Kate Athens Anne Haldane AP Statistics Final Project: Gender and Handwriting Our project investigates whether there is a difference in the handwriting size of teenage boys and girls. We consider the number of words per line to be a good indicator of how much space a person’s handwriting takes up. The Experiment: We asked 60 subjects – 30 boys and 30 girls – to write one line of text as we dictated it to them. For each person, we used the same Alice in Wonderland extract, which we felt contained a good distribution of short, medium, and long words. We removed all punctuation from the paragraph. We told our subjects to write freely, without attempting to squeeze more words into one line than they would normally write. When reviewing the samples, we adjusted for differences in where subjects chose to end on the page (i.e. if one subject left a margin on the right side of the sheet, we asked them to go to the next line and then measured how many more words they would have fit into the margin). We then tallied the number of words per line. Data and Calculations: Table A – Number of words per line Male Subject 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 # Words per line 15 15 15 10 18 14 15 14 9 16 13 15 9 8 12 Male Subject 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 # Words per line 18 12 7 12 9 9 10 17 15 11 12 10 16 9 15 Female Subject 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 # Words per line 10 12 9 10 15 12 15 12 13 16 15 16 12 8 7 Female Subject 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 # Words per line 18 10 9 9 11 15 8 10 9 13 7 9 11 9 5 Step 1: Population of interest: Princeton High School teens Parameter: mean number of words per line H0: μM = μF Ha: μM > μF The mean number of words per line is identical in both teenage males’ and females’ handwriting The mean number of words per line is greater in teenage males’ handwriting than in teenage females’ handwriting Step 2: Inference procedure: 2-Sample t test of significance, as we do not know the population mean or standard deviation. Conditions: Are the samples two SRS’s from two distinct populations? Though our samples were not simple random samples, we approached the selection of the samples as randomly as possible, asking the first 60 people we could effectively use as subjects. We will treat these samples as SRS’s. Are the two samples independent from one another? The two samples, teenage females and males, are independent from one another, and we measured the same variable (words per line) for each sample. Are the samples normally distributed? INSERT Female Histogram Histogram: Female 8 7 Frequency 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 4 9 14 19 Words/Line INSERT Male Histogram Histogram: Male 8 7 Frequency 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 4 9 14 Words/Line 19 INSERT Female Words/Line Normal Probability Plot Normal Probability Plot for Female Words/Line 99 ML Estimates 95 Percent 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 5 1 5 10 Data INSERT Male Words/Line Normal Probability Plot 15 20 Mean: 11.1667 StDev: 3.09928 Normal Probability Plot for Male Words/Line 99 ML Estimates 95 Mean: 12.6667 StDev: 3.09121 Percent 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 5 1 5 10 15 20 Data Though the two samples are not completely normal, they have a similar, slightly symmetrical shape. The 2-sample t test is more robust than the one-sample t test, and we have a large enough sample to use the t distribution. Also, the normal probability plots for the two samples show a linear pattern, which means the distribution is approximately normal. Because the conditions are met, we can continue with the 2-sample t test. Step 3: Procedure: 2 sample mean t test of significance at the .05 level Using calculator software, we calculated the following from the data in Table A: Sample mean words per minute (X Bar) male = 12.66 Sample mean words per minute (X Bar) female = 11.16 Sample standard deviation words per minute (s) male = 3.14 Sample standard deviation words per minute (s) female = 3.15 df = 57.999 t-value = 1.845 P(t>1.845) = .035 Step 4: Interpretation: The low p-value of .035 allows us to reject the null hypothesis H0: μM = μF and conclude that at the 0.05 significance level, the mean words per line in teenage males’ handwriting is greater than the mean words per line in teenage females’ handwriting. Therefore, we can accept the alternative hypothesis: Ha: μM > μF Overall, we believe that this indicates that teenage boys’ handwriting is smaller/takes up less space than teenage girls’ handwriting. This is also supported by the modified boxplots of the two samples, which indicate that the median number of words per line for teenage males (12.5) is higher than for females (10.5), as is the entire spread. Also, the 1.5 x (Inter-quartile range) formula indicates that there were no outliers. INSERT Female Words/Line Boxplot Histogram: Female 8 7 Frequency 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 4 9 14 Words/Line INSERT Male Words/Line Boxplot 19 BOXPLOT Words/Line 15 10 5 Male Female Sex Sources of Error: Response bias – because we briefly explained the purpose of the experiment beforehand, this may have influenced the number of words that subjects attempted to write in one line. Also, subjects seeing previous subjects’ lines may have been a source of influence. “Space” is an objective term; for example, some people write letters with greater height and width than others, but use shorter spaces between their words, while others may write letters with short height and width but use wide spacing. However, even with this variation, we believe our conclusion about space usage to be generally true. Further experiments might be useful for testing hypotheses that teenage girls on average tend to group their words more closely together than teenage boys, or that teenage boys tend to write with smaller letters.