psychology, 1994, 79, 108-120. Levy, RJ, Using 'Scientific' Testimony to Prove Child Sexual Abuse", Family Law Quarterly, 23, 3, Fall, 1989, 383 -409. Lewis, Stanger and Sullivan, Deception in Three-Year-Olds, Developmental Psychology, 25, 3, 1989, 439-443. Lewis, P, Delayed Prosecution for Childhood Sexual Abuse, Oxford University Press, 2006. Lilienfield, SO, Wood JM and Garb, HN The scientific status of projective techniques, Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2000, 1, 27-66. Lindsay, DS, and Read, JD, Memory Work and recovered Memories of Childhood Sexual Abuse: Scientific Evidence and Public, Professional and Personal Issues, Psychology Public Policy and Law, 1995, 1, 4, 846-908. Lockhart J quoted in Judicial Commission of New South Wales and Australian Institute of Judicial Administration, Concurrent Evidence: New Methods with Experts (DVD, 2005). Loftus, EF, and Palmer, JC, Reconstruction of Automobile Destruction: An Example of the Interaction between Language and Memory, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal behaviour, 1974, 13, 585-589. Loftus, EF and Ketcham, K, Witness For The Defence, New York, St. Martin’s Press 1991. Macfarlane, K, Child abuse allegation in divorce proceedings, in K Macfarlane, J Waterman, S Conerly, L Damon, M Durfee and S Long (Eds) Sexual Abuse of Young Children NY: Guildford, 1986. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 54 Magner, ES, An alternative view: recovered memories and the Australian Courts, Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 28, 2000, 30-41. Manning, F and Griffith, G, Victims Rights and Victims Compensation: Commentary on the Legislative Reform Package, (1996) NSW Parliamentary Library Research Service, Briefing Paper 12. Mapes, B, Child eye witness testimony in sexual abuse investigations Brandon VT: Clincal Psychology Publishing, 1995. McDougall, R, Paper given to the Institute of Arbitrators and Mediators, Australia on 13 February 2004. McClellan, The Hon Justice Peter, Paper delivered to the National Judicial College of Australia “The Australian Justice System In 2020”, 25 October 2008. McIver, W, Wakefield, H, and Underwager, R, Behaviour of Abused and Non-Abused Children in Interviews with Anatomically-Correct Dolls, Issues In Child Abuse Accusations, 1989, Vol. 1, No. 1, pp. 39-48 Melton, GB, Petrila J, Poythress NG and Slobogin C, Psychological evaluations for the courts: a handbook for mental health professionals and lawyers (2nd Ed) NY: Guildford, 1997. Melton, GB, Children's Competency to testify, Law and Human Behaviour, 5, 73, 1981. Misener, M, Children's Hearsay Evidence in Child Sexual Abuse Prosecutions: A Proposal for Reform, Criminal Law Quarterly, 1991, 364 - 384. Nathan D and Snendeker M, Satan's Silence, BasicBooks, New York NY (1995), Neoh, J and Mellor, D, Professional issues related to allegations and assessment of child sexual abuse in the context of Family Court litigation, Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, July 2009, 16(2), 303-321. Neuman, DA, Houskamp BM, Pollock VE and Briere J, The long term sequelae of childhood sexual abuse in women: a meta-analytic review, Child Maltreatment, 1996, 1, 6-16. New South Wales Child Protection Council, Review of Seminar by Ralph Underwager & Hollida Wakefield, 1989. Nicholson, A, ‘Court management of cases involving child abuse allegations’ Paper presented at 7th Australasian Conference on Child Abuse and Neglect, Perth, October, 1999. Nikou, O, ‘Reliability of expert evidence in children’s cases’ Paper presented at 10th Family Law Conference, Melbourne, March 2002. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 55 Nucrome, B, The Child as Witness: Competence and Credibility, Journal AM. ACAD. Child Psychiatry 25 1986, 473 – 474. Oates, RK, Children as Witnesses, Australian Law Journal, 64, March, 1990, 129 - 134. Odgers, S, A Uniform Evidence Law 2nd Ed. (1997) Federation Press, Sydney. O’Leary, DK and Moerk, KC, Divorce, children and the courts: the use of parental alienation syndrome in custody disputes, Expert Evidence, 1999, 7, 127-146. Orbach, Y and Lamb, ME, Assessing the accuracy of a child’s account of sexual abuse: A case study, Child Abuse and Neglect, 1999, 23, 91-98. Orbach, Y, Hershkowitz, DI, Lamb, ME, Sternberg, K, Esplin, P and Horowitz, D Assessing the value of structured protocols for forensic interviews of alleged abuse victims, Child Abuse and Neglect, 2000, 24, 733-752. Ornstein, P-A, Larus, D-M and Clubb, P-A, Understanding children’s testimony: Implications of research on the development of memory, In R Vasta (Ed) Annals of Child Development 8, 145-176 London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers, 1991. Paolucci, EO, Genuis, ML and Violato, C, A meta-analysis of the published research on the effects of child sexual abuse, Journal of Psychology, 2001, 135, 17-36. Parker, J, Bahrick, L, Lundy, B, Fivush, R and Levitt, M “Effects of stress on children’s memory for a natural disaster” In CP Thompson, DJ Herrmann, JD Read, D Bruce, DG Payne and MP Toglia (eds) Eyewitness Memory: Theoretical and Applied Perspectives Mahwah NJ: Erlbaum, 1999. Petersen, C and Bell, M Children’s memory for traumatic injury Child Development, 1996, 67, 3045-3070. Petersen, C and Briggs, M Interviewing children about trauma: problems with ‘specific’ questions Journal of Traumatic Stress, 1997, 10, 279-290. Perner, J, and Wimmer, H, John thinks that Mary thinks that....: Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5-10 year old children”, Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1985, 39, 437-471. Pithers, WD, Gray, A, Busconi, A, and Houchens, P Children with sexual behaviour problems: identification of five distinct child types and related treatment considerations, Child Maltreatment, 1998, 3, 384-406. Poole, DA and White, LT “Tell me again and again: stability and change in the repeated testimonies of children and adults” In MS Zaragoza, JR Graham, GCN Hall, R Hirschman and YS Ben-Porath (Eds) Memory and Testimony in the Child Witness Thousand Oaks Ca: Sage, 1995. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 56 Powell, M, Improving the reliability of child witness testimony in court: the importance of focusing on questioning techniques, Current Issues in Criminal Justice, 2005, 17(1), 137-143. Raskin, DC, and Yuille, J,C, Problems in evaluating interviews of children in sexual abuse cases, in Ceci, SJ, Ross, DF and Toglia, MP, (Eds) Perspectives on children's testimony, New York, Springer-Verlag, 1989, 184-207. Rayner, M, The Right to Remain Silent: The Interrogation of Children, Monash University Law Review, 15, 1989, 30 -47. Rendell, K, Rathus, Z and Lynch, A, An Unacceptable Risk Brisbane: Women’s Legal Service, 2000. Report of the Advisory Committee on the Investigation, Detection and Prosecution of Offences Against Children, London, date. Rhoades, H, The “no contact mother”: reconstructions of motherhood in the era of the “new father”, International Journal of Law, policy and the Family, 2002, 16, 7194. Richards, K “Child complainants and the court process in Australia” Trends and Issues in Crime and Criminal Justice (Australian Institute of Criminology), July 2009, No 380. Robertson, B, Exert Evidence in Child Sex Abuse Cases: A Comment, New Zealand Law Journal, May 1989, 163-166. Rose, Hon Justice P, The evolution of the expert witness, Australian Journal of Family Law, 2009, 23, 1-9. Rudy, L and Goodman, GS, Effects of Participation on Children's Reports: Implications for Children's Testimony, Developmental Psychology, 1991, 27. Salmelainen, P and Coumarelos, C, Adult Sexual Assault in NSW, Crime and Justice Bulletin, 20, July 1993. Salter, A Accuracy of Expert Testimony in Child Sexual Abuse Cases: A Case Study of Ralph Underwager and Hollida Wakefield, Unpublished. Sas, LD and Cunningham, AH, Tipping the balance to tell the secret: Public discovery of child sexual abuse London, Canada: London Court Clinic, 1995 Sauzier, M, Disclosure of child sexual abuse: For better or for worse Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 1989, 12, 455-469. Saywitz, KJ and Snyder, L , Improving Children’s Testimony with Preparation, In GS Goodman and BL Bottoms (Eds) Child Victime, Child Witnesses: Undertanding and Improving Testimony NY: Guildford Press, 1993. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 57 Saywitz, KJ and Snyder, L, Narrative elaboration: test of a new procedure for interviewing children, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,1996, 64, 1347-1357. Schneider, W and Pressley, M Memory development Between 2 and 20 NY: Springer-Verlag, 1989. Scott, S and Dickens, A, Police and the Professionalisation of Rape, in Dunhill, C.(Ed) The Boys in Blue, London, Virago, 1989. Selby, H, The Expert In Court: Educator Or Puppet, New Doctor Legal Service Bulletin, 1984, 9, 1, 11-13. Shapiro, LR and Purdy, TL Suggestibility and source monitoring errors: blame the interview style, interviewer consistency and the child’s personality Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2005,19, 489-506. Sixty Minutes (Australia) “Witness for Mr Bubbles” Transcript of story Aired August 5, 1990 Channel Nine Network. Producer Anthony McClellan; Reporter Mike Munro. Sjöberg, R and Lindblad, F Limited disclosure of sexual abuse in children whose experiences were documented by videotape American Journal of Psychiatry, 2002(a), 158, 312-314. Sjöberg, R and Lindblad, F Delayed disclosure and disrupted communication during forensic investigations of child sexual abuse: a study of 47 corroborated cases Acta Paediatrica, 2002(b), 91, 1391-1396. Smallbone, SW and Wortly RK, Child sexual abuse: offender characteristics and modus operandi, No 193 in Trends and Issues in Crime and Criminal Justice Series, Canberra: 2001. Smith, DW, Letourneau, EJ, Saunders, BE, Kilpatrick, DG, Resnick, HS and Best, CL Delay in disclosure of childhood rape: results from a national survey, Child Abuse and Neglect, 2003, 2, 273-287. Smyth, B, Moloney, L, Weston, R, Richardson, N, Qu Lixia and Grays, M, Allegations of family violence and child abuse in children’s proceedings: A pre-reform empirical snapshot, Australian Journal of family Law, 2007, 21, 252-287. Snow, P and Powell, M, Getting the Story in Forensic Interviews with Child Witnesses: Applying a story grammar framework Canberra: Criminology Research Council, 2007. Solomon, JC, ‘Child sexual abuse by family members: a radical feminist perspective’ Sex Roles: A Journal of research, 1992, 27, 473-487. Spencer, JR and Flin, R, The Evidence of Children, Blackstone, London 1990. Spencer, J. “Child Witnesses, Corroboration and Expert Evidence” The Criminal Law Review, 1987, 239-251. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 58 Strand, VC, ‘Clinical social work and the Family Court: A new role in child sexual abuse cases’ Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 1994, 11, 107-122. Steller, M and Koehnken, G, Criteria-based statement analysis, in Raskin, D.C. (Ed) Psychological methods in clinical investigation and evidence, New York, Springer, 1989, 217-245. Steller, M, Recent developments in statement analysis, in Tuille, JC, (Ed) Credibility assessment, Norwell, Kluwer, 1989, 135-154. Stephen, A and Newman, J, Assessing the Quality of Expert Testimony in Cases Involving Children, The Journal of Psychiatry Law, 1994, 22, 2, 181-234. Stevenson, K, and Sood, U, Pigot: the need for a good look at videos, The Law Society's Gazette, 18, 16 May 1990, 23, 25. Summit, R, The Child Sexual Abuse Accommodation Syndrome, Child Abuse and Neglect, 7, 1983, 177-193. Sumner-Armstrong, C and Newcombe, P, The education of jury members: influences on the determinations of child witnesses, Psychology, Crime and Law, 2007, 13(3), 229-244. Suwa, M, Tversky, B, Nicoll, SB, Radin, S, Santos, EM and Tuan, RS, The prevalence and nature of child sexual abuse in Queensland Australia, Child Abuse and Neglect, 1997, 21, 489-499. Svedin, CG and Back, M, Why don’t they tell? Being used in child pornography Falun: Rädda Barnen, 2003. Taylor, NJ, Geddis, DC and Henaghan, RM, Child sexual abuse: Obtaining accurate testimony from child victims, New Zealand Law Journal, November 1990, 388-391; December 1990, 425- 431. Timnick, L Early Memory; Children in Court; How Reliable, L.A. Times, April, 18, 1985 at 1, 32 reporting the findings of child psychiatrist Lenore, C, in Dale, J, Ensuring Reliable Testimony from Child Witness in Sexual Abuse Cases: Applying Social Science Evidence to a New Fact Finding Method, Albany Law Review 57, 1993, 187-212, p.187. Tobey, A and Goodman, GS Children’s eyewitness memory: effects of participation and forensic context Child Abuse and Neglect, 1992, 16, 779-796 Thoennes, N and Pearson, J, Summary of findings from the sexual abuse and allegations project, In EB Nicholson and J Bulkley (Eds) Sexual Abuse Allegations in Custody and Visitation cases: a resource Book for Judges and Court Personnel, Washington DC: American Bar Association, 1988. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 59 Thoennes, N and Tjaden, PJ, The extent, nature and validity of sexual abuse allegations in custody/divorce disputes, Child Abuse and Neglect, 1990, 151, 153-154. Turtle, JW and Wells, GL, Setting the stage for psychological research on the child eyewitness, in Ceci, MP, Toglia, MP and& Ross, DF, (Eds) Children's eyewitness memory, New York, Springer-Verlag, 1987, 230-248. Turkat, ID, Child visitation interference in divorce, Clinical Psychology Review, 1994, 14, 737-742. Udwin, O, Annotation: children’s reactions to traumatic events, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1993, 34, 115-127. Vrij, A and Bush, N Differences in suggestibility between 5-6 and 10-11 year olds: the relationship with self-confidence Psychology, Crime and Law, 2000, 6, 127138. Wakefield, H and Underwager, R, Accusations of Child Sexual Abuse Institute for Psychological Therapies, 1988. Wakefield, H and Underwager, R, The Alleged Child Victim and Real Victim of Sexual Misuse, in Krivacski, J and Mahoney, J (Eds) Handbook of Forensic Sexology Buffalo, Prometheus Books, 1994 . Walsh, G, Re-Establishing balance in the Criminal Justice System, Law Society Journal, July, 1991, 70 - 79. Walters, S, Holmes, L, Bauer, G and Vieth, V Finding Words: Half a Nation by 2010 – Interviewing Children and Preparing for Court Alexandria, VA: National Center for Prosecution of Child Abuse, 2003. Warner, K, Child Witnesses in Sexual Assault Cases, Criminal Law Journal, 12, 1988, 286 -302. Waterman, J and Lusk, R, Psychological Testing in Evaluation of Child Sexual Abuse, Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 1993, 145-159. White, S, Should Investigators Use of Anatomical Dolls Be Defined by The Court?, Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 3, 1988, 471-475. Wilson, P, False complaints by children of sexual abuse, Legal Service Bulletin, 11, 2, April 1986, 80 - 83. Wittaker, M, The recurring nightmare of Mr. Bubbles, The Australian, Tuesday February 12 1991 p.4. Wolfe, DA and Legate, BL, Expert opinion on child sex abuse: separating myths from reality, Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, Aug 2003, 10 (3); Health Module, 339-343. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 60 Zaragoza, MS, Memory, Suggestibility and Eyewitness Testimony in Children and Adults, in Ceci, Toglia and Ross (Eds) Children's Eyewitness Memory, 1987, 53-78. Zarb, LH, Allegations of childhood sexual abuse in custody and access disputes. What care is in the best interests of the child?, Canadian Journal of Family Law, 1994, 12, 91-114. Matthew Myers: Towards a safer and more consistent approach to allegations of child sexual abuse in Family Law Proceedings - Expert Panels and Guidelines. Page 61