This thesis is submitted as partial fulfillment for the award of Bachelor of Arts in International Business Management Behavioral Aspects in Accounting and their Impact on the Auditing Process Katrin Wittke Student No. 271363 First Supervisor: Prof. Dr. Nadja Jehle Second Supervisor: Prof. Dr. Madeleine Janke 19/08/2013 15.780 words Katrin Wittke International Business Management Table of Contents List of Tables........................................................................................................................ iv List of Figures ........................................................................................................................v List of Abbreviations............................................................................................................ vi Affidavit .............................................................................................................................. vii Introduction ............................................................................................................................8 1. Literature Review .........................................................................................................10 1.1 Behavioral Accounting ...............................................................................................10 1.2 Behavioral Aspects in Auditing .................................................................................11 2. Developments in Rational Thinking ............................................................................13 2.1 Homo economicus - Origins, Adaptations, Criticism ................................................13 2.2 Innovative Concepts of Human Behavior ..................................................................16 2.3 Developments in Organizational Theory ...................................................................19 3. Selected Aspects of Organizational Behavior ..............................................................23 3.1 Judgment and Decision-Making .................................................................................23 3.2 Cognitive Fallibilities in Accounting and Auditing ...................................................24 3.2.1 Availability..........................................................................................................25 3.2.2 Adjustment and Anchoring .................................................................................27 3.2.3 Confirmation bias ................................................................................................29 3.3 Control, Incentives and Their Effects on Behavior ....................................................32 4. From The Auditor’s Perspective ..................................................................................35 4.1 Auditor Independence – Where Do Loyalties Lie? ....................................................35 4.2 Measures to Ensure Audit Quality .............................................................................37 5. Survey – Behavioral and Environmental Influences on Auditing Processes ...................39 5.1 Research Objectives ...................................................................................................39 5.2 Research Design and Method .....................................................................................39 5.3 Findings & Analysis ...................................................................................................42 Conclusion ...........................................................................................................................53 ii Katrin Wittke International Business Management List of References ................................................................................................................54 Appendix ..............................................................................................................................59 A. Prospect Theory Formula and EUT Formula ........................................................59 B. Survey Outline .......................................................................................................59 C. Distribution of respondents by age ........................................................................64 D. Positions held by respondents within the firm ......................................................64 E. Distribution of size of audited firms .....................................................................65 F. Cross tabulation: Team size and audit firm size ...................................................65 G. Frequency of answers on atmosphere ...................................................................66 H. Preciseness and Atmosphere .................................................................................67 iii Katrin Wittke International Business Management List of Tables Table 1: Active employment in the industry ......................................................Page 41 Table 2: Distribution of team size ......................................................................Page 43 Table 3: Cross tabulation between firm size and perceived degree of involvement in planning process ..................................................................................Page 46 Table 4: Cross tabulation between firm size and perceived degree of involvement in planning process ..................................................................................Page 47 Table 5: Frequencies for question 11: preciseness of audit tasks ......................Page 48 Table 6: Cross tabulation deadline quality and negative atmosphere ................Page 49 Table 7: Cross tabulation preciseness and quality .............................................Page 50 Table 8: Cross tabulation: Frequency of sample adaptations and audit quality .Page 51 iv Katrin Wittke International Business Management List of Figures Figure 1: Work Experience by Firm Size ..........................................................Page 42 Figure 2: Firm size and positive characteristics of atmosphere .........................Page 44 Figure 3: Firm size and negative characteristics of atmosphere ........................Page 45 Figure 4: Frequency of sample size adaptation in large and small audit firms ..Page 50 v Katrin Wittke International Business Management List of Abbreviations BAR Behavioral Accounting Research e.g. Latin: “exempli gratia” (English signification: “for example”, “such as”) EUT Expected Utility Theory i.e. Latin: “ita est” (English signification: “this means” JDM Judgment and Decision-Making PCAOB Public Company Accounting Oversight Board (PCAOB) SEM Standard Economic Model SOX Sarbanes-Oxley Act of 2002 vi Katrin Wittke International Business Management Affidavit I declare that I wrote this thesis independently and on my own. I clearly marked any language or ideas borrowed from other sources as not my own and documented their sources. The thesis does not contain any work that I have handed in or have had graded as a Prüfungsleistung earlier on. I am aware that any failure to do so constitutes plagiarism. Plagiarism is the presentation of another person's thoughts or words as if they were my own—even if I summarize, paraphrase, condense, cut, rearrange, or otherwise alter them. I am aware of the consequences and sanctions plagiarism entails. Among others, consequences may include nullification of the thesis, exclusion from the BA program without a degree, and legal consequences for lying under oath. These consequences also apply retrospectively, i.e. if plagiarism is discovered after the thesis has been accepted and graded. My name: Katrin Wittke Title of my thesis: Behavioral Aspects in Accounting and their Impact on the Auditing Process Date: 19/08/2013 Signature: _________________________ vii Katrin Wittke International Business Management Introduction The worlds of accounting and auditing are ruled by hard facts and numbers. How does the idea of motivation and irrational behavior fit into such a world? If the models that are in place today are built on the idea of homo economicus, where does an actual human being fit in? In a world controlled by numbers, cost- and benefit analyses and ruled by the cold-hearted goal of profit maximization, the behavioral movement has taken it upon itself to create a space for homo sapiens. The idea of economic man as a rational human being has been challenged with such force that it has spawned new organizational theories, novel definitions of rationality and an overall new look on nearly all aspects of behavior. Internal financial reporting represents a necessary and significant process in and of itself, but, especially for large firms that are listed on stock exchanges and small firms that voluntarily present their financial information to financial investors, it has become an integral part of everyday business life. The function of the auditor and its role in the process of a financial (external) audit become inextricably linked to the reporting system of the organization that is being audited, at least for the duration of the audit. Therefore, when examining human behavior, it is indispensable to regard both sides of the coin – the preparer of financial information and the auditor. This makes the combination of accounting and auditing a unique field of study with myriad possibilities for conflict and unconscious cognitive processes which influence everyday life. The aim of this thesis is to examine the way that the view of human behavior in accounting has developed over time and to analyze in what way these findings apply to and influence the auditing process. First, the evolution of the view of rationality and behavior in economics and accounting will be retraced in order to understand the basis of classical theory and the developments of the behavioral view. The influence of behavioralist perspective on aspects of decision making, organizational theory and sociological aspects will be presented and the consequences on management, accounting and auditing will be discussed. Starting always from the accounting side, the impact of behavioralist approaches on financial systems in organizations demand a new look. Finally, after examining selected aspects of auditor 8 Katrin Wittke International Business Management behavior, performance and audit quality, the study which was conducted to to examine whether the changes on auditing that were discussed find their way into actual auditing processes will be presented and its results analyzed. The thesis concludes with the discussion of the theoretical and practical implications of the results. 9 Katrin Wittke International Business Management 1. Literature Review The following literature review will give an overview of the developments in Behavioral Accounting Research (BAR) over the last sixty years as well as of the more specialized research area of behavioral aspects of auditing. 1.1 Behavioral Accounting Ever since the inception of economics, man has been viewed as a rational human being. This view has its origin in the works of John Stuart Mill and followed the general characteristics which Adam Smith already outlined in The Wealth of Nations (1776, reprinted in 1991).After its inception the concept has been embraced by economists and was developed further over the following decades. Its evolution will be outlined in section 2.1 Homo economicus. Criticisms of the theory of homo economicus, as the rational man has been called shortly after his inception, have started to arise towards the end of World War II. Starting with criticism of the idea of unlimited cognitive abilities by Herbert A. Simon, over the years experiments brought to light a multitude of characteristics of homo economicus which were not accounted for by the classical model. It became clear that this model was not able to accurately represent reality. Gradually, through the development of the principle of bounded rationality by Simon (1964), neoclassical economic theory was extended by the inclusion of elements of psychology, marking the birth of behavioral economics. More dramatic changes were made when the behavioralism movement gained momentum in the 1960s and almost immediately swept over to include accounting, auditing, finance and finally, investment. In the 1970s, researchers like Hofstedt (1976) and Birnberg and Nath (1967) have attempted to draw a preliminary result from the explosive growth in BAR, with the latter authors having correctly predicted a profound increase in interest in behavioral aspects of budgeting processes and control systems. Up to the point in time when Hofstedt (1976) attempts an illustration of the state of BAR, the focus has mostly lied upon such topics as controlling and judgment and decisionmaking (JDM), but the theories and paradigms have not been formulated to a degree where they could be molded into all-encompassing theories. While internal accounting and controlling already had enjoyed a high degree of attention by scholars which resulted in a large body of research, external financial reporting and auditing had not been put under the 10 Katrin Wittke International Business Management behavioral microscope with as much scrutiny. A major concern of Hofstedt’s was that research mainly occurred on the academic side and lacked input from the practical side, indicating a possible absence of interest of practitioners. He feared that the reconciliation between academics and practicing accountants, specifically the practical adaption of theoretical results, would be hindered if the transition from theoretical research into practice would not improve (Hofstedt 1970, p. 41 f) Colville (1981) has offered a detailed look at the connection between accounting and organizations, and attempted to predict what the implications of a behavioral view of accounting would be for the organizational aspects of society. He suggests a reciprocal exchange between organizational theorists, behavioral researchers and sociologists in order to ensure that the organization is not examined without giving the individual his due place in it. A similar link between accounting and organizational theory was also drawn by Covaleski and Aiken (1968), which has been deemed “an excellent synthesis of […] two literatures whose intersection has not been subject to systematic review” by esteemed researcher Jacob Birnberg (1986). In the following two decades researchers began to focus on several specialized aspects, such as cognitive processes and other specific auditing topics. As Bamber (1993) noted, a general focus on managerial accounting remained, while emphasis shifted to information processes by decision makers. This view has been confirmed in an empirical analysis of the articles published in Behavioral Research in Accounting during the 1990s by Meyer and Rigsby (2001). The two authors found that the focus on research still relied on three core schools, namely managerial control systems, accounting information processing and audit (p. 255 f) 1.2 Behavioral Aspects in Auditing Considering behavioral aspects in auditing, Shields (2009) argues that a significant impetus which jumpstarted research in auditing was the Research Opportunities in Auditing program, funded by KPMG in 1976. A first study on the topic of auditor judgments was conducted by Ashton only two years prior, in 1974 (Trotman 2011, p. 203). As was the case in BAR during this period, studies in auditing focused heavily on cognitive issues and judgment and decision making. Knechel (2000) proposes that the reason for the surge in JDM research lies in the fact that auditing “is inherently a judgment process" (p. 695). This viewpoint was supported by Gillenkirch and Arnold (2008) who gave a very broad and concise overview of the state of research up to the publishing date. Trotman (2011) offers a 11 Katrin Wittke International Business Management more personal chronological overview of the literature on audit judgment and decision making in his aptly called article “A Different Personal Perspective through the Behavioral Accounting Literature”, which is supposed to be regarded as a counterpart to the overview of management accounting literature by Shields. He confirms that the developments by Kahneman and Tverksy in the field of biases and heuristics as well as investigations into the role of memory and other cognitive approaches had a strong impact on the research field. The author also points out that while during the 1990s focus lay mainly on elements influencing the auditor’s judgment performance, such as time pressure and accountability, the arrival of new auditing standards and the implementation of the business risk approach influenced the direction of research in the new millennium and steered it towards betweenauditor interactions. Trotman also predicts that possible directions for the future might influenced by further “[c]hanges in regulation and audit processes” which offer new opportunities for experiments (Trotman 2011, p. 205) 12 Katrin Wittke International Business Management 2. Developments in Rational Thinking Over the last five decades, economists and academics with a background in psychology have begun to move away from the thitherto generally accepted idea of the rational person and developed more realistic concepts of human behavior. In the following sections, after an overview of the development of homo economicus, selected concepts of behavioral theories will be presented. Then, the judgment-oriented approach as well as the convergence of organizational and behavioral theories will be examined. 2.1 Homo economicus - Origins, Adaptations, Criticism For more than a century, the concept of homo economicus represented the foundation of economic thought, yet there is still dissonance among economists and researchers about where the model originated. While there is overall agreement that the general characteristics of what would eventually become a well-developed framework can be found in Adam Smith’s works, specifically in The Wealth of Nations (1776, reprinted in 1991), it is debatable whether it was Leon Walras, Eduard Spranger or John Stuart Mill who defined the concept. As Joseph Persky notes in The Ethology of Homo Economicus (1995), the economist and philosopher John Stuart Mill is usually credited with formulating the more detailed characteristics in 1848, and the term homo economicus was derived in reaction to his work. As we shall see, the evolution of homo economicus over the 20th century was marked by heavy criticism from the sociologists and earned outright denunciation by advocates of the behavioralism movement. The cornerstones of this development and their implications on related fields of studies will be discussed in detail. In Adam Smith’s works, the - at the time - nameless economic man was described as a self-loving or self-interested individual which pursues maximum utility, or welfare in any given situation (Smith 1776, reprinted in 1991). This general idea was developed further by Mill (1848, reprinted in 2012) until it became a highly detailed model of a rational human being. Karafyllis (2002, p. 19) argues that a person can be described as rational when she chooses the alternative which, given her overall objectives, opinions and values as well as the underlying conditions, promises to fulfill her goals in the optimal way. Other criteria for rationality which can be applied to attitudes and preferences are their adherence to logic and probabilities, the fact that they should be coherent, i.e. stable and consistent, 13 Katrin Wittke International Business Management be based on relevant and material factors and must not be in conflict with empirical facts and observations known to the choosing person (Wilkinson 2008, p. 391). Over the following decades the view of rational man evolved through discussion and reinterpretation into a “rational self-interested utility maximizer” (Wilkinson 2008, p. 11), while the degree of assumed selfishness varies strongly among researchers. While many interpreted the classical “Butcher statement“1 from Smith’s Wealth of Nations (1776, reprinted in 1991) as a sign of selfishness, others suggested that Smith’s earlier work, the Theory of Moral Sentiments (1759, reprinted in 1977), gives more insight into the relationship of homo economicus with his environment and definitely describes him as pursuing his “self-interest with sympathy for others and within the moral system of society” (Altman 2006, p. 7). Even though this misunderstanding is widely settled today with the latter view prevailing, some critics still believe the selfish aspect of homo economicus provides reasonable ground for criticism. The reason why homo economicus was so beloved by those who would become representatives of neoclassical economics was because of the characteristics that were assumed. In the neoclassical view, homo economicus was endowed with “full information and unlimited information-processing capacity” (ibid., p. 238), with which he was able to analyze the costs and benefits of any given situation to come to the most utile decision. This model of a rational man offered a simplicity that allowed economists to make predictions which were thought to represent actual patterns of behavior and could be used to rationalize these actions (Frerichs 2011, p. 298). This aspect of homo economicus was necessary for the functioning of neoclassical economic models of equilibrium and supply and demand. However, there are numerous examples of proponents of neoclassical economics and economic man that did not turn a blind eye to the inherent flaws of this model2. While they conceded that a generalization in the form of patterns is necessary in order to depict the complexity of human emotions that affect decision making behavior, critics of rational economic man disagreed with several main points of the concept, the basic tenet of the argument being that the model was reflecting real human behavior. One point of criticism was the fact that social relationships are not included in the neoclassical model. Homo economicus is thereby effectively separated from his environment, and the only influences that theoretically guide his behavior are those of 1 2 "It is not from the benevolence of the butcher, the brewer, or the baker that we expect our dinner, but from Among the Milton Friedman and, of course, Herbert A. Simon. 14 Katrin Wittke International Business Management prices, changes in income and other economic variables (Steele 2004). In this view, fundamental aspects of humanity such as values, beliefs and social interrelationships are effectively ignored by neoclassical economics. Another important aspect of criticism which relates to the incongruence with reality lies in the assumption of complete information and processing capabilities. The most comprehensively developed criticism of this postulate was provided by Herbert A. Simon (1964), who, in the search for a more realistic model, developed the concept of bounded rationality. Even though Simon agreed that man is a “social and rational animal” (ibid., p. vii), he argued that, due to our limited powers of comprehension, humans can only process a small amount of information at once. The idea of omniscience and unlimited intellectual capacities is hence unrealistic and will lead to improbable outcomes when the situation becomes too complex or the individual factors are uncertain (Simon 1964). A further issue which psychologists and especially sociologists took with the idea of homo economicus was the characteristically egoistical nature of such a rational being that is intent on maximizing its own welfare, regardless of the needs of others (Põder 2006, p. 10). However, the view of a selfish homo economicus was heavily misinterpreted from the aforementioned “Butcher statement” by Smith (Alterman 2006, p. 7). One can argue, nevertheless, that the welfare of others is only increased either as a by-product of the decisions that a rational human being makes or because of the self-interested strive for acceptance by others (Jasay 2011). When it comes to the main point of critique, namely the closeness to reality of the model of homo economicus, the opinions differ widely. While some argue that anomalies reflect irrational behavior that is out of the norm and should therefore not be included in the model, others argue the exact opposite: They suggest that rational economic man “does not exist, has never existed” (Vasek 2012), and a model based upon homo economicus does not reflect the behavior of homo sapiens. According to the behavioralist view, human beings are irrational and do not always behave in their own optimal self-interest. Therefore a model which is designed to predict human behavior should take this important aspect of humanity into account. In short, when the model does not reflect reality, the assumptions about humans should not be changes, but instead the model should be adapted (Altman 2006). Behavioral economics, in combination with psychology, aims at explaining those anomalies through adaptations of the assumptions of human rationality, and will be described further in the next section. 15 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Others brought forward the counter-argument that the benefits of a simplified model of human behavior greatly outweighed the shortcomings of a more realistic model (Sen 1977, p. 335 ff.) which would have to be as individualized as the great number of different social norms and roles. They claim that the idea of homo economicus is merely an approximation, a hypothetical construct to be used as an aid to depict behavior of humans and market mathematically and was never meant to be more. The desire of economics to be more like natural sciences demanded mathematical models, clear assumptions about actors and conditions, as Heuser (2008) argued. This “mathematization” of economics required a simplified model of man (Guckelsberger 2005). As Steele notes in his article “Understanding Economic Man”, by employing mathematical approximations inconsistencies and ambiguities can be brought to light and, as he argues, “[i]ts very usefulness lies in its contrast to reality”. (Steele 2004, p. 1030) At this point, it is important to note that while simplicity may be desirable, disregarding empirical evidence which strongly suggests that basic human instincts and behaviors are not perfectly rational cannot lead to an accurate model of markets that consist of the sum of those behaviors. In the following section, selected areas of research and corresponding theories will be discussed, since their observations identified aspects of human behavior which acutely contradicted those of homo economicus and could no longer be ignored. 2.2 Innovative Concepts of Human Behavior Even though empirical evidence was gathered after the end of World War II which contradicted the standard economic model and its view of rationality, economists were still reluctant to alter their model of homo economicus in a drastic way. In order to account for some of the fallibilities, Herbert A. Simon (1964) pioneered the idea of bounded rationality with which he aimed to adapt the neoclassical model into a concept that would represent actual human behavior. Simon argues that in neoclassical theory, the assumptions that are made about economic man do not give detailed information about his psychological characteristics, offering only the description of a rational human being that is intent on maximization of utility. He acknowledges the traditional view of economic man as a powerful tool, yet in order to apply the theory to actual behavior of humans that is observed, the aspect of “global 16 Katrin Wittke International Business Management rationality” (Simon 1964, p. 241) has to be adapted to make it more realistic. It is important to point out that Simon does not dismiss neoclassical theory in its entirety. In the core of the theory of bounded rationality stands the idea that people neither have perfect information nor the cognitive abilities and resources to calculate the probabilities of every choice available to them and their respective results. Instead of optimizing their choice, Simon claims that they tend to “satisfice”, i.e. they do not necessarily look for the best outcome, but the one that is acceptable in that situation by simplifying the choices available to them. In his collection of essays, Models of Man, Simon (1964) describes the individual computing mechanisms that would be involved in the process of maximization of utility (p.241). They include the comparison of alternatives, their relative pay-offs and the ordering of preferences of these payoffs. All of these factors are taken into account to achieve the best solution. He claims that the results of these calculations under psychological limits, specifically “with respect to computational and predictive ability […] can at best be an extremely crude and simplified approximation” (Simon 1964, p. 243). In order to save figurative computing power, humans tend to make use of so-called computational shortcuts, or heuristics. They can be described as rules of thumb and are necessary to derive approximate results from a complex situation with masses of information and with the cognitive capacities available to them. For instance, instead of being able to predict pay-offs of all possible outcomes and ordering them by preference, Simon developed simplifications of pay-off functions. In this proposed approach, decision makers would categorize potential outcomes into two categories, satisfactory or unsatisfactory, and search for the behavioral alternative which would be most satisfactory. This involves a process of ordering and partial ordering of the pay-offs, which, in order to comply with the scope of this thesis, will not be described in detail. Simon points out, however, that the proposed process is drastically simplified and hence less complex than the assumed computations proposed by neoclassical economists. Before moving on to explain the next stage in the development of behavioral theories, it is important to point out that Simon’s hypotheses were not based on “empirical knowledge of the decisional processes”, since they were not examined at that point in time, but instead rest upon common experience (Simon 1964, p. 246). This approach to human behavior, particularly when it comes to biases and the uses of heuristics, however, has since been picked up by several researchers and been proven in experiments and studies. 17 Katrin Wittke International Business Management As empirical examples of violations of rationality began to accumulate 3 an answer needed to be found to explain how irrationality would fit into the view of homo economicus. Opinions differ widely among researchers whether the standard economic model actually aims at depicting real human behavior or whether it is merely a simplifying tool used for predictions, and hence should not be held up to such high standards. A point where disagreement generally subsides however is the fact that standard economics “seems to suggest that empirical ‘anomalies’ indicate human failures (such as a lack of rationality) and not model failures” (Frerichs 2011, p. 293), when the reverse seems to be true, since there exist alternative models which are able to account for many of the observed anomalies. Some of the more well-known experiments and related papers that started to propose alternative theories to the standard economic model were those by Kahneman and Tversky (1979), and Thaler (1980), the former having introduced the so-called prospect theory. It is a nonconventional theory, meaning it is an alternative to the concepts which assume utility maximization (Wilkinson 2008, p. 98). The goal of this descriptive theory was not to model how people should act, but to depict how they actually behave and disregards the idea of perfect rationality. In particular, it aimed at explaining the anomalies which the SEM, specifically the Expected Utility Theory (EUT), failed to account for. Prospect Theory presented a new way to account for cognitive biases and seemingly irrational behaviors which became apparent in experiments. Specifically, the adapted EUT proposes that people employ a decisionweighting mechanism when evaluating several alternatives. The adapted utility formula (see Appendix A) incorporates four elements through which these anomalies can be explained. These four elements are: reference dependence, loss aversion, diminishing sensitivity and probability weighting (Barberis 2013, p. 175). In order to remain within the boundaries of this thesis, the formula will not be discussed in detail. The importance lies especially in the ways in which the formula has been used by Kahneman and Tversky, for example as a way to modernize the decision making process. Most importantly, the authors pioneered the novel concepts of framing and endowment effects and other cognitive biases in subsequent research, all of which have been developed further through studies and are now considered well-known cognitive biases. Through 3 Experiments which fostered further studies were developed by such innovative thinkers as Allais and, of course, Tversky and Kahneman 18 Katrin Wittke International Business Management these, economists were now capable of behavior in financial markets and everyday economic actions in a more realistic way. A selection of these biases and cognitive fallibilities and their potential impact on management accounting processes, auditing processes and organizational behavior will be presented in more detail in the third main section of this thesis. Beforehand, it is important to examine the predominant theories in organizational behavior and its connection with accounting. 2.3 Developments in Organizational Theory With the advent of trade, manufacturing and industrialization, the organization has become an integral part of society. It is where individuals spend a significant amount of their time and energy, which they exchange for money or other inducements. In this section, an overview of the organization and its relationship with accounting will be given. An organization can be described as “a social system that is created to achieve certain specific goals or objectives” (Riahi-Belkaoui 2002, p.18). Caplan (1968) offers an overview of the differences between traditional and modern organizational theory in detail. Born out of the necessity to organize a large amount of people and keep them motivated as a result of industrialization (Caplan 1966, p. 500), organizational theory in its traditional form incorporated the basic ideas of rationality and classical economics. The goals of the organization are inherently the goals of the main decision maker, since the organization itself is not a sentient being. The goal of the decision maker is assumed to be profit maximization and, in turn, cost reduction. He, the entrepreneur, chooses among several available alternatives concerning the combinations of factors of production to produce corresponding outputs. Thanks to perfect information and complete rationality the decision maker can analyze costs and benefits of the different combinations of factors of production in a way to maximize profit (Simon 1964, p. 171). In fact, behavior in the traditional model is assumed to be bias-free. In this original theory of the firm, or “F-theory”, as Simon called it, the rational individuals are inherently driven by the desire for utility maximization. The goal of management, next to profit maximization, is to “secure effective participation” (Caplan 1966, p. 502) from the individuals in the organization. This is done in part by a mix of economic rewards and punishments, and through authoritative control from above. He goes on to describe their reasons to enter an organization as purely instrumental (Simon 1964, p. 166). Individuals 19 Katrin Wittke International Business Management are assumed to regard work as an unpleasant task which they try to avoid, and are hence even considered “lazy”. Once the individuals have entered an organization, they become passive instruments (Caplan 1966, p.500). Since the assumptions about behavior of participants are extremely limited in the traditional view, more faceted motivations which go beyond economic incentives and could lead to a more optimal functioning of organizations are almost entirely ignored. This failure to take the needs of individuals into consideration has been shown to lead to frustration and conflict, mistrust and hostility in organizations (ibid., p. 506). These limited assumptions about individual goals and motivation make the traditional model a very simple one, with a much more uncomplicated view of behavior than the more modern model which will be presented next. In contrast to this traditional theory of the firm, the organizational decision-making perspective, or O(rganizational)-Theory has been developed on the basis of behavioral assumptions which correspond with Simon’s principle of bounded rationality. This model is shaped by assumptions about communication and decision making of the individual participants, and has been shaped by March and Simon (1958). One of the main differences between the traditional model and the decision-making approach to organizational theory lies in the assumption of goals of individual participants. Unlike the rational man in F-theory, who has “no place in the theory of organization” (Simon 1964, p. 198), the individuals under modern organizational theory join organizations to reach personal goals. They do not merely consist of economic incentives, but also of social and psychological aspects. Similarly, in response to criticisms that the singular goal of profit maximization is unrealistic and does not reflect actual organizations, the goal of the organization in modern theory is assumed to be less one-sided. One of the proposed goals is organizational survival, which can include more faceted objectives, like innovation, increase in market share or productivity and growth. In order to fulfill these aims, it is necessary that the goals of the individuals are not in conflict with the goals of the organization. Therefore, the overall objective is to achieve goal congruence. As was the case in traditional organizational theory, it is the objective of management to ensure that participants cooperate efficiently. However, the idea of authority from above has been abandoned. While management still strives to influence the behavior of individuals, this influence is only effective if the individual is willing to accept such 20 Katrin Wittke International Business Management influence (Caplan 1966, p. 501). The decision to accept is affected by social, psychological and economical factors. In terms of acceptance of the two main models, Caplan (ibid.) suggests that, while the traditional model has faults, its perseverance as a widely accepted theory suggests that there are some inherent truths in it and benefits of its practical uses. The two views could be regarded as opposite sides of a spectrum, with less extreme possibilities which combine aspects of both theories in the middle. Naturally, the traditional model and the modern organizational theory are not the only possible models of organizational behavior. Other alternatives include the Shareholder Wealth Maximization Model, the Social Welfare Maximization Model and the Problem Solving Model. For the sake of a concise overview, these will not be discussed in detail. The crux of the difference between the two theories, as suggested by Colville (1981), is that one is aimed at explaining the organization while the other concerns itself with the individuals and their behavior. It is important to point out that these factors should not be viewed as two separate entities, since they are inextricably linked. Contingency theory, as developed by Thompson (1967) actually managed to combine the insights on behavior and decision-making from March and Simon with sociological aspects, which allowed for a closer look on “control systems, budgeting and planning. When it comes to the importance of accounting in organizations, Colville (1981) stated it best when he said that it “is not only a product of organizational reality, it also influences and helps produce that very reality” (p.129). Management accounting in particular does not merely serve management as a delivery system of information – it is an integral part of it. Management accountants as individuals are themselves members of the organization and, as such, are motivated by similar psychological and social objectives such as security, prestige, power, and self-actualization. Motivation could also derive from sub-unit loyalty – i.e., through the departmentalization of the organization, accountants could end up focusing on the performance of the accounting department while ignoring other important aspects which would benefit the organization as a whole (Caplan 1966, p. 508) It is not the aim of this discussion to determine which of these theories represents actual behavior in organizations more realistically, or in which accounting system could produce the best results. More importantly, the context in which accounting as a decision support 21 Katrin Wittke International Business Management system functions needed to be outlined in order to examine judgment and decision making processes in detail. 22 Katrin Wittke International Business Management 3. Selected Aspects of Organizational Behavior In this section, two fields of study which cannot be left out in a discussion of behavior and accounting will be discussed. First of all, the judgment and decision-making process will be outlined and selected cognitive biases will be examined. Secondly, the influences of control on behavior and motivation are investigated. These two aspects have received the most attention in the literature on behavior (Hofstedt 1976, p. 42) and are deeply ingrained in both accounting and auditing research fields. 3.1 Judgment and Decision-Making JDM processes are integral parts of both accounting and auditing, not to mention everyday managerial processes. As such, it is important to look at the way that the human mind performs such processes, in what situations they malfunction, and why it is important to investigate these problems and try to resolve them. The first half of the process, the judgment, refers to the cognitive part of a two-part process. It refers to the formation of an idea or opinion and reflects one’s beliefs (Bonner 1999). This can involve an assessment of risk, predictions for the future or other phenomena. The decision involves “making up one’s mind” about the evaluated issue and choosing the appropriate behavior according to one’s preferences. The decision-making process itself can be grouped into steps. For the purposes of this section it is sufficient to summarize the steps as they are proposed by Bazerman and Moore (2013, p. 2). The first step, which is identifying and defining the problem, seems to be intuitive, but it represents an important means to understanding where the roots of a disruption lie. An insufficient investigation of the problem can lead to the treatment of superficial symptoms instead of underlying causes.. The second and third steps are sometimes left out in other descriptions of the process, since they concern the criteria which are relevant in the decision-making process, for example different attributes and factors that can influence the result. This means that all factors that affect the final decision are regarded in detail and are weighed according to their importance. The fourth and fifth points according to Bazerman and Moore deal with the generation and rating of alternatives. This is the one of the main points which have come under criticism in the past: An objective identification of all possible alternative courses of actions and the rating of the outcomes of each alternatives is one of the cornerstones of decision-making, 23 Katrin Wittke International Business Management but virtually impossible in practice. Individuals neither have the necessary information to imagine all possible alternatives, nor the cognitive capacities to compute the probabilities and resulting outcomes under uncertainty. Last but not least, this process, if done “rationally” would take an inappropriate amount of time. In order to derive the optimal decisions, the values which were assigned to the alternatives have to be weighted by multiplying these values with the weight of the criteria. At the end of this process, the rational decision-maker chooses the alternative with the highest rating. It seems highly implausible that any person in an organizational setting has the knowledge, cognitive (or mathematical) abilities or time to follow each of these steps in detail. On top of that, external conditions such as environmental factors and value structures are constantly changing, which inhibits an accurate assessment of criteria, probabilities and the final value. As Tversky and Kahneman (1974) have argued, to cope with these uncertainties, individuals make use of heuristics and other shortcuts. Simon (1981) proposed that instead of maximizing the value of the outcome, humans tend to choose the alternative which represents the best solution under the present conditions. Reasons for possible errors which can occur as a consequence of these shortcuts are presented in the next section as well as possible remedies and debiasing techniques. 3.2 Cognitive Fallibilities in Accounting and Auditing Some of the more human, integral cognitive effects such as loss aversion have already been observed and described as early as the 18th century by Adam Smith (1759, reprinted 1977) while other, less obvious cognitive fallibilities and biases have only been formulated in the last fifty years. These fallibilities result from the fact that, as argued before, humans have limited cognitive abilities and must rely on “simple probabilistic rules” (Wilkinson 2008, p.393), or shortcuts, better known as heuristics to cope with a complex environment. As Tversky and Kahneman (1974) have argued, the use of rules of thumb to simplify situations can result in “severe and systematic errors” (p. 1124). Three of the cognitive biases which they developed and which have been heavily researched in reaction to their work will be presented in this section. The selection was made in order to include those biases which have been observed to impact both management accounting and auditing processes. 3.2.1 Availability One of the biases that were developed by Tversky and Kahneman (1973) in a paper with the very same name was the availability bias. It is a heuristic which is said to affect an 24 Katrin Wittke International Business Management individual’s judgment of the estimated probability of an event by the “ease with which instances or associations” (p. 208) come to mind. The individual's estimation of this probability can be distorted by evidence from the environment, for example by recent examples which are still fresh in memory. The concreteness or vividness of these examples and can increase the availability effect and, if the examples are not related, lead to errors in judgment. In everyday behavior, people use heuristics in order to make more or less accurate predictions about the probability that any given event might occur. As has been argued when it comes to the safety of driving compared to the likelihood of an airplane crash or, especially after 9/11, a terrorist attack, events that attract more attention in the news because of their highly scandalous or sensationalized attributes tend to skew our probabilistic capabilities. The vividness of the images that come to mind, in part because of more extended television coverage, can influence or estimation of probabilities and result in biased judgments. In accounting, the research on the effect of this bias on judgment and estimation of probabilities is scarce. It does not seem too unlikely, however, that accounting processes are influenced by this cognitive heuristic process which whose effects on probability estimation in our everyday lives has already been proven. One example of availability bias in accounting could occur when it comes to budget planning. When trying to estimate the likelihood of an event, such as failure to stay within the budget, the accountant might recall an unrelated event of great vividness which in turn influences his estimation of the probability of the actual planning subject, for example the “fiscal cliff” which threatened in the United States. This hypothetical example is simply a thought experiment to illustrate how availability biases could affect accounting decisions. Additionally, as Peters (1993) points out, heuristics and biases can affect the way that accounting information is recalled, especially by decision makers such as auditors. When it comes to the examination of availability bias in auditing, the body of research is far more voluminous, albeit not necessarily definite in its evaluation of its consequences. One example of how audit planning might be influenced by availability heuristics is the development of the current auditing plan. During this process, auditors are usually tempted to base their estimations on last year’s audit results. The bias that comes into play is not only availability bias but also the (similar) familiarity bias, which refers to the tendency of individuals to prefer familiar alternatives, which are similar to past decisions, over new ones (Knapp & Knapp 2012). On the other hand, research on the effects of availability bias 25 Katrin Wittke International Business Management suggests that new information factors in highly in the estimation of events. Therefore, one could argue that last year’s audit results might not favor in too markedly and the two biases cancel each other out. Nonetheless, it is important to point out that some anchoring and adjustment is taking place during this process. The effects of this bias will be discussed in the following section. One of the more notable studies to examine auditor’s susceptibility to availability bias was conducted by Libby (1985) and looked at the development of error hypotheses which he assumed to be “influenced by their expected frequency of occurrence” (p. 650). The results of the experiments confirm Libby’s hypothesis and he proposes that there is a positive connection between the availability of errors in memory and the estimated frequency by which these occur (p. 657). The responses in the subsequent literature to these findings were manifold. Some researchers such as Shanteau (1989) have argued that it might not have been availability bias which was proven in these experiments, but actually accessibility. Others posited and were able to prove that the influence of availability bias significantly decreased for more experienced or specialized auditors who were able to judge the probability of errors with more accuracy than inexperienced auditors (Libby and Frederick 1990). A similar immunity of experienced auditors for irrelevant, yet highly available information such as audit results from the previous year was also found by Bedard and Wright (1994) One more example can be found by Knapp and Knapp (2012): They argue that since client fraud is a “rare event” (p. 41), a vivid event might not easily come to mind and hence might be underestimated. In order to explain the failure of an auditing team to detect fraudulent behavior by retailer Crazie Eddie Inc., the authors argue that the auditors simply underestimated the likelihood of such an event because of the scarcity of readily available examples. The presumption made by Knapp and Knapp in this example is not supported by empirical studies and it is debatable whether it is a very realistic example, especially when one considers the relative recentness of scandals such as Enron, Bernie Madoff and the general chaos in the aftermath of the financial crisis. 3.2.2 Adjustment and Anchoring Adjustment and Anchoring refers to the heuristic used by people when they make estimates by starting from an initial value - the anchor - and then adjusting the values to yield the final answer (Tversky & Kahneman 1974). It is therefore a two-step process 26 Katrin Wittke International Business Management where the anchor can either be selected by the individual or given by an external source. In the original experiment by Tversky and Kahneman, subjects were asked to estimate the percentage of African member countries in the UN. At the same time, a random number was generated on a spinning wheel. Between the estimated numbers of the control group and of those who generated random number, the unrelated number had a clear effect on the estimations which were made. These effects persisted even though the subjects were reminded that it was a randomly generated number and they adjusted from the anchored number. These results have been replicated in further experiments, for example when subjects were asked to estimate the population of Chicaco, or the willingness to pay for certain items (Wilkinson 2008, p. 395). The argument that has been put forward in the subsequent research was that the adjustment that was made to the initial value was insufficient and left the result too close to the anchor, therefore leading to a bias. Epley and Gilovich (2006) argued that these insufficient adjustments are the result of a practice which resembles the idea of satisficing, originally presented by Simon (1964) in the context of bounded rationality. They argue that the adjustment process is only continued until the value reaches “a range of plausible values” (p. 312), which implies a kind of cost-saving method of cognitive capacities. A practical utilization of anchoring and adjustment in managerial as accounting proposed by Ahmed Riahi-Belkaoui (2002) refers to the development of cost forecast based on an anchor of last year’s results (p. 215), which is then adjusted to account for differences in conditions, such as raw material prices or inflation. Unlike the previously studied examples, this process, arguably a widely used practice in accounting and budgeting, would represent a conscious process made by the accountant. When it comes to auditing tasks, there have been several areas where the use of anchoring heuristics has been posited and studied in corresponding experiments. The results from these studies vary significantly. One domain wherein possible effects of availability bias have been studied is the process of developing the extent of substantive tests for the auditing period. Researchers proposed that the range of tests and sample sizes from a previous period or engagement are used as an anchor, which will then be adjusted to changes in the environment of the firm, for instance of internal control mechanisms. This would mean that for a firm which has increased its control mechanisms, the magnitude of tests would be smaller. While this may seem like a useful process, the process can be 27 Katrin Wittke International Business Management erroneous if the adjustments are not moving “far enough from the anchor” (Joyce & Biddle 1981, p.122). This hypothesis was tested by Joyce and Biddle and the results of their experiments were largely inconclusive. While experiments which were marked by low auditor familiarity confirmed their assumption, a clear impact of anchoring bias was not found in other evinronments. They vaguely summarize: “sometimes anchoring happens, and sometimes it does not” (ibid., p.143). One important point of discussion among researchers of heuristics in auditing is the influence of experience as a mitigating factor. Experimental results by Shields et al. (1988) revealed that more experienced auditors are able to concentrate their utilization of this heuristic to those circumstances when it is efficient to make use of them and hence reduce the impact of biases. It has been argued, though, that the results of the research on this specific subject are not conclusive enough. Wüstemann and Koch (2007) make the point that the reason why the influence of experience on the effect of heuristics is hard to measure lies in the design of experiments. In order to accurately measure the impact of biases, there would have to be a control group which performs the same tasks as the uncontrolled group, consisting of experienced auditors. In order to be reliable, the control group would have to be inexperienced – but at the same time they need to have enough experience to understand and perform the tasks, a difficult research setting to attain (p. 23). When it comes to the task of mitigating anchoring effects, before discussing possible debiasing techniques, it has to be reviewed whether or not it is necessary to attempt to do so. Since the research on the extend of the heuristic effects are inconclusive, and practical work experience seems to have positive influences on their magnitude, it could be argued that a restructuring of judgment processes in auditing might do more harm, in the form of costs, than good. On top of that, the conservative behavior of auditors which is driven by motivation and incentives has been observed as a natural debiasing technique by Cohen & Kida (1989). For example, auditors might be reluctant to reduce the extent of tests of detail in cases where analytical review did not indicate any problems. On the other hand, when the conservative behavior of auditors is paired with adjustment to anchors, it might be important to consider some debiasing strategies. 28 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Most of the mitigating techniques proposed in the literature which are supposed to lessen the effects of heuristics involve conscious deliberation of the information available. As evidenced by Epley and Gilovich (2005), the mere “forewarning of bias” (p. 209) improved the adjustment process of participants when dealing with internally provided anchors. Since anchors can also be provided from external sources, the authors argued that the outside stimuli should be either avoided or given conscious consideration to determine in what way they could be wrong. To sum up, it can be argued that the most effective and cost-efficient technique might be to encourage auditors to give information conscious and critical deliberation. 3.2.3 Confirmation bias Confirmation bias, which is also known in the literature as positive test strategy, refers to the tendency of people to selectively search for and prefer information which confirms their own beliefs and expectations (Knapp & Knapp 2012) and, in turn, to favor evidence less which does not support their hypothesis. Our brain usually has “reasons” why it employs certain shortcuts. One of the reasons why we selectively retrieve information which supports instead of contradicts our initial beliefs is that this way, we are more likely to gather useful information. Also, due to our limited cognitive abilities to process information, confirmation bias can act as a useful shortcut which requires fewer cognitive capabilities. Instead of analyzing every single piece of information or evidence, individuals tend to concentrate on certain facts which are useful to them. In everyday behavior confirmation bias can present itself in the form of unconsciously preferring to watch political shows which support their beliefs, or to put less weight on information which contradicts them. Another reason why people might favor information that supports their own point of view is to reduce cognitive dissonance. Armet (2013) argues that people do not like to be at odds with themselves, i.e. their actions and their values should, optimally, reflect one another. Incongruence between one’s values and evidence might lead to feelings of cognitive dissonance. The focus on evidence that supports our values is a mechanism to reduce such negative feelings. An important characteristic of confirmation bias as it is understood by psychologists as well as in this context is the fact that it is done unconsciously; i.e. subjects do not intend to prefer information which favors their own point of view, and they may not 29 Katrin Wittke International Business Management even be aware of the effects of the bias. Numerous studies have been conducted in areas such as gambling, astrology, descriptions and judgments of personality traits and reinterpretation of observed behavior to confirm a hypothesis 4, all of which have shown that there exists an asymmetry in the weighting of confirming and disconfirming information in favor of a preset belief. Confirmation bias can also present itself in disproportional criticism of evidence that is more likely to refute our hypothesis. When it comes to the effects of confirmation bias in accounting settings, the research has focused largely on tax professionals, with a significant amount of evidence in studies pointing at the existence of confirmation bias5. One of the reasons why accountants might have a predetermined opinion which would lead to the favoring of information that supports that belief is the client relationship. During the evidence collecting phase, which lower level accountants are responsible for, confirmation bias can occur in the shape of accountants relying more heavily on information which coincided with their initial opinion of the client. In a study by Hatfield (2001) the supervising function of higher-level accountants has been shown to mitigate this bias during the review process. This is in accordance with previous findings that work experience has a mitigating effect on biases, the same way that practical experience has been shown to reduce the effect of availability bias in the previous section. In auditing, this bias can come into play in such a way that auditors may selectively search for evidence that supports initial assessments and hypotheses about the “fairness of a client’s financial statements” (McMillan & White 1993, p. 445) rather than for information which rejects those. Another similarity between confirmation bias and availability bias is the ambiguity of the research results when it comes to the magnitude of the effects in auditing. During the assessment of the influence of confirmation bias on the behavior of auditors, it is important to evaluate the reciprocating or even counteracting effects of professional skepticism, also known as conservative bias. In the International Standard on Auditing (2009, p. 78), professional skepticism is defined as 4 Numerous examples can be found, cf. for example studies by Snyder & Uranowitz (1978), Darley and Gross (1983), Duncan (1976). 5 Cf. Hatfield (2001), Johnson (1993), Cloyd and Spilker (1999) 30 Katrin Wittke International Business Management “An attitude that includes a questioning mind, being alert to conditions which may indicate possible misstatement due to error or fraud, and a critical assessment of audit evidence.” This initial mindset which focuses on error-related evidence can have various effects on confirmation bias: If the hypothesis of the auditor is that the client’s financial information is correct, professional skepticism could lessen confirmation bias, since without skepticism the individual would be prone to put more weight on information which supports their initial belief. On the other hand, if conservative bias is engrained in an individual’s mindset to a point where their conscious and unconscious beliefs are that the financial information is wrong, confirmation bias and professional skepticism could reinforce one another. That is, the instead of balancing each other out, the auditor would shift his focus largely on information that would prove financial statements to be faulty. While this is inarguably the point of auditing, a disproportionate weighting of error-related information can lead to unnecessary and ineffective audit procedures (McMillan 1993). As has been implied above, in an effort to ascertain whether auditors focus on confirming rather than disconfirming evidence, research findings have been contradictory. McMillan and White (1993) conducted a study whose findings suggest that the susceptibility to and magnitude of confirmation bias depends on the potential source for discrepancies. They found that depending on whether an individual believes that fluctuations in financial information result from erroneous financial reporting or from changes in the economic and financial environment, the auditors were more or less likely to be influenced by confirmation bias, respectively. Those that believed in erroneous financial statements were more prone to be influenced by confirming and disconfirming evidence. Although some of the discrepancies among research findings can be traced back to differences in research methods, a similar inconclusiveness can be observed when it comes to the attempt to identify the relationship between work experience and confirmation bias. Scientific research6 as well as psychological studies have found that confirmation bias prevails over several levels of expertise, while there have also been contradicting studies which suggest that experience mitigates the effects of confirmation bias7. While it seems intuitive that work experience and expertise help with the mitigation of biases, a 6 7 Cf. Bamber et al. (1997), Jones and Sugden (2001) Cf. Kaplan and Reckers (1989) 31 Katrin Wittke International Business Management conclusion has not yet been reached. Therefore it is necessary to take debiasing techniques into consideration. One such proposed method is the encouraging of auditors to identify multiple possible reasons for fluctuations and sources of problems. This evaluation of alternatives promotes critical thinking and can lead to a more balanced analysis of evidence. Another suggestion which aims at engaging auditors in further reflection upon the given information and their corresponding decisions is the implementation of extended required documentations (Emby & Finley 1997). It can be argued, however, that his is a way to make the auditing process less efficient. Results of the study by Hatfield (2001) suggest that the review process already mitigates some of the effects of confirmation bias, since it combines both aspects that were mentioned above: The review is performed by a more experienced auditor and it imposes upon the lower-level auditors the knowledge that their results will be subject of scrutiny, which can encourage critical, yet balanced thinking. 3.3 Control, Incentives and Their Effects on Behavior Modern organizations have become large, multifaceted, multinational, departmentalized and hierarchical conglomerates. Is indispensable that there be a mechanism to make sure that the operations run smoothly and individuals are held accountable for their actions (Riahi-Belkaoui 2002, p. 47). Control systems therefore fulfill an essential function of the management activity. The different kinds of control and their effects on performance are therefore examined in this section. According to Caplan (1971), the accounting system serves as “a control device which permits management to identify and correct undesirable performance” (p. 13). Accounting information itself provides the basis for the control of costs and people (Jones 1976, p.17), and can motivate and influence the behavior of individuals working in the organization, from factory workers to managers. It is a means to achieve guide motivation of individuals towards the direction of the goals of the organization to achieve goal congruence. It is used to keep track of the performance of individual departments and to verify whether the goals of the organization are being met. Riahi- Belkaoui (2002) provides an overview over different classifications of control and their uses. For behavioral accounting, a few selected means of control are important. One 32 Katrin Wittke International Business Management of the most important types of control is the setting of standards of performance. Such standards can aim for example at an increase in efficiency or a lowering of costs. In order the performance of the firm and the degree of conformance among the individuals, the actual performance needs to be measured and compared to the previously set standard. Differences can lead to errors or sources of control. This represents the traditional model of control through predetermined budgets or standard costs. Additional classifications of control include the standardization of quality for external suppliers, and, of course, the motivation of individuals within a firm. The exertion of control of one human being over another does not occur if there is no basis for control. One of the most logical bases for control lies in the fact that organization compensates individuals for specific tasks. The basis of control in this case lies within the ability of the firm to give out rewards for performance, be they monetary, through promotions or by increasing benefits or responsibilities of individuals. On the flipside of that coin, the organization can also punish individuals for behavior which does not coincide with the goals of the organization or directly conflict the standards of performance. Punishments can include demotions, stripping of benefits or simply a bad reputation. The basis of power depends of the individual situation Riahi-Belkaoui (2002). The aim of accounting system and information is to motivate individuals to act in a way that is beneficial for the information and helps achieve the goals of the organization (04, 18). But, as has been argued before, the organization is made up of a number of people whose individual goals might not be aligned with the goals of the organization. This goal incongruence might stem from personal economic goals, intergroup conflict or the will to increase the performance of a specific department at the expense of the benefit of the organization as a whole. The desired consistency between personal goals and those of the organization might be unattained. In order to motivate people in such a way as to influence their behavior so that it might benefit the firm as a whole, individual needs have to be taken into consideration and “be met by appropriate economic rewards” (Jones 1976, p. 19). According to the modern organizational theory, these needs go beyond simple economic incentives to include fair treatment, recognition, self-actualization and other less tangible aspects of human needs. An important aspect of organizational control is the reaction of the individuals towards the control mechanisms: Argyris’s widely cited research on “Human Problems with Budgets” 33 Katrin Wittke International Business Management (1953) has found that budgets can be viewed as negative when they are perceived to be used by management to manipulate or control them. If an individual feels like he is being intentionally steered towards the goals of the organization, control can be viewed as an unnecessary exercise of power over others and yield strong, negative emotional responses to control. One of the most commonly proposed solutions for the development of common goals and the directing of individuals towards wanting to achieve them is participative budgeting. The cooperation between a manager and his superior on goals and budgets for the next periods has been shown to have many beneficial effects: First of all, the communication between the two hierarchically different individuals improves. Secondly, if the manager is allowed to make active contributions towards the setting of goals and budgets, he is more likely to identify himself with the goals. It is important to point out that failure to include managers in communication and active participation can have just the opposite effect. Lastly, if done right, the managers internalize the collaboratively set goals and are hence more likely to work towards achievement of these goals. Another aspect of control which can influence the compliance of individuals with firm policy has been observed Birnberg and Nath (1967). They argue that assert that internal auditing can alter worker behavior in a positive way. The research on the relationship between internal or external auditing and manager and accountant behavior has been sporadic at best. Future research in this field would offer a deeper insight into why and how individuals prepare for the audit and whether they continue their “good” behavior over a prolonged period of time – or simply until the audit is over. 34 Katrin Wittke International Business Management 4. From The Auditor’s Perspective As was already indicated in the introduction of this thesis, it is impossible to regard aspects of internal accounting completely separately from auditing. The objective appraisal of the financial statements has become a normal part of business for many medium and large firms, specifically public companies and those who wish to obtain external financial investments or satisfy a stakeholder’s wishes. Therefore, in the influences of independence and objectivity on performance are examined and proposed measures to offset these adverse effects and alternatives are explores. 4.1 Auditor Independence – Where Do Loyalties Lie? Auditing is a judgment decision-making process with many factors, both behavioral and technical, which can influence the final results among the way. Attempts to define which characteristics of an audit environment have the strongest effects on audit quality have been manifold. One such example is an empirical study by Carcello, Hermanson and McGrath (1992), which has confirmed the assumption that attributes of the audit team are perceived as more important than those of the audited firm. Especially aspects of experience, both industry-specific and client-related of the audit team and of the firm itself as well as the auditor’s responsiveness to the needs of the client have been found to exert the highest influence on audit quality. In subsequent research, the relationship between auditor tenure in a specific firm and independence has been examined in detail. Reasons for the seemingly ceaseless interest in this field partly result from the fact that assumptions of a correlation between tenure are both intuitive and controversial: More often than not does an organization stay with the same auditing company for several years. This means that auditors, be they managers or partners, tend to come back year after year and spend a significant amount of time with the preparers of financial information and members of the management. While the benefits which result from auditors being familiar with the firm, its members and internal processes are plentiful, the closeness between auditor and client that is built up over time can adversely affect auditor independence and, in turn, audit quality. This conflict can be illustrated by regarding the hierarchical layers in auditing: The auditor receives his wage from the audit firm. The audit firm is in turn paid by the audited organization, which contracted the audit firm due to regulations or interest of external share- and/or 35 Katrin Wittke International Business Management stakeholders. Going back to the auditor, although he spent a significant amount of time working for the auditing firm, he also resides in close contact with the client for what can shape up to be a large amount of time, depending on the size of the organization. Auditors might therefore try to avoid discord and relax stern measures such as large sample sizes or in depth-analysis of certain processes to the client’s benefits. Knechel and Vanstraelen (2007) propose further possible reasons for loss of independence, for example the desire of auditors to avoid conflict with the client in order to keep him as such. The pressure on auditors to keep lucrative long-term relationships with clients is enormous (Kroll 2012), since the costs of acquiring new clients includes marketing and initial fees, not to mention the process of familiarization with the new client which can take up to two years (Daugherty et al. 2012, p. 99), can be significant. Hence, auditors might rather unwittingly turn a blind eye and indulge client request for leniency (Daugherty et al. 2013, p. 29) But one study which concerned itself with the relationship between client and auditor, specifically with service ethic instilled upon members of audit firms stands out: AndersonGough et al. (2000) conducted a detailed study in order to determine the ways how “client discourse” impacts the training of new members of the organization and found that “the client” has become an abstract symbol which is used to justify long work hours, tight budgets and other requirements of behavior. In the past, research on the relationship between auditor tenure and audit quality, which can be measured by the likelihood of the auditors to issue correct going concern opinion when the company subsequently goes bankrupt, has yielded contradicting results8. The issues of competence and industry-specific expertise are also possible reasons for variations in audit quality, but are not subject of the debate on hand. Instead, possible measures of increasing audit objectivity, performance and independence will be examined after having introduced another important aspect of everyday auditing behavior that can influence audit quality. 8 Cf. James N. Myers, Linda A. Myers, and Thomas C. Omer (“Exploring the Term of the Auditor-Client Relationship,”Accounting Review, vol. 78, no. 3, 2003,pp. 779–799), and Van E. Johnson, Inder K. and Khurana, and J. Kenneth Reynolds (“Audit-Firm Tenure and the Quality of Financial Reports,” Contemporary Accounting Research,vol. 19, no. 4, 2002, pp. 637–660) 36 Katrin Wittke International Business Management 4.2 Measures to Ensure Audit Quality Having examined the most widespread effects on audit quality, an answer needs to be found on how to enhance the objectivity of auditors, their independence and reinforce their inherent skepticism to capitalize on its effectiveness. One of the possible measures to prevent the negative effects of long tenure is the introduction of mandatory auditor rotation. A topic which has been under discussion since the 1970s, it has been proposed again by the “Regulation of the European Parliament and of the Council on Specific Requirements Regarding Statutory Audit of Public-Interest Entities” in 2011, and by the Public Company Accounting Oversight Board (PCAOB) in August of 2012. Both institutions proposed to introduce specific term limits after which firms would be obliged to change audit firms. The proposed term lengths vary from periods of six to seven years. Proponents of such measures stress the importance to “protect” auditor independence, objectivity and skepticism towards clients which can be overshadowed by the pressure to keep long client relationships. Some of the more famous failures of auditors to discover fraud or to issue going concern opinions9 have been blamed on decreased auditor skepticism due to long, close auditor-client-relationships (Daugherty 2013, p. 29). It could be claimed that a mandatory rotation of audit firms lowers the probability of the development of close relationships with clients. Additionally, the prospect of another firm reviewing the work in the next fiscal year after the rotation has taken place can act as powerful incentive to identify and report problems. While it is incontestable that a “new set of eyes” can offer a new point of view on a given situation, the disadvantages of auditor rotation are numerous and should not be disregarded lightly. Many have argued that positive effects of audit firm rotation on audit quality are not clearly discernible from empirical evidence. On the contrary, longitudinal studies in countries which have introduced mandatory audit rotation such as Korea and Italy have shown that quality actually increases with tenure (Kroll 2012, p.45). The observed drop in audit quality in the beginning of a new rotation can actually have the adverse effect of increasing audit failures instead of reducing them. 9 The most prominent examples are the failures of American International Group Inc. and Lehman Brothers Holdings Inc. 37 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Another important aspect is that of client- and industry-specific knowledge which is accumulated over time. If auditors are forced to leave a firm after a specific time period, the building of institutional expertise becomes more difficult, and in the worst-case scenario, cannot be applied to other firms in similar industries. Lastly, the cost of mandatory audit rotation is one of the most frequently cited negative consequences of the introduction of mandatory rotation. With a new client comes the process of gathering knowledge about the client and familiarizing with the organizational individualities, which costs a lot of time and hence would drive up audit fees. The increased costs would be passed along to the client, while other market forces remain stagnant and put increased competitive pressures on audit firms (Daugherty 2013, p. 33). The plethora of resistance against the proposed regulation, supported by research findings that should not be ignored, poses the question whether there are more (cost-) effective alternatives to the problem of auditor-client relationships. In fact, one alternative to the stringent mandatory rotation of auditor firms has been in effect since 2002: the SarbanesOxley Act of 2002 included a provision which requires audit engagement partners to change clients after a maximum period of five years. This rotation of individual auditors instead of the entire firm has several advantages: First of all, more junior member of the engagement team are allowed to stay with the firm. Secondly, even if the engagement partner is required to rotate, inner-company knowledge exchange among the auditors can help retain at least some of client-and industry-specific knowledge through documentation, interaction, planning and strategy. Daugherty et al. (2013) points out that a significant amount of the research has stressed the beneficial aspects of tenure and expertise in comparison with adverse effects of auditor (in)dependence. They suggest increased focus and investment on training of auditors – a remedy which has been proposed with respect to debiasing techniques as well. So, in the words of senior vice president of Pfizer, Loretta Cangialosi as cited by Kroll (2012), instead of “using an elephant gun to kill an ant”, it might be more beneficial for everyone involved to focus on the most important assets in any organization – the people. 38 Katrin Wittke International Business Management 5. Survey – Behavioral and Environmental Influences on Auditing Processes 5.1 Research Objectives This study has been designed in a way as to determine whether those aspects of behavior which have been found in the theoretical framework of behavioral research actually are observable in auditing processes. In the core of the analysis lies the question of the effect of the working environment and conditions on the auditing team’s behavior and the quality of the audit report. Particular areas of interest were auditor behavior under different sources of pressure, the relationship between firm and team size and working atmosphere as well as perceived pressures, and the differences between planned and actual auditing processes. An additional factor which will be interesting to observe is whether there are observable differences between Big Four10 audit firms and smaller organizations, especially when it comes to participation in audit planning and perceived pressure from superiors or the client. Since actual auditing processes are notoriously difficult to reproduce in practice and would have gone beyond the scope of this thesis, a survey was conducted to gather answers from those who worked in the field. In the next section, the design of this survey will be outlined. 5.2 Research Design and Method The chosen method of data collection was the conduction of an online survey, since it was hoped that through social networking sites a vast population could be reached. Because of the specific nature of the research topic, the survey was directly targeted at students of business management, finance and accounting who gained firsthand experience in audit firms as interns or student workers. It was also directed at former students who are or were working in an audit firm or department. The reason for targeting former interns and student workers in particular was that it was expected for them to be more inclined to answer honestly about business practices which might not shed a flattering light on their (former) team or the organization as a whole, such as changing sample sizes or pressures from the team or superiors. In contrast, auditors who were in management positions are expected to 10 Big Four is the name commonly used to group the four biggest professional service firms together. These include Deloitte & Touche Tohmatsu, Price Waterhouse Coopers, Ernst & Young and KPMG. 39 Katrin Wittke International Business Management be more prone to either justify frequent changing of audit plans and sample sizes or underestimate such practices. Naturally, the possibility existed that interns and student workers would not have enough work experience in year-end audit to fully recognize the magnitude of these decisions. However, it is commonly known that interns and student workers are “thrown into the deep end” almost immediately and gather work experience very quickly especially in Big Four audit firms. As can be seen in Figure 2 (p. 44), the majority of respondents who worked for less than six months were employed by one of the Big Four audit firms, which suggests that they felt confident enough about their work experience to participate in this study. The importance of work experience in year-end auditing was also made clear in the description of the survey, which was posted in five student networking groups on Facebook as well as in the official HWR Berlin School of Economics and Law group on Xing. Additionally, the survey was directly forwarded to students and alumni who have worked or are still working in audit firms. They were also asked to forward the survey to (former) colleagues, yet the limited number of responses suggests that this was rarely the case. Layout: In order to reach the largest population possible, the questionnaire was available in either English or German. The English version can be found in its entirety in the Appendix B. The survey consisted of fourteen questions and was organized in four parts: Part 1: Part 2: Part 3: Part 4: Part 5: Introduction General Information Work Experience Auditing Process General Satisfaction The questions were mainly posed in the form of multiple choice questions which sometimes included the ability to answer openly in case the possible answers were not deemed sufficient by the participants. The reason for primarily choosing closed questions over open questions lies in the fact that closed questions are designed not necessarily to promote certain answers, but rather stimulate and encourage the participants to regard the subjects from a different angle. Those questions which formed the third and fourth part of the questionnaire are of particular interest for this thesis. Among them were specific questions about the auditing process that concentrated on methods and parameters (such as estimated risk or sample 40 Katrin Wittke International Business Management sizes) which the management team defines during the planning process. The aim was to ascertain whether these parameters were changed on a regular basis as the auditing process progressed, particularly in regard to approaching deadlines or tight budgets. Respondents were asked to evaluate particular aspects of the working environment such as team size and working atmosphere, as well as to estimate whether they observed divergences between planned and actual processes. If so, participants could estimate to what degree the audit quality was affected, and whether or not they deemed the involvement of the auditing team in planning processes sufficient and satisfactory. The survey was posted at the end of June and “pushed”, i.e. commented upon by the author or fellow students in order to keep it at the top of the groups on social networking websites. In spite of that, the number of usable responses was 35, which was much lower than was hoped for and expected. The survey link was opened 150 times in eight weeks, which amounts to a response rate of 23.3 %. It can be argued that the low response rate was partly due to the timing of the survey posting: In June and July, many students were either busy studying for exams or writing their own theses, and auditors were most likely on vacation, which was gathered from the automatic e-mails which were sent as a response. Even if some of the auditors who were still working in the field came back from vacation in time to fill out the survey, it is unlikely that this represented a top priority for them. Nevertheless, there were 23 answers in total from people who were still working in the industry and took time out of their schedule to fill out the survey. Consequently, they made up more than two thirds of the total population, as can be seen in Table 1 below. Table 1: Active employment in the industry Question 4: Are you still working in the auditing sector? Yes 23 65,7 Valid Percent 69,7 No 10 28,6 30,3 Total System 33 2 94,3 5,7 100,0 35 100,0 Frequency Percent Valid Missing Total Cumulative Percent 69,7 100,0 41 Katrin Wittke International Business Management 5.3 Findings & Analysis Age and Work experience As was expected, a majority of the respondents, namely 26 out of 35 (or 76,5 %), have worked in one of the Big Four audit firms. The total population of respondents is broken down by age (see Appendix C for detailed table) which shows the surprising amount of responses from more tenured auditors: While half of the respondents were age 25 or younger, 8.6 % of respondents were more than forty years old, leading to a median age of respondents of 28.26 years. Similarly, 11.4 % of respondents had a staggering work experience of more than ten years (see Figure 1 below). Still, one third of respondents have only worked in the auditing industry for less than six months and another third between one and five years. As can be seen, the distribution of people who were working as either interns or working students and those who held positions as full-time employees (professionals) or higher is fairly even, with 54.3 % and 45.7 % respectively (see Appendix D for details). All in all, the responses make up a relatively even sample, which can give some interesting insights into the differences between more tenured auditors and the perceptions of interns and young professionals. Figure 1: Work Experience by Firm Size *Percentages are based on total number of respondents. 42 Katrin Wittke International Business Management In order to estimate the actual work experience gathered by the respondents during their employment, they were asked to state the firm size of the companies whose year-end audits they participated in. While a total of 8 respondents answered that they participated only in one year audit for a small, medium or large firm – who were evenly represented – the majority of respondents, namely 71,4 %, participated in more than one year end-audit. Most of the participants who belonged to the latter category have worked for one of the Big Four companies (For more details, please see Appendix E). This supports our aforementioned hypothesis that even interns and working students, who made up half of the respondents, quickly participate in practical work. Teamwork and Atmosphere The participants were also asked to describe some key aspects of their working environment. First of all, they stated the team size in terms of team members in audit teams. As can be seen below in Table 2, the majority of respondents worked in smaller teams of two to three or four to five people. Larger audit teams of six to nine people were much rarer, with only 17 % of respondents stating that they worked in teams of that size, and merely 8,6 % declaring of having worked in teams of ten people or more. However, only the respondents from Big Four audit firms answered having worked in such large teams, which was to be expected11. Table 2: Distribution of team size Valid Missing Total 2-3 people 4-5 people 6-9 people 10 people or more Total System Frequency 10 10 6 3 29 6 35 Percent 28,6 28,6 17,1 8,6 82,9 17,1 100,0 Valid Percent 34,5 34,5 20,7 10,3 100,0 When asked about the atmosphere in their teams, participants were given a multitude of adjectives to choose from. They had the possibility to choose more than one answer, and could also answer openly. The results are displayed in Appendix G and will be summarized henceforth. While the possible answers were randomized in the survey itself, they have been arranged so that the positive attributes, such as “friendly” and “informal” are clearly visible in contrast to more negative aspects, such as time pressure, fearfulness 11 For a detailed crosstabulation between firm size and team size, see Appendix F. 43 Katrin Wittke International Business Management towards the client or the boss. The aspect of competition was grouped with the negative attributes, but it can be argued that a competitive atmosphere can have positive effects on performance. With a response rate of 11,2 % and 20,2 % of all responses, the attributes “stressful” and “under time-pressure” respectively were the most commonly cited negative aspects of the atmosphere. Especially the aspect of time-pressure is pervasive here, since, with a total response quantity of 18, it has been cited by half of the participants. Only one other attribute has been selected more often, namely the aspect of “friendliness”, which can be viewed as a nice balance against the negative aspect. Having been chosen by 19 of the 35 respondents, it is with 21,3 % of all responses the most frequently selected answer for this question. A general conclusion from this can be drawn: Even though the teams are under a lot of time pressure, the atmosphere is still friendly – and not marked by competition, as the low response rate of 2,2 % underlines. The aspect of time-pressure seems to impact people differently, as can be gathered from the contrasting answers “relaxed” and “stressful”, who have been chosen at almost the same frequency, namely nine and ten times, respectively. Figure 2: Firm size and positive characteristics of atmosphere Percentages are based on the total number of responses. 44 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Figure 3: Firm size and negative characteristics of atmosphere Percentages are based on the total number of responses. Involvement in Planning Processes Before investigating possible changes in preciseness of auditing tasks in the face of approaching deadlines and the frequency of sample size adaptation, the aspect of involvement in planning processes in Big Four firms and smaller firms is examined. Tables 3 and 4 show two cross tabulations of the firm sizes with the perceived degree of involvement of the respondents in the planning process and the desire for change, respectively. What is remarkable is the nearly identical distribution of perceived involvement between the respondents of the two firm sizes. There is only a slight difference on the extreme ends of the spectrum: The percentage of respondents who worked in a Big Four audit firm and perceived to be involved to a low degree in the planning process is slightly higher than that of the participants who worked in the smaller firms. The opposite is true for respondents who felt very involved in the planning processes. 45 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Table 3: Cross tabulation between firm size and perceived degree of involvement in planning process Perceived Degree of Involvement Total Low Medium High involvement involvement involvement Was the firm Yes one of the Big Four companies? No Total Count % of Firm Size Count % of Firm Size Count % of Firm Size 4 11 7 22 18,2% 50,0% 31,8% 100,0% 1 3 2 6 16,7% 50,0% 33,3% 100,0% 5 14 9 28 17,9% 50,0% 32,1% 100,0% When it comes to the desire to change the degree of involvement, those who worked in larger firms (and are hence slightly more likely to have uninvolved in the planning process) expressed the desire to be more involved in planning. While it is to be expected that those who feel uninvolved would like to change the degree of involvement in the future, the scale of the differences between large and small firms does not correspond to the marginal discrepancies in perceived degree of involvement, but is disproportionately higher. The differences which are referred to are highlighted in Table 4 on the next page. More than half of the respondents who worked in large audit firm would prefer to be more involved in the planning of audits and projects. In comparison, only 16 % of the smaller firm audit participants desired any change at all, be it towards higher degree of involvement or less participation in planning processes. A two-third majority was satisfied with their degree of involvement. 46 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Table 4: Cross tabulation between firm size and perceived degree of involvement in planning process Desire for change More Less No change involved involved necessary Was the firm one of the Big Four companies? Total Yes Count No % of Firm Size Count % of Firm Size Count % of Firm Size 11 1 9 Total 21 52,4% 4,8% 42,9% 100,0% 1 1 4 6 16,7% 16,7% 66,7% 100,0% 12 2 13 27 44,4% 7,4% 48,1% 100,0% These differences in desire to change the status quo can stem from the organizational structure of audit firms and the amount of audit engagements that are held by the individual partners. It is, for example, much more probable that the person responsible for the planning of the audit, most likely the managers, is available for direct contact with the interns, student workers and young professionals. In Big Four companies however, where interns and students sometimes have to be borrowed from other departments during the busy season, contact with and knowledge of the responsible individuals can be more indirect. The perceived degree of involvement in planning of the individual’s own participation in the audit, for example at what date and time they will be working for which client, might be similar in both kinds of organizations, because a functioning scheduling process is vital. A possible reason for the wish for more participation in planning can lie in the desire to gain more experience in planning processes or in differences in the ways how possible scheduling conflicts are resolved. Again, the aspect of availability of the person in charge might play a role here: If the manager is located on the same floor or even in the same office as the individual, the planning process can run much more organically. Additionally, audit partners in larger firms are more likely to oversee several audit engagements at once and are hence less flexible when it comes to reacting to scheduling conflicts. 47 Katrin Wittke International Business Management This section of the questionnaire was dedicated to aspects of audit quality and the preciseness with which it is executed. First of all, a general look at the results for Question No. 11 which deals with preciseness of tasks towards the deadline: Table 5 shows the frequencies of answers for each category. Table 5: Frequencies for question 11: preciseness of audit tasks Frequency Valid Cumulative Percent 6 17,1 23,1 23,1 Small decrease preciseness 6 17,1 23,1 46,2 2 5,7 7,7 53,8 12 34,3 46,2 100,0 26 74,3 100,0 No change preciseness Total Total Valid Percent Significant decrease in preciseness in Team-decision to lessen preciseness Missing Percent System in 9 25,7 35 100,0 With nine answers missing, the remaining 26 responses are fairly evenly distributed: Slightly less than half of all respondents answered that, in their opinion, the preciseness of tasks did not decrease towards the end of the deadline. 23 % of respondents each stated that they estimate a significant and a small decrease in preciseness. While the answer category “Small decrease in audit preciseness” can be interpreted a positive influence on audit quality, it still means that sample sizes are adapted towards the end of the audit. This would suggest that the auditors were under a lot of pressure – which can be seen in Table 6: Cross tabulation on the next page. Here, the frequency of responses for time pressure (17 counts among all participants) catches one’s eye. Several interesting aspects can be gained from this table. Firstly, the only other aspect of atmosphere next to time-pressure which was also selected by respondents (one respondent to be exact) who estimated decrease in preciseness to be significant is pressure from the client. Fearfulness towards the boss one the other hand which, in reference to Appendix F made up only 1,1 % of total answers, seemingly does not have any correlation with a decrease in preciseness. On the contrary: The supervisor is more likely to be looking over the auditor’s shoulder to examine the results and hence the auditing team is more motivated to work with full scrutiny. If the pressure comes from the client, the likelihood that he might strike fear into the hearts of auditors when he sees that they are slacking is miniscule – Since he most likely cannot estimate whether the auditing team is working less precisely or not. 48 Katrin Wittke International Business Management From the significant percentage of responses that selected both having felt time-pressure and estimating the effects of budget deadline on audit preciseness to be significant or, at least, existing, a very simple conclusion can be drawn: Pressure from the client or the boss can be alleviated through communication. Pressure from time that is running out cannot be mitigated. Therefore, when push comes to shove, sometimes the only solution is to work hastily and less precise or discuss with your team members to focus on the most important tasks. Table 6: Cross tabulation deadline quality and negative atmosphere Deadline and Significant Exactness decrease preciseness Count in % of Total Count Small decrease in % of preciseness Total Total Team-decision to Count lessen preciseness % of Total No change in Count preciseness % of Total Count % of Total Fearful towards the boss 0 Negative Atmosphere Pressure Under timefrom the Stressful pressure client 1 0 5 Total 6 0,0% 3,3% 0,0% 16,7% 20,0% 0 1 2 4 7 0,0% 3,3% 6,7% 13,3% 23,3% 0 0,0% 0 0,0% 0 0,0% 1 3,3% 1 3,3% 1 3,3% 1 3,3% 7 23,3% 7 23,3% 16 53,3% 1 3 9 17 30 3,3% 10,0% 30,0% 56,7% 100,0% Percentages and totals are based on responses. Before moving on to the investigation of sample sizes, a look at the connection between other, more positive attributes of atmosphere and changes in preciseness towards the deadline is merited (please see Appendix H). The two other attributes which also showed a connection with significant decreases in audit task preciseness are those that are normally considered positive, nice working atmospheres. Both relaxed and friendly atmospheres seem to encourage a working environment in which it is acceptable to relax scrutiny for audit tasks. For Question No.12 participants were asked to estimate how often they think it happens that sample sizes, which have been predetermined in the audit planning stage, are adapted in order to save time or simply make a task less strenuous. Again, first of all the differences between the smaller and larger audit firms are examined. While on first glance it seems that the smaller audit firms are less likely to change sample sizes during the audit 49 Katrin Wittke International Business Management process, a more detailed look might beg to differ. The percentages in Figure 4 are based on total number of responses in each firm size category. This means that a quarter of all respondents who worked in one of the Big Four audit firms do not think that sample sizes are changed retroactively to save time. When it comes to the distribution of answers from participants who think that sample sizes are adopted “often” or “all the time”, it seems that the Big Four auditors are “worse” – 10 % of respondents from the larger firms stated that retroactive adaptation happen “all the time”. Nevertheless, it lies in the eye of the beholder to determine which is “worse – The cumulative percentage of both answers taken together, i.e. 25 % of Big Four auditors estimate adaptations happening “all the time” or “often” – or the 33 % of smaller firm auditors who answered “often”? In order to answer this, admittedly very detail-oriented question, one would have to be able to determine what frequencies the respondents associate with both categorizations. 60% 50% 50% Percentage * 40% 40% 33% 30% 25% Big Four Audit Firm 20% 17% 15% 10% Smaller Audit Firm 10% 10% 0% 0% 0% All of the time Often On a regular basis Only in extreme situations Never Frequency of Sample Size Adaption Figure 4: Frequency of sample size adaptation in large and small audit firms *Percentages are based on the total number of responses in each firm size category Next, the influence on quality will be examined, which is arguably a lot more significant than sheer frequency of changes in audit procedures. Tables 7 and 8 are cross tabulations between audit quality and audit preciseness and frequency of sample adaptations respectively. What is remarkable that there were only four respondents who stated that in their opinion the audit was less precise due to measures taken and should have been carried out as planned: In Table 7, 33 % of participants who stated that towards the end of the 50 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Table 7: Cross tabulation preciseness and quality Impact on audit quality Audit was less precise Significant Count decrease in % of changes preciseness in preciseness Small Count decrease in % of changes preciseness in preciseness Changes in preciseness Teamdecision to lessen preciseness No change in preciseness Total 3 1 6 33,3% 50,0% 16,7% 100,0% 0 5 1 6 0,0% 83,3% 16,7% 100,0% 0 2 0 2 0,0% 100,0% 0,0% 100,0% 0 8 4 12 0,0% 66,7% 33,3% 100,0% 2 18 6 26 7,7% 69,2% 23,1% 100,0% Count % of changes in preciseness Count % of changes in preciseness N/A 2 Count % of changes in preciseness No negative impact on audit quality Total Table 8: Cross tabulation: Frequency of sample adaptations and audit quality Impact on audit quality Audit was less precise All of the Count time % of Frequency Often Count % of Frequency Frequency On a regular Count of Sample basis % of Size Frequency Adaptations Only in Count extreme % of situations Frequency Never Count Total % of Frequency Count % of Frequency No negative impact on audit quality Total N/A 1 1 0 2 50,0% 50,0% 0,0% 100,0% 0 4 1 5 0,0% 80,0% 20,0% 100,0% 1 1 0 2 50,0% 50,0% 0,0% 100,0% 0 10 1 11 0,0% 90,9% 9,1% 100,0% 0 3 3 6 0,0% 50,0% 50,0% 100,0% 2 19 5 26 7,7% 73,1% 19,2% 100,0% audit, preciseness of audit tasks decreased significantly, also stated that the audit was less precise. 51 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Similarly, the audit was deemed of lower quality by those people who estimate that sample sizes are adapted “all of the time” or at least “on a regular basis” in order to save time. This leaves the reader with the consoling solution that in order to have a negative impact on the overall quality of the audit, there has to be a marked decrease in preciseness, and the majority of respondents did not think that there were any negative effects on audit quality due to changes in sample size frequency or decrease in preciseness. 52 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Conclusion The purpose of this thesis was to give an overview of the developments in behavioral research from three angles: from an economic standpoint, from the accounting point of view, and from the perspective of the auditor. All of these aspects have something in common: They are human constructs, built by individuals who were trying to represent life, society, cooperation with other individuals. Admittedly, this was not a thesis on sociology, but the idea of finding humanity in all these different systems that spread across the globe is comforting – even though the homo sapiens only recently began inhabiting them. Through the development of a habitable environment built on assumptions which reflect reality and his characteristics, the possibilities for future research and the areas for practical application are plentiful Research in each of the three areas has had its peaks and its pitfalls, it had geniuses and obstacles which barred the way to further accomplishments. Regarding the current state of studies and the possibilities for the future, it is safe to say that research has not yet peaked fully. There is still a lot of possibilities where humans can come together – scientists and psychologists, economists and sociologists, since while the building of bridges between these gaps has commenced, we are just not there yet. Every one of these fields of research has unknowns which will be discovered and understood at some point. And if interdisciplinary research is encouraged, humanity might just get there faster. Because what we need, is not homo economicus. It is homo sapiens. 53 Katrin Wittke International Business Management List of References Altman, M 2006, Handbook of contemporary behavioral economics. Foundations and developments, M.E. Sharpe; Eurospan [distributor], Armonk, N.Y, London. Argyris, C 1952, The Impact of Budgets on People, Controllership Foundation, New York. Available from: http://hdl.handle.net/2027/mdp.35128000213536 [15 August 2013]. Argyris, C 1953, 'Human Problems with Budgets', Harvard Business Review, vol. 31, no. 1, pp. 97–110. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=6775504&site=ehost-live [18 August 2013]. Armet, N 2013, 'Buyer's remorse - but no sour grapes', Money Management, vol. 27, no. 21, pp. 22–23. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=88935832&site=ehost-live [03 August 2013]. Bamber, EM 1993, 'Opportunities in Behavioral Accounting Research', Behavioral Research in Accounting, vol. 5, p. 1. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=3957603&site=ehost-live [03 July 2013]. Barberis, NC 2013, 'Thirty Years of Prospect Theory in Economics: A Review and Assessment', Journal of Economic Perspectives, vol. 27, no. 1, pp. 173–196. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=85292941&site=ehost-live [03 July 2013]. Bazerman, MH & Moore, DA 2013, Judgment in Managerial Decision Making, John Wiley & Sons, Chichester. Bedard, JC & Wright, AM 1994, 'The functionality of decision heuristics: Reliance on prior audit adjustments in evidential.', Behavioral Research in Accounting, vol. 6, p. 62. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=9510180831&site=ehost-live [19 August 2013]. Birnberg, JG 1986, 'Discussion of Covaleski and Aiken', Accounting, Organizations & Society, vol. 11, 4/5, pp. 321–325. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=5417626&site=ehost-live [08 April 2013]. Birnberg, JG & Nath, R 1967, 'Implications of Behavioral Science for Managerial Accounting', Accounting Review, vol. 42, no. 3, pp. 468–479. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=4481967&site=ehost-live [02 July 2013]. Bonner, SE 1999, 'Judgment and Decision-Making Research in Accounting', Accounting Horizons, vol. 13, no. 4, pp. 385–398. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=2811298&site=ehost-live [10 March 2013]. Caplan, EH 1966, 'Behavioral Assumptions of Management Accounting', Accounting Review, vol. 41, no. 3, pp. 496–509. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=4515926&site=ehost-live [16 August 2013]. 54 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Caplan, EH 1971, Management accounting and behavioral science, Addison-Wesley Pub. Co, Reading, Mass. Carcello, JV, Hermanson, RH & McGrath, NT 1992, 'Audit Quality Attributes: The Perceptions of Audit Partners, Preparers, and Financial Statement Users', Auditing, vol. 11, no. 1, pp. 1–15. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=9703122164&site=ehost-live [25 March 2013]. Cohen, J & Kida, T 1989, 'The Impact of Analytical Review Results, Internal Control Reliability, and Experience on Auditors' Use of Analytical Review', Journal of Accounting Research, vol. 27, no. 2, pp. 263–276. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=6414667&site=ehost-live [05 August 2013]. Colville, I 1981, 'Reconstructing "Behavioural Accounting"', Accounting, Organizations & Society, vol. 6, no. 2, pp. 119–132. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=5417607&site=ehost-live [20 March 2013]. Covaleski, M & Aiken, M 1986, 'Accounting and Theories of Organizations: Some preliminary considerations', Accounting, Organizations and Society, vol. 11, 4–5, pp. 297–319. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0361368286900024 [28 May2013]. Daugherty, BE, Dickins, D, Hatfield, RC & Higgs, JL 2012, 'An Examination of Partner Perceptions of Partner Rotation: Direct and Indirect Consequences to Audit Quality', Auditing, vol. 31, no. 1, pp. 97–114. Available from: 10.2308/ajpt-10193 [18 August 2013]. Daugherty, B, Dickins, D, Higgs, J & Tatum, K 2013, 'The Question of Mandatory Audit Firm Rotation', CPA Journal, vol. 83, no. 1, pp. 28–33. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=87368391&site=ehost-live [18 August 2013]. Emby, C & Finley, D 1997, 'Debiasing Framing Effects in Auditors' Internal Control Judgments and Testing Decisions', Contemporary Accounting Research, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. 55–77. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=9710215724&site=ehostlive [19 August 2013]. Frerichs, S 2011, 'False Promises? A Sociological Critique of the Behavioural Turn in Law and Economics', Journal of Consumer Policy, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 289–314. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=65020952&site=ehost-live [21 April 2013].. Gillenkirch, RM & Arnold, MC 2008, State of the Art des Behavioral Accounting. Available from: https://www.google.de/url?sa=t&rct=j&q=&esrc=s&source=web&cd=1&cad=rja&ved=0CDMQFj AA&url=http%3A%2F%2Fwww.unigoettingen.de%2Fde%2Fdocument%2Fdownload%2F412bdf7bc8d608b6327f1504ff87d8b7.pdf% 2Frgma_behacc.pdf&ei=RbGkUZyVJqeu4ATC8oGoBQ&usg=AFQjCNEXorJniBVPCKfcDai8j1 BhEFU7eg&sig2=m1g_FV76ps_WLP9TrBFyGg&bvm=bv.47008514,d.bGE [28 May 2013]. Guckelsberger, U 2005, Das Menschenbild in der Ökonomie. Ein dogmengeschichtlicher Abriß, Ludwigshafen. Available from: http://web.fhludwigshafen.de/fb2/guckelsberger.nsf/Files/1D61686B4BEEC666C1256F9E00412045 [08 July 2013]. 55 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Hatfield, RC 2001, 'The Effect of Staff Accountant Objectivity in the Review and Decision Process: A Tax Setting', Journal of the American Taxation Association, vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 61–74. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=6479753&site=ehost-live [11 August 2013]. Heuser, UJ 2008, Humanomics. Die Entdeckung des Menschen in der Wirtschaft, Campus-Verl., Frankfurt, M, New York, NY. Hofstedt, TR 1976, 'Behavioral Accounting Research: Pathologies, Paradigms and Prescriptions', Accounting, Organizations & Society, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 43–58. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=10237876&site=ehost-live [08 April 2013]. Hofstedt, TR & Kinard, JC 1970, 'A Strategy for Behavioral Accounting Research', Accounting Review, vol. 45, no. 1, p. 38. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=4483415&site=ehost-live [11 August 2013]. International Federation of Accountants 2009, International Standard on Auditing 200, Geneva, Switzerland. Jasay, A de 2011, 'Suckers, punters, pathbreakers: when homo oeconomicus is selflessly selfish', CATO Journal, vol. 31, no. 2, pp. 377–387. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=63192492&site=ehost-live [21 April 2013]. Jones, DM 1976, 'Behavioural Aspects of Management Accounting', Management Decision, vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 17–24. Joyce, EJ & Biddle, GC 1981, 'Anchoring and Adjustment in Probabilistic Inference in Auditing', Journal of Accounting Research, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 120–145. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=6406102&site=ehost-live [31 July 2013]. Kahneman, D & Tversky, A 1979, 'Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision Under Risk', Econometrica, vol. 47, no. 2, pp. 263–291. Karafyllis, NC 2002, Zugänge zur Rationalität der Zukunft, Metzler, Stuttgart. Knapp, MC & Knapp, CA 2012, 'Cognitive Biases in Audit Engagements', CPA Journal, vol. 82, no. 6, pp. 40–45. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=87403260&site=ehost-live [25 July 2013]. Knechel, WR 2000, 'Behavioral Research in Auditing and Its Impact on Audit Education', Issues in Accounting Education, vol. 15, no. 4, pp. 695–712. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=3854226&site=ehost-live [25 March2013]. Knechel, WR & Vanstraelen, A 2007, 'The Relationship between Auditor Tenure and Audit Quality Implied by Going Concern Opinions', Auditing, vol. 26, no. 1, pp. 113–131. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=24690198&site=ehost-live [19 August 2013]. 56 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Kroll, K 2012, 'Mandatory Auditor Rotation: Where Things Stand', Financial Executive, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 42–45. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=74476418&site=ehost-live [18 August 2013]. Libby, R 1985, 'Availability and the Generation of Hypotheses in Analytical Review', Journal of Accounting Research, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 648–667. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=6437936&site=ehost-live [01 August 2013]. Libby, R & Frederick, DM 1990, 'Experience and the Ability to Explain Audit Findings', Journal of Accounting Research, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 348–367. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=6414812&site=ehost-live [19 August 2013]. March, JG & Simon, HA 1958, Organizations, Wiley, New York. McMillan, JJ & White, RA 1993, 'Auditors' Belief Revisions and Evidence Search: The Effect of Hypothesis Frame, Confirmation Bias, and Professional Skepticism', Accounting Review, vol. 68, no. 3, pp. 443–465. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=9312211767&site=ehost-live [30 July 2013]. Meyer, M & Rigsby, JT 2001, 'A Descriptive Analysis of the Content and Contributors of Behavioral Research In Accounting 1989-1998', Behavioral Research in Accounting, vol. 13, p. 253. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=4284270&site=ehost-live [28 May 2013]. Mill, JS 1848, reprinted in 2012, The Principles of Political Economy, CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform. Persky, J 1995, 'The Ethology of Homo Economicus', Journal of Economic Perspectives, vol. 9, no. 2, pp. 221–231. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=9506261428&site=ehost-live [04 July 2013]. Peters, JM 1993, 'Decision making, cognitive science and accounting: An overview of the intersection', Accounting, Organizations and Society, vol. 18, no. 5, pp. 383–405. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0361368293900388 [01 August 2013]. Riahi-Belkaoui, A 2002, Behavioral management accounting, Quorum Books, Westport, CT. Sen, AK 1977, 'Rational Fools: A Critique of the Behavioral Foundations of Economic', Philosophy & Public Affairs, vol. 6, no. 4, pp. 317–344. Shields, MD, Solomon I.R.A. & Waller, WS 1988, 'Auditor's usage of unaudited book values when making presampling audit value estimates', Contemporary Accounting Research, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 1–18. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=10938566&site=ehost-live [05 August 2013]. Shields, MD 2009, '"What a Long, Interesting Trip it's Been" through the Behavioral Accounting Literature: A Personal Perspective', Behavioral Research in Accounting, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 113– 116. Available from: 57 Katrin Wittke International Business Management http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=43248474&site=ehost-live [16 April 2013]. Simon, HA 1964, Models of man, Wiley & Sons, Inc. Simon, HA 1981, Entscheidungsverhalten in Organisationen. Eine Untersuchung von Entscheidungsprozessen in Management und Verwaltung, Verl. Moderne Industrie, Landsberg am Lech. Smith, A 1759, reprinted 1977, Theorie der ethischen Gefühle, Felix Meiner, Hamburg. Smith, A 1776, reprinted in 1991, The wealth of nations, David Campbell Publishers, London. Steele, GR 2004, 'Understanding Economic Man', American Journal of Economics & Sociology, vol. 63, no. 5, pp. 1021–1055. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=15286038&site=ehost-live [16 April 2013]. Thaler, R 1980, 'Toward a positive theory of consumer choice', Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 39–60. Thompson, JD 1967, Organizations in action; Social science bases of administrative theory, McGraw-Hill, New York. Trotman, KT 2011, 'A Different Personal Perspective through the Behavioral Accounting Literature', Behavioral Research in Accounting, vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 203–208. Available from: http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=bth&AN=57736272&site=ehost-live [12 May 2013]. Tversky, A & Kahneman, D 1974, 'Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases', Science (New York, N.Y.), vol. 185, no. 4157, pp. 1124–1131. Tversky, A & Kahneman, D 1973, 'Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability', Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), pp. 207–232. Available from: http://dtserv2.compsy.unijena.de/__C1257641005FF6B0.nsf/0/1538BC6A54743092C125764700444391/$FILE/TVERSKY %20Availability%20A%20heuristic%20for%20judging%20frequency%20and%20probality.pdf [31 July 2013]. Vasek, T 2012, 'Ohne Vernunft und Verstand' 29 November. Available from: http://www.theeuropean.de/thomas-vasek-2/5448-den-homo-oeconomicus-gibt-es-nicht [10 July 2013]. Wilkinson, N 2008, An introduction to behavioral economics, Palgrave Macmillan, New York. Wüstemann, J & Koch, C 2007, A review of bias research in auditing: combining psychological and economic research. Working Paper, Mannheim. Available from: http://aaahq.org/meetings/AUD2009/AReviewOfBias.pdf [04 August 2013]. 58 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Appendix A. Prospect Theory Formula and EUT Formula Example of a gamble. Source: Barberis, NC 2013, 'Thirty Years of Prospect Theory in Economics: A Review and Assessment' B. Survey Outline Part 1: Welcome Thank you for agreeing to take this survey. Your answers will be handled with complete anonymity and will strictly be used for analysis in my Bachelor Thesis. The survey consists of fourteen questions and should take between five to ten minutes. We will begin with general information and move on to your experience in audit firms and with auditing processes. Part 2: General Information 1. How old are you? [Open Question] 2. How long have you worked in an auditing firm / department? Less than 3 months 3 to 6 months 7 to 12 months If you have worked for more than one year, please fill in the overall duration of work experience: _____________ [Open Question] 59 Katrin Wittke 3. International Business Management Did the company belong to one of the big four auditing firms (KPMG, Ernst & Young, PWC, Deloitte & Touche)? Yes No I’d rather not say 4. Are you still working in the auditing sector? Yes No 5. What position did you hold in that firm? Intern Student worker Full-time employee Other (please specify): __________ [Open Question] Part 3: Work Experience 6. Did you participate in one or more year end audits? Yes, for a small firm Yes, for a medium firm Yes, for a large firm Yes, for more than one firm Other (please specify): ________ [Open Question] 7. How large were your audit teams in general? Please answer even if you answered the previous question with no / other. 2-3 people 4-5 people 6-9 people 10 people or more 8. How would you describe the working atmosphere in your audit team? More than one answer may be Relaxed Serious Informal Under time-pressure Stressful Friendly given. Competitive Fearful (towards “the boss”) Pressure from “the client” Other (please specify): __________ 60 Katrin Wittke International Business Management 9. How would you rate the degree of involvement of the auditing team in the planning process of the audit? Low Medium High NA 10. Regarding your previous answer, do you think the auditing team should be: More involved in the planning process Less involved in the planning process No change is necessary. Other (please specify): _________ [Open Question] Part 4: Auditing Process 11. Would you say that towards the deadline of the auditing process the preciseness for some tasks decreased? Yes, significantly Yes, but not in a large way Yes, it was a team-decision to focus on other tasks No, even towards the deadline we would work with full scrutiny and preciseness. Other (please specify) : ________ [Open Question] 12. Based on your personal experience in auditing, how often do you think it happens that previously set sample sizes are reduced in order to make a task easier or less time-intensive? All of the time Often On a regular basis Only in extreme situations Never Other (please specify): _________ [Open Question] 13. If the previous answer was “Yes”: In your opinion, were there consequences to the audit result and its quality due to the changes? I think the audit was less precise and should have been carried out as planned I do not think there was a negative impact on the overall quality of the audit NA Other (please specify): ___________ [Open Question] 61 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Part 5: General Satisfaction 14. Please comment on any factors you were not satisfied with. If you have any suggestions for improvement, we'd like to hear them. [Open Question] [End of Survey] 62 Katrin Wittke International Business Management C. Distribution of respondents by age Age Valid 23 24 25 27 28 29 30 35 37 39 42 43 Total Missing System Total Frequency 3 8 6 1 6 3 1 1 1 1 2 1 34 1 35 Percent 8,6 22,9 17,1 2,9 17,1 8,6 2,9 2,9 2,9 2,9 5,7 2,9 97,1 2,9 100,0 Valid Percent Cumulative Percent 8,8 23,5 17,6 2,9 17,6 8,8 2,9 2,9 2,9 2,9 5,9 2,9 100,0 8,8 32,4 50,0 52,9 70,6 79,4 82,4 85,3 88,2 91,2 97,1 100,0 Mean 28,26 Median 26,00 D. Positions held by respondents within the firm Job Position Frequency Valid Intern Working Student Full-time Employee Manager Partner Total Percent Valid Percent Cumulative Percent 11 31,4 31,4 31,4 8 22,9 22,9 54,3 14 40,0 40,0 94,3 1 2,9 2,9 97,1 1 2,9 2,9 100,0 35 100,0 100,0 63 Katrin Wittke International Business Management E. Distribution of size of audited firms Audited Firms 70,0% 17 Percentage of total 60,0% 50,0% 40,0% 30,0% Big Four Firm 20,0% Smaller Firm 10,0% 1 1 3 3 1 3 0 0,0% Small firm Medium firm Large firm More than one firm Audited Firms F. Cross tabulation: Team size and audit firm size Team Size Yes Count % within Firm Big Four Size Audit Firm? No Count % within Firm Size Total Count % within Total Answers 2-3 4-5 6-9 10 people people people people or more Total 9 5 6 3 23 39,1% 21,7% 26,1% 13,0% 100,0% 1 5 0 0 6 16,7% 83,3% 0,0% 0,0% 100,0% 10 10 6 3 29 34,5% 34,5% 20,7% 10,3% 100,0% 64 Katrin Wittke International Business Management G. Frequency of answers on atmosphere Pressure from the client 3,4 % Fearful towards the boss 1,1 % Atmosphere Competitive 2,2 % Stressful 11,2 % Under time-pressure 20,2 % Serious 19,1 % Informal 11,2 % Friendly 21.3 % Relaxed 10,1 % 0 2 4 6 8 10 Frequency of Answers* 12 14 16 18 20 *Percentages are based on total amount of answers. 65 Katrin Wittke International Business Management Preciseness and Atmosphere 66