Ethnic differences on students’ approaches to learning: Self-regulatory cognitive and motivational predictors of academic achievement for Latino/a and White college students. Robert Rueda Hyo Jin Lim, Harry O’Neil University of Southern California Noelle Griffin University of California, Los Angeles Shel Bockman Barbara Sirotnik California State University, San Bernardino NOTE: The work report herein was supported at the University of Southern California by the Office of Naval Research, under Award No. N00014-06-1-0711 at UCLA. The work was supported by a subcontract from UCLA. The opinions, findings and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not reflect the views of the Office of Naval Research. 1. Page |1 Abstract We examined (a) the construct validity across White and Latino college students in terms of motivational and the use of learning strategies constructs; and (b) the differences in the relationships among sociocultural variables and motivation/learning related factors and academic achievement. Results of multi-group confirmatory factor analyses revealed that the scales of motivation and learning-related factors proved to be equivalent across the groups. We tested a theoretical model of relationships among sociocultural backgrounds and motivational factors which affected achievement using SEM methods. In general, the patterns of relationships were in the expected direction. There were significant ethnic differences in terms of specific effects of background characteristics on motivational and/or learning related factors. Ethnic differences were also found in the relationships between the use of adaptive and maladaptive learning strategies and academic achievement. The research is discussed with respect to relatively sparse literature on sociocultural aspects of motivation and their effects on academic achievement with college-level Latino students. 1. Page |2 Table of Contents Introduction.................................................................................................................. Achievement Differences Among Subgroups in Higher Education: Social and Cultural Factors........................................................................................................................... Ethnicity............................................................................................................ SES..................................................................................................................... Secondary effects: Opportunity to learn and prior knowledge......................................... Gender............................................................................................................. Cognitive and Motivational Factors in Learning and Achievement.............................. Self-Efficacy...................................................................................................... Goal Orientation................................................................................................. Learning Strategies and Self-regulated Learning............................................... Help-seeking...................................................................................................... Text Anxiety and Worry............................................................................................. Summary and Purpose of the Study................................................................................ Method............................................................................................................................ Research Context................................................................................................ Subjects............................................................................................................... Measures............................................................................................................. Background variables................................................................................. Motivation and attitudes............................................................................ Personal motivational beliefs...................................................................... 1. Page |3 Strategy use...................................................................................................... Academic achievement.................................................................................... Procedure............................................................................................................ Telephone interview................................................................................... Online survey.............................................................................................. Data Analysis...................................................................................................... Results............................................................................................................................. Reliabilities, descriptive statistics and intercorrelations........................................... Reliabilities................................................................................................. Correlational analyses based on theoretical expectations.......................................... Correlations among motivation/ learning strategies variables.................... Correlations between learning strategies and achievement........................ Multi-group Confirmatory Factor Analysis (MG-CFA)............................................ Preliminary CFA for overall model............................................................ Multiple-group CFA................................................................................... Structural Equation Model (SEM) Analysis............................................................. SEM MIMIC model for all students.......................................................... Two single-group SEMs (Latino vs. White)............................................. Summary Findings.................................................................................................... Discussion and Conclusion................................................................................................. References........................................................................................................................... Tables..................................................................................................................................... . 1. Page |4 Figures................................................................................................................... Appendix A............................................................................................................................. B.............................................................................................................................. 1. Page |5 The national move toward accountability, primarily focused on K-12 schools, has begun to focus on postsecondary education as well. Accountability demands for student academic performance and persistence rates is growing (Carey, 2004, 2005; Hearn & Holsworth, 2002). In addition, postsecondary education is widely seen as only a minimum goal in education, as a high school diploma is no longer seen as sufficient preparation for contemporary workforce demands. However, there are wide disparities in the success rates among groups in the country. In 2007-08, for example, there were 2,757 four year postsecondary institutions in the United States, and one third of these (33.1%) were public institutions. Within these institutions, 67% of the degrees conferred were awarded to White, non-Hispanic students. In contrast, 8.5% and 7.1% of the degrees were awarded to Black, non-Hispanic and Hispanic students respectively (Knapp, Kelly-Reid, & Ginder, 2008). These data belie significant differences in other college-related outcomes between Latino and other students in the U.S. Latinos have been found to lag every other population group in attaining college degrees, especially bachelor’s degrees. To better understand that problem and help identify policy responses, the Pew Hispanic Center conducted a new analysis of the educational performance of Latino high school graduates. This analysis is based on Current Population Survey data collected by the U.S. Census Bureau from 1997 to 2000. The data was combined and averaged to create a solid statistical basis for assessing different forms of college attendance for Latinos as compared to other groups and for making important distinctions among sub-groups of the Latino population. The report (Fry, 2002) revealed the following enrollment patterns: 1. Page |6 Latino high school graduates are enrolled in some form of college at a surprisingly high rate (10 %, compared to 7% of the total population). Moreover, after the age of 24, the proportion of Latino high school graduates enrolled in college courses is higher than that of whites (5% vs. 7%) ,although those students who finish at an earlier age tend to see greater economic rewards There is a substantial enrollment gap between Latinos and all other groups among 18-24 year olds – only 35% of Latino high school graduates in that age group are enrolled in college compared to 46% of whites Latinos are far more likely to be enrolled in two year colleges than any other group – about 40% of Latinos in the 18 to 24 year old range) compared to about 25% of white and black students in that age group Latinos are more likely to be part time students (about 85% of Whites are enrolled full time compared to about 75% of Latinos) Latinos lag behind in the pursuit of graduate and professional degrees in the 25-34 year old age group compared to Whites (1.9 % compared to 3.8%) There are within-group differences that have been reported as well. For example, first-generation students (those whose parents did not attend college and who are most often African American or Latino) also have a lower rate of postsecondary attainment than White students. Based on the National Education Longitudinal Study of 1988 (National Center for Education Statistics [NCES], 2005), 43% of the first-generation students who enrolled in postsecondary education between 1992 and 2000 “left without a degree” and 25% had attained an undergraduate degree by 2000. In contrast, 20% of the 1. Page |7 students whose parents had a bachelor’s degree or higher “left without a degree” from a postsecondary institution, while almost 70% attained an undergraduate degree by 2000. These inequities in outcomes are troubling for a number of reasons, yet they only document the current situation rather than suggesting a solution. Attempts to examine the underlying causes of these achievement differences have focused on a number of areas including correlates resulting in hypothesized or possible causes of the achievement patterns noted above. These are described below. Achievement Differences Among Subgroups in Higher Education: Social and Cultural Factors College enrollment has been found to be influenced by a variety of academic, personal, social and economic factors. Cultural and family-related values play an integral role in modeling student perceptions and motivation as well, especially in populations of students from minority backgrounds (Alfaro, Umana-Taylor, & Bamaca, 2006; Esparza & Sanchez, 2008; Fuligni, Tseng, & Lam, 1999; Plunkett & Bamaca-Gomez, 2003; Plunkett, Henry, Houltberg, Sands, & Abarca-Mortensen, 2008). For instance, Eccles, Vida, and Barber (2004) found that high school achievement, family expectations, mother’s education level and family income were predictors of college enrollment. Abrego (2006) took a qualitative approach to examining the problem of undocumented youth in high school, and her findings revealed how these students faced the difficult and often impossible task of entering college or finding a job due to their undocumented status, thus resulting in decreased motivation and increased tendency to dropout. In regards to families, Goldenberg, Gallimore, Reese, and Garnier (2001) looked at how the aspirations and expectations of immigrant Latino families impacts student 1. Page |8 performance. This study was found that low performing students do not come from families with low expectations. In fact, the contrary was found, in that immigrant families hold high expectations and aspirations for their children’s current and future performance in school. Additionally, these beliefs are influenced by their children’s’ performance in school, rather than the beliefs influencing performance (Goldenberg et al., 2001), and these beliefs later affect students’ enrolment in college (Davis-Kean, Vida, & Eccles, 2001). Other commonly examined sociocultural factors include ethnic/racial background, socioeconomic status, and gender. Ethnicity, SES, and gender have been found to either predict or moderate the prediction of college retention and GPA (Nettles, Thoeny, & Gosman, 1986; Nguyen, Allen, & Fraccastoro, 2005; Robbins, Lauver, Le, Davis, Langley, & Carlstrom, 2004; Robbins, Allen, Casillas, Peterson, & Le, 2006). We consider each of these in the following paragraphs. Ethnicity. Almost all discussions about the achievement gap in U.S. education include ethnic, racial, and cultural aspects. As noted earlier, achievement based on ethnic and cultural differences are prominent features of the educational system. Surveys conducted by the National Center for Education Statistics (NCES) indicated that, on average, minority students (primarily Latino and African American) lagged behind their White peers in terms of academic achievement (U.S. Department of Education, 2000). While early work tended to focus on presumed cultural deficits as a primary cause of ethnic/racial/cultural achievement gaps, this view has been widely discredited (Graham & Hudley, 2006; Gutierrez & Rogoff, 2003). More recently, other factors have been suggested, including the effects of living in low-income households or in single parent 1. Page |9 families; low parental education levels, and schools with lower resources. All of these factors are components of SES and linked to academic achievement (National Commission on Children, 1991). We consider some of these factors below. SES. Socioeconomic status (SES) is one of the most widely used contextual variables in education research, and is often looked at in relation to academic achievement (Bornstein & Bradley, 2003; Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997; Coleman, 1988). There seems to be relatively widespread agreement that SES is comprised of three components: parental income, parental education, and parental occupation (Gottfried, 1985; Hauser, 1994). While these variables are often moderately correlated, there is evidence that the components are unique and that each one measures a substantially different aspect of SES that should be considered to be separate from the others (Bollen, Glanville, & Stecklov, 2001; Hauser & Huang, 1997). In the current study, we used combination of mother’s education and parental income. As these findings suggest, it is only a convenience to consider SES as a stand-alone variable, since it is linked directly as well as indirectly to academic achievement. These include, for example, students’ racial and ethnic background, grade level, and school/neighborhood location (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998; Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997;; Eccles, Lord, & Midgley, 1991; Lerner, 1991). Social capital appears to be necessary to success in school. That is, the supportive relationships among structural forces and individuals (e.g. parent–school collaborations) that promote the sharing of societal norms and values(Coleman, 1988; Dika & Singh, 2002) is one of the indirect factors linked to family SES, in addition to such directly linked factors as home resources. 1. P a g e | 10 An early, comprehensive meta-analysis of SES and academic achievement (White, 1982) showed that the relation varies significantly with a number of factors such as the types of SES and academic achievement measures, and later studies have reported inconsistent findings. Correlations have ranged from strong (e.g., Lamdin, 1996; Sutton & Soderstrom, 1999) to no significant correlation at all (e.g., Ripple & Luthar, 2000; Seyfried, 1998). A more recent analysis (Sirin, 2005) examined journal articles published between 1990 and 2000, and included 101,157 students, 6,871 schools, and 128 school districts gathered from 74 independent samples. The results showed a medium to strong SES–achievement relation. However, the relation was moderated by the unit (e.g., the individual student vs. an aggregated unit), the source (e.g. secondary sources vs. self-report), the range of SES variable, and the type of SES–achievement measure. The relation was also contingent upon school level, minority status, and school location. In terms of grade level, the mean effect size (ES) was .19 for the kindergarten students, .27 for the elementary school students, .31 for middle school students, and .26 for high school students. In addition, the mean ES for White student samples (.27) was significantly larger than the mean ES for minority student samples (.17). Unfortunately, the review did not consider information on college level effects. Overall, the average correlation in Sirin’s (2005) review was .30, as compared with White’s (1982) average correlation of .34. In sum, the best evidence indicates that using Cohen’s (1977) effect size guidelines, the overall ES of Sirin’s analysis suggests a medium level of association between SES and academic achievement at the student level and a larger degree of association at the school level. Importantly, the review also suggests that parents’ location in the overall SES hierarchy has a strong impact on 1. P a g e | 11 students’ academic achievement. As Coleman (1988) has suggested, family SES sets the stage for students’ academic performance both by directly providing resources at home and by indirectly providing the social capital that is necessary to succeed in school. This same factor also helps to determine the kind of school and classroom environment to which the student has access (Reynolds & Walberg, 1992a). These indirect influences of SES are often subsumed under the conceptually larger label of opportunity to learn, which is considered next. Secondary effects: Opportunity to learn and prior knowledge Opportunity to learn (OTL) is a way of measuring and reporting whether students have access to the different ingredients that make up quality schools, for example access to challenging curriculum and to qualified teachers. The more OTL ingredients that are present in an individual students’ educational history, the more opportunities students have to benefit from a high quality education. OTL standards provide a benchmark against which the opportunities that a school provides can be measured. The notion of OTL is important since it opens the possibility that student outcomes are not solely a function of inherent student or family characteristics. Using OTL standards as a guide, students can estimate the quality of their educational experience and whether they have a realistic possibility of competing for college. These learning opportunities can either enhance or limit students’ equal access to a high quality education and future educational achievement. Examples of OTL indicators include students’ access to qualified teachers, clean and safe facilities, up-todate books and quality learning materials, high quality coursework, and related school conditions that provide students a fair and equal opportunity to learn and achieve 1. P a g e | 12 knowledge and skills. For example, taking a high-level math course is the one consistent course predictor of college preparedness. Adelman (1999) found that the single greatest predictor of successful college completion was the taking of high level mathematics courses during high school. If students do not successfully complete algebra, they are unlikely to succeed in institutions of postsecondary education (Checkley, 2001). Thus, if students find themselves in a school that does not offer high level courses or teachers qualified to teach such content, a negative relationship to academic outcomes would be expected. Several research studies (Adelman, 1999; Alexander, Pallas & Holupka, 1987; Cabrera & La Nasa, 2000a, 2000b; Horn & Kojaku, 2001; Kane & Spizman, 1994; McDonough, 1997; Stage & Rushin, 1993) [NOTE: HON to add CRESST study here] have looked at other factors that fall under the general umbrella of OTL. These studies have shown the following to be the strongest predictors of college attendance and completion, particularly for minority and low income students: academic preparation, social support, access to information, and parental involvement and knowledge about college, and financial aid. As might be expected, there is evidence that there are important differences in OTL between low-SES schools and higher-SES schools. Differences have been found in the following areas: instructional arrangements, materials, teacher experience, and teacherstudent ratio (Wenglinsky, 1998). Reinforcing the complex interrelationships among sociocultural factors, family SES has been found to influence the quality of the relationship between school personnel and parents (Watkins, 1997). 1. P a g e | 13 Gender. There is a significant amount of work on the issue of gender differences in academic achievement in general, and math achievement specifically, spanning several decades (Caplan & Caplan, 1997; Chipman, 1996; Favreau, 1997). Historically, males generally outperformed girls on measures of math and science achievement, with the gap increasing in middle and high school (Byrnes, 2001, 2005; Halpern, 2000). Although some more recent large-scale studies have documented girls performing lower on math and science assessments than their math peers, (Braswell, Lutkus, Grigg, Santapau, TayLim, & Johnson, 2001; NCES, 2002; OECD, 2000) other researchers have found this trend diminished or even reversed (see Baker, Griffin, & Choi, 2008 for a review). Women in particular tend to outperform men in terms of many post-secondary outcomes, with differences particularly evident for African American and Hispanic students (Freeman, 2004). For example, females currently have greater success than males in attaining post-secondary education, higher aspirations than males while in high school, are more likely to enroll in college immediately after graduating from high school, and persist and complete degrees at higher rates than males. Measures of postsecondary academic achievement (e.g., GPA) also tend to be higher for women than men. More than half of all bachelor’s and master’s degrees are awarded to females. Nevertheless, gender differences in majors still exist, with female bachelor’s degree recipients much less likely than their male peers to major in computer science, engineering, and physical sciences. Females also still lag behind males in enrolment in first–professional and doctoral programs, and these gaps are particularly evident in fields such as science, engineering, and mathematics (Snyder, Dillow, & Hoffman, 2007.). These differences persist into the world of work as well, with women continuing to be 1. P a g e | 14 under-represented in math, science, and technology professions (Commission on Professionals in Science and Technology, 2006).Thus, while gaps have narrowed in some areas, gender differences in some aspects of math achievement and attainment continue. Many factors have been suggested as explanations for the patterns just reported (Ben-Zeev, Fein, & Inzlicht, 2005; Geary, Saults, Lui, & Hood, 2000; Halpern & Lemay, 2000). For example, Byrnes (2005) posited an approach to the relationship between gender and math outcomes that integrates several types of underlying factors: exposure/opportunity to learn, motivation, and aptitude/ability. Byrnes suggests that this constellation of conditions, influenced by a range of social, psychological, and physiological variables, creates the gender differences often found in math achievement. The role of socialization and stereotyped gender expectations, particularly in the classroom setting, has also been highlighted in considering the origins and maintenance of gender differences in math outcomes (Huguet & Requer, 2007) One important line of investigation in this area has targeted metacognitive and motivational variables as contributors to differences in outcomes. For example, as part of the large scale PISA study, Marsh, Hau, Artelt, Baumert, & Peschar (2006) found gender differences in math-related metacognitive, motivational, and affective factors. Hong, O’Neil, and Feldon (2005) found no significant gender effects on math achievement, however they did identify gender differences in state-based anxiety and self-regulation, which themselves predicted math outcomes. Given this background, it is clear that gender is an important variable in examining group differences in achievement, and it was included in the present study. Cognitive and Motivational Factors in Learning and Achievement 1. P a g e | 15 It is clear that social and cultural factors are critical in considering achievement differences in college and other academic settings. In addition, cognitive and motivational factors have been shown to be related to a wide variety of academic outcomes, and here we describe some of the relationships that have been found in the literature. We begin by examining some of the key motivational variables. Rooted in the Latin word “to move”, motivation generally describes the relationship between the internal processes of beliefs, values and goals with the external expression of action, such as choice, persistence and performance (Eccles & Wigfield, 2002; Schunck, Pintrich, & Meece, 2008; Wigfield & Eccles, 2000). We consider two of the motivational variables that have been heavily studied and which have been shown to be important influences on learning, self-efficacy and goal orientation. Self-efficacy. Self efficacy is defined as “[P]eople’s judgments of their capabilities to organize and execute courses of action required to attain designated types of performances” (Bandura, 1986, p. 391). Although there are many motivational constructs, self-efficacy is central to promoting students' engagement and learning. These context-specific and domain-specific beliefs affect behavior by influencing the choices that people make and the courses of action they follow (Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2004). Pajares (1996) points out that people take on tasks in which they feel knowledgeable, experienced and confident and avoid those in which they do not. Self-efficacy motivates students because when people expect to do well, they tend to try hard, persist, and perform better (Schunk, Pintrich & Meece, 2008). By believing that they are able and have the ability to do well, students are lead to be more motivated in terms of effort, persistence, and behavior than students who do not (Bandura, 1997). 1. P a g e | 16 One reason that self-efficacy is important is that it has been associated with a variety of other motivational and learning-related variables. For example, Ross, Shannon, Salisbury- Glennon, and Guarino (2002) point out that a large number of studies have demonstrated a positive relationship between mastery goal orientation and self-efficacy. There is also evidence that when students feel confident they can learn, they tend to use more self-regulatory strategies (Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Zimmerman, 2000b). Much of the work on self-efficacy has been carried out with young students. One important study with college age students was reported by Chemers, Hu, and Garcia (2001). The study involved the entire freshman class at a major university in California. Academic self-efficacy (among other variables) was measured along with high school GPA and first year college GPA. While high school grades were significantly related to college grades, self-efficacy at the time of college entrance was related to grades at the end of the first year of college, even when prior GPA was controlled. Other recent studies (Pietsch, Walker, & Chapman, 2003; Pintrich, 2003) as well as a recent review of selfefficacy studies (Valentine, DuBois, & Cooper, 2004) have confirmed these patterns. Consistent with this body of research, it was expected that self-efficacy would be related to learning strategies and achievement. Goal orientation. Students’ perceptions about achievement behaviors and the meanings they assign to such behaviors is the root of goal orientation-theory. The classroom environment influences the goals students adopt. Mastery and performance goals are two major constructs of goal orientation. Students with a mastery goal orientation have the desire to learn for understanding, knowledge or insight, whereas students with a performance goal orientation want to be perceived as competent in 1. P a g e | 17 comparison to their peers, and may seek to outperform their classmates (Ames, 1992; Dweck, 1986; Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Elliot, 1999). Goal theorists have made an important distinction between those achievement goals that are performance-approach in which students want to look good in comparison to their peers, and those that are performance-avoidance in which students want to not look bad in comparison to their peers. In general, the research suggests that mastery and performance-approach goals tend to be positively related to academic achievement, and performance-avoidance goals sometimes related negatively to academic achievement (Harackiewicz, Barron, Pintrich, Elliot, & Thrash, 2002). In spite of these generalizations, there is some controversy about the benefits of mastery versus performance goals (Elliot, 2007). There is little disagreement about the negative consequences of performance avoidance goals, but more disagreement about the role of performance approach goals. For example, Midgley, Kaplan, and Middleton (2001) argued that performance approach goals may lead to low level strategy use such as memorizing to do well on tests, may be differentially beneficial to some groups or in some situations more than others (e.g. males, older students, and students in classrooms that are competitive in nature). In contrast, in a pair of related studies, Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, and Elliot (2002) and Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, Carter, and Elliot (2000) found that in studies with college students, mastery goals predicted increased interest, enjoyment, and course-taking (in psychology courses), but not to grades in the course. These researchers and others (Pintrich, 2003) have argued for a multiple goal perspective in which mastery and performance approach goals play a positive role in academic performance. 1. P a g e | 18 One consistent finding in the achievement goal literature is that students who support mastery goals are more likely to use deep-level strategies such as elaboration and organization than students who support performance goals (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, and Paris, 2004; Ross, Shannon, Salisbury-Glennon & Guarino, 2002;Wolters, Yu & Pintrich, 1996). A great deal of research, as Patrick, Kaplan, and Ryan (2007) point out, has documented that mastery goals are connected with students’ use of cognitive and selfregulatory strategies and interacting with others about their ideas and understanding. Multiple studies found that performance goals, especially performance avoidance, were positively correlated with low levels of cognitive engagement (Patrick et al, 2007). Mastery goals are usually associated with many positive cognitive, motivational, and behavioral outcomes (Pintrich, 2003). Improvements in student achievement are found when students go beyond the classroom requirements and do more work than is required (Fredrick et al., 2004). Given the preceding research, it was expected that mastery goals would be positively related to the use of learning strategies, and negatively related to maladaptive help-seeking and worry. Learning strategies and self-regulated learning. Research has suggested that self- regulated learning incorporates cognitive, motivational and metacognitive dimensions (Zimmerman & Schunk, 2001) and suggests the importance of self-regulatory skills in academic achievement (Zimmerman, 1990; Zimmerman & Martinez-Ponz, 1990). Research also indicates that students who self-monitor their learning progress and engagement with tasks generate internal feedback. This internal feedback works to assess possible gaps in performance, and the result may lead to improved learning outcomes (Azevedo & Cromley, 2004; Butler & Winne, 1995; Ley &Young, 2001). 1. P a g e | 19 Students who are capable of effective learning tend to choose appropriate learning goals, use adaptive knowledge and skills to direct their learning. These students are also able to select effective learning strategies appropriate to the task at hand. In other words, they are able to regulate their learning and take responsibility for the acquisition and maintenance of new skills. Boekaert (1997, 1999)’s model suggests that self-regulated learning is a complex, interactive process involving motivational as well as cognitive self-regulation. According to the theory of self-regulated learning, there is a relationship between different types of knowledge, strategy use, and goals. One of the central aspects of self-regulated learning is students’ ability to select, combine, and coordinate learning activities (Zimmerman & Martinez-Ponz, 1990). The cognitive aspects of effective self-regulated learning are elaboration (e.g. exploring how the information might be useful in the real world) and control strategies. Additionally, effective learning requires adaptive motivational beliefs related to factors such as selfefficacy, interest, goal orientation, attributions, and other variables which in turn affect effort and persistence in learning [Note to HON to add study here]. There is a significant body of research findings that support the connection between motivational variables, learning strategies, self-regulatory behaviour, and academic achievement (Bandura, 1997; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Zimmerman, 1999, 2000a). With respect to performance goals in particular, some work has found that they are negatively related to self-regulation of learning, decreasing the use of deep cognitive processing strategies that lead to better understanding (Graham & Golan, 1991; Meece, Blumenfeld, & Hoyle, 1988; Nolen, 1988). In contrast, mastery goal orientations held by individuals have been found to influence self-regulatory strategies (e.g., surface 1. P a g e | 20 processing, deep processing, and help-seeking behaviors) and academic achievement (e.g., Elliot, McGregor, & Gable, 1999; Middleton & Midgley, 1997). However, other research has found no relation between classroom performance goals and self-regulated learning (Ames & Archer, 1988), and one study (Ryan & Pintrich, 1997) found no relation between performance goals and adaptive help seeking, one form of selfregulated learning. In line with this research, it was expected that students’ selfregulatory and learning strategies would be related to achievement because of the centrality of these factors to a wide range of variables associated with improved academic outcomes. Help-seeking. Adaptive help-seeking has been described as asking for help needed in order to learn independently, not simply to obtain the correct answer. It is manifested when students monitor their academic performance, show awareness of difficulty they cannot overcome on their own, and exhibit the wherewithal and self-determination to remedy that difficulty by requesting assistance from a more knowledgeable individual, they are exhibiting mature, strategic behaviour (Newman, 2002, p. 132). While help seeking was once viewed as an indication that students were dependent, or immature, learning researchers have come to view help seeking as an adaptive strategy, especially when it is used to overcome difficulties in order to achieve mastery and autonomy in learning (Schunk & Zimmerman, 1994). Research indicates that students who tend to seek more help are also more likely to seek instrumental help (Karabenick, 2004; Tobias, 2006). Research further indicates that students who engage in instrumental help-seeking become more competent and independent learners, and they are more self-regulated and metacognitively aware, which 1. P a g e | 21 influences their awareness of a need for help (Tobias, 2006; Karabenick, 2004). Karabenick (2004) found that mastery oriented students not only tend to seek more help, but also perform better. In contrast, performance oriented students tend to have lower achievement due to their threat of help-seeking and resistance to obtaining the help they need, thereby decreasing their chances of success (Karabenick, 2004). Studies have also shown that help-seeking behaviors can lead to increased academic achievement (Karabenick, 2003; Wigfield & Eccles, 2002).Help-seeking is related to motivational variables as well. For example, students who have higher efficacy are more likely to seek help (Ryan, Gheen, & Midgley, 1998). Students may modify their engagement by adjusting or setting new goals. They may further reexamine their learning tactics and strategies and select more productive approaches, adapt available skills, or even generate new learning or monitoring procedures (Butler & Winne, 1995). Unfortunately, students who need help the most are often those least likely to seek it out (e.g. Ryan & Pintrich, 1997; 2001). In addition, while help-seeking can be an important self-regulatory strategy, it is not necessarily always positive. Students who are motivated by work-avoidance goals are reluctant to seek help or seek help for the wrong reasons. Students may ask for assistance when it is not necessary, for example, or when they have not attempted the work on their own. They may also be motivated to seek help in order to complete a task and move on to other activities, or they may seek help for social rather than academic reasons. There are a variety of reasons that students can engage in maladaptive helpseeking. These include factors such as lack of awareness of the need for help, concern about one’s competence, concern about negative evaluations by teachers or peers, 1. P a g e | 22 classroom structures that are judged to sanction help-seeking, concern about the amount of effort needed to seek help, negative judgments abouthe competence or utility of available help, etc.(Ryan, Pintrich, & Midgley, 2001). There is evidence that there is a social dimension to help-seeking as well. The goal to form and maintain positive peer relationships has been found to be negatively related to avoidance of help seeking (Ryan et al., 1997). Group differences. Though the literature on learning strategies that has examined ethnic differences is small, there are important studies that examine different approaches used by different groups. For instance, Garcia, Yu, and Coppola’s (1993) study unpacked gender and ethnic differences in science achievement. The design was a pre-post test at two different times to examine changes in patterns of motivation and learning strategies. Latino and African-American students’ success was correlated with motivation and prior achievement, and not with learning strategies. Also, minority students (African American, Latino and Asian) reported a higher extrinsic motivation orientation over their White counterparts. These strategies included metacognitive awareness (e.g. planning, monitoring, regulating) and effort management. The authors conclude that gender and ethnicity do not affect achievement outcomes, but preparedness, motivation and use of learning strategies do. [Note for HON to add study here] Stough and Songeroth (1994) interviewed 28 college students to examine the role of teacher instruction and learning strategies for successful students (identified as persisting in school and having a GPA of over 2.0). Learning strategies defined in this study were reading books, working supplemental problems, asking for help and working with peers on homework. Additionally, successful students, over non-successful students, were more 1. P a g e | 23 articulate about their use of learning strategies and more metacognitively aware of their application. In one of the first studies to look at cross-cultural differences in selfregulated learning and learning strategies, Purdie, Hattie, and Douglas (1996) looked at conceptions of learning and use of strategies, suggesting that conceptions inform strategies used. They found that Japanese students use strategies of rote memorization, the use of textbooks and do not seek help from others. Both groups used learning strategies (e.g. “environmental structuring”, “self-evaluating”) at similar frequencies and the only difference in strategies was which ones were used more by both groups (“reviewing previous work” was the least used strategy of both groups). In other words, there was no between-group difference in types of strategies used. Test anxiety and worry. There is a significant amount of work in the area of motivation that has examined to role or affect or emotion on academic performance (Hembree, 1988; Zeidner & Matthews, 2005). Because of the variety of related terms that are often used in this work, Forgas (2000) proposed a distinction among affect (a broad and inclusive term which encompasses specific emotions as well as general moods), mood (relatively low-intensity, diffuse, but enduring affective states that have no salient antecedents and that have little cognitive content), and emotions (short-lived, intense phenomena that usually have a salient cause in the immediate environment of which the person is aware, and which have clear cognitive content – such as pride at doing well on an exam). In addition, Pekrun (1992) and Pekrun and Frese(1992) proposed a taxonomy which divides emotions in to task-related and social, and which further subdivides taskrelated emotions according to whether they are experienced during a task (process- 1. P a g e | 24 related), in anticipation of a task (prospective), or after a task (retrospective). In this framework, anxiety falls under the category of task-related, prospective emotions. One important emotion is test anxiety because of the obvious connections to achievement. Test anxiety is often seen as a specific form of generalized anxiety, in reference to test or evaluative situations. It is recognized as having phenomenological, physiological, and behavioural responses that occur in conjunction with concern about possible negative consequences of low performance (Zeidner, 1998). In addition, researchers often distinguish trait anxiety, which is more stable and enduring, from state anxiety, which is more context-specific (Covington, 1992; Spielberger, 1972). Test anxiety is also commonly seen as having a cognitive component (worry) and an emotional component (emotionality such as fear and uneasiness) (Zeidner & Matthews, 2005). There are two general patterns in this literature. First, the negative effects of test anxiety on performance are strong and consistent (Hembree, 1988; Hill & Wigfield, 1988). One review found a correlation coefficient of up to -.60 between test anxiety and achievement (Hill & Wigfield, 1988). Second, the worry component of test anxiety is more closely linked to performance and achievement than the emotionality component (Covington, 1992). The effects on performance have been suggested as resulting from various sources including interference with attentional processes and thus increased cognitive load, as well as to a more generalized lack of access to efficient learning strategies which impacts encoding and retrieval (Schutz & Davis, 2000; Zeidner, 1998). Given these findings, it was hypothesized that worry would be negatively related to achievement. 1. P a g e | 25 Summary and Purpose of the Study Pintrich (2003) provided a comprehensive review and summary of recent work and unanswered questions and issues in research and theory that connects motivation, selfregulation, learning strategies, and achievement. In the review, Pintrich provided summary generalizations about the role of motivational beliefs in learning and achievement, but a raised a key question about whether they hold for all ethnic groups (e.g. African American, Asian American, Latino, Native American, etc.) in western cultures as well as in other nonwestern cultures. There is some evidence from large scale research studies that there is invariance among a broad range of cultural groups on selfregulatory, motivational, and learning strategy constructs (Marsh, Hau, Artlet, Baumert, & Peschar, 2005). However, there is also some evidence for cultural specificity in these domains (McInerney & Van Etten, 2001; Urdan, 2004; Urdan & Giancarlo, 2001; McInerney, Watkins, Akande, & Lee, 2003; Rueda & Chen, 2005; Vansteenviste, Zhou, Lens, & Soenens, 2005). While the learning and motivation literature has increasingly recognized sociocultural factors as important, there is only limited work exploring the robustness of the relationships among these factors for students from different ethnic, linguistic, and cultural groups, especially in the U.S. context with specific groups such as the increasingly large Latino population. To address this gap, this study included constructs tapping students’ approaches to learning, motivation related factors (e.g. academic efficacy and goal orientation), opportunity to learn (e.g. advanced level of math and science education in high school), and demographic/background variables (e.g. gender, ethnicity, SES and prior knowledge). The first specific purpose of this study was to examine cross-ethnic 1. P a g e | 26 invariance across White and Latino students in terms of self-regulatory beliefs, learning strategies, and motivational constructs in a four year college setting. In addition, a second purpose of the study was to develop and test a structural model of relationships between, student background characteristics, opportunity to learn, learning strategies, motivational variables and academic achievement. Method Research Context The study examined the relationship between learning strategies and academic achievement and the differences between 1Hispanic and non-Hispanic students’ use of strategies, educational attitudes, and motivation to learn in a four year state college setting in the Southwest. This college selects from among the top 30% of high school students in California. At the time of the study, the college had a total of 16,479 enrolled students. The majority of students attending were of Mexican American, Hispanic descent. The breakdown for the campus by ethnicity was 36.9% Hispanic, 13.0% African American, .9% Native American, 6.3% Asians, 0.5% Pacific Islander, 36.4% White, 3.0% Filipino, and 3.1% other ethnicities. The percentage of new freshmen was 30.41% and 44.2% of the new transfers attended local community colleges. [Noelle, is there a CSUSB reference we can cite here?] 1 There are a variety of terms found in the literature including the generic terms Latino and Hispanic as well as more population-specific terms such as Mexican-American or Cuban. There is not widespread agreement about the use of a single term, and arguments for different choices based on political and other considerations are numerous. In this report we use the term Hispanic because it is the designation used by the research site., 1. P a g e | 27 Subjects There were two components to the data collection, a phone survey and an online survey. The final number of students contacted for the phone survey was 2232 nonHispanics (63.4% of the total sample) of which the majority were White (1,402, 39.8%) and the remainder of the sample were Hispanic/Latino (1,295, 38.7%). The final number of students responding to the online survey was 2,012 (1,307 65% non-Hispanics and 705 35% Hispanics). Measures The measures included questions about students’ demographic characteristics, educational attitudes and beliefs, academic efficacy, and learning strategies such as elaboration and control. Background variables. For the students’ background variables, Socioeconomic Status (SES) was measured by using a combination of mother’s highest degree of education (1 = didn’t finish high school, 4 = graduated from college) and income (1 = less than $25,000, 4 = more than $75,000). Two questions assessed students’ Prior achievement level (Prior knowledge) including high school grade point average (GPA, M = 3.13, SD = 0.47), SAT1 verbal score (M = 455.33, SD = 91.27) and SAT1 math score (M = 462.18, SD = 86.93). Opportunity to learn. This construct measured whether students had received different educational or instructional ingredients that make up quality school experience. Our questions included “how many semesters of Advanced Placement (AP) math did you take in high school?” and “how many semesters of AP science did you take in high school?” The questions ranged from 1 to 8. 1. P a g e | 28 Motivation and attitudes. The questions about educational attitudes and motivation were revised from Marsh et al. (2006)’s Student Approaches to Learning (SAL) instrument. Academic efficacy consisted of four items asking students’ beliefs in their capabilities to master the skills being taught and to overcome barriers through their own efforts. The Worry scale was derived from Spielberger, Gonzlez, and Taylor (1980)’s Text Anxiety Inventory (α = .92) and is conceptualized as cognitive concern over failure. Eight items were used to indicate how they felt while taking the exam and how they felt anxiety on failing the exam. Students were instructed to select a number ranging from 1(almost never) to 4 (almost always) for each item of these scales. Personal motivational beliefs. The items from this scale were from Anderman, Urdan and Roeser (2003)’s Patterns of Adaptive Learning Scale. Three personal goal orientations were assessed. Mastery goal orientation included five items reflecting students’ emphasis on learning and their concern with understanding, developing competence and improvement. (α = .85). Performance approach goal orientation consisted of five items that tapped into students’ desire to demonstrate competence, for example by outperforming others in their class work (α = .89). Four items assessed performance avoidance goal orientation, reflecting students’ concern with not appearing incompetent or less competent than others in their class work (α = .74). Students indicated their response to each item on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all true, 5 = very much true). Learning strategies. Students’ use of different learning strategies was assessed with items derived from Marsh et al. (2006)’s Student Approaches to Learning (SAL) instrument. Elaboration consisted of four items measuring how often students engage in 1. P a g e | 29 construction, integration, and transfer strategies when completing class work. Control included five items and measured the extent to which students adopt a self-regulating perspective during the learning process. This scale asked questions about whether students engage in activities such as checking that they remember what they have already learned or what they do not yet understand (α = .83). Effort included four items tapping into students’ levels of effort expended, as well as their willingness to continue to try even when presented with difficult material. Students responded on a 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always) in those scales. Finally, six questions were adapted from Meuschke, Dembo, and Gribbons’s (2006) Adaptive Help-Seeking Scale to assess students’ use of maladaptive help-seeking strategies (e.g. guessing, simply avoiding the work, or giving up the task without asking for help). The items were scored on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 5 (very much true) (α = .72). Each of the scales above was averaged to form 9 distinct constructs (Elaboration, Control, Maladaptive help-seeking, Effort, Worry, Academic efficacy, Mastery goal orientation, Performance approach goal orientation, and Performance avoidance goal orientation). Academic achievement. The outcome variable of this study was measured by students’ total grade point average (GPA) (M = 2.92, SD = 0.52). Procedure Data were obtained from the Office of Institutional Research in the form of a list of all 16,488 students registered at the college for the 2006 Fall quarter. Graduate students were deleted from the list, as were students who were not American citizens. Further, 1. P a g e | 30 students with no contact information were deleted. The final sampling frame contained contact information for 11,569 undergraduate students. Our data were collected in the fall of 2006 using two methods of data collection—a phone survey that included 3,523 students (2,232 non-Hispanics and 1,295 Hispanics) and an online survey that included 2,012 students (1,306 non-Hispanics and 705 Hispanics). Telephone interview. Students were initially contacted by phone in order to explain the purpose of the research. Interviewers asked the student respondents a few questions to elicit some basic demographic data as well as some initial questions regarding their learning strategies. In addition, interviewers confirmed the student’s e-mail address so that a link to a web survey could be sent. The link to the web survey was then sent to each student, with at least two follow-up reminders sent to all non-respondents. In addition, a lottery system was used by which students could win money/coupons to be used to purchase items at the University’s bookstore. In the telephone survey part of the data collection, telephone surveys on demographics and students’ background were conducted by trained staff from a research institute of the college, using computer assisted telephone interviewing (CATI) equipment and software. In order to maximize the chances of finding a student at home, telephone surveys were conducted Monday through Friday, from 9:00 AM – 9:00 PM and weekends (Saturday 10am – 5pm and Sunday 1pm – 7pm). In an effort to ensure the quality and reliability of the interviews, institute research staff supervised all interviews conducted. To further ensure quality control, supervisory personnel randomly selected completed interviews (at least one completion per interviewer) and made call-backs for verification. A pairwise method was used for missing data. 1. P a g e | 31 Online survey. For the second part of the data collection, on-line surveys related to academic motivation and the use of learning strategies were completed from the students’ own computer or in computer labs in the college. [Note from HON to Noelle: cite PISA?] Data Analysis The first part of the analysis focused on evaluating the psychometric characteristics of the measures. We examined internal consistency of scales by calculating Cronbach’s alpha coefficients. Also, in order to evaluate responses to the same instrument by different groups (in this case Latino and white students) separately, the second part of analysis examine whether the constructs were same across White and Latino/a groups. Therefore, multiple group confirmatory factor analysis (MG-CFA) was employed to test factorial invariance between those groups. MG-CFA test traditionally posits a series of nested models in which the end-point is the least restrictive model with no invariance constraints and the most restrictive (total invariance) model with all parameters constrained to be equal across all groups. Thus, we first examined the extent to which the theoretical structure of the instrument was invariant to ensure the structural equivalence of underlying constructs between White and Latino/a groups. We also investigated the extent to which the factor patterns and weighing of loadings were invariant, in order to ensure the equivalent interpretation of item content across two different groups. Finally, we aimed to determine how demographic and background information related to motivation/learning strategies and academic achievement for White and Latino/a college students. We initially developed a model for the entire sample (n = 3,257) to test the theoretical structural equation model (SEM) and then we estimated two single-group 1. P a g e | 32 SEMs using White students and Latino students to compare the significant path coefficients between two groups. Results Reliabilities, Descriptive Statistics and Intercorrelations Reliabilities. Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients were computed for 9 scales on motivation/learning strategies items. Scale reliabilities for all scales were generally satisfactory (see Table 1). Table 1. Reliability coefficients for all students (N = 3527) N of items Cronbach’s α Elaboration 4 .82 Control 5 .71 Maladaptive Help6 .86 seeking Academic Efficacy 4 .82 Effort 4 .80 Worry 8 .89 Mastery Goal 5 .87 orientation Performance Approach 5 .93 Performance Avoidance 4 .86 All 45 .84 For the total sample of 3,527 participants, reliability coefficients varied from .71 to .93 (M = .84). This average reliability and the reliabilities of each of the 9 scales are higher than at least .70, suggesting that the measures used in this study appears to meet basic psychometric criteria. Table 2 presents the reliability estimates for Latino/a and White students. Generally, the coefficients of White students were higher than those of Latino/a students. 1. P a g e | 33 Table 2. Reliability coefficients for Latino/a and White students N of items Elaboration Control Maladaptive Helpseeking Academic Efficacy Effort Worry Mastery Goal orientation Performance Approach Performance Avoidance All 4 5 6 α .80 .71 .86 Latino/a (n = 1,125) Mean SD 2.81 .65 3.07 .53 2.25 .83 4 4 8 5 5 4 45 .81 .81 .88 .85 .91 .85 .86 2.72 3.05 2.09 4.08 1.98 2.26 2.65 .62 .62 .70 .73 .96 .99 .35 α .83 .72 .87 White (n = 1,156) Mean 2.90 3.06 2.05 SD .66 .54 .77 .83 .82 .90 .88 .93 .87 .84 2.84 3.06 1.83 3.90 2.17 2.41 2.61 .64 .62 .67 .76 1.00 .99 .32 Descriptive analyses were conducted to provide overall information of the study variables. Table 3 presents means and standard deviations of primary variables, and Tables 4 and 5 present the data by group. Table 6 provides a comparison of mean differences between groups, and Figure 1 provides a graphical representation. Table 3. Descriptive statistics for study variables (N=3,527) Variable High School GPA SAT1 verbal SAT1 math Elaboration Control Maladaptive Help-seeking Academic Efficacy Effort Worry Mastery Goal orientation Performance Approach Performance Avoidance Total GPA M 3.10 455.6 462.4 2.88 3.08 2.12 2.78 3.05 1.96 3.99 2.12 2.32 2.84 SD .47 90.7 86.3 .66 .54 .79 .64 .62 .69 .77 .99 .99 .53 Observed Range 1.1-4.3 200-730 200-790 1-4 1-4 1-5 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-5 1-5 1-5 .67-4.25 1. P a g e | 34 Table 4. Descriptive statistics for study variables for White students (N=1,156) Variable High School GPA SAT1 verbal SAT1 math Elaboration Control Maladaptive Help-seeking Academic Efficacy Effort Worry Mastery Goal orientation Performance Approach Performance Avoidance Total GPA M 3.21 486.3 495.9 2.90 3.06 2.05 2.84 3.06 1.83 3.90 2.17 2.41 3.04 SD .49 90.7 86.3 .66 .54 .79 .64 .62 .67 .77 1.0 .99 .50 Observed Range 1.6-4.3 200-730 200-790 1-4 1-4 1-5 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-5 1-5 1-5 1.26-4.25 Table 5. Descriptive statistics for study variables for Latino students (N=1,125) Variable High School GPA SAT1 verbal SAT1 math Elaboration Control Maladaptive Help-seeking Academic Efficacy Effort Worry Mastery Goal orientation Performance Approach Performance Avoidance Total GPA M 3.10 436.6 444.5 2.81 3.07 2.25 2.72 3.03 2.09 4.08 1.98 2.26 2.78 SD .43 90.7 86.8 .65 .53 .82 .62 .63 .70 .73 .96 .99 .52 Observed Range 1.6-4.3 200-670 200-680 1-4 1-4 1-5 1-4 1-4 1-4 1-5 1-5 1-5 1.15-4.00 1. P a g e | 35 Table 6. Mean differences between White and Latino students Elaboration Control Maladaptive Helpseeking Academic Efficacy Effort Worry Mastery Goal orientation Performance Approach Performance Avoidance High School GPA Total GPA White (n = 1,156) 2.90 3.06 2.05 Latino (n = 1,125) 2.81 3.07 2.25 F (1, 2,218) 6.54** .03 22.37*** p .011 .857 .000 2.84 3.06 1.83 3.90 2.17 2.41 3.05 3.05 2.72 3.03 2.09 4.08 1.98 2.26 2.78 2.78 11.30*** 1.03 49.85*** 19.70*** 12.46*** 7.42** 19.10*** 129.1*** .001 .310 .000 .000 .000 .007 .000 .000 1. Figure 1. White and Latino comparison on study variables P a g e | 36 1. P a g e | 37 Correlational Analyses Based on Theoretical Expectations Correlations among motivation/ learning strategies variables. In evaluating the nature of relations among scales, we began by examining the patterns of correlations consistent with theoretical expectations. The relationships among scales revealed that all of the correlations (see Table 7 for the entire sample and Tables 8 and 9 for each group) among the adaptive learning strategies (elaboration, control, and effort) measures were positive and statistically significant (rs = .38 to .59, p < .01). Elaboration, control, and effort scales were positively correlated with the efficacy scale (rs =.40, .39, .39, respectively, p < .01) but negatively correlated with worry (rs = −.07, −.09, −.07, p < .01). Maladaptive help-seeking had a negative correlation with efficacy (r = −.33, p < .01) but it was positively correlated with worry (r = .38, p < .01). With regard to personal motivational beliefs, two of the performance goal orientation scales (performance approach and performance avoidance) were found to be highly correlated with each other (r = .74, p < .01). However, those performance goal orientation scales showed no statistically significant correlations with mastery goal orientation. Mastery goal orientation scale was positively correlated with all of the learning strategies (elaboration; r = .39, p < .01, control; r = .48, p < .01, effort; r = .54, p < .01). Both of the performance goal orientation scales were correlated only with elaboration (rs = .07 to .03, p < .01) and effort (r = −.09, p < .01). Finally, performance goal orientation scales showed positive correlations with worry (rs = .13 to .18, p < .01) whereas mastery goal orientation showed no statistically significant correlation with worry. 1. P a g e | 38 Table 7. Bivariate correlations among primary indicators of motivation and learning strategies (White and Latino students N=2,281) 1 - 2 1. Gender (Female=0) 2. Ethnicity (White=0) .06** - 3. AP Math .01 .03 - 4. AP Science .01 .00 .61** - 5. High school GPA -.12** .12** .09** .05 - 6. SAT1 verbal .09** .27** .10** .16** .25** - 7. SAT1 math .20** .29** .16** .16** .30** .62** - 8. Efficacy .18** .09** .10** .11** .11** .25** .17** - 9. Worry -.12** -.19** -.07* -.04 -.19** -.34** -.34** -.38** - 10. Elaboration .05 .07* .05 .06* .07 .14** .11** .40** -.07** - 11. Control -.07* -.01 .05 .03 .12** .02 -.02 .39** -.09** .45** - 12. Effort -.12** .03 .05 .01 .05 -.11** -.11** .39** -.07** .38** .59** - 13. Mastery goal .00 -.12** .05 .05 -.01 -.07 -.12** .35** -.03 .39** .48** .54** - .04 .10** .05 .07* .05 .04 .04 .03 .13** .07** -.03 -.09** -.02 - .01 .08** .02 .02 .08 .06 .07 -.03 .18** .03 -.01 -.09** -.02 .74** - .03 -.13** -.05 -.01 -.09* -.05 .00 -.33** .38** -.25** -.33** -.42** -.28** .22** .30** - -.03 .25** .01 .01 .38** .39** .32** .24** -.34** .12** .17** .20** .05 .04 .02 -.26** 14. Performance approach 15. Performance avoidance 16. Maladaptive helpseek 17. Total GPA 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 - 1. P a g e | 39 Table 8. Bivariate correlations among primary indicators of motivation and learning strategies (White students N=1,156) 1. Gender (Female=0) 1 - 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 2. AP Math .01 - 3. AP Science .01 .68** - 5. SAT1 verbal 6. SAT1 math -.15** .09** .18** .06 .06 .13** .06 .13** .17** .24** .31** .62** - 7. Efficacy .15** .10** .10** .11 .31** .18** - 8. Worry -.13** -.06 -.05 -.14** -.42** -.34** -.44** - .01 .03 .04 .07 .12* .08 .40** -.12** - 10. Control -.10* .05 .01 .14* .02 -.01 .38** -.14** .46** - 11. Effort -.18** .02 -.03 .04 -.18** -.11* .33** -.10** .40** .61** - -.04 .04 .01 .02 -.06 -.04 .30** -.07 .44** .51** .54** - .02 .05 .04 .10 .03 .00 .05 .12** .05 -.05 -.09* .02 - -.02 .00 -.03 .14* .07 .04 -.02 .18* .02 .03 -.09* .00 .75** - .04 .00 -.02 -.06 .00 .05 -.32** .35** -.28** -.39** -.46** -.34** .19** .26** - -.07* -.01 .00 .41** .38** .25** .26** -.33** .13** .20** .26** .15** .05 .04 -.25** 4. High school GPA 9. Elaboration 12. Mastery goal 13. Performance approach 14. Performance avoidance 15. Maladaptive helpseek 16. Total GPA 16 - 1. P a g e | 40 Table 9. Bivariate correlations among primary indicators of motivation and learning strategies (Latino students N=1,125) 1. Gender (Female=0) 2. AP Math 3. AP Science 1 - 2 .00 - 3 4 5 6 .19** .24** .57** - 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 .01 .53** - 5. SAT1 verbal 6. SAT1 math -.11** .06 .19** .10** .11** .17** .04 .19** .17** 7. Efficacy .21** .10** .12** .09 .17** .13* - 8. Worry -.10* -.07 -.03 -.19** -.21** -.27* -.29** - 9. Elaboration .08* .07 .08 .05 .13* .11* .39** .01 - 10. Control -.04 .05 .06 .10 .03 -.02 .41** -.05 .44** - 11. Effort -.04 .09* .05 .04 -.07 -.14* .46** -.04 .36** .57** - 12. Mastery goal .06 .06 .10* .00 -.02 -.13* .44** -.04 .36** .45** .56** - .06 .04 .11** -.02 .00 .03 .00 .19** .09* .00 -10* -.04 - .05 .04 .08 .00 .01 .07 -.05 .21** .03 .02 -.10* -.01 .73** - .03 -.11** .00 -.08 -.03 .05 -.32** .38** -.21** -.28** -.39** -.25** .29** .37* - -.01 .02 .01 .34** .36** .31** .18** -.27** .09* .16** .13** -01 -.01 -06 -.22** 4. High school GPA 13. Performance approach 14. Performance avoidance 15. Maladaptive helpseek 16. Total GPA 16 - 1. P a g e | 41 With regard to personal motivational beliefs, two of the performance goal orientation scales (performance approach and performance avoidance) were found to be highly correlated with each other (r = .74, p < .01). However, those performance goal orientation scales showed no statistically significant correlations with mastery goal orientation. Mastery goal orientation scale was positively correlated with all of the learning strategies (elaboration; r = .39, p < .01, control; r = .48, p < .01, effort; r = .54, p < .01). Both of the performance goal orientation scales were correlated only with elaboration (rs = .07 to .03, p < .01) and effort (r = −.09, p < .01). Finally, performance goal orientation scales showed positive correlations with worry (rs = .13 to .18, p < .01) whereas mastery goal orientation showed no statistically significant correlation with worry. Correlations between learning strategies and achievement. Consistent with the findings from prior research, learning strategies were positively correlated with the achievement variable (elaboration; r = .12, p < .01, control; r = .17, p < .01, effort; r = .20, p < .01). However, maladaptive help-seeking had a negative correlation with achievement (r = −.26, p < .01), Academic efficacy was also positively correlated with achievement (r = .24, p < .01), whereas worry was negatively correlated (r = −.34, p < .01). Research on worry supports the negative effects of worry and academic outcome (Elliot & McGregor, 1999; Vagg & Papsdorf, 1995). The intercorrelations among key variables are presented in Tables 7-9. Multi-group Confirmatory Factor Analysis (MG-CFA) Preliminary CFA for overall model. Preliminary confirmatory factor analyses were conducted with M plus 3.13 (Muthén & Muthén, 2004) using maximum likelihood 1. P a g e | 42 estimation. Usually, a researcher posits a priori structure of hypothesized constructs and tests the fit of a solution based on the structure of the data. Thus, we hypothesized a 9factor model in which the items for each factor load on their respective latent variable. When using a chi-square test of fit, although nonsignificant p values are preferred, large samples increase the likelihood of obtaining significant p values (Hu & Bentler, 1995). Thus, we used additional indices to evaluate model fit: (a) Comparative Fit Index (CFI), (b) Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), and (c) root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA). The results supported the hypothesized theoretical model, indicating that all the factor loadings were significant and our 9-factor solution had a good fit to the data with regard to acceptable levels of fit (Comparative Fit Index [CFA] = .96; Tucker-Lewis Index [TLI] = .96; root-mean-square error of approximation [RMSEA] = .03), although the chi-square value was statistically significant (χ2= 2429.14, df = 878, p = .000). It should be noted that chi-square is sensitive to sample size, and the larger the sample size, the more likely the rejection of the model and the more likely a Type II error. Thus, as the case here, with large samples, even small differences between the observed model and the perfect-fit model may be found significant. One fit index that is less sensitive to sample size is the chi-square/df statistic (Bryant & Yarnold, 2001). In this case, the value of χ2/df was 2.77, within acceptable limits. Table 10 presents the factor structure and standardized factor loadings for this model. Table 10. Standardized factor loadings and 9 factor solutions for all students. Factor Item Standardized factor loading Elaboration Q1 .64 Q2 .34 Q3 .69 Q4 .86 Control Q9 .69 1. Maladaptive Help-seeking Efficacy Effort Worry Mastery goal Performance approach Performance avoidance Q5 Q6 Q7 Q8 Q19 Q16 Q17 Q18 Q20 Q21 Q25 Q22 Q23 Q24 Q29 Q26 Q27 Q28 Q30 Q31 Q32 Q33 Q34 Q35 Q36 Q37 Q46 Q44 Q45 Q47 Q48 Q53 Q49 Q50 Q51 Q52 Q56 Q54 Q55 Q57 P a g e | 43 .39 .55 .63 .56 .75 .71 .60 .66 .76 .72 .80 .53 .69 .79 .68 .63 .67 .75 .62 .79 .60 .66 .81 .76 .69 .74 .79 .64 .79 .76 .68 .94 .70 .84 .78 .90 .81 .67 .79 .84 Multiple-group CFA. For this aspect of the analysis, we selected the two ethnic groups focused on in the study, including White (n = 1,156, 32.8%) and Latino (n = 1,125, 31.9%). Our confirmatory factor analysis next dealt with the question of whether 1. P a g e | 44 the 9 measures were equivalent across the White and Latino/a subgroups. The assumption of structural equivalence was tested using a multi-group CFA that allows factorial invariance across groups. Testing for factorial invariance involves comparing a set of models in which factor structure and factor loadings are held equal across groups, and assessing fit indices when elements of these structures are constrained (Marsh et al., 2006). Thus, we developed an initial baseline model (Model 1), which is totally noninvariant in that there were no assumptions being made about group invariance across groups. Then we imposed invariance constraints for factor loadings and factor means (Model 2), and factor variances (Model 3). Table 11 presents the fit indices for the model comparison for 9 factors. 1. P a g e | 45 Table 11. Goodness of fit statistics for the multiple-group comparison for the 9 factors. Model 1 2 3 Invariance Constraints None (Totally non-invariant) Invariance of factor loadings, factor means Invariance of factor loadings , factor means, and factor variances *** p < .000, ** p < .01 χ2 df CFI TLI RMSEA Δ χ2 (Δdf) AIC BIC 3199.21 χ2 / df 1.79 1792 .951 .946 .038 -- 106571.46 108417.91 3360.47 1.82 1837 .947 .943 .039 161.26(45) *** 106642.72 108263.37 3397.08 1.83 1855 .946 .943 .039 36.61(18)** 106643.33 108173.67 P a g e | 46 Model 1 examined whether the items in each of the 9 factors grouped in accordance with theoretical expectations (χ2= 3199.21, df = 1,792, p = .000, χ2/df = 2.14, CFI=.95, TLI=.95, RMSEA=.04). Model 2 tested whether factor loadings and factor means were the same across groups. This model is important because a minimal criterion for structural equivalence is that the factor loadings relating each indicator to its hypothesized factor are the same across all groups. (χ2= 3360.47, df = 1,837, p = .000, χ2/df = 1.83, CFI=.947, TLI=.943, RMSEA=.039). The difference in chi-square between the constrained and unconstrained models was significant (Δχ2 = 161.26, Δdf = 45, p <.000), however, the differences in RMSEA and other fit indices were very small. We then tested the assumption that factor variances as well as factor loadings and factor means were equal across groups (Model 3). The most restrictive model provided a good fit to the data (χ2 = 3397.08, df = 1,855, p = .000, χ2/df = 1.83, CFI=.946, TLI=.943, RMSEA=.039), and the Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) showed that this model best fit to the data. Although all three models showed good fit, Model 3 is most parsimonious in terms of number of parameters to be estimated. In summary, there was good support for the invariance of factor loadings, factor means and factor variances across White and Latino/a groups. Structural Equation Model (SEM) Analysis SEM MIMIC model for all students. An approach based on a multiple-indicatormultiple-indicator cause model (MIMIC; Kaplan, 2000, Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1993) was used for evaluating relations between motivational and learning strategy factors and background variables. This extended SEM MIMIC model was seen as appropriate here given that it is based on latent constructs having measurement error and that it allows for a test of the P a g e | 47 underlying structural model (Marsh et al., 2006). We hypothesized a structural model (see Figure 2) among cognitive and motivational factors and other criterion variables. P a g e | 48 Figure 2. A hypothesized structural equation model for cognitive and motivational factors Learning strategies self efficacy Gender SES academic achievement us min OTL min u s performance approach worry Ethnicity performance avoidance maladaptive help-seeking Prior Knowledge mi mastery goal s nu P a g e | 49 Figure 3 shows the results of SEM estimating for all students. Learning strategies factor was indicated by three 1st order factors (control, effort, and elaboration). −−For the overall sample, the results indicated that the model produced a good fit to the data (χ2 = 3694.90, df = 1,371, p = .000, χ2/df = 2.70, CFI=.94, TLI=.93, RMSEA=.03). Figure 3. Estimation of cognitive and motivational factors of academic achievement (all groups) (N= 2,281) control .97 .32 0.1 self efficacy Gender effort 0.38 elaboration learning strategies 2 0. 1 .98 .98 18 0. OTL 0.07 5 0. 1 performance approach -0 .0 7 academic achievement -0.3 SES 0.5 -0.1 8 7 4 0. 1 7 0.0 0 .2 performance avoidance 0.08 7 0 .55 0.14 .59 4 .98 3 .1 -0 0.3 worry .83 9 0 .2 Ethnicity -0.27 prior knowledge -0.24 -0.1 8 maladaptive help-seeking .78 mastery goal .70 .94 Most of the path coefficients were statistically significant. In particular, SES had positive effects on opportunity to learn (measured by number of semesters taking AP math and science) (β = .15, p < .001) and prior knowledge (measured by high school GPA, SAT verbal and math scores) (β = .25, p < .001), suggesting that students who had higher SES had higher opportunities to learn and a higher level of achievement in high school. Ethnicity had a direct effect on prior knowledge (β = −.27, p < .001), showing that White students P a g e | 50 achieved better in high schools than Latino/a peers. In terms of the relationships between the motivational variables and background variables, we found significant effects of opportunity to learn on efficacy (β = .12, p < .001), performance approach goal orientation (β = .07, p < .001), and mastery goal orientation (β = .07, p < .001). Students who experienced more learning opportunities in high school showed higher academic efficacy and higher performance approach and mastery goal orientations in college. The use of adaptive learning strategies (a latent factor indicated by effort, control and elaboration) was strongly affected by motivational factors including academic efficacy (β = .38, p < .001) and mastery goal orientation (β = .57, p < .001). As expected, students with high mastery goal orientation reported greater use of learning strategies, which in turn, positively predicted academic achievement (measured by GPA, β = .18, p < .001). Also, efficacy was found to have a positive influence on learning strategies (β = .38, p < .001). However, students who reported higher use of maladaptive help-seeking strategies were found to have a stronger performance avoidance goal orientation (β = .29, p < .001). There was a relatively high positive influence of maladaptive help-seeking (β = .29, p < .001) and performance avoidance (β = .07, p < .001) on worry. Students were found to have lower GPA when they felt more worry (β = −.18, p < .001) and when they employed more maladaptive help-seeking (β = −.13, p < .001). Single-group SEMs (Latino vs. White). Next, we removed ethnicity from the overall model and performed two single-group SEM analyses for White and Latino/a students in order to compare the differences in the relationships among motivation, learning strategies and academic achievement (See Figures 4 and 5). The SEM results produced good fits to data (Latino/a group - χ2 = 2498.05, df = 1,322, p = .000, χ2/df = 1.90, CFI=.92, TLI=.92, P a g e | 51 RMSEA=.03; White - χ2 = 2540.59, df = 1,322, p = .000, χ2/df = 1.92, CFI=.94, TLI=.93, RMSEA=.03). Figure 4. Estimation of cognitive and motivational factors of academic achievement for White students. (N = 1,156) .99 .29 self efficacy Gender .99 0.33 learning strategies 9 0.0 0. OTL 4 -0.1 academic achievement 25 performance approach 5 -0 . 0.53 0.6 SES 21 .98 .97 prior knowledge .97 worry 0.11 0.18 performance avoidance 4 0.2 0.25 .86 maladaptive help-seeking -0.3 mastery goal .99 4 .68 .56 P a g e | 52 Figure 5. Estimation of cognitive and motivational factors of academic achievement for Latino students. (N = 1,125) .95 .31 self efficacy 0.39 0.1 6 OTL 6 0.2 0.1 3 .98 academic achievement 35 -0. performance approach .94 SES learning strategies -0 .1 6 0.14 Gender 0.1 worry 0.11 prior knowledge .79 0. 5 3 5 .99 -0 .15 0.46 0. 46 performance avoidance -0 .2 0.3 0.39 .83 maladaptive help-seeking 8 .69 mastery goal .91 For Latino/a students, the overall relationships among opportunities to learn, prior level of achievement and motivation factors were similar to those of all students (e.g. positive effects of motivation factors (efficacy, mastery goal) on learning strategies). However, unlike the overall SEM, the Latino subgroup did not show any significant effect of prior knowledge on performance avoidance, although they showed a positive effect of opportunity to learn on performance approach (β = .12, p < .001). We also found that male Latino students reported more academic efficacy than female Latino students (β = .14, p < .001), as expected in findings from the studies on gender and self-efficacy. Efficacy and mastery influenced learning strategies positively (β = .39 and β = .53, respectively, p < .001) but efficacy had negatively influence help-seeking (β = −.34, p = .001). Maladaptive helpseeking strategies were positively associated with worry (β = .30, p < .001). The pattern of .71 P a g e | 53 effects on GPA (positive effects of prior knowledge, negative effects of worry and helpseeking) was similar to those of the overall model. For White students, the results showed different patterns in relations of SES to prior knowledge and opportunity to learn. White female students achieved more in high school than their male peers (β = −.14, p < .001) and students who had more OTL reported higher self-efficacy (β = .09, p < .001). Also, there was no effect of opportunity to learn on mastery goal orientation, whereas there was a positive effect of prior knowledge on performance avoidance (β = .19, p < .001). Self-efficacy and mastery goal orientation positively affected learning strategies (β = .33 and β = .65, respectively, p < .001). Like Latino students, both of maladaptive help-seeking and performance avoidance positively affected worry (β = .24 and β = .11, respectively, p < .001) and worry had a negative effect on achievement (β = −.25, p < .001). Prior knowledge and learning strategies were found to be two prominent predictors of academic achievement (β = .53 and β = .21, respectively, p < .001). Multiple-group SEM: White and Latino groups. Finally, we analyzed a multi-group SEM for White and Latino groups to examine the extent to which parameter estimates in our theoretical model differ between White and Latino groups. (See Figure 6). We first estimated the constrained model, which fixed all common parameters between groups. Our constrained model resulted in a CFI of .91 and TLI of .90 with χ2 = 6083.70 (df = 2,773, χ2/df = 2.19, p < .000). We then performed chi-square difference tests several times, in order to identify which pairs of path coefficients were significantly different in multiple groups. For example, we freed the parameter of the path from gender to prior knowledge to test whether there was a significant chi-square decrease. The model improved P a g e | 54 significantly (Δχ2 = 82.11, Δdf = 1, p <.001) which means that the there was a significant difference in parameter estimates between two groups. Figure 6. Multiple-group (White/Latino) estimation of cognitive and motivational factors of academic achievement (White=1,156, Latino=1,125) learning strategies self efficacy .55 /.2 1 Gender 0/ .0 9 OTL -.12/ns performance approach .0 1/ -.0 SES academic achievement 1.0 .9 2/0 - .2 6/ - .4 9 4/0 -.2 4 .11 /ns 4 worry prior knowledge .25/.17 performance avoidance maladaptive help-seeking 8/-.0 .49 mastery goal In other words, although both of the standardized path coefficients were negative, the effect of gender on prior knowledge was slightly greater for Latino students (β = −.04, p <.001) than for White students (β = −.01, p <.001). Figure 4 shows different effects for White and Latino students for our theoretical model.2 2 All of the unconstrained model results in this section produced good fit indices (CFI and TLIs = .90 to .91 and RMSEAs < .04). All of the chi-square difference tests reported in this section showed significant model improvement, indicated by significant chi-square decrease. P a g e | 55 Latino students had a positive effect of OTL on self-efficacy (β = .09, p <.001) but White students showed no effect of OTL. Prior knowledge had a negative effect on mastery goal orientation but had a stronger relation for Latino students (β = −.08 for White and β = −.49 for Latino, p < .001). Prior knowledge was also one of the strong predictor of academic achievement (β = 1.02 for White and β = .99 for Latino, p < .001). Interestingly, it revealed that there was a weaker effect of prior knowledge on performance avoidance for Latino students (β = .17, p <.001) than for White students (β = .25, p <.001). Performance approach goal orientation showed a different pattern of relationship between groups. It had a negative impact on GPA for White students (β = −.12, p <.001) but no effect for Latinos. The effects of adaptive/maladaptive learning strategies also showed some interesting differences in White and Latino groups. First, while both groups had positive relations of learning strategies to GPA, the latent factor had a greater impact on GPA among White (β = .55, p <.001) than Latino (β = .21, p <.001). Second, maladaptive help-seeking had negative effects for both White and Latino groups, but the relation was stronger for White (β = −.46, p <.001) than for Latino (β = −.24, p <.001). Lastly, worry had a negative effect on achievement for White students (β = −.24, p <.001) but no effect for Latinos. Summary Findings Scale reliabilities were generally satisfactory: For the total sample of 3,523 participants, reliability estimates of the measures ranged from .71 to .93. Confirmatory factor analysis results for the total combined sample showed that the cognitive and motivational factors of effective learning strategies were well defined in a 9-factor solution (elaboration, control, maladaptive help-seeking, efficacy, effort, worry, mastery goal orientation, performanceapproach goal orientation, performance- avoidance goal orientation) and the results of the P a g e | 56 multiple-group CFA indicated that there was good support for a totally invariant model with a 9-factor solution across White and Latino groups. For the overall sample, SEM results indicated that the model fit the data, suggesting that prior knowledge and learning strategies were positively related to students’ achievement measured by GPAs, while maladaptive helpseeking and worry were negatively related. In general, sociocultural factors were related to factors predictive of achievement and were consistent with what would be expected given previous research and theory. For example, SES was positively related to OTL and prior knowledge. Male students have higher self-efficacy than female students. In addition, students who had higher self-efficacy endorsed a performance-approach goal orientation more than student who had lower efficacy. In addition, the positive impact of prior knowledge on performance goals but negative effects on mastery goal orientation is consistent with motivational theories regarding students who may have weak academic backgrounds. The results of goal orientation variables were also largely consistent with prior research. A mastery goal orientation was strongly related to learning strategies and negatively to maladaptive help-seeking, while a performance avoidance goal approach related positively to maladaptive help seeking and worry. As expected, worry and maladaptive help-seeking negatively related to achievement. In terms of ethnic differences, gender was related negatively to prior knowledge for both of groups, but Latino groups had slightly greater relations among those. SES was positively related to prior knowledge for White students but not Latino students. For White students, having a family with higher income and more educated mothers significantly affected students’ prior levels of achievement, but this relation was not statistically P a g e | 57 significant for Latino students. Latino students showed a stronger association between OTL and academic efficacy than White students. For White students, the effect of prior knowledge on the achievement was greater than for Latino students. White group also appeared to be more influenced by maladaptive help-seeking and worry, compared to Latino group. Although both groups had positive effects of learning strategies on GPA, the relation of White students was greater than that of Latino students. Discussion and Conclusion The results of this study provide support for factors that previous literature and theory has shown to be important for self-regulated learning and the relationships to motivational and learning strategy use. From an educational perspective, this is encouraging news, since the motivational and learning strategies of the type examined in this investigation are amenable to instruction and modifiable. This is important because of a tendency of postsecondary institutions, as well as the field of education in general, to consider low achievement a function of unmodifiable student or family deficits (Bensimon, 2005, Valencia, 1997). While there is some concern in psychological research about the tendency to focus on a limited range of sociocultural contexts and populations (Arnett, 2008), there is good evidence that there are similarities across sociocultural groups with respect to the motivational and learning-related constructs examined here, as evidenced by the PISA (Program in International Student Assessment) international data (Marsh et al, 2006). This is consistent with the significant body of research findings that support the connection between motivational variables, learning strategies, self-regulatory behaviour, and academic achievement (Bandura, 1997; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Zimmerman, 1999, 2000a). This does not negate the importance of sociocultural factors, however. P a g e | 58 In spite of the invariance of the constructs investigated, there were some differences between the two groups examined. Latinos were somewhat lower on elaboration, academic efficacy, performance approach, and performance avoidance for example, and somewhat higher on maladaptive help-seeking, worry, and mastery goal orientation. There is no simple explanation for these differences, as the literature focusing on comparisons between the two groups in this study is too small. One factor that may be important is the select nature of the Latino students in this sample, Given that they were in a four year college, they may not be comparable to the general population of Latino students. While their prior grades and SAT scores were lower than those of White students, the fact that they were admitted to a relatively selective institution may have been a factor in higher mastery goal orientation, and lower levels of prior knowledge (high school GPA and SAT verbal and math) may have been related to lower efficacy and increased maladaptive help-seeking. The findings here were somewhat consistent with Garcia, Yu, and Coppola’s (1993) study with Latino and AfricanAmerican students which found that success was correlated with motivation and prior achievement, and not with learning strategies. Given the extensive literature that suggests the impact of learning strategies on academic outcomes (Azevedo & Cromley, 2004; Butler & Winne, 1995; Ley &Young, 200; Zimmerman, 1990; Zimmerman & Martinez-Ponz, 1990: Zimmerman & Schunk, 2001), this deserves more exploration. In addition, SES was related to OTL and prior knowledge for Latino but not for White students. This may be a function of other prior work on differences in OTL between low-SES schools and higher-SES schools. As noted earlier, differences have been found in instructional arrangements, materials, teacher experience, and teacher-student ratio (Wenglinsky, 1998). Interestingly, while OTL was not found to have significant direct effects P a g e | 59 on achievement, it was related to other motivational variables self-efficacy, performance approach, and mastery goal orientation) in the study relative to White students (academic self-efficacy only). One of the most interesting findings was the very strong effect of prior knowledge on GPA in comparison to the relatively smaller effects of background variables (and other variables as well) such as OTL and SES compared to what we expected. For example, prior knowledge was found to be strongly and positively related to achievement for both White and Latino students. This is a different pattern that what other studies have found, in particular the relatively stronger impact of self-efficacy compared to prior knowledge. Chemers, Hu, and Garcia (2001), for example, found that while high school grades were significantly related to college grades, self-efficacy at the time of college entrance was related to grades at the end of the first year of college, even when prior GPA was controlled. Other recent studies (Pietsch, Walker, & Chapman, 2003; Pintrich, 2003) as well as a recent review of self-efficacy studies (Valentine, DuBois, & Cooper, 2004) have confirmed these patterns. Because the focus of the current study was on math in particular, this may have had an impact on the present findings. An additional interesting finding was the lack of direct effects of the motivational variables on achievement. As the previous literature has suggested, arguments have been made for a multiple-goal approach in which mastery and performance approach goals play a positive role in academic performance (Pintrich, 2003). Previous studies have found, similar to the present results (Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, & Elliot, 2002: Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, Carter, & Elliot, 2000) that in studies with college students, mastery goals did not predict course grades, but rather predicted increased interest, enjoyment, and course-taking P a g e | 60 (in psychology courses). While these variables were not measured in the present study, future studies should consider adding them as important mediators. The present study was also consistent with findings in the achievement goal literature that students who support mastery goals are more likely to use deep-level strategies such as elaboration and organization than students who support performance goals (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004; Patrick, Kaplan, & Ryan, 2007; Ross, Shannon, SalisburyGlennon & Guarino, 2002; Wolters, Yu & Pintrich, 1996). A great deal of research, as point out, has documented that mastery goals are connected with students’ use of cognitive and self-regulatory strategies. Yet it is puzzling why these factors did not exhibit stronger effects on achievement for either group. A more significant and stronger (negative) relationship to achievement was found for worry, the cognitive component of stress and anxiety, consistent with the work in this area (Zeidner & Matthews, 2005). This study is one of the few studies which have investigated sociocultural, learning, and motivation related factors with college-aged Latino students. Because relatively little work has been done with ethnic differences among college-level students’ approach to learning, this study contributes to the knowledge that aims to describe the relationships among those aspects of learning students have already obtained in a given domain, those aspects that are still being developed, and those where students need further support. Rather than exclusively focusing on programs which rely on social and academic integration or financial aid, colleges should continue investigating the sociocultural, motivational, and learning aspects of academic achievement, addressing issues of students’ different approaches to learning and how these can be bolstered both prior to and after students arrive on campus. Few faculty members in college teaching positions receive educational experiences which provide the P a g e | 61 knowledge to consider these factors in their classroom instruction. This is increasingly important, however, as college campuses experience both increasing diversity as well as continued pressure for accountability and student learning outcomes. Drawing upon studies such as this, institutions of higher education should be able to establish professional development programs in order to train faculty at all levels in motivational principles and learning strategies methods which are appropriate to the needs of at-risk students, informed by possible between-group as well as individual differences. P a g e | 62 References Adelman, C. (1999). Answers in the tool box: Academic intensity, attendance patterns, and bachelor’s degree attainment. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement. Alexander, K. L., Pallas, A., & Holupka, S. (1987). Social background and academic determinants of two-year versus four-year college attendance: Evidence from two cohorts a decade apart. American Journal of Education, 96(1), 56-80. Alfaro, E. C., Umana-Taylor, A. J., & Bamaca, M. Y. (2006). The Influence of Academic Support on Latino Adolescents' Academic Motivation. Family Relations, 55(3), 279291. Anderman, E. M., Urdan, T. U., & Roeser, R. (2003, March). The patterns of adaptive learning survey: History, development, and psychometric properties. Paper prepared for the Indicators of Positive Development Conference, Washington, DC. Ames, C. (1992). Achievement goals and the classroom motivational climate. In J. Meece & D. Schunk (Eds.), Student perceptions in the classroom (pp. 327-348). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students' learning strategies and motivation processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260-267. Arnett, J.J. (2008). The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. American Psychologist, 63(7), 602-614. Azevedo, R., & Cromley, J, G. (2004). Does training on self regulated learning facilitate students’ learning with hypermedia? Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(3), 523535. P a g e | 63 Baker, E.L., Griffin, N.C. & Choi, K. (2008). The achievement gap in california: Context, status , and approaches for improvement. Davis, California: University of California, Davis School of Education Center for Applied Policy in Education. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Bandura, A. (1989). Social cognitive theory. In R. Vasts (Ed.), Annals of child development (Vol. 5, pp. 1-60). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Bandura, A., & Shunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 586-598. Bensimon, E. M. (2005), Closing the achievement gap in higher education: An organizational learning perspective. In A.Kezar (Ed.),Organizational Learning in Higher Education. New Directions for Higher Education (No. 131, 99-111). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Ben-Zeev, T., Fein, S., & Inzlicht, M. (2005). Arousal and stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 174-181. Boekaerts, M. (1997). Self-regulated learning: A new concept embraced by researchers, policy makers, educators, teachers and students. Learning and Instruction, 7(2), 161186. Boekaerts, M. (1999). Self-regulated learning: Where we are today. International Journal of Educational Research, 31, 445-475. Bollen, K., Glanville, J. A., & Stecklov, G. (2001). Socioeconomic status and class in P a g e | 64 studies of fertility and health in developing countries. Annual Review of Sociology,27, 153–185. Bornstein, M. C., & Bradley, R. H. (Eds.). (2003). Socioeconmic status, parenting, and child development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Braswell, J.S., Lutkus, A.D., Grigg, W.S., Santapau, S.L., Tay-Lim, B., and Johnson, M. (2001). The Nation’s Report Card: Mathematics 2000, NCES 2001–517. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Education. Office of Educational Research and Improvement. National Center for Education Statistics. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theory (5th ed.). New York: Wiley. Brooks-Gunn, J., & Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. The future of children, 7(2), 55–71. Bryant, F.B., & Yarnold, P.R. (2001). Principal components analysis and exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. In LG. Grimm and P.R. Tarnold (Eds.), Reading and understanding multivariate statistics. (pp. 99-136). Washington, DC: American Psychological Assn. Butler, D., & Winne, P. H. (1995). Feedback and self-regulated learning: A theoretical synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 65(3), 245-281. Byrnes, J.P. (2001). Cognitive development and learning in instructional contexts (2nd ed). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Byrnes, J.P. (2005). Gender differences in math: Cognitive processes in an expanded framework. In Gallagher, A.M. & Kaufman, J.C. (Eds.), Gender differences in P a g e | 65 mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (pp73-98). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cabrera, A. F., & La Nasa, S. M. (2000a). On the path to college: Three critical tasks facing America’s disadvantaged. University Park, PA: The Pennsylvania State University, Center for the Study of Higher Education. Cabrera, A. F., & La Nasa, S. M. (2000b). Understanding the college choice process. In A.F. Cabrera, & S. M. La Nasa (Eds.), Understanding the college choice of disadvantaged students: New directions for institutional research, No. 107 (pp. 5-22). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Caplan, P.J., & Caplan, J.B. (1999). Thinking critically about research on sex and gender (2nd ed.). New York: Addison-Wesley Longman Inc. Carey, K. (2004). A matter of degrees: Improving graduation rates in four-year colleges and universities. Washington, DC: The Education Trust. Carey, K. (2005). One step from the finish line: Higher college graduation rates are within our reach. Washington, DC: The Education Trust. Checkley, K. (2001). Algebra and activism: Removing the shackles of low expectations: A conversation with Robert P. Moses. Educational Leadership, 59, 6-11. Chemers, M.M., Hu, L., & Garcia, B.F. (2001). Academic self-efficacy and first-year college student performance and adjustment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 55-64. Chipman, S.F. (1996). Female participation in the study of mathematics: The U.S. situation. In G. Hanna (Ed.), Towards gender equity in mathematics education (pp. 285-296). Boston: Kluwer Academic. Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (rev. ed.). New P a g e | 66 York: Academic Press. Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of Sociology, 94, S95–S120. Commission on Professionals in Science and Technology, (2006). Professional women and minorities: A total human resources data compendium (16th ed.). Washington, DC. Covington, M.V. (1992). Making the grade: a self-worth perspective on motivation and school reform. New York: Cambridge University Press. Davis-Kean, P. E., Vida, M., & Eccles, J. (2001). Influences on Parental Expectations for Post-High School Transitions. Paper presented at the Society for Research on Child Development Biennial Conference. Deci, E. L. & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press. Dika, S., & Singh, K. (2002). Applications of social capital in educational literature: A critical synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 72(1) 31–60. Dweck, C.S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41, 1040-1048. Dweck, C.S., & Leggett, E.S. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273. Eccles, J. S., Lord, S., & Midgley, C. (1991). What are we doing to early adolescents? The impact of educational context on early adolescents. American Journal of Education, 99, 521–542. P a g e | 67 Eccles, J., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109-132. Eccles, J. S., Vida, M. N., & Barber, B. (2004). The Relation of Early Adolescents' College Plans and Both Academic Ability and Task-Value Beliefs to Subsequent College Enrollment. Journal of Early Adolescence, 24(1), 63-77. Elliot, A.J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, 169-190. Elliot, A.J. (2007). A conceptual history of the achievement goal construct. In A.J. Elliot & C.S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation. (pp. 52-72). NY: Guilford Press. Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (1999). Test anxiety and the hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 628-644. Elliot, A.J., McGregor, H.A., and Gable, S. (1999). Achievement goals, study strategies, and exam performance: A mediational analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 549-563. Esparza, P., & Sanchez, B. (2008). The role of attitudinal familism in academic outcomes: a study of urban, latino high school seniors. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14(3), 193-200. Favreau, O.E. (1997). Sex and gender comparisons: Does null hypothesis testing create a false dichotomy? Feminism & Psychology, 7(1), 63-81. P a g e | 68 Forgas, J. (2000). The role of affect in social cognition. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 1-28). New York: Cambridge University Press. Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74 (1), 59-109. Freeman, C.E. (2004). Trends in Educational Equity of Girls & Women: 2004 (NCES 2005– 016). U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Fry, R. (2002). Latinos in higher education: Many enroll, too few graduate. Washington, DC: The Pew Hispanic Center. Garcia, T., Yu, S. L., & Coppola, B. P. (1993). Women and Minorities in Science: Motivational and Cognitive Correlates of Achievement. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association Geary, D., Saults, S.J., Liu, F., & Hoard, M.K. (2000). Sex differences in spatial cognition, computational fluency and arithmetical reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 337-353. Goldenberg, C., Gallimore, R., Reese, L., & Garnier, H. (2001). Cause or Effect? A Longitudinal Study of Immigrant Latino Parents' Aspirations and Expectations, and Their Children's School Performance. American Educational Research Journal, 38(3), 547-582. Gottfried, A. (1985). Measures of socioeconomic status in child development research: Data and recommendations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 31(1). 85–92. P a g e | 69 Graham, S., & Golan, S. (1991). Motivational influences on cognition: Task involve ment, ego involvement, and depth of information processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 187-194. Graham, S.,, & Hudley, C. (2007). Race and ethnicity in the study of motivation and competence. In A.J. Elliot and C.S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation. (pp. 392-413). NY: Guilford Press. Gutierrez, K.D., & Rogoff, B. (2003). Cultural ways of learning: Individual traits or repertoires of practice. Educational Researcher, 32 (5), 19-25 Halpern, D.F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Halpern, D. F. & Lemay, M. L. (2000). The smarter sex: A critical review of sex differences in intelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 12, 229-246. Harackiewicz, J.M., Barron, K.E., Tauer, J.M., & Elliot, A.J.. (2002). Predicting success in college: A longitudinal study of achievement goals and ability measures as predictors of interest and performance from freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 562-575. Harackiewicz, J.M., Barron, K.E., Tauer, J.M., Carter, S.M., & Elliot, A.J. (2000). Short term and long-term consequences of achievement goals: Predicting interest and performance over time. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 316-330. Harackiewicz, J.M., Barron, K.E., Tauer, J.M., & Elliot, A.J. (2002). Predicting success in college: A longitudinal study of achievement goals and abilitymeasures as predictors of interest and performance from freshman year through graduation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 562-575. P a g e | 70 Hauser, R. M. (1994). Measuring socioeconomic status in studies of child development. Child Development, 65(6), 1541–1545. Hauser, R. M. & Huang, M. H. (1997). Verbal ability and socioeconomic success: A Trend Analysis. Social Science Research, 26, 331–376. Hearn, J.C.,& Holdsworth, J.M. (2002). Influences of state-level policies and practices on college students’ learning. Peabody Journal of Education, 77, 6-39. Hembree, R. (1988). Correlates, causes, effects, and treatment of test anxiety. Review of Educational Research, 58, 47-77. Hill, K.T., & Wigfield, A. (1984). Test anxiety: A major educational problem and what can be done about it. Elementary School Journal, 85, 105-126. Hong, E., O’Neil, H. F., & Feldon, D. (2005).Gender effects on mathematics achievement. In Gallagher, A.M. & Kaufman, J.C. (Eds.), Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (pp. 264-293). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Horn, L., & Kojaku, L. K. (2001). High school academic curriculum and the persistence path through college: Persistence and transfer behavior of undergraduates 3 years after entering 4-year institutions. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55. Huguet, P. & Regner, I (2007). Stereotype threat among school girls in quasi-ordinary P a g e | 71 classroom circumstances. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99 (3), 545-560.Jöreskog, K.G., & Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8: Structual equating modeling with the SIMPLIS command language. Chicago IL: Scientific Software International. Kane, J., & Spizman, L. M. (1994). Race, financial aid and college attendance: Parents and geography matter. American Journal of Economics and Sociology, 53, 85-97. Kaplan, D. (2000). Structural Equation Modeling: Foundations and extentions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Karabenick, S.A. (2003). Seeking help in large college classes: A person-centered approach. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 37-58. Karabenick, S. A. (2004). Perceived achievement goal structure and college student help seeking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(3), 569-581. Knapp, L.G., Kelly-Reid, J.E., and Ginder, S.A. (2008). Postsecondary Institutions in the United States: Fall 2007, Degrees and Other Awards Conferred: 2006-07, and 12Month Enrollment: 2006-07 (NCES 2008-159). National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Washington, DC. Retrieved October 1, 2008 from http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch. Lamdin, D. J. (1996). Evidence of student attendance as an independent variable in education production functions. Journal of Educational Research, 89(3), 155–162. Lerner, R. (1991). Changing organism-context relations as the basic process of development: A developmental contextual perspective. Developmental Psychology, 27, 27–32. Ley, K. & Young, D. B. (2001). Instructional principles for self regulation. Educational Technology, Research and Development, 49(2), 93-103. P a g e | 72 Linnenbrink, E.A., & Pintrich, P.R. (2004). Role of affect in cognitive processing in academic contexts. In D.Y. Dai & R.J. Sternberg (Eds.), Motivation, emotion, and cognition: Integrative perpectives on intellectual functioning and development (pp. 5787). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Marsh, H. W., Hau, K., Artelt, C., Baumert, J., & Peschar, J. L. (2006). OECD’s brief selfreport measure of educational psychology’s most useful affective constructs: Crosscultural, psychometric comparisons across 25 countries. International Journal of Testing, 6(4), 311-360. McDonough, P.M. (1997). Choosing colleges: How social class and schools structure opportunity. Albany, NY: State University of New York (SUNY) Press. McInerney, D., & Van Etten, S. (2001). Research on sociocultural influences on motivation and learning. Greenwich, Conn: Information Age Publishers. Meece, J. L., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Hoyle, R. H. (1988). Students' goal orientations and cognitive engagement in classroom activities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 514-523. Meuschke, D.M., Dembo, M.H., & Grobbons, B. (2006). Relationship between goal orientation, help-seeking, math self-efficacy, and mathematics achievement in a community college. San Francisco, CA: Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association. Middleton, M.J. and Midgley, C. (1997). Avoiding the demonstration of lack of ability: An underexplored aspect of goal theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(4), 710718. P a g e | 73 Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. (2001). Performance-approach goals: Good for what, for whom, and under what conditions, and at what cost? Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 77-86. National Center for Education Statistics. (2005). First-generation students in postsecondary education: A look at their college transcripts. NCES 2005-171, by X. Chen. Project Officer. C. Dennis Carroll. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. National Commission on Children (U. S.) (1991). Beyond rhetoric: A new American agenda for children and families. Washington, D.C.: National Commission on Children. Nettles, M.T., Thoeny, A.R., & Gosman, E.J. (1986). Comparative and predictive analyses of black and white students’ college achievement and experiences. Journal of Higher Education, 37, 289-318. Newman, R.S. (2002). How self-regulated learners cope with academic difficulty: The role of adaptive help seeking. Theory into Practice, 41(2), 132-138. Nguyen, N.T., Allen, L.C., & Fraccastoro, K. (2005). Personality predicts academic performance: Exploring the moderating role of gender. Journal of Higher Education Policy and Management, 27 105-116. Nolen, S. B. (1988). Reasons for studying: Motivational orientations and study strategies. Cognition and Instruction, 5, 269-287. Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD). (2000). Knowledge and skills for life: First results from PISA 2000. Paris: Author. Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in achievement settings. Review of Educational Research, 66, 543-578. P a g e | 74 Patrick, H., Kaplan, A., Ryan, A. (2007). Early Adolescents’ Perceptions of the Classroom Social Environment, Motivational Beliefs, and Engagement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99 (1), 83-98. Pekrun, R., (1992). The impact of emotions on learning and achievement: Towards a theory of cognitive/motivational indicators. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 41 359-376. Pekrun, R., & Frese, M. (1992). Emotions in work and achievement. In C. Cooper & I. Robertson (Eds.), International review of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 153-200). Cichester, UK: Wiley. Pintrich, P.R. (2003). A motivational science perspective on the role of student motivation in learning and teaching contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4), 667–686. Reynolds, A. H. & Walberg, H. J. (1992b). A structural model of science achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(1), 97–107. Ripple, C. H., & Luthar, S. S. (2000). Academic risk among inner-city adolescents: The role of personal attributes. Journal of School Psychology, 38(3), 277–298. Ross, M. E., Shannon, D. M., Salisbury-Glennon, J. D., Guarino, A. (2002). The patterns of adaptive learning survey: A comparison across grade levels. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62 (3), 483-497. Ryan, A. M., and Pintrich, P. R. (1997). Should I ask for help?: The role of motivation and attitudes in adolescents’ help seeking in math class. Journal of Educational Psychoogy. 89: 329–341. P a g e | 75 Ryan, A. M., & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). "Should I ask for help?": The role of motivation and attitudes in adolescents' help-seeking in math class. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 329-341. Ryan, A.M., Pintrich, P.R.,& Midgley, C. (2001). Avoiding seeking help in the classroom: Who and why? Educational Psychology Review, 13(2), 93-114. Ryan, A. M., & Shim, S. O. (2005). Social achievement goals: The nature and consequences of different orientations toward social competence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1246-1263. Schunk, D.H., Pintrich, P.R., & Meece, J.L. (2008). Motivation in education: Theory, research, and applications. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall. Schutz, P., & Davis, H. (2000). Emotions and self-regulation during test-taking. Educational Psychologist, 35, 243-256. Snyder, T., Dillow, S. A., & Hoffman, C. M. (2007). Digest of Educational Statistics 2007. US Department of Education, Report # NCES 2008-022. Spielberger, C.D. (1972). Anxiety as an emotional state. In C.D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety: Current trends in theory and research (pp. 23-49). New York: Academic Press. Sutton, A., & Soderstrom, I. (1999). Predicting elementary and secondary school achievement with school-related and demographic factors. The Journal of Educational Research, (6), 330–338. Pietsch, J., Walker, R., & Chapman, E. (2003). The relationship among self-concept, selfefficacy, and performance in mathematics during secondary school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 589-603. P a g e | 76 Pintrich, P.R. (2004). Motivation and classroom learning. In W.M. Reynolds & G.E. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Vol. 7. Educational psychology (pp. 103-122). New York: Wiley. Pintrich, P.R & De Groot, R.V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33-40. Plunkett, S. W., & Bamaca-Gomez, M. Y. (2003). The Relationship between Parenting, Acculturation, and Adolescent Academics in Mexican-Origin Immigrant Families in Los Angeles. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 25(2), 222-239. Plunkett, S. W., Henry, C. S., Houltberg, B. J., Sands, T., & Abarca-Mortensen, S. (2008). Academic Support by significant others and educational resilience in Mexican-origin ninth grade students from intact families. Journal of Early Adolescence, 28(3), 333355. Purdie, N., Hattie, J., & Douglas, G. (1996). Student Conceptions of Learning and Their Use of Self-Regulated Learning Strategies: A Cross-Cultural Comparison. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(1), 87-100. Robbins, S.B., Lauver, K., Le, H., Davis, D., Langley, R., & Carlstrom, A. (2004). Do psychosocial and study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis. Psychologtcal Bulletin, 130, 261-288. Robbins, S.B., Allen, J., Casillas, A., Peterson, C., & Le, H. (2006). Unraveling the differential effects of motivational and skills, social, and self-management measures from traditional predictors of college outcomes. Journal of Educational Pschology, 98(3), 598-616. P a g e | 77 Ross, M. E., Shannon, D. M., Salisbury-Glennon, J. D., Guarino, A. (2002). The patterns of adaptive learning survey: A comparison across grade levels. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62 (3), 483-497. Ryan, A. M., Gheen, M. H., & Midgley, C. (1988). Why do some students avoid seeking help? An examination of the interplay among students’ academic efficacy, teachers’ social-emotional role, and the classroom goal structure. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(3), 528-535. Rueda, R., & Chen, C. (2005). Assessing motivational factors in foreign language learning: Cultural variation in key constructs. Educational Assessment, 10(3), 209-229. Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (1994). Self- regulation of learning and performance: Issues and educational applications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl baum. Sirin, S.R. (2005). Socioeconomic Status and Academic Achievement: A Meta-Analytic Review of Research. Review of Educational Research, 75(3), 417–453 Stage, F. K., & Rushin, P. W. (1993). A combined model of student predisposition to college and persistence in college. Journal of College Student Development, 34, 276-82. Seyfried, S. F. (1998). Academic achievement of African American preadolescents: The influence of teacher perceptions. American Journal of Community Psychology, 26(3), 381–402. Stough, L. M., & Songeroth, M. S. (1994). Minority Students Who Persist: A Three-Year Study of Undergraduate Engineering Majors. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Southwest Educational Research Association. P a g e | 78 Tobias, S. (2006). The importance of motivation, metacognition, and help seeking in webbased learning. In H. F. O’Neil & R. S Perez (Eds.). Web-based learning: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 203-220). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Urdan, T. (2004). Predictors of academic self-handicapping and achievement: Examining achievement goals, classroom goal structures, and culture. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(2), 251-264. Urdan, T., & Giancarlo, C. (2001). A comparison of motivational and critical thinking orientations across ethnic groups. In D. M. McInerney and S. V. Etten (Eds.), Research on sociocultural influences on motivation and learning, Volume 1 (pp. 37-60). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing. U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2000). The Condition of Education 2000, NCES 2000-602. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Valencia, R.R. (1997). The evolution of deficit thinking: Educational thought and practice. Bristol, PA: Falmer Press, Taylor & Francis Inc. Valentine, J.C., DuBois, D.L., & Cooper, H. (2004). The relation between self-beliefs and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychologist, 39, 111133. Vansteenviste, M., Zhou, M., Lens, W., & Soenens, B. (2005). Experiences of autonomy and control among Chinese learners: Vitalizing or immobilizing? Journal of Educational Psychology. 97(3), 468-483. Watkins, T. J. (1997). Teacher communications, child achievement, and parent traits in parent involvement models. Journal of Educational Research, 91(1), 3–14. P a g e | 79 Watkins, D., McInerney, D., Akande, A., Lee, C. (2003). An investigation of ethnic differences in the motivation and strategies for learning of students in desegregated South African schools Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Vol. 34( 2), 189-194. Wenglinsky, H. (1998). Finance equalization and within-school equity: The relationship between education spending and the social distribution of achievement. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 20(4), 269–283. White, K. (1982). The relation between socioeconomic status and academic achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 461–481. Wigfield, A. and Eccles, J.S. (Eds.) (2002). Development of achievement motivation. San Diego: Academic Press. Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 68-81. Wolters, C., Yu, S., Pintrich, P. R. (1996). The relation between goal orientation and students’ motivational beliefs and self-regulated learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 211-238. Zeidner, M. (1998). Test anxiety: The state of the art. New York: Plenum. Zeidner, M., & Matthews, G. (2005). Evaluation anxiety: Current theory and research. In A.J. Elliot & C.S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 141-163). New York: Guilford Press. Zimmerman, B.J. (2000a). Attaining self-regulation: A social-cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P.R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 13-39). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. P a g e | 80 Zimmerman, B.J. (2000b). Self-efficacy: An essential motive to learn. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 82-91. Zimmerman, B. J. (1990). Student differences in self-regulated learning: Relating grade, sex, and giftedness to self-efficacy and strategy use. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 284-290. Zimmerman, B. J., & Schunk, D. H. (Eds.). (2001). Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theory, research and practice. New York: Springer-Verlag. P a g e | 81 APPENDIX A Phone Survey Questionnaire PHONE SURVEY QUESTIONNAIRE HELLO Hello, I am calling from XXXX. HEA Have I reached [INSERT STUDENT’S NAME]? 1. Yes [SKIP TO INTRO] 2. No [CONTINUE] 9. REFUSED [TERMINATE SURVEY] HEAD HEAD Is [INSERT STUDENT’S NAME] at home? 1. Yes [SKIP TO INTRO] 2. 2. Not here right now [SKIP TO APPT] 3. 3. No longer live here…new phone # (record on next screen) 4. 4. No one here by that name [TERMINATE] 5. 5. No longer lives here…don’t have a new phone # [TERMINATE] 6. REFUSED [TERMINATE – CALL BACK LATER] What time can we call back to talk with [INSERT STUDENT’S NAME]? __________________ TERMINATE AFTER GETTING TIME INTRO We’re conducting a study of XXX students regarding their learning styles. I have just a few questions to ask you right now, and then the main part of the study is a 10 minute on-line survey. If you agree to participate in both parts you will be entered into a drawing for a chance to win up to $100 in Coyote bookstore gift certificates. Is it alright to ask you these questions now? YES [SKIP TO BEGIN] NO APPT Is it possible to make an appointment to ask you the survey questions at a more convenient time? 7. Yes (SPECIFY)________________ 8. No BEGIN Before we start, let me assure you that your identity and your responses will remain completely confidential, and of course, you are free to decline to answer any particular survey question. I should also mention that this call may be monitored by my supervisor for quality control purposes only. First, since part of the survey is on-line, I need to verify your e-mail address. According to our database, your e-mail address is [INSERT E-MAIL FROM DATABASE]. Is that correct? Yes [SKIP TO QUESTION4] No [SKIP TO 2C] 8. DON’T KNOW [SKIP TO TRANSJB] 9. REFUSED [SKIP TO TRANSJB] 2C. Do you have an e-mail address? 1. Yes 2. No [SKIP TO TRANSJB] 8. DON’T KNOW [SKIP TO TRANSJB] 9. REFUSED [SKIP TO TRANSJB] 3. What is your e-mail address? [INTERVIEWER: TYPE CAREFULLY AND READ BACK TO RESPONDENT] TRANSJB Students who do not have an e-mail address can come to JB 277 to complete the second part of the survey. Please come to JB 277 after next Wednesday, November 8, to complete the on-line portion. Your name will be entered into the bookstore drawing after you complete both the phone and on-line survey. 4. What was the highest grade of school your father completed? Didn’t finish high school Graduated from high school Had some education from college Graduated from college DON’T KNOW REFUSED 5. How about your mother…how far did she go in school? Didn’t finish high school Graduated from high school Had some education from college Graduated from college DON’T KNOW REFUSED 6. What does your father do for a living?______________________ IF RETIRED OR DECEASED, ASK WHAT HE USED TO DO 7. What does your mother do for a living? ______________________ IF RETIRED OR DECEASED, ASK WHAT SHE USED TO DO 8. Yes No Are you receiving financial aid to help pay for school? 9. 9. REFUSED Please tell me whether you Strongly Agree, Agree, Disagree, or Strongly Disagree with the following statements: My family emphasized the importance of reading when I was growing up 1. STRONGLY AGREE 2. AGREE 3. NEUTRAL / NO OPINION 4. DISAGREE 5. STRONGLY DISAGREE 8. DON'T KNOW 9. REFUSED 9a. My family emphasizes the need for a college education. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 8. 9. STRONGLY AGREE AGREE NEUTRAL / NO OPINION DISAGREE STRONGLY DISAGREE DON'T KNOW REFUSED 10. When you were growing up, how often did people in your home talk to each other in a language other than English? Would you say it was…. 1. Never, 2. Once in a while, 3. About half of the time, or 4. All or most of the time 8. DON’T KNOW 9. REFUSED 11. Is English your first language? 1. Yes 2. No 9. REFUSED 12. Were you born in the United States? 1. Yes [SKIP TO TRANS] 2. No 9. REFUSED [SKIP TO TRANS] 13. How long have you been living in the United States? ____ years TRANS Now I’d like to ask you about the math and science courses you took in high school. Specifically, 14. How many semesters of AP math did you take in high school?______ [ENTER 98 FOR DON’T KNOW/FORGOT, 99 FOR REFUSED] 15. How about AP science?_______ [ENTER 98 FOR DON’T KNOW/FORGOT, 99 FOR REFUSED] 16. And now, focusing on your courses at CSUSB, about how many pages of reading do you do for your courses each day? 1. 5 or fewer, 2. 6-10, 3. 11-15, 4. 16-20, 5. 21-40, 6. More than 40 8. DON’T KNOW 9. REFUSED TRANS Now I have just a few questions about your preferred learning style. Please rate each of the following statements on a scale from 1 to 7, with 1 meaning strongly agree and 7 meaning strongly disagree. First statement: 17. I prefer to learn visually…how strongly do you agree on the 1 to 7 scale? 18. I am good at learning from labeled pictures, illustrations, graphs, maps, and animations 19. I prefer to learn verbally 20. I am good at learning from printed text 21. Yes No 8. 9. Finally, a question about your background. Are you of Hispanic or Latino origin? DON’T KNOW REFUSED 22. How would you describe your race or ethnicity? [MULTIPLE RESPONSE ALLOWED] White Black or African American Asian American Indian or Alaska Native Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander Hispanic [SAY IF THEY HAVE E-MAIL] Thanks, that’s it. In a day or so you will find in your email account our on-line questionnaire. We’ll need you to return it as soon as possible, and then you will be entered into the bookstore drawing. Have a great day. OR [SAY IF THEY DON’T HAVE E-MAIL] Thanks, that’s it. Please come to JB-277 in a few days to do the brief on-line questionnaire. Once you do that, you will be entered into the bookstore drawing. INTERVIEWER QUESTIONS IQ1. How cooperative was the respondent? 1. Cooperative 2. Uncooperative 3. Very Uncooperative IQ2. How well did the respondent understand the questions? 1. Very easily 2. Easily 3. Some difficulty 4. Great deal of difficulty APPENDIX B Written Survey Questionnaire WRITTEN SURVEY Hello: Thank you for agreeing to participate in our survey about XXX students' learning styles and strategies. It will take approximately 10 - 15 minutes to complete the questionnaire. Your participation in this study is completely voluntary. If you feel uncomfortable answering a particular question, feel free to continue to the next question without answering. Answer as much as you can. It is very important for us to learn your opinions. Your survey responses will be strictly confidential and data from this research will be reported only in the aggregate. If you have questions at any time about the survey or the procedures, you may contact XXX or by email at the email address specified below. Thank you very much for your time and support. Please start with the survey now by clicking on the Continue button below. When I study, I try to relate new material to things I have learned in other subjects. When I study, I figure out how the information might be useful in the real world. When I study, I try to understand the material better by relating it to things I already know. When I study, I figure out how the material fits in with what I have already learned. When I study, I start by figuring out exactly what I need to learn. When I study, I force myself to check to see if I remember what I have learned. When I study, I try to figure out which concepts I still haven’t really understood. When I study, I make sure that I remember the most important things. When I study, and I don’t understand something I look for additional information to clarify this. 1. Almost never 2. Sometimes 3. Often 4. Almost always 10) help. 11) 12) 13) 14) 15) When I don't understand my work, I often guess instead of asking someone for I don't ask questions in my classes, even when I don't understand the work. If my course work is too hard for me, I just don't do it rather than ask for help. I usually don't ask for help with my work, even if the work is too hard to do on my own. When I don't understand my work, I often put down any answer rather than ask for help. If I need help to do part of my coursework, I skip it. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Not at all true Somewhat true Very true 16) I’m certain I can understand the most difficult material presented in texts. 17) I’m confident I can understand the most complex material presented by the teacher. 18) I’m confident I can do an excellent job on assignments and tests. 19) I’m certain I can master the skills being taught. 1. Almost never 2. Sometimes 3. Often 4. Almost always 20) 21) 22) 23) 1. 2. 3. 4. When studying, I work as hard as possible. When studying, I keep working even if the material is difficult. When studying, I try to do my best to acquire the knowledge and skills taught. When studying, I put forth my best effort. Almost never Sometimes Often Almost always 24) 25) 26) 27) 28) 29) 30) 31) Thinking about my grade in a course interferes with my work on tests I freeze up on important exams During exams I find myself thinking about whether I'll ever get through school The harder I work at taking a test, the more confused I get Thoughts of doing poorly interfere with my concentration on tests I seem to defeat myself while working on important tests During tests I find myself thinking about the consequences of failing During examinations I get so nervous that I forget facts I really know 1. Almost never 2. Sometimes 3. Often 4. Almost always 32) 33) 34) 35) My work in math was frequently encouraged by my instructors College and high school math instructors often praised me When I did well in math, my instructors confirmed that I feel useless in math classes 36) 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Math makes me feel bad about myself Strongly agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly disagree 37) 38) 39) 40) 41) It is important to me that I learn a lot of new concepts this year One of my goals in class is to learn as much as I can One of my goals is to master a lot of new skills this year It’s important to me that I thoroughly understand my class work It’s important to me that I improve my skills this year 1. Not at all true 2. 3. Somewhat true 4. 5. Very true 42) work 43) 44) 45) 46) It’s important to me that other students in my class think I am good at my class 47) 48) 48) class 50) It is important to me that I don’t look stupid in class One of my goals is to keep others from thinking that I’m not smart in class It is important to me that my teacher doesn’t think that I know less than others in One of my goals is to show others that I’m good at my class work One of my goals is to show others that class work is easy for me One of my goals is to look smart in comparison to the other students in my class It is important to me that I look smart in comparison to others in my class 1. Not at all true 2. 3. Somewhat true 4. 5. Very true One of my goals in class is to avoid looking like I have trouble doing the work 1. Not at all true 2. 3. Somewhat true 4. 5. Very true 51) I am more comfortable working in a group than working alone 52) I feel that high quality learning can take place without having face-to-face interaction Strongly agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly disagree 53) 54) 55) 56) 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. I am comfortable using computers If an assignment requires a computer, I will need help getting started I am among the first of my peers to play with new computer technologies I like experimenting with computers Strongly agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly disagree 57) Are you Hispanic or Latino? Select one or more No, I am not Hispanic or Latino Yes, I am Mexican, Mexican American, or Chicano Yes, I am Puerto Rican or Puerto Rican American Yes, I am Cuban or Cuban American Yes, I am from some other Hispanic or Latino background 58) Which of the following best describes you? Select one or more: White Black or African American Asian American Indian or Alaska Native Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander Hispanic Other 59) My career plans after graduation are the following: Select one or more Enter the world of work Go to graduate school Enter the U.S. Military Take some time off Give back to the community through volunteer work Other I don’t know 60) Which of the following categories best describes your parents’ total household or family income before taxes, from all sources? If you are self-supporting, list your total household income. 3. 4. 8. 9. 61) take 1. Less than $25,000 2. $25,000 to $49,999 $50,000 to $74,999 $75,000 or more DON'T KNOW REFUSED True or false: My ELM score determines how many college math classes I must 1. 2. 8. 62) True False DON’T KNOW True or false: Remedial courses don’t count toward graduation 1. True 2. False 8. DON’T KNOW Thank you! Your response has been saved and recorded with ID# ________. You will be notified within a month if you will be receiving one of the bookstore coupons. Thank you for your help!