1 1 CHAPTER 6 2 THE DEVELOPING BRAIN 3 Draft: 3-14-2000 4 My changes are tracked in red or green and marked by an *asterisk 5 My comments (red) are enclosed in {curly brackets}--bill 6 The brain is the ultimate organ of adaptation. It takes in information and orchestrates 7 complex behavioral repertoires that allow us to act in sometimes marvelous, sometimes terrible 8 ways. For humans, most of what we think of as the “self” –what we think, what we remember, 9 what we can do, how we feel--is acquired by the brain from the experiences we have after birth. 10 Some of this information is acquired during critical or sensitive periods of development when the 11 brain appears uniquely ready to take in certain kinds of information, while other information can 12 be acquired across broad swaths of development that can extend into adulthood. This spectrum 13 of possibilities is well captured by coinciding evidence of the remarkably rapid brain 14 development that characterizes early development and of the brain’s lifelong capacity for growth 15 and change. The balance between the enduring significance of early brain development and the 16 impressive plasticity of the brain lies at the heart of the current controversy about the effects on 17 the brain of early experience. 18 The past 20 years have seen unprecedented progress in our understanding of how the 19 brain develops and, in particular, of the phenomenal changes in both the circuitry and 20 neurochemistry of the brain that occur during prenatal and early postnatal development. As 21 discussed in Chapter 3, our knowledge of the ways in which genes and the environment interact 22 to affect the development of the brain has expanded by leaps and bounds. The years ahead will Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 2 23 bring even more breathtaking progress as, for example, the completion of the human genome 24 project provides a map of the genome. This promises an explosion of our ability to understand 25 the interweaving of genetic and environmental influences as they affect both brain and 26 behavioral development. 27 The growth in “brain knowledge” naturally leads to demands to understand what this 28 means for how we should raise children and what we can do to improve their development. 29 Accordingly, efforts to translate this emerging knowledge for public consumption have 30 proliferated in recent years. Some of this information has been well and accurately portrayed; 31 but some has also been misportrayed. The challenge of deciphering what this information means 32 for what we should do as parents, guardians, and teachers of young children is enormous. There 33 are few neuroscience studies of very young children and those that exist have not usually focused 34 on the brain regions that affect cognition, emotions, and other complex developmental tasks. 35 Much of our fundamental knowledge about brain development is actually based upon 36 experimental studies in animals. The translation of this information from basic neuroscience into 37 “rules” for application to humans can be quite straightforward when very similar mechanisms are 38 involved in humans and animals, as is the case with the developing visual system. But, the 39 interpretation of other data from animals, or even some data from humans (such as estimates of 40 the density of synapses in various brain regions at various ages), can be extraordinarily complex 41 or inappropriate when the brain mechanisms of cognition, language, and social-emotional 42 development are addressed. In this context, it is essential to balance excitement about all that we 43 are learning with caution about the limits of what we understand today. Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 3 44 This chapter’s synthesis of what science now tells us about the developing brain focuses 45 on the role of experience in early brain development. Following a brief discussion of how we 46 study the developing brain, we provide an overview of early brain development from conception 47 through the early childhood years. We then turn to a discussion of how early experiences 48 contribute to brain development. Four themes run throughout this section: 49 1. Developmental neuroscience research tells us a great deal about the conditions that pose 50 dangers to the developing brain and from which young children need to be protected. It tells 51 us virtually nothing about what we can do to create enhanced or accelerated brain 52 development. 53 2. The developing brain is open to influential experiences across broad swaths of development. 54 The openness to experience is part of what accounts for the remarkable adaptability of the 55 developing mind. Although there are a few aspects of brain growth that require particular 56 kinds of experience at particular points in development, as far as we know at present, this is 57 exceptional rather than typical for human brain growth. 58 3. The kinds of early experiences on which healthy brain development depends are ubiquitous 59 in typical early human experience—just as nature intended. This means, however, that our 60 greatest concerns should be devoted to children who, for reasons of visual impairments, 61 auditory processing deficits, major perceptual-motor delays, and other basic deficits, cannot 62 obtain these experiences on which the developing nervous system depends. 63 4. Abusive and neglectful care, growing up in a dangerous or toxic environment, and related 64 conditions are manifest risks for healthy brain development. Beyond these extremes, the 65 nature and boundaries of the environmental conditions necessary for healthy brain growth are Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 4 66 less well known*, partly owing to the complexity and the cumulative achievements of 67 cognitive, language, and socioemotional growth. This is cutting-edge research. 68 69 HOW DO WE STUDY THE DEVELOPING BRAIN? 70 Neuroscience techniques have advanced significantly, rendering studies of young 71 children's brains more feasible and informative than in the recent past. These techniques have 72 enabled scientists to learn more about how babies' brains change with development and how 73 vulnerable or resilient they are to environmental insults. The repertoire of techniques that can be 74 used with preschool-age and young children is circumscribed because some of the more direct 75 methods (i.e., looking from inside the brain) of studying brains are either invasive (e.g., Positron 76 Emission Tomopraphy requires the injection of a radioactive substance) or require long periods 77 of remaining* still (e.g., functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging). Nevertheless, by tracking the 78 brain's activity from the outside with the electroencephalogram, event-related potentials, and 79 magnetic encaphalography, researchers can record the electrical and/or magnetic field* activity 80 of the brain while the baby or child is presented with different stimuli (e.g., speech sounds) and 81 identify which parts of the brain are active and how active they are when children are "doing" 82 different things. This approach has been used to reveal that the neural substrate for recognizing 83 faces and facial expressions is remarkably similar in the infant and the adult (de Haan and 84 Nelson, 1997, 1999), and that babies' brains change as they learn their native language (Neville, 85 Bavelier, Corina, Rauschecker, Karni, Lalwani, Braun, Clark, Jezzard, and Turner, 1998). <* 86 In addition, children with focal brain damage can be studied using neuropsychological 87 tools that involve giving young children behavioral tasks that have been shown to depend on Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 5 88 specific neural circuits (e.g., working memory, spatial planning) and observing how performance 89 varies with the particular part of the brain that is damaged (Luciana and Nelson, 1998). This 90 approach has been used in a longitudinal study of language development in children who 91 suffered focal brain damage in the first months of life and revealed the extensive capacity for 92 recovery of language functioning in these children (Bates and Roe, in press). Finally, among 93 children for whom medical reasons have required that their brains be studied, PET has revealed 94 the patterns of synaptic growth and pruning that characterize early development (Chugani, 1994). 95 (See Appendix B for a fuller discussion of technologies for studying the developing human 96 brain.) 97 98 99 100 For Chugani’s stuff, could one say “revealed metabolic patterns consonant with synaptic growth and pruning occurring in early development”* WHAT “DEVELOPS” IN EARLY BRAIN DEVELOPMENT? The development of the brain has a long trajectory, beginning within a few days after 101 conception and continuing through adolescence. The nervous system undergoes its most 102 dramatic development during the first few years of life. Yet the processes that establish the 103 structure and functioning of the brain, made possible by the developing networks of synapses 104 that interconnect nerve cells and by the progressive fine-tuning of the neurons for the roles they 105 will play within their synaptic networks, continue well into adolescence. The milestones of brain 106 development from the prenatal period up to school entry involve the development and migration 107 of brain cells to where they belong *in the brain, embellishments of nerve cells through the 108 sprouting of new axons or by expanding the dendritic surface; the formation of connections, or 109 synapses, between nerve cells; and the postnatal addition of other types of cells, notably glia. Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 6 110 Fascination with the earliest stages of brain development is understandable. During this period, 111 the spinal cord is formed, nearly all of the billions of neurons of the mature brain are produced, 112 the dual processes of neural differentiation and cell migration establish their functional roles, and 113 synaptogenesis proceeds apace. As we discussed in Chapter 3, these processes represent an 114 elaborate interplay between gene activity and the surrounding environments within and outside 115 of* the child. 116 There have been significant changes over time in the parameters of brain development 117 that have captivated public attention. Twenty years ago, we were fascinated by our ability to 118 measure developmental changes in the degree to which* neurons in different areas of the brain 119 became wrapped in the white, fatty matter—myelin—that insulates nerve cells and affects the 120 speed with which nerve impulses are transmitted from one cell to another. Myelination is, in fact, 121 affected by the young child’s behavioral experiences and nutrition, as we discuss below. Today, 122 we are focused on information--not all of it new--about the rate of synapse development, 123 particularly studies by Huttenlocher and his colleagues (detailed below) showing that there is a 124 tremendous burst of synapse formation early in life followed by a decline in synapse number 125 apparently extending into adolescence in some areas of the brain. Combined with evidence that 126 synapses that are used are retained and those not used are eliminated, there has been a frenzy of 127 concern about “using it or losing it” in the first years of life. In comparison to the brain’s wiring, 128 far less attention has been paid to the neurochemistry of early brain development. Yet, the 129 neurochemistry of brain development is essential to the brain’s capacity to learn from experience 130 and is likely to play an important role in the regulation of behavior. We discuss this critical 131 aspect of early brain development, as well. Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 7 132 133 Development of the Brain’s Wiring Diagram Brain development proceeds in overlapping phases: making the brain cell (neurulation and 134 neurogenesis), getting the cells to where they need to be (migration), growing axons and 135 dendrites—structures they need to link with other nerve cells (neuronal differentiation and 136 pathfinding), developing synapses, the* or points of communication with other cells 137 (synaptogenesis), refining those synapses (maturation and pruning), and, finally, forming the 138 supportive tissue that surrounds the nerve cells and makes for efficient communication among 139 them (gliogenesis and myelination*). 140 The brain and spinal cord arise from a set of cells on the back (dorsal part) of the 141 developing embryo called the neural plate. Two rows of rapidly dividing cells arise from the 142 plate on each side along its length and fold over centrally into the neural tube. The anterior or 143 head end of the neural tube forms a set of swollen enlargements that give rise to the various parts 144 of the brain—the forebrain containing the cerebral hemispheres, the midbrain containing 145 important pathways to and from the forebrain, and the hindbrain containing the brainstem and 146 cerebellum. The remainder of the neural tube becomes the spinal cord, peripheral nerves, and 147 certain endocrine, or hormone, glands in the body. Under the control of regulatory genes, the 148 brain cells migrate to where they belong in accord with the functions they will ultimately serve. 149 These genes provide developmental directions to particular groups of cells, which tell them what 150 to do and where to go in the embryonic brain. Both cell proliferation and migration vary from 151 area to area but these processes are generally complete by six prenatal months. The exceptions to 152 this rule include the cerebellum, whose development is more prolonged. The preceding two 153 sentences are definitely inaccurate. Both the hippocampal formation and the cerebellum Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 8 154 generate neurons postnatally. I doubt that cerebral cortical neurogenisis and migration are 155 complete by the 2nd trimester, but I can check on this. I am not sure how much specificity is 156 warranted or whether there might not be a better way to say this.* 157 Within the neural tube, the innermost cells divide repeatedly, giving rise first to the cells 158 that primarily become nerve cells, or neurons, and later giving rise to both neurons and the 159 supportive tissue components called glia. Once the nerve cells are formed and finish migrating, 160 they rapidly extend axons and dendrites and begin to form connections with each other, called 161 synapses, often over relatively long distances. These connections allow nerve cells to 162 communicate with each other. This process starts prenatally and continues well into the 163 childhood years. There is evidencein many* parts of the nervous system, that* the stability and 164 strength of these synapses is largely determined by the activity, that is the firing, of these 165 connections. The speed with which neurons communicate with each other across the synapses is 166 determined by the development of myelin, a substance that wraps itself around nerve axons. By 167 insulating the nerve cell axon, myelin increases conduction velocity. The development of myelin 168 *is a protracted developmental process extending well into the postnatal period. The rate and 169 extent of myelination is also affected by experience.* Most myelinated pathways are laid down 170 in the first ten years, but in some cases, such as the frontal cortex, myelination continues into the 171 third decade of life. The unique “wiring diagram” that this process produces in each individual 172 brain guides our thoughts, memories, feelings and behaviors. 173 Synaptic Overproduction and LossI would skip Rakic below and just cite Huttenlocher. 174 Rakic’s monkey work does not support the idea of differential timing across cortical regions and 175 was also derivative of a long line of research by others and hardly a landmark. “Over 20 years Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 9 176 ago, Huttenlocher (e.g., Huttenlocher, 1979; Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997) first showed 177 that there is a pattern to synaptogenesis in the human cerebral cortex, characterized by… 178 Beginning 20 years ago, the work of Rakic with monkeys (e.g., Rakic, Bourgeois, Eckenhoff, 179 Zecevic, and Goldman-Rakic, 1986) and Huttenlocher with humans (e.g., Huttenlocher, 1979; 180 Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997) made landmark contributions to our understanding of the 181 phenomenon of synapse development. Specifically, there is a pattern to synaptogenesis 182 characterized by the rapid proliferation and overproduction of synapses, followed by a phase of 183 synapse elimination or pruning that eventually brings the overall number of synapses down to 184 their adult levels. This process is most exhuberant during the first few years of life, although it 185 can extend well into adolescence. Within this developmental span, however, different brain 186 regions with different functions appear to develop on different time courses (see Figure 1 – 187 Nelson). Huttenlocher estimated that the peak of synaptic overproduction in the visual cortex 188 occurs about midway through the first year of life, followed by a gradual retraction until the 189 middle to the end of the preschool period, by which time the number of synapses has reached 190 adult levels. In areas of the brain that subserve audition and language, a similar although 191 somewhat later time course is observed. However, in the prefrontal cortex (the area of the brain 192 where higher-level cognition takes place), a very different picture emerges. Here the peak of 193 overproduction occurs at around one year of age, and it is not until middle to late adolescence 194 that adult numbers of synapses are obtained1. For some reason the text reversed color below 1 Many of the human findings regarding synaptic overproduction and loss were based upon measurements of the density of synapses, rather than upon measurements of the actual number of synapses. Density measures reflect both how many synapses are present and how many other things (e.g., nerve cell bodies, dendrites and axons; glial cells, and blood vessels are present in addition to synapses. The human brain adds lots of cells to the cerebral cortex postnatally (almost 2/3 of the mass of the cerebral cortex is added after birth), and this makes density estimates very difficult to interpret. Thus, evidence available to date does not allow us to determine how ubiquitous synapse overproduction and loss is in brain development generally or in humans specifically. Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 10 195 196 Scientists have pondered the purpose of synaptic overproduction and loss for a very long 197 time. One of the earliest observations was made by the turn of the century Nobel laureate 198 Spanish neuroanatomist Santiago Ramon y Cajal: 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 “I noticed that every ramification, dendritic or axonic, in the course of formation, passes through a chaotic period, so to speak, a period of trials, during which there are sent out at random experimental conductors most of which are destined to disappear. …What mysterious forces precede the appearance of the processes, promote their growth and ramification … and finally establish those protoplasmic kisses, the intercellular articulations, which seem to constitute the final ecstasy of an epic love story?” [Recollections of My Life, 1917] 207 cat visual cortex produced a greater number of synapses during development than it actually 208 retained into adulthood. Subsequent work in monkeys and cats by Hubel and Weisel and their 209 collaborators (e.g., LeVay, Hubel & Weisel, 1980) demonstrated that as the physiological 210 functioning of the visual cortex became more refined and precise, the anatomical synaptic 211 connections were also refined. Those that fit the intended pattern were retained, while those that 212 did not were eliminated. 213 A more modern formulation of the love story began with the Cragg (1975) report that the The scientists also showed that visual experience played a necessary role in this process. 214 If experience was distorted, such that one eye got much more stimulation than the other, its 215 connections were pared back less drastically than usual, and the connections with the 216 inexperienced eye were pruned more than usual. In short, the development of patterned 217 organization in the visual cortex was dependent upon visual experience and involved the 218 selective loss of connections that were not appropriate to the pattern. Synapses appear to be 219 programmed to be eliminated if they are not functionally confirmed, based upon some not fully Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 11 220 known aspects of their activity history. In general, frequently active connections, like those of 221 the more experienced eye, are more likely to survive. 222 While the data are not as complete for other sensory modalities, it is reasonably clear that 223 building the organized neural systems that guide sensory and motor development involves the 224 production of excess connections followed by some sort of pruning that leaves the system in a 225 more precisely organized pattern. Moreover, in both humans and animals, the effects of 226 experience on these systems--normal or abnormal--become increasingly irreversible over time. 227 In kittens, irreversible deficits in vision will result with deprivation lasting for only 2-3 months 228 after birth. In humans, irreversible deficits in vision are present when corrections for optical 229 conditions such as strabismus in which, due to muscular weakness, one eye deviates from and 230 cannot be brought into alignment with the other normally functioning eye, are not made by the 231 time the child reaches elementary school. The deficits become more pronounced with more 232 prolonged visual deprivation. Thus, a sensitive period exists for vision, but rather than being 233 sharply demarcated, it gradually tapers off. 234 A very useful heuristic for considering how experience becomes incorporated into the 235 developing synaptic connections of the human brain, discussed briefly in Chapter 3, has been 236 offered by Greenough and Black (1986, 1992). They distinguish between experience-expectant 237 and experience-dependent mechanisms guiding brain development. Experience-expectant 238 synaptogenesis refers to situations in which a species-typical experience (that is, something that 239 all members of a species experience barring highly aberrant conditions) plays a necessary role in 240 the developmental organization of the nervous system. Normal brain growth relies on these 241 forms of environmental exposure. For example, the visual cortex “expects” exposure to light and Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 12 242 patterned visual information, and is genetically programmed to utilize these inputs for normal 243 development. Deprivation of these ubiquitous and essential forms of environmental input can 244 permanently compromise behavioral functioning, which is why it is essential to detect and treat 245 early sensory deficits (e.g., cataracts, strabismus, auditory deficits) that interfere with the 246 detection and registering of expected experiences. 247 “Experience-dependent” synaptogenesis, in contrast, refers to encoding* new experiences 248 that occur through-out life, foster new brain growth and the refinement of existing brain 249 structures, and vary for every individual. This process optimizes the individual’s adaptation to 250 specific and possibly unique features of the environment. Whereas in experience-expectant 251 development, all brains depend on the same basic experiences to develop normally, in 252 experience-dependent development, individual differences in brain development depend upon 253 the idiosyncratic experiences that we encounter across the life span. Experience-dependent 254 development is also linked to synaptogenesis, but in this case all we know is that experience 255 triggers more plentiful connections among neurons. We do not know if this occurs through a 256 process of overproduction and pruning, or if a more continuous pattern of growth is involved. 257 Whatever the specific mechanism, experience-dependent brain development is a source of 258 enduring plasticity and of adaptability to the demands of everyday life. {“our special” conflicts 259 with the fact that most of our real information comes from animal studies}** Importantly, there 260 appears to be no abrupt transition from the utilization of experience-expectant processes to 261 utilization of experience-dependent processes of brain development. In fact, it seems likely that 262 the greater potential for recovery that characterizes young animals probably reflects the 263 availability of both mechanisms. Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 13 264 265 Postnatal Neurogenesis We now need to add the possibility of postnatal neurogenesis—the postnatal production 266 of new nerve cells—to the repertoire of mechanisms by which the human brain continues to 267 develop after the early childhood years. Prevailing knowledge about brain development, notably 268 that the adult human brain does not produce new neurons has recently been challenged by new 269 insights into adult brain development. Specifically, important forebrain regions, such as the 270 hippocampal dentate gyrus (which is involved in establishing memory for facts and relationships 271 among events and places in one’s experience), continue to receive new nerve cells into adulthood 272 in humans (e.g., Eriksson, Perfilieva, Bjork-Eriksson, Alborn, Nordborg, Peterson, & Gage, 273 1998). Recent findings in monkeys indicate that new neurons are also being formed each day and 274 migrating to areas including the prefrontal cortex, the seat of planning and decision-making 275 (Gould et al.*, 1999). Although it remains to be determined how significant this neuronal 276 addition is to the functioning of the brain, it certainly lends further support to the argument that 277 the brain continuously remodels itself. 278 The Neurochemistry of Early Brain Development 279 Once the neurons and their* synapses, axons, and dendrites have been established, the sending 280 and receiving of messages in the nervous system depends on chemical messengers. A number of 281 these chemical messengers affect gene expression in nerve cells in ways that have long-lasting 282 effects on how nerves grow, respond to stimulation, and function. They are thus intimately 283 involved in the growth and development of the nervous system and in neural plasticity. The past 284 two decades have seen an explosion of information about these chemical messengers. In addition 285 to the classic neurotransmitters, over 60 other peptide and steroid molecules have been identified Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 14 286 that have direct effects on the brain. Currently there is little from this field that we can apply 287 directly to human development with confidence. However, because the study of neurochemistry 288 promises to revolutionize the way we think about the nervous system, a brief overview of some 289 core ideas from this work is warranted. 290 Chemical messengers that affect the brain operate through receptors, most of which are located 291 in the dendrites and synapses of nerve cells. Like locks and keys, the physical structure of the 292 messenger (the key) has to fit the physical structure of the receptor (the lock) for the chemical 293 messenger to have any effect on the nerve cell. Receptors are specific. They typically recognize 294 or bind with only one natural* molecule. For many years, this type of specificity gave rise to the 295 hope that science would be able to link specific neurochemicals to specific behaviors, allowing 296 highly focused manipulations of behavior through pharmacological intervention (i.e., drug 297 therapy). However, despite what filters its way into the popular press (e.g., low serotonin levels 298 cause aggression), the way the biochemistry of the brain operates is vastly more complex than 299 one chemical-one behavior. For example,It says above that receptors only bind with one 300 molecule. Conflict with point below.*** it now appears that many of the chemicals that affect 301 brain function are able to unlock several different receptors. This allows the same (or quite 302 similar) chemical to have different functions and to play a role in multiple (albeit often related) 303 behavioral systems. 304 The brain is also able to alter its sensitivity to a chemical messenger by changing the 305 presence, conformation (structure), and availability of the chemical’s receptors. Often receptor 306 changes reflect the history of the nerve cell’s experience with its neurochemical. High levels of 307 the chemical operating on the receptor frequently result in a decrease in the nerve’s receptors for Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 15 308 that chemical (a process called down regulation); sometimes a dearth of a chemical important in 309 a nerve’s functioning results in an increase in receptor number (i.e., up-regulation). Up- and 310 down-regulation take* place over hours and days, partially explaining why some psychoactive 311 drugs take time before they begin to influence behavior and why some drugs, with time, need to 312 be taken in higher and higher dosages to have the same effects. Some of these shifts in chemical 313 messenger/receptor systems appear to be relatively permanent, perhaps especially those that 314 occur during periods of rapid development; others are more transient, reflecting the normal 315 turnover (production, decline, replacement) of receptors. This complexity is hardly user-friendly 316 for those who are trying to decipher the mysteries of the brain, but it does allow the brain to be 317 highly plastic, toning its functioning in highly nuanced ways, often quite rapidly, without the 318 need to prune or grow dendrites or synapses. 319 Neurochemical/receptor systems also lie at the heart of how the brain alters its physical 320 structure. A variety of different nerve growth factors (i.e., chemicals that play a role in the 321 growth of dendrites and synapses) have been identified. These growth factors are present in 322 different quantities and locations at different points in development of the brain, regulated by 323 genes involved in normal brain development. They also change in their concentration in response 324 to nerve damage, playing a role in the brain’s attempts to adapt to and restore functioning 325 following trauma. Receptor systems also play critical roles in both experience-dependent and 326 experience-expectant neural plasticity. The NMDA receptor is one receptor, but not the only one, 327 which plays a role in neural plasticity. It appears* to support learning by helping to foster what is 328 termed “long-term potentiation”. Long-term potentiation, a memory “model” involving increased 329 synaptic strength,* is brought about by* sustained rapid* activity in the neural circuits involved Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 16 330 in newly acquired information, analagous* to repeating a new phone number in order to 331 memorize it. It also appears that at critical points in the development of neural systems there is 332 sometimes an increase in NMDA receptors. This increase seems to “open the window” for the 333 development of that neural system, allowing stimulation to have large effects, with the window 334 closing when the number of NMDA receptors decreases. 335 Changes in chemical messenger systems and their receptors tend to tone the nervous 336 system, altering sensitivity to stimuli and probabilities of responses, rather than necessarily 337 causing particular behaviors. The following thought experiment, provides a good example. You 338 have been on a low calorie diet (and have stuck to it) for several weeks. Numerous 339 neurochemical changes in your brain have been set into motion by this starvation. All of these 340 changes do not mean that you will eat that luscious steak the waiter just set in front of you (the 341 fact that you are dieting, are a vegetarian, and did not order the steak will hopefully rule the day). 342 But the myriad of neurochemical changes in your brain set into motion by starvation will 343 probably make you more sensitive to how good the steak smells, make you salivate more, make 344 you remember that steak for a long time, and so on-- all changes orchestrated to help increase the 345 probability that you will break down and eat the steak that your body might, in fact, “need”. As 346 this thought experiment indicates, the behavioral impact of changes in neurochemistry are 347 dependent on the context and the individual’s history. Like our temperaments, they tend to 348 orchestrate a bias or propensity to respond in particular ways rather than rigidly determine that a 349 behavior will always be expressed. A number of researchers believe that in order to understand 350 the neural bases of temperament and emotions, we will need to understand the genetic and Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 17 351 experiential processes that regulate these complex neurochemical systems of the brain 352 throughout development. 353 Characteristically, the neurochemical systems of the brain are open both to input from 354 the environment and to events occurring in the body below the neck. There is increasing animal 355 evidence that the environment plays a role in regulating aspects of brain neurochemistry. For 356 example, the licking and grooming that the mother rat does of her pups (infant rats) appears to 357 enhance the production of serotonin and thyroid hormone, both important in the neurochemistry 358 of brain development. There is also increasing evidence that elements of early caregiving may 359 help modulate the neurochemicals involved in pain and distress. Thus, the fats and sugars in 360 breast milk appear to stimulate taste receptors linked to central opioid (natural pain killer) 361 pathways, stimulating mild analgesia. Similarly, tactile stimulation of the mouth appears to 362 operate through non-opioid pain-killing neurochemistry affecting brain pathways controlling 363 distress. Some of these effects have been demonstrated in human infants. The evidence that the 364 regulation of neuroactive chemical systems extends into basic caregiving activities is exciting, 365 even though much of it still has been demonstrated only in animals. This evidence promises to 366 help explain how alterations in the environment early in life may have wide-ranging effects on 367 brain development and may alter, probabilistically, patterns of behavioral responding for 368 children with different rearing histories. 369 370 HOW IS THE BRAIN AFFECTED BY EARLY EXPERIENCE? 371 Our account of early brain development emphasizes the ways in which the nervous 372 system is designed to recruit and incorporate experience into its developing architecture and Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 18 373 neurochemistry. Normal experience (e.g., good nutrition, patterned visual information) supports 374 normal brain development and abnormal experience (e.g., prenatal alcohol exposure, occluded 375 vision) can cause abnormal neural and behavioral development (Black, Jones, Nelson, & 376 Greenough, 1998). As such, plasticity is a double-edged sword that leads to both adaptation and 377 vulnerability. We have seen that the process of synaptic overproduction and loss is dependent 378 upon environmental information, although our evidence is largely restricted to sensory systems. 379 Similarly, the brain’s neurochemistry is exquisitely sensitive to behavioral and environmental 380 stimuli. We are, however, far from linking specific types or amounts of experience to the 381 developing structure or neurochemistry of the immature human brain, and, conversely, from 382 understanding how early brain development affects the ways in which young children process 383 the abundance of information and experiences that their environments present to them. Answers 384 to questions about when during development particular experiences must occur and when, in 385 fact, timing is important and when not also lie, to a large extent, beyond the boundaries of 386 current knowledge. Research on the developing brain can, nevertheless, provide a framework for 387 considering the effects of early experience on development more generally. The questions that 388 have been asked by neuroscientists have their parallels in research on behavioral development, as 389 we will discuss in the following chapters. 390 Two issues have played pivotal roles in guiding scientific inquiry about early experience 391 and the brain. The first issue concerns the nature of early experiences. Those who raise and work 392 with young children are deeply concerned about whether they are providing them with the 393 “right” experiences and protecting them from “harmful” ones. What damage is done by exposure 394 to inappropriate experiences, and how reversible is this damage? What degree of enhancement Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 19 395 can be achieved by exposure to enriched experiences, and how long do beneficial effects last? 396 We actually know much more about the negative consequences for brain development of 397 harmful environments than we do about the benefits of advantageous environments. We also 398 know relatively more about the effects of pre- and perinatal environments on the developing 399 nervous system than we do about environmental influences after the first few months of life. 400 The second issue concerns the timing of experience and is often expressed in terms of 401 critical or sensitive periods. Much of the contemporary discussion of the importance of the first 402 three years of life is framed in the terminology of sensitive periods. But, does it really matter 403 when the child is exposed to particular experiences? Do specific experiences need to occur 404 during specific windows of time in order for the brain to develop normally? Can the brain 405 recover or compensate when critical experiences are missed? In addition to the examples 406 regarding the visual system, described above, there are some very dramatic instances of timing 407 effects. For example, an injury to the rat’s cortex on the first day after birth causes more 408 ultimate* damage to brain tissue and greater loss of normal behavioral functioning than a similar 409 injury on day 5 (add cite this is Bryan Kolb* ). The presence of testosterone in the third 410 trimester of human fetal development organizes the physiological characteristics of brain regions 411 such as the hypothalamus in the male direction such that release of hormones that govern sexual 412 and reproductive functions follows the tonic, non-cyclic pattern seen in the post-adolescent male 413 (add cite). Prior to or after this critical period of sensitivity to the hormone, the presence of 414 testosterone does not have the same* organizing effects upon brain structure and physiology. The 415 zebra finch must be exposed to the father’s song between X and Y or it will fail to develop the 416 capacity to produce the full* adult song specific to its species (add cite). Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 20 417 In developmental science, the term “sensitive periods” is preferred to “critical periods” 418 because it implies less rigidity in the nature and timing of formative early experiences 419 (Immelman & Suomi, 1981). Sensitive periods can be defined as unique episodes in 420 development when specific structures or functions become especially susceptible to particular 421 experiences in ways that alter their future structure or function (Bornstein, 1989; Thompson, in 422 press). This susceptibility can operate in two ways: First, certain early experiences uniquely 423 prepare the young children for the future by establishing certain capabilities at a time when 424 development is most plastic and responsive to stimulation. Second, the young child is highly 425 vulnerable to the absence of these essential experiences, and the result may be permanent risk of 426 dysfunction. In fact, it is extraordinarily difficult to study issues of timing in human development 427 given that it is profoundly unethical to deprive children of needed experiences in order to 428 “introduce” them at different developmental stages. We are thus dependent upon animal studies 429 that are generating fascinating evidence of timing effects (see, for example, Bornstein, 1989; 430 Knudsen, 1999) but have dubious translations to humans and on so called “experiments of 431 nature”, such as prenatal exposures that occur at different points in fetal development (which we 432 discuss below) and research on children with sensory deficits, as in the case of deaf children who 433 are not exposed to normal language inputs (which we discuss in Chapter 8). 434 Within these parameters, it is well known that a variety of environmental factors play a 435 significant role in modulating early brain development. Our greatest insights have come from 436 research on the detrimental consequences of stress and of early biologic insults and deprivations. 437 We have also learned a great deal from efforts to trace the neurobiological consequences of 438 prematurity. Following a brief overview of the research that stimulated excitement about the Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 21 439 brain’s receptivity to environmental influence, we turn to the research on biologic insults, stress 440 and prematurity. We close by highlighting several cross-cutting issues, including a brief reprise 441 on timing and comments on the confluence of adverse inputs, individual variability in responses, 442 common pathways of influence, and modifiability and recovery. 443 444 The Contribution of Environmental Variation Documented differences in the brains and behaviors of animals that have experienced 445 markedly discrepant early environments has emerged from the laboratory of Greenough and his 446 colleagues (Black and Greenough, 1998; Black et al., 1998;* Greenough and Black, 1992; ). 447 Rats, not babies, are the subjects of study. They are either housed from infancy or placed as 448 adults in cages that vary in the degree of stimulation they offer to the rat. The “complex” cages 449 contain play objects and other animals. Animals reared or housed in these cages outperform rats 450 raised alone or placed in typically barren laboratory cages on a variety of learning and problem- 451 solving tasks (e.g., making their way through a maze, finding a hidden platform in a pool of 452 water, learning a relationship between a cue such as a light and a reward, etc.*). The brains 453 of the rats reared in the complex environments also showed more mature synaptic structure, 454 more dendritic spines, larger neuronal dendritic fields, more synapses per neuron, more 455 supportive glial tissue* and increased capillary branching that increases blood volume and 456 oxygen supply to the brain*. Importantly, these effects do not appear to be characterized by a 457 critical period. The indicators of both superior performance and more developed brains 458 characterized the rats exposed to the complex environments as adults, as well as those reared in 459 these environments, although the effects occured more rapidly and to a greater degree in the Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 22 460 younger animals*. Thus, both early and later exposure to greater environmental stimulation had 461 beneficial consequences. 462 Studies of complex environments in rats have also revealed the role that such 463 environments can play in processes of recovery. For example, the detrimental behavioral effects 464 of prenatal exposure to low to moderate levels of alcohol in rats (e.g., ataxia and impairments in 465 learning spatial tasks) can be attenuated by raising the animals in a complex environment 466 (Hannigan et al., 1993). A program of forced motor skill training in alcohol exposed rats nearly 467 eliminated motor dysfunction and it subsequently increased synapse number in their cerebellar 468 cortex (Klintsova et al., 1997, 1998*). Finally, increasing the complexity of the environment 469 before and/or after brain damage in developing and adult rats enhances recovery from the 470 impairments produced by damage to various brain areas, probably through mechanisms that 471 involve the development of alternative strategies rather than the direct recovery of lost functions 472 (see Jones et al., 1998*). 473 Two additional facets of this research are important to highlight, in part because they 474 have been the subject of serious misinterpretation. First, these findings bear more directly on 475 issues of deprivation than of enrichment. In fact, the complex cages in Greenough’s research 476 presented the rats with fewer challenges and learning opportunities than rats would typically* 477 confront in their natural wild habitats.. {The preceding statement is untrue or at least not 478 demonstrated. While we have not looked at long intervals, we see very little decline of the effects 479 on synapses over periods equivalent to the length of the exposure. Glial effects diminish over 480 that same time period. The following sentence is OK.} Second, while long-term neuron and 481 synapse studies have not been conducted, the effects of exposure to a complex environment on Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 23 482 learning ability diminish over time if the rats are removed from the environment.* The 483 intervention provided by the complex cages thus functioned more like a tetanus shot that requires 484 boosters than the small*pox vaccine that provides a permanent inoculation against disease. As 485 we shall see later, most of the risks for which we design early interventions for humans act more 486 like tetanus than small pox. 487 Most of the research that we can turn to for evidence of experience-brain connections 488 does not concern improvements upon deprivation, as with the complex environment* rats, but 489 rather explores the detrimental consequences of harmful influences. We now turn to the research 490 on biologic insults and the developing brain. 491 {I still think something is missing here and that we have thrown out the baby with the 492 bashing (of zero to 3). The foregoing sections bend over backwards to say that bad things can 493 happen but that we know nothing about positive environmental qualities beyond the sub-neutral. 494 I disagree, as I have at several points in our meetings. I believe that, despite falling short of the 495 real environment, the complex environment studies point to a continuum that implies positive 496 effects of a better than average environment. I believe that the studies of verbal interactions in 497 the home and subsequent child performance similarly imply the existence of a multidimensional 498 continuum with anchor points that could be described as “impoverished” and “enriched” with 499 meaningful implications for cognitive, linguistic and social development. And I think it is a 500 mistake to imply that, as long as some minimal threshold exceeded by most kids is reached, we 501 “don’t have to worry.” Maybe it’s just that I like to take some credit for my outstanding 502 daughter…. So erase this and say whatever you wish, but remember that I objected.} 503 Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 24 504 505 Early Biologic Insults and the Developing Brain Research on early biologic insults provides fundamental insights into the vulnerability 506 and resilience of the developing central nervous system. This area of research also offers a 507 compelling illustration that plasticity cuts both ways, leaving the developing fetus and young 508 child simultaneously vulnerable to harm and receptive to positive influences. Finally, this 509 research suggests that the current emphasis on the birth-to-three years may have unwittingly 510 bypassed the stage of development, namely the prenatal period, when damaging environmental 511 conditions may have among the most devastating effects on development and, consequently, 512 when preventive efforts may have the greatest benefits. 513 Environmental factors that play a significant role in modulating prenatal and early 514 postnatal brain development include substances and circumstances that are necessary for normal 515 brain development, as well as exposures to chemicals, diseases, and stressors that are toxic or 516 disruptive. Tables 1 and 2 note some that are beneficial or detrimental, respectively. The factors 517 listed are by no means exhaustive. They are examples selected on the basis of clinical 518 importance, availability of basic research on brain effects, and/or existence of relevant clinical 519 studies of human infants. In this section, we consider a few of these biologic insults in more 520 detail: and an infectious disease (rubella); a developmental neurotoxin (alcohol), and a nutrient 521 deficiency (iron). 522 Infectious Diseases 523 Rubella is a classic example of an infectious disease that causes harm in utero. Exposure to 524 rubella early in prenatal development affects the organs (e.g., eyes, ears) that are developing at the 525 time if the virus crosses the placental barrier. Because the development of organs is largely Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 25 526 compete by the end of the first trimester, fetal development during the second and third trimesters 527 of pregnancy is largely protected from the negative effects of the rubella virus. 528 The rubella story demonstrates how long it has often taken to recognize that a particular 529 condition or exposure can put the fetus or child at risk. It was widely believed that few diseases 530 were as benign as rubella, or German measles, until 1942 when the first report of the devastating 531 effects of maternal rubella during pregnancy was published {1998}. One of the puzzles is why the 532 medical community did not figure out the connection between maternal rubella and congenital 533 malformations earlier. Some qualities of rubella which exist in other conditions as well, made it 534 difficult to make the connection {1981}. It is not always clear that a fetus has been exposed to a 535 particular infectious illness or toxic agent during pregnancy. In the case of rubella, there are many 536 causes of fever, rash, and other symptoms that are variably seen. To complicate matters further, 537 effects on the developing fetus or child may also be quite variable. For instance, rubella may affect 538 the fetus’ eyes, ears, brain, and/or heart, among other organs. Furthermore, the very idea that the 539 fetus could be vulnerable to harm was novel before the rubella syndrome was accepted. We now 540 know this to be true for many conditions, including BETSY – PLZ Add a couple of examples. 541 The rubella story also illustrates a triumph in prevention. As better methods of diagnosing 542 rubella became available in the 1960s {1983}, there was more certainty about which rashes and 543 nonspecific symptoms in early pregnancy were due to rubella and which were not. Parents then 544 had the option of terminating the pregnancy to prevent the birth of a devastated child. With the 545 advent of vaccination, however, a far better solution is now available. Public health policy 546 requiring universal immunization against rubella has virtually eliminated the problem of the 547 congenital rubella babies in the US. Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 26 548 Developmental Neurotoxins 549 Substances such as drugs and chemicals that are damaging to the developing nervous 550 system are known as developmental neurotoxins. Table 2 indicates a number of these agents. 551 Their effects on brain and behavior have been summarized in several comprehensive volumes 552 {1616,1597}, as well as in hundreds of original research reports. We use prenatal alcohol 553 exposure as an example of this class of early biologic insult. The effects of prenatal alcohol have 554 been studied extensively, and the current state of knowledge was recently considered in depth in 555 another Institute of Medicine report {1967}. Major points related to questions of early 556 brain/behavior development are highlighted here. 557 The adverse effects of prenatal alcohol exposure are now so widely known and accepted, 558 that it is hard to believe that the first report was issued only 30 years ago. Fetal Alcohol 559 Syndrome was first described in the English-language medical literature in 1973 {2000}. 560 Maternal alcohol consumption during pregnancy can lead to facial deformities, loss of neurons, 561 and severe neurobehavioral impairment, among other problems. It is the most common cause of 562 mental retardation and its consequences appear to persist throughout life 563 {1967,1958,1959,1963,1474,1426} [Connor & Streissguth, 1996]. Survey data collected by 564 the Centers for Disease Control show that the incidence of drinking at levels that put the fetus at 565 risk for neurobehavioral impairment was 3.5% in 1995 (the most recent year for which data are 566 available), with binge drinking the predominant pattern (87% of the cases) {1960}. The 567 proportion of women who consume alcohol during pregnancy has decreased since the mid 568 1980’s (Serudula et. al., 1991), although much of the decline is due to changed habits of light 569 drinkers. Women who drink heavily, who pose the greatest risk to the fetus, appear to be more Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 27 570 resistant to prevention efforts. Heavy drinking, and thus the incidence of FAS, is much higher 571 among African-Americans than among European-Americans (Abel, 1995; Faden et al., 1997), 572 and also appears to be high among American Indian populations ( Duimstra et al., 1993). 573 Fetal Alcohol Syndrome (FAS) is the most severe form of prenatal alcohol effects. 574 Defined by a specific pattern of facial and other physical deformities accompanied by growth 575 retardation, FAS identifies a relatively small proportion of children prenatally affected by 576 alcohol. The Institute of Medicine (1996) recently suggested that the term “alcohol-related 577 neurodevelopmental disorder” (ARND) be used to focus specifically on brain dysfunctions in the 578 presence of significant prenatal alcohol exposure but without physical deformities. FAS is 579 estimated to occur at a rate of 1-3 per thousand live births; ARND is likely to be at least ten 580 times more prevalent. Brain dysfunctions in alcohol-exposed children without FAS are often as 581 severe as those in children with FAS. 582 A variety of neurobehavioral changes have been observed in children exposed to alcohol 583 prenatally. These effects range from problems with attention and memory to poor motor 584 coordination to problems with problem-solving and abstract thinking. Infants and toddlers may 585 be delayed in reaching important milestones, have difficulty “tuning out” excess sensory stimuli, 586 and often are hyperactive. About half of all individuals with FAS are mentally retarded (IQ < 587 70). Both severely and more mildly affected children demonstrate slower information processing 588 and longer reaction times, and appear to have specific problems with performance in arithmetic 589 {1833,1874}. These effects have been documented through the early adolescent years and into 590 adulthood. Such results demonstrate the importance of assessing functions other than IQ. In fact, 591 these more sensitive measures often detect effects of early biologic insults in the absence of IQ Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 28 592 differences, and behavioral disturbances may create more functional impairment than a lower IQ. 593 In addition, the more specific and sensitive measures may indicate specific and differing effects 594 of various developmental neurotoxins {1875}. 595 The importance of considering timing (when a condition occurs during development), 596 severity (degree or dose), and chronicity (how long it lasts) in attempting to understand the 597 effects of early biologic insults is well illustrated by prenatal alcohol exposure. In general, the 598 prenatal period appears to be distinguished by its sensitivity to a large array of harmful 599 conditions. But, even within the prenatal period, timing matters. For instance, alcohol exposure 600 early in gestation has different effects on the developing brain from similar exposure later on. 601 Case reports from autopsies and more recently, neuroimaging studies {1992,1904,1906} give an 602 indication of central nervous system effects in the human. However, animal models—with 603 experimental manipulation of alcohol exposure and direct examination of brain tissue—continue 604 to provide crucial information. In the mouse, for example, exposure to alcohol on days 7 and 8 of 605 gestation not only results in the typical facial deformities of FAS but also brain anomalies, such 606 as small size overall and deficiencies in cerebral hemispheres, striatum, olfactory bulbs, limbic 607 structures, the corpus callosum, and lateral ventricles. Exposure later in gestation generally does 608 not produce such gross structural malformations but nonetheless kills nerve cells, interferes with 609 synaptogenesis, formation of myelin, and other biochemical processes, including reduction of 610 NMDA receptor binding in the hippocampus. 611 Research with humans also shows that the timing of prenatal alcohol exposure has 612 differential effects {1967, 1958,1959,1963,1426,1474} [same problem with Connor & 613 Streissguth]. The unusual facial features of fetal alcohol syndrome in the human infant (e.g., lowEarly Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 29 614 set ears, short philtrum, cleft palate, cleft lip) appear to be due to heavy exposure early on, in the 615 first trimester, when the structures that come together to form the face are developing. Fetal 616 exposure to alcohol during the second and especially the third trimester of pregnancy appears to 617 be a time of particular vulnerability for the impaired neurobehavioral development discussed 618 above, although some data suggest that these effects extend throughout pregnancy. Dividing cells 619 appear to be particularly sensitive to the toxic effects of alcohol, and hence a period during 620 which extensive neurogenesis occurs would be a time of acute sensitivity to alcohol insult. The 621 cognitive effects associated with exposure to alcohol later in pregnancy, for example, may be 622 associated with the high level of neuronal cell division in pertinent parts of the brain that occurs 623 during the third trimester. 624 The severity of exposure is another important factor in understanding ill effects, perhaps 625 as important as the timing. For prenatal alcohol exposure, greater exposures are associated with 626 worse effects. In addition, episodic binge drinking appears to be more harmful to the developing 627 brain than equivalent levels of alcohol consumed gradually. Experimental animal studies indicate 628 that ingestion of a given dose of alcohol over a short period of time generates a greater peak 629 blood alcohol concentration than the same dose ingested over several days {1961}. Thus, the 630 developing fetus is actually exposed to a higher level of alcohol in binge drinking, and has been 631 found in animal research to experience greater neuronal {1961} and behavioral impairment 632 {1962). In humans, binge drinking is more of a problem than usually recognized, because 633 “moderate” drinkers, who consume 1-2 drinks per week on average, in fact, tend to concentrate 634 their drinking on 1-2 days per week, thus drinking 4-6 drinks {numbers don’t add}* per occasion 635 {1834}. When juxtaposed with evidence on the timing of alcohol exposure, the detrimental Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 30 636 effects of binge drinking suggest that any bouts of drinking during pregnancy run the risk of 637 damaging some aspect of the developing brain. 638 Chronicity is another important factor in understanding the effects of early biologic 639 insults. In the case of prenatal alcohol exposure, it appears that the effects on the fetus worsen 640 with successive pregnancies. Specifically, older mothers who are moderate-to-heavy drinkers are 641 at higher risk for having an affected offspring (1413). This may be due to reduced ability to 642 metabolize alcohol by women who have been drinking heavily for several years {1959,1874}. In 643 the case of alcohol exposure, chronicity should thus be thought of as both a within- and between- 644 pregnancy dimension of risk. 645 Research on early biologic insults has also yielded information on modifiability or brain 646 plasticity. Environmental interventions to reduce the effects of alcohol exposure (other than 647 specific treatment of a toxin or deficiency, e.g. iron replacement for iron deficiency) have been 648 studied in only a few conditions. Prenatal alcohol exposure is perhaps the best-researched in 649 recent years. In animal models, a variety of interventions has been shown to ameliorate some of 650 the central nervous system effects of alcohol {690,1612,1987}. Effective interventions include 651 motor training, procedures that enhance maternal caregiving behaviors, and a postweaning 652 environment that is physically and socially stimulating. However, one should not conclude that 653 the process is trivial. For instance, getting a rat to do motor training may require quite heroic 654 efforts on the part of the investigator, and it is not clear that the intervention brings the brain and 655 behavior of exposed animals fully back to the levels of animals who never experienced the 656 biologic insult. As common sense would suggest, protecting the developing brain from early Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 31 657 biologic insults is a more desirable and effective strategy than trying to correct the deficits once 658 they have occurred. 659 Malnutrition \ 660 Prenatally and postnatally, nutritional adequacy is essential for optimal brain 661 development and function (see Georgieff and Rao {1985}, Morgan & Gibson, 1991; for recent 662 reviews) because of the growing brain’s reliance on folic acid, iron, vitamins, and other nutrients. 663 The effects of generalized undernutrition (lack of sufficient protein, energy, and other nutrients) 664 on the developing brain have been studied extensively over several decades 665 {1598,1975,509,1980}. Although adequate nutrient intake is important throughout life, certain 666 nutrients have a more profound effect on the developing brain than others. The timing of nutrient 667 supplementation or deficiency is also important. For example, nutritional deprivation in the 668 second trimester of pregnancy has been shown to result in deficient numbers of neurons, whereas 669 deprivation in the third trimester affects numbers of glial cells and the maturation of neurons 670 (e.g., Dickerson, 1981). Postnatal nutrition also appears to show timing effects, with the first 671 two to three years of life being an especially vulnerable time for sustaining serious impairments 672 in brain growth and experiencing related behavioral consequences. The earlier the malnutrition 673 occurs, the greater the reduction in brain size, and the longer the malnutrition continues, the 674 greater the effect on the brain (Moran & Winick, 1985; Winick, 1976). Moreover, a nutrient that 675 is essential for normal development at one time of life may be superfluous or even toxic at 676 another time. 677 678 Iron deficiency is our nutritional example of how early insults affect brain development, as there has been a recent burst of relevant research. Iron deficiency is probably the world's most Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 32 679 common single nutrient disorder. Approximately 20-25% of babies worldwide has iron- 680 deficiency anemia, and a much higher proportion has iron deficiency without anemia 681 {402,1997}. The latter is common even in countries where public health interventions have 682 reduced anemia. In the U.S., for instance, the prevalence of iron-deficiency anemia has decreased 683 dramatically {1547}, due to fortification of infant formula and cereal and increased breast 684 feeding, among other factors. However, poor and minority children are still at considerable risk 685 for iron deficiency with or without anemia {1425}. Non-poor white toddlers had the lowest 686 prevalence of iron deficiency (about 3%) in a recent U.S. national survey, while Mexican- 687 American toddlers were at highest risk regardless of economic status, affecting approximately 688 18% and 12% of poor and non-poor Mexican-American children, respectively {1425}. 689 Altered behavior and development are among the most worrisome concerns about iron 690 deficiency in infancy. Iron-deficient anemic infants generally test lower in mental and motor 691 development {183,184,185,186,89,164,526,1078}. Other behavioral differences, such as 692 increased fearfulness, fatigue, and wariness have also been noted {184,89,1078,168,92,218,805}. 693 Although one study reported that test scores improved with a full course of iron treatment {164}, 694 the other available studies found that a majority of infants with iron deficiency anemia continued 695 to have lower developmental test scores {89,1078,186,400} despite iron therapy for 2-6 months 696 and correction of anemia. Other behavioral differences were also still observed {805}. 697 Differences thus appear to persist. 698 At early school age, children who were anemic as infants continue to have lower test 699 scores than their peers who did not experience anemia {165,182,191,192,193}. A comprehensive 700 follow-up at the transition to adolescence {1544} found that children who had been treated for Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 33 701 severe, chronic iron deficiency in infancy still scored lower on measures of mental and motor 702 functioning, specifically in arithmetic achievement and written expression, motor functioning, 703 and some specific cognitive processes such as spatial memory and selective recall. They were 704 also more likely to have repeated a grade. Parents and teachers rated the formerly anemic 705 children as showing more anxiety/depression, social problems, and attention problems. In a 706 different, population-based study {1354}, children who were anemic in infancy (presumably due 707 to iron deficiency) were at increased risk for mild/moderate mental retardation at 10 years of age. 708 Thus, severe, chronic iron deficiency in infancy identifies children who continue to be at 709 developmental and behavioral risk more than 10 years later. 710 Basic science research and animal studies indicate some possible mechanisms for such 711 behavioral and developmental differences. Iron is required for enzymes that regulate central 712 nervous system cell division, neurotransmitter synthesis (especially dopamine), myelination, and 713 oxidative metabolism (reviewed in {1985}). Maximal transport of iron into the brain corresponds 714 with the brain growth spurt, and iron deficiency during this period results in a deficit of brain 715 iron in animal models. These observations suggest that the developing brain may be particularly 716 vulnerable to the effects of this nutrient deficiency. Conversely, free or excess iron is toxic to 717 cell membranes and may contribute to neuronal damage following a brain injury. 718 New studies that utilize neurophysiologic and electrophysiologic methods are now providing data 719 on iron-deficient human infants and demonstrating close links to results in animal models. In one 720 such study {1039}, 6-month-old infants with iron deficiency anemia had slower nerve 721 conduction in the auditory pathway. Differences in nerve conduction velocity between anemic 722 and nonanemic infants increased over the following year despite iron therapy. A disruption or Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 34 723 defect in myelination was considered to be a promising explanation given that brain iron is 724 required for myelination, young iron-deficient animals have been noted to be hypomyelinated, 725 and the auditory system is rapidly myelinating in the first two years after birth in the human 726 infant (reviewed in {1039}). 727 The hippocampus, which controls recognition memory among other functions, also 728 appears to be vulnerable to early iron deficiency {1912,1451}. In animal models, iron deficiency 729 results in markedly reduced neuronal metabolism (as indicated by cytochrome coxidase activity) 730 in all subareas of the hippocampus and other regions involved in higher cognitive functions 731 {1912}. Preliminary evidence from a study of infants of diabetic mothers (who are at risk for 732 lower levels of iron in the liver, heart, and brain), using electrophysiologic techniques, has 733 revealed seriously impaired recognition memory despite normal iron status at 6-8 months of age 734 {1986}. These findings are consistent with a hippocampally-based memory deficit tied to iron 735 deficiency. Disruptions in recognition memory, in turn, may be a subtle early effect that could 736 contribute to the later learning disabilities observed in iron-deficient children. 737 Prematurity and Early Brain Development 738 One of the true marvels of human brain development is that an infant can be born 739 prematurely in the early part of the third trimester and not only survive, but achieve something 740 resembling his or her genetic {???????!!!}* potential in mental and motor behavior. Highly 741 sophisticated intensive care techniques have improved survival rates of premature infants, 742 although the borders of viability (approximately 24 weeks gestation) have not changed since 743 1980 (Richardson, et al. 1998). Greater than 95% of infants born after 28 weeks gestation and 744 greater than 50% of infants born at 24-28 weeks survive (Hack, et al. 1991). At the very borders Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 35 745 of viability (22-24 weeks gestation), mortality remains high and of the infants that survive, a 746 high percentage have sustained damage to developing neurologic structures and have significant 747 neurologic morbidity (Allen, et al. 1993). Nevertheless, it is safe to say that over the past decade 748 neonatology has begun to concern itself less with survival (mortality) and more with outcome 749 (morbidity) (Richardson, et al. 1998). 750 It is useful to consider preterm infants as fetuses who develop in extrauterine settings at 751 the time when their brains are growing more rapidly than at any other time in their life (Als, 752 1997; McClellan, 1972). Prematurity has two main negative effects on brain development. First, 753 premature birth predisposes the infant to pathologic events that directly injure the brain. These 754 events can be thought of as damage committed by factors that the human at this gestation would 755 not normally be exposed to. These can be as seemingly benign as the wrong mixture of nutrients 756 to more obvious neuropathologies such as intracranial hemorrhage. Second, premature birth 757 interrupts the normal process of intrauterine brain development by denying it of expected 758 intrauterine stimuli and factors important for growth (e.g. nutrients such as docosohexaenoic 759 acid). One can consider this to be damage due to omission of factors which are critical for normal 760 development. Ultimately, the morbidity seen at any gestational age will be the result of the 761 combination of the number and severity of exposure to both types of factors. 762 The first principle* of assessing the effect of prematurity on neurologic outcome is to 763 note that the child’s general developmental status and intelligence scores decrease with 764 reductions in gestational age (Saigal, et al. 1991). Thus the 24 weeker is at greater risk than the 765 26 weeker who in turn is at higher risk than the 28 weeker. The 24 weeker not only has a less 766 “complete” brain than the 26 weeker, he is also far more prone to intracranial hemorrhage, Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 36 767 hypoglycemia, and postnatal malnutrition, all of which adversely affect his more “primitive” 768 brain. The superimposition of lower socio-economic status on prematurity causes a further 769 decrement in developmental status and intelligence at each successively lower gestational age 770 (Saigal, et al. 1991). 771 Insults Due to Prematurity 772 The neonatal outcome literature is replete with studies assessing the effects of intracranial 773 hemorrhage (Papile, et al. 1983), periventricular leucomalacia (Feldman, et al. 1990, Lowe and 774 Papile 1990), hypoglyecmia (Duvanel, et al. 1999), and malnutrition (Georgieff, et al. 1985, 775 Georgieff, et al. 1989, Hack and Breslau 1986) on head growth and developmental outcome. 776 Besides gestational age and SES, the next most important factor in assessing risk of adverse 777 neurologic outcomes is the degree of illness of the infant during the newborn period. Infants 778 whose overall physiology is more compromised are more developmentally delayed at 2 years 779 and appear to be at greater risk of prefrontal deficits at 8 years of age (Brazy, et al., 1991; 780 Luciana, et al., 1999). 781 Intracranial hemorrhage (also known as intraventricular hemorrhage or IVH) is the most 782 extensively studied noxious event that affects the premature infant’s brain. This is likely due to 783 the fact that IVH is easily visualized by cranial ultrasonography and quantifiable into Grades I 784 (least severe) to IV (most severe). Approximately 20% of infants between 28 and 34 weeks 785 gestation have IVH, with the ???*majority (>60%) rated as Grade I or II. In contrast, 60% of 24 786 to 28 week infants have IVH and their hemorrhages tend to be the more severe Grade III and 787 Grade IV varieties. Accordingly, the risk of major handicaps, both motor and cognitive, is 788 increased. Infants with lower grade hemorrhages do not appear to be at any greater risk of major Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 37 789 handicap (cerebral palsy or mental retardation) than infants who did not bleed (Papile, et al. 790 1983), although they are at higher risk of minor handicaps (e.g., behavior problems, attention 791 problems, memory deficits) (Lowe and Papile 1990, Ross, et al. 1996). 792 Omission of Factors Important for Normal Brain Development 793 A premature infant with a benign neonatal course nevertheless remains at increased risk 794 of neurologic morbidity. Although one can never be assured that all noxious events (both pre- 795 and postnatal) have been accounted for in any given study, there is mounting evidence that 796 transferring brain growth and development from an intrauterine to an extrauterine environment 797 before 38 weeks gestation is less than optimal even in the absence of other definable neurologic 798 risk factors (Chapieski and Evankovich 1997, Cherkes-Julkowski 1998, Huppi, et al. 1996). 799 Recent research, for example, has demonstrated poorer performance on elicited imitation tasks (a 800 medial temporal lobe function) at 18 months of age in 27- to 34- week gestational age preterm 801 infants with completely benign neonatal courses compared with term infants tested at the same 802 post-conceptional age (deHaan et al, 2000). These emerging data strongly suggest that the human 803 brain continues to develop in a unique way in utero until the end of gestation and that early 804 termination of pregnancy disrupts that development with subsequent behavioral consequences. 805 A more pernicious effect of extrauterine life on brain development in small preterm 806 infants is the general problem of malnutrition. Neonatal illness not only predisposes preterm 807 infants to definable neurotoxic events (e.g. IVH, hypoxia) but at the same time does not allow 808 the provision of adequate nutritional substrates to promote normal brain growth and 809 development. Studies have estimated that greater than 50% of very low birth weight infants fall 810 below the 5th percentile for head growth sometime during their hospitalization (rendering them, Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 38 811 by definition, microcephalic) (Georgieff, et al., 1985). Fortunately, one of the most amazing 812 aspects of early human life is the ability of the head (and brain) to demonstrate catch-up growth. 813 After a period of no growth, the head exhibits a remarkable increase in growth velocity to double 814 or triple normal rates, given adequate protein-energy intakes (Georgieff, et al. 1985, Sher and 815 Brown 1975). There is, however, a point of diminishing return. If the infant has had no growth 816 for more than a month, the subsequent catch-up rate is markedly reduced, almost as if the 817 potential for catch-up has been lost (Georgieff, et al. 1985, Hack and Breslau 1986, Sher and 818 Brown 1975). The premature infants with more striking postnatally acquired microcephally due 819 to malnutrition indeed have smaller head circumferences and poorer scores on the Bayley Scales 820 of Infant Development at 12 months of age (Georgieff, et al. 1985). Reduced head circumference 821 at 8 months postnatally bodes poorly for developmental outcomes measured at 3 and 8 years of 822 age (Hack and Breslau 1986). These studies suggest that although catch-up head growth is a 823 marvelous compensatory response, it is better to have never accrued the growth deficit in the first 824 place. Extrapolating further, it argues for important windows of opportunity for brain growth in 825 the late third trimester that, if interrupted by premature birth and lack of head growth, may result 826 in the brain being “constructed” in an alternative manner (deHaan, et al., 2000). 827 In sum, prematurity confers a significant risk to the developing brain. The risk emanates 828 from both insults to the brain that arise during the course of illness in the premature infant and 829 from interruptions of the provision of the expected substrates and environment apparently 830 necessary for normal brain development. We have used examples for which there is a substantial 831 literature (e.g. IVH), but hasten to add that multiple other potentially neuropathologic factors that 832 are more difficult to isolate and quantify (e.g. hypoxia-ischemia, hypoglycemia, neurotoxic Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 39 833 medications such as steroids) likely play important roles as well. The ultimate risk to any one 834 premature infant is likely to be a composite of all the known and unknown risk and protective 835 factors that characterize that infant, and on the infant’s general extent of biological vulnerability. 836 Thus the premature infant born to a lower SES mother who received poor prenatal care is likely 837 to have a much more difficult neonatal course and therefore be at higher neurodevelopmental 838 risk than an infant of the same gestation born to a mother who received better prenatal care and 839 has more resources. Perhaps this helps explain the overall down-shifting of developmental scores 840 in lower SES premature infants (Saigal, et al. 1991). 841 Growing awareness of environmentally-based differences in the outcomes of premature 842 infants has fueled multiple intervention efforts ranging from dramatic changes in the care these 843 infants receive in neonatal intensive care units (NICU’s) (see reviews by Als, 1997; Hernandez- 844 Reif & Field, 2000) to comprehensive initiatives that provide a range of services to the infants 845 and their families from the time they leave the hospital to several months or years post-discharge. 846 The best known of the comprehensive approaches is the Infant Health and Development Program 847 (see box) (Gross, Spiker, & Haynes, 1997), which included a randomized trial and extensive 848 follow-ups of the participating families. The evaluation literature on these interventions offers 849 good news about the capacity of NICU-based stimulation programs, approaches that emphasize 850 individualized developmental care, and initiatives focused on parental coping and training in 851 optimal parenting skills to improve health outcomes and decrease developmental delays in 852 premature infants. Infants participating in the IDHP intervention demonstrated improved 853 behavioral functioning (e.g., higher IQ scores, vocabulary gains, and fewer behavioral problems) 854 at the conclusion of the intervention (when they were 3 years old) (IHDP, 1990), however, at 5 Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 40 855 years of age, only the heavier low birthweight infants (i.e., < 2000 grams) continued to show 856 gains that distinguished them from the children that did not receive the intervention (Brooks- 857 Gunn et al., 1994). By 8 years of age, even the gains of the heavier infants had been substantially 858 diminished (McCarton et al., 1997). The authors have speculated about the outcomes that might 859 have emerged if they had continued the program up to school entry. While it appears, therefore, 860 that the developmental problems associated with prematurity and low birthweight can be 861 mitigated by intervention, because this is such a complex, multifaceted biological phenomenon, 862 relatively nonspecific interventions may not be the most productive approach. Moreover, 863 virtually all experts in this area agree that efforts focused on preventing the birth of low 864 birthweight babies need to be the top priority. 865 866 Stress and the Developing Brain Research on premature infants has provided substantial evidence of the importance of the 867 baby’s caregiving environment for later prognosis. This theme emerges, as well, from research 868 on animals regarding how stress affects the developing brain. This research provides preliminary 869 insights into how alterations of the early caregiving environment affect neurochemical aspects of 870 early brain development. It is premature to extend this evidence to our own species. There is, for 871 example, only one scientifically-reviewed study that has imaged the brains of maltreated children 872 (DeBellis, Keshavan et al., 1999b), which we discuss in Chapter 10. The animal evidence, 873 however, is suggestive of the physiological processes that may underlie associations found 874 between highly dysfunction caregiving and problematic child outcomes and thus points to 875 promising directions for future collaborative research among behavioral and brain scientists. Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 41 876 The term stress is used by psychologists, physiologists, and the lay public. It means 877 different things to each (Engle, 1985). We adopt the convention of using stress to refer to the set 878 of changes in the body and brain that are set into motion when there are overwhelming threats to 879 our physical or psychological wellbeing (Selye, 1973; Selye, 1975). Stress can have dramatic 880 effects on health and development (Johnson, Kamilaris, Chrousos, & Gold, 1992b). This happens 881 because the physiology of stress produces a shift in the body’s priorities. When threats begin to 882 overwhelm our immediate resources to manage them, a cascade of neurochemical changes that 883 begin in the brain temporarily puts the processes in the body that can be thought of as future- 884 oriented--finding, digesting and storing food; fighting off colds and viruses; learning things that 885 don’t matter right now but may be important sometime in the future; reproducing and rearing 886 offspring--on hold. Many of these neurochemical changes take place in the very same brain 887 structures (e.g. hypothalamus and brainstem) that function to regulate heart rate, respiration, food 888 intake and digestion, reproduction, growth, and the building up versus breaking down of energy 889 stores (Stratakis & Chrousos, 1995). 890 These brain regions also play a role in regulating the production of stress hormones “ 891 below the neck”. Specifically, the adrenal glands, located on the top of the kidneys, produce 892 adrenaline and cortisol (Axelrod and Reisine, 1984). Adrenaline is part of the sympathetic 893 nervous system (SNS). Increases in SNS activity support vigilance, narrow attention, increase 894 heart rate, shunt blood to muscles and away from the digestive system, break down fat stores 895 making energy available to cells, and dampen activity of the immune system. Cortisol is a steroid 896 hormone that plays a myriad of roles in stress physiology. It helps to break down protein stores, 897 liberating energy for use by the body. It suppresses the immune system, suppresses physical Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 42 898 growth, inhibits reproductive hormones, and affects many aspects of brain functioning, including 899 emotions and memory. 900 Our understanding of how psychological stimuli, such as experiences of fear and anxiety, 901 recruit stress physiology is currently centered on an area of the brain called the amygdala (Davis, 902 1997; Rolls, 1992; Schulkin et. al., 1994) which has close back and forth communication with 903 areas of the brain involved in attention, memory, planning, and behavior control. In animals, 904 experimentally causing a hyper-stimulation of the amygdala (a process termed ‘kindling’) seems 905 to create a hyper-sensitization of the fear-stress circuits of the brain and changes in behavior that 906 look like an animal version of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Rosen, Sitcoske, & Glowa, 907 1996). It is as if the fear circuits get locked in the “on” mode and have trouble shutting off. These 908 circuits course through the amygdala and an area called the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. 909 They appear to be pathways through which circumstances outside the body set in motion the 910 cascade of events inside the body and the brain that undergird fear-stress responses. These events 911 involve the elevation of cortisol and stimulation of the sympathetic arm of the stress response. In 912 animals, flooding the brain with cortisol for prolonged periods of time produce changes in this 913 process that may lower the threshold for activating the fear-stress system (Makino, Gold, & 914 Schulkin, 1994). The result is an animal that more readily experiences fear, anxiety, and the 915 stress that accompanies these emotions and may have a harder time dampening or regulating 916 these responses. 917 The amygdala is a fairly mature brain area at birth in humans and seems to be fully 918 mature at least as early as a child’s first birthday. All anatomical evidence suggests that by the 919 end of the first year, young children should be capable of experiencing psychologically-driven Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 43 920 fear, anxiety, and stress. Indeed, fear reactions to strangers (Bronson, 1971; Schaffer, 1966; 921 Waters, Matas, & Sroufe, 1975) and anxiety reactions to separation from familiar caregivers 922 (Ainsworth & Bell, 1970; Bowlby, 1973; Sroufe, 1979) are hallmarks of emotional development 923 in late infancy. Brief periods of stress are not expected to be problematic. Indeed, survival 924 requires the capacity to mount a stress response. However, because the stress system functions to 925 put growth-oriented processes on hold, frequent or prolonged periods of stress may negatively 926 affect development. 927 Evidence from research on rodents and primates suggests that experiences of neglect 928 early in life constitute the kinds of stressful experiences to which young offspring are especially 929 sensitive and may result in a more reactive stress system. In studies of rats, for example, when 930 experimenters do things to the nest (such as handle the pups ) that affect maternal behavior, they 931 can affect the development of the rat’s stress system (Denenberg, 1999; Levine & Thoman, 932 1970). Doing things to the nest that result in better organized maternal behavior results in infant 933 rats that develop into less fearful, less stress-reactive adults, whereas doing things that disrupt 934 maternal behavior results in more fearful and stress-reactive adult rats. Researchers have also 935 shown that strains of rodents that are known to be more stress reactive are characterized by 936 maternal care that involves less licking and grooming (Liu et al., 1997; Meaney, 1996; Plotsky & 937 Meaney, 1993). Cross-fostering genetically high stress-reactive infants to mothers from low 938 stress-reactive strains results in the development of a more stress resilient animal. These early 939 experience effects in the rat appear to operate through the development of the receptor system in 940 the brain that influences the reactivity of the fear-anxiety circuits. Plenty of input early in life 941 that keeps the stress system dampened down results in the development of a stress-modulating Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 44 942 receptor system that can quickly turn off stress reactions. Absent this input, the fear-stress system 943 appears to get “shaped*” so that the rat pup becomes a more highly reactive adult who has 944 difficulty modulating these responses. In short, the development of a less stress-reactive rat 945 seems to revolve around enhancing and supporting qualities of the caregiving environment. 946 There are monkey analogues of these rat studies, although details of the bio-behavioral 947 mechanisms have not been worked out as elegantly. Infant monkeys deprived of normal social 948 stimulation grow into socially incompetent, fearful adults (Harlow, Harlow, & Suomi, 1971; 949 Laurens, Suomi, Harlow, & McKinney, 1973). More recent studies have documented that 950 monkeys reared on cloth surrogates, but exposed every day to several hours of play with other 951 infant monkeys, are not as socially incompetent as monkeys raised in isolation, but they show 952 numerous physiological signs of being very anxious and fearful (Suomi, 1991). They produce 953 higher levels of stress hormones when threatened and they have high levels of anxiety-related 954 brain neurochemicals in the cerebrospinal fluid which flows from the brain into the spinal cord. 955 Monkeys reared only with other infant monkeys (i.e., no cloth surrogates to call their own), show 956 similar patterns of high stress reactivity (Champoux, 1989; Champoux, Byrne, DeLizio, & 957 Suomi, 1992). A high stress-reactive adult monkey can also be produced by procedures that 958 cause stress to its mother (Schneider, 1992a; Schneider, 1992b; Schneider et al., 1998; 959 Schneider, Coe, & Lubach, 1992 Coplan et al., 1996; Coplan, Rosenblum, & Gorman, 1995; 960 Rosenblum & Andrews, 1994; Rosenblum et al., 1994). One technique for stressing the mother is 961 to make her food resources unpredictable. This has the effect of deeply disturbing the mother’s 962 social relationships with other adult monkeys in her group. The infant monkeys in these 963 unpredictable food studies (who are roughly equivalent in developmental age to 1- to 2-year old Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 45 964 human children) experience high levels of stress hormones (like their mothers) and grow up into 965 highly fearful, socially less competent adult animals (Rosenblum & Andrews, 1994; Rosenblum 966 et al., 1994). These effects were obtained even though food was never uncertain for the young 967 monkeys themselves, and thus seem to be mediated by what this uncertainty and disturbance in 968 the social environment does to their mothers. 969 We have a great deal to learn about how the social environment connects with the 970 biology of growth and the regulation of stress physiology in human infants and children. 971 Intriguing research is emerging, however, which suggests that the development of stress 972 regulation in young children may be a very promising place to look for brain-experience 973 dynamics. For example, both failure to thrive and psychosocial dwarfism (Gohlke, Khadilkar, 974 Skuse, & Stanhope, 1998; Skuse, 1985), in which children’s pituitary glands fail to secrete 975 sufficient growth hormone (Skuse, Albanese, Stanhope, Gilmour, & Voss, 1996), are associated 976 with failures in the social environment (Alanese et al., 1994). Removing the child from the 977 problematic social system reverses the disorder and growth increases rapidly. A significant 978 implication of this research, as well as that on orphanage-reared infants discussed in Chapter 10, 979 is that the human brain may be capable of greater plasticity and self-righting tendencies than 980 have been observed in the animal studies. In general, we have much to learn about the extent to 981 which the neurological pathways between caregiving environments and dysfunctional behavior 982 that are emerging in the animal literature apply to human offspring and about the effects of 983 remedial experiences that attempt to enhance the development of children from early abusive and 984 neglectful environments. Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 46 985 In sum, neuroscience evidence from animal research is increasingly pointing to 986 experiences of neglect, stress and trauma within the caregiving environment as a source of 987 compromised brain development. Research on rodents and primates indicates that the ways in 988 which the brain learns to respond to stressful and fear-inducing circumstances are profoundly 989 affected by the capacity of the infant’s caregivers to regulate the developing stress system. 990 Disruptions to the caregiving environment that produce stress in the mother appear to alter the 991 offspring’s developing stress reactivity, as seen behaviorally in high levels of fearfulness and 992 neurologically in how the brain releases and modulates stress hormones. Alternatively, 993 supportive and nurturant caregiving can protect offspring from these consequences. Although 994 this evidence is compelling with regard to the significance of early rearing environments as they 995 affect the developing brain, we are at the frontier of exploring these issues in human babies. In 996 the future, research that blends neuroscience and behavioral research promises to help explain 997 how alterations in the environment early in life may have effects on brain development and may 998 alter, probabilistically, patterns of behavioral responding for children with different rearing 999 histories. The capacity of the developing human brain to reorganize itself when beneficial 1000 caregiving environments are substituted for highly depriving circumstances also warrants 1001 expanded research attention. 1002 1003 Cross-Cutting Issues We have reviewed a range of evidence regarding the influence of early environments on 1004 brain development. When examined topic by topic, however, issues that are common to or cut 1005 across specific areas of research do not necessarily surface. We close this section, therefore, with 1006 a brief discussion of several such issues. Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 47 1007 Timing, Severity, and Chronicity 1008 The effects of early biologic insults vary depending on when the insult or stressor occurs, 1009 how severe it is, and how long it lasts, even within the prenatal period. We know less about these 1010 issues with regard to exposure to stress, although the brain systems that are affected are 1011 developing rapidly during the early childhood years. Issues of timing, severity, and chronicity are 1012 critical to understanding what aspects of brain development are particularly vulnerable, when 1013 they are vulnerable, which functions recover spontaneously, and which can be corrected or 1014 improved with intervention. Few conditions have been studied systematically enough to provide 1015 information on each of these issues, despite the tremendous clinical importance for the many 1016 infants exposed to early biologic insults. There is however, little doubt that timing, severity, and 1017 chronicity matter. We also saw that severity and chronicity can be different paths to similar 1018 outcomes. Episodic binge drinking early in pregnancy may be as bad for the developing brain as 1019 lower levels of alcohol consumption throughout gestation. Despite the urgent questions involved, 1020 separating the effects of timing, severity, and chronicity is often impossible in human studies and 1021 requires ingenuity and elaborate experiments even in animal models. 1022 Confluence of Risks 1023 Early biologic risks and insults often do not occur in isolation [Figure in Child Dev. 1024 paper]. In fact, they typically are increased among infants who also grow up in disadvantaged 1025 environments. Low birth weight, elevated lead levels, and iron deficiency, for example, are all 1026 more prevalent among poor and/or minority infants in the U.S.{Lozoff, NRC race paper. The co- 1027 occurrence of environmental and biologic risks requires cautious interpretation of research 1028 findings given how difficult it is to disentangle poorer developmental and behavioral outcomes Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 48 1029 that are due to the biologic exposure, rather than to the problematic environment. The confluence 1030 of risks is not confined to the overlap of environmental disadvantage and early biologic factors. 1031 The early biologic insults themselves are often intertwined or induce other risks: women who 1032 drink heavily during pregnancy are also more likely to smoke, take other drugs, and have poor 1033 diets; iron deficiency exacerbates lead absorption, and so on. The confluence of risks is the 1034 reality of many babies' lives, and the overlap raises important questions. If a baby is exposed to 1035 alcohol and nicotine prenatally, is that worse than either chemical by itself? Does it make a 1036 difference if a baby born prematurely then experiences a nutritional deficiency postnatally? 1037 When biologic and environmental risks co-occur, are the effects on child development additive 1038 (e.g., 2 + 2 = 4) or synergistic (2+2=6)? Answering these questions in humans is difficult, and 1039 even in animal models there has been relatively little research on co-occurring risks. 1040 Nonetheless, such studies promise to provide fundamental information about the developing 1041 brain. 1042 Individual Variability 1043 There is a great deal of variability in the response to early biologic risks. For example, 1044 low birth weight babies are a heterogeneous group consisting of those born prematurely and term 1045 babies who did not grow optimally in utero. They are frequently categorized as low birthweight 1046 (2500 grams or less), very low birthweight (1500 grams or less), or extremely low birthweight 1047 (1000 grams or less). Neurocognitive differences, observed at all levels of low birth weight, are 1048 greater the lower the birth weight.{1411} Research showing clear differences, on average, 1049 between infants who experienced a early biologic insult and normal controls tells us little about 1050 the effect on an individual child. Even though an exposed group may generally do less well than Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 49 1051 a non-exposed group, some children may show no ill effects. As we understand more about 1052 environmental factors and genetic influences on brain development, some of this variation may 1053 be explained. In the meantime, it is important to be cautious in applying research findings of 1054 average differences between groups to the clinical care of an individual child. 1055 Common Pathways 1056 Investigators understandably seek to identify specific central nervous system effects of 1057 the particular biologic insult they study. Yet, the search for specific effects, while undeniably 1058 important, may be misguided in some respects. Many of the biologic risks in Tables 1 and 2 are 1059 involved in numerous central nervous system processes.{Morgane; Neurotox issue} Thus, 1060 diffuse, subtle, or multiple effects would be expected for many of these factors, and their 1061 characterization may be as important as the search for specific effects. Certain brain functions 1062 may be particularly vulnerable, regardless of the specific biologic insult or stressor. For example, 1063 animal research indicates that the hippocampus is affected by a number of conditions, including 1064 hypoxia-ischemia (common in premature infants), lead, iron deficiency, and exposure to stress in 1065 the form of maternal separation. Similarly, human studies suggest that certain behavior patterns 1066 are frequently observed in infants exposed to a variety of early biologic insults. For instance, 1067 longitudinal studies of children born with low birth weight, exposed to alcohol prenatally, with 1068 elevated lead levels, or who experienced iron deficiency in infancy all show increased attention 1069 problems. Memory functions and behavioral wariness are additional candidates. More 1070 coordinated choices of outcome measures in studies of developmental and behavioral outcome in 1071 human infants who experience different early biologic insults and stressors might identify more Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 50 1072 outcomes in common and suggest testable hypotheses about basic mechanisms underlying the 1073 observed ill effects. 1074 Modifiability and Recovery 1075 Both observational and experimental studies provide crucial information about whether 1076 the ill effects of early biologic insults are modifiable by specific treatment or spontaneous 1077 recovery. Results from these studies address fundamental questions about both sensitive periods 1078 that constrain modifiability and the ongoing plasticity of the developing brain that keeps it open 1079 to influence by the environment, as well as about the extent of adaptation that is possible 1080 following damage. Research on animals clearly indicates that brain structure and behavioral 1081 functioning can be modified even in the face of clear biologic insult. 1082 The research on stress further suggests that more than adequate caregiving may be required 1083 following detrimental early experiences to return the young animal to a normal developmental 1084 pathway. 1085 Because there is minimal neuroscience data on young children who experience conditions 1086 of early psychosocial adversity, we know little about the capacity of the human brain to 1087 reorganize itself following conditions of disrupted caregiving, biologic insults, privation of 1088 stimulation, and so on early in life. We noted that a variety of interventions have been shown to 1089 ameliorate some of the central nervous system effects of prenatal alcohol exposure, and the 1090 behavioral effects of prematurity, but that sometimes extensive if not heroic efforts are required 1091 and that functioning is seldom fully restored. Other insults appear to leave even more lasting 1092 damage, as appears to be the case with iron deficiency anemia. There is an urgent need for 1093 continued basic science and clinical studies of the effects of specific environmental interventions Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 51 1094 in the whole array of early biologic insults and stressors. It may turn out that the brain is 1095 considerably more plastic than is often assumed. Alternatively, it may provide even more 1096 compelling evidence that preventing early biologic insults is the best avenue for ensuring healthy 1097 development. 1098 1099 1100 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Basic research on the development of the brain is a rapidly moving frontier. Abundant 1101 evidence indicates that brain development begins well before birth, extends far into the adult 1102 years, and is specifically designed to recruit and incorporate experience into its emerging 1103 architecture and functioning. For some systems, environmental inputs need to occur prenatally or 1104 relatively early in life after which time the brain becomes decreasingly capable of developing 1105 normally. But, available evidence indicates that such critical periods are more exceptional than 1106 normative in human development. For the vast majority of brain development, including areas of 1107 the brain involved in cognitive, emotional, and social development, we either have not explored 1108 questions regarding critical or sensitive periods or it appears that the brain remains open to 1109 experiences across broad swaths of development. This makes sense. Adaptation depends upon 1110 the rapid consolidation of capabilities essential to survival and the life-long flexibility to adjust to 1111 changing circumstances and learn new skills. As a result, assertions that the “die has been cast” 1112 by the time the child enters school are not supported by neuroscience evidence and can create 1113 unwarranted pessimism about the potential efficacy of interventions that are initiated after the 1114 preschool years. On the other hand, what happens early matters. Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 52 1115 Concerns about protecting the developing brain need to begin well before birth. During 1116 the prenatal months, the developing brain is highly vulnerable to intrinsic hazards (such as neural 1117 migration{??????!!!!!*** how is migration a hazard?}) and external insults resulting from drug 1118 or alcohol exposure, viral infection, malnutrition (including deprivation of iron, folic acid, and 1119 other essential nutrients), and environmental and other teratogens. This directs attention to efforts 1120 to protect brain development during pregnancy and the earliest months of life, including the 1121 importance of prenatal and postnatal care, as well as expanded health and public health efforts to 1122 improve nutritional quality and reduce drug and viral exposure. It also argues for continued 1123 efforts to reduce the incidence of premature births and to ameliorate the adverse consequences of 1124 prematurity. Neuroscience evidence may, in fact, provide a stronger scientific basis for 1125 preventive efforts and interventions aimed at the prenatal and perinatal periods than for 1126 initiatives designed to enrich the environments of older children. In general, existing evidence 1127 about the developing brain has much more to tell us about the conditions that deprive the brain of 1128 what it needs than about those that enhance development beyond what might otherwise be 1129 expected. 1130 For some fundamental aspects of development, the contemporary concern with early 1131 environments may actually be misplaced insofar as the vast majority of children receive the 1132 kinds of experiences that they need. {This lack of appreciation of Parents Do Matter is, in my 1133 opinion, inappropriate and incorrect. Not that it does not apply to sensory systems, but where do 1134 you say that it might apply to cognitive, language and other complex facets of development?}* 1135 This appears to be the case, for example, for the development of sensory systems. For most 1136 children, the question regarding sensory development is less whether the environment is Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 53 1137 providing the necessary experience and more whether the child can detect or register the 1138 experience and process it adequately. This shifts our focus to early detection, identification, and 1139 treatment of problems such as visual impairments, auditory processing deficits, and major 1140 perceptual-motor delays that have profound effects upon the child’s capacity to access and 1141 incorporate the stimulation needed to organize the developing nervous system. For these aspects 1142 of development, there is solid evidence that the timing of corrective efforts matters a great deal. 1143 A final implication of research on early brain development concerns the detrimental 1144 effects of early and sustained stressful experiences, particularly those that derive from aberrant or 1145 disrupted caregiving environments. Evidence from research on animals suggests that such 1146 experiences over-activate neural pathways that regulate fear-stress responses in the immature 1147 brain, perhaps placing them on a “high alert” setting that may alter, probabilistically, patterns of 1148 behavioral responding in adult animals with different rearing histories. Translations to human 1149 development are purely speculative, however, emerging evidence (reviewed in chapter 10) 1150 regarding the physiology of children subjected to serious deprivation and trauma early in life are 1151 consistent with the animal studies, as is the richer body of behavioral data on young children 1152 exposed to such early adverse experiences. This is an especially promising arena for research 1153 that integrates animal and human studies using both neuroscience and behavioral approaches, 1154 and explores not only the negative consequences of early stress and trauma but also the capacity 1155 of the brain to reorganize itself following highly depriving circumstances early in life. 1156 What, in sum, does neuroscientific research on early brain development tell us about 1157 caring for children? It reminds us that the young children warranting the greatest concern are 1158 those growing up in environments, starting prenatally, that fail to provide them with adequate Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE 54 1159 nutrition and other growth-fostering inputs, expose them to biologic insults, and subject them to 1160 abusive and highly neglectful care. Children with undetected sensorimotor difficulties (whose 1161 developing brains may not receive the stimulation they need) also warrant great concern. The 1162 brain research also, however, reassures us that brain development is probably on course for the 1163 vast majority of young children who are protected from these conditions, and, in many instances, 1164 can be affected positively by timely corrective interventions focused on early insults and deficits. 1165 {see comment above}* 1166 Early Childhood Development 03/09/16 Chapter 6 CONFIDENTIAL - DO NOT QUOTE, CITE, OR REPRODUCE