More on Reconstructive memory Bartlett's classic studies offered good support for his theory of reconstructive memory. Many subsequent studies have further supported the theory and helped clarify some of the details. We will look at these other studies now, as well as at the factors that influence reconstructive memory. Other research related to reconstructive memory; The concept of schema: Definition: Schemas are cognitive frameworks that guide memory, aide in the interpretation of events, and influence how we retrieve stored memories. Schemas therefore lie at the core of Bartlett's theory because this theory states that our beliefs generate expectations and these expectations reconstruct memory. The reconstructive nature of memory guided by our use of schemas can be seen in a number of studies. For example: In a classic study by Carmichael et al. (1932) two different groups of participants were shown the same set of drawings which are shown in the table below. However while being shown the above drawings each group was given a different set of descriptions at the side of each drawing. That is one group was given the descriptions in condition 1 (see below) but the other group was given the descriptions in condition 2 (see below). Cond. 1 Curtains in a window Bottle Eye glasses Kidney bean Crescent moon Two Ship’s wheel Hour glass Beehive Pine tree Gun Seven Cond. 2 Diamond in a rectangle Stirrup Dumb bell Canoe Letter C Eight Sun Table Hat Trowel Broom Four When the participants were asked to recall the drawings at a later time findings showed that the descriptions they had been given affected the drawing they subsequently produced – see examples of these below Cond. 1 Cond. 2 This shows that the language used affected their memory. In essence, language conjures up a set of beliefs and expectations about an object, and this expectation affects the memory. So, in Carmichael's study, we might say that memory was affected by the schema provided. Other studies of the effects of schema Brewer and Treyens (1981): In a study by Brewer and Treyens (1981), individual participants were asked to wait in an office. The experimenter said that this was his office and that they should wait there whilst he checked the laboratory to see if the previous participant had finished. After 35 seconds, he returned and took the participant to another room where they were asked to recall everything in the room in which they had been waiting. Findings showed that people showed a strong tendency to recall objects consistent with the typical ‘office schema’. For example: Nearly everyone remembered the desk and the chair next to it. Only eight out of the 30 recalled the skull, few recalled the wine bottle or the coffee pot, and only one recalled the picnic basket. Some recalled items that had not been there at all: 9 remembered books. This shows how people may introduce new items consistent with the schema. Sulin and Dooling (1974): Sulin and Dooling gave one group of participants a story about a man called Gerald Martin which, amongst other information, stated that 'he strove to undermine the existing government to satisfy his political ambitions. He became a ruthless, uncontrollable dictator. The ultimate effect of his rule was the downfall of his country'. They gave another group of participants exactly the same story apart from in this version of the story the name of the man was changed from Gerald Martin to Adolph Hitler. Findings showed that the participants who were given the story with the name Adolph Hitler were significantly more likely to indicate that they had read 'He hated the Jews particularly' in the story than those whose story hade been about Gerald Martin. We can therefore assume that it was probably their schema about Adolph Hitler that distorted their memories of the content of the story. The role of Stereotypes in reconstructive memory: Definition: Stereotypes are a type of schema that we have about a particular class or group of people. However unlike other schema stereotypes are (usually) a much more simplistic type of schema. Stereotypes are very useful as they help us to maintain relative simplicity in our very complex social world in which we are constantly being bombarded with new information that we must try to remember. They also help us to predict the behaviour of those around us because stereotypes enable us to resort to commonly held beliefs about certain people so reducing the amount of information that we need to process to be ready to think about and/or interact with people. However research has shown that while stereotypes are a useful and necessary ’rules of thumb’ that make us more effective in our social world it has also shown that stereotypes may also lead to a reconstruction of memory. The reconstructive effects of stereotypes on memory can be seen in a classic study by Allport and Postman (1947): Allport and Postman (1947) had his participants take a brief look at a drawing of several people on a subway train, including a seated black man and a white man standing with a razor in his hand (see picture below). Findings: After a brief look at a drawing such as this one, half of the participants reported having seen the razor in the black man's hand and that the black man had been threatening the white man. Conclusion: It would appear that those participants who reported having seen the razor in the black man's hand and that the black man had been threatening the white man had distorted the information in keeping with stereotypical beliefs about black and white men, thus supporting claims about the reconstructive nature of memory. Interviewing techniques and reconstructive memory: The way in which a witness is interviewed has been shown to influence what they recall - the more leading or suggestive the questions, the lower the accuracy. This can be clearly seen in the two studies by Elizabeth Loftus and John Palmer . Interviewers may unintentionally communicate their expectations in various ways (facial expressions, tone of voice). Such expectations are found to affect the reconstruction of a memory and may distort a witness's version of an event. NB as this research relates to the effects of leading questions of memory it can also be used to support Loftus and Palmer’s findings and conclusion.