This article was downloaded by: [72.152.245.219] On: 11 September 2011, At: 21:45 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Creativity Research Journal Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcrj20 Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking as Predictors of Personal and Public Achievement: A Fifty-Year FollowUp a a a Mark A. Runco , Garnet Millar , Selcuk Acar & Bonnie Cramond a a Torrance Creativity Center, University of Georgia, Athens Available online: 09 Nov 2010 To cite this article: Mark A. Runco, Garnet Millar, Selcuk Acar & Bonnie Cramond (2010): Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking as Predictors of Personal and Public Achievement: A Fifty-Year Follow-Up, Creativity Research Journal, 22:4, 361-368 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2010.523393 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material. CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL, 22(4), 361–368, 2010 Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1040-0419 print=1532-6934 online DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2010.523393 Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking as Predictors of Personal and Public Achievement: A Fifty-Year Follow-Up Mark A. Runco, Garnet Millar, Selcuk Acar, and Bonnie Cramond Downloaded by [72.152.245.219] at 21:45 11 September 2011 Torrance Creativity Center, University of Georgia, Athens This article presents the results of the 50-year follow-up of the longitudinal study E. Paul Torrance initiated 5 decades ago. The Torrance Test of Creative Thinking (TTCT) was administered in the late 1950s and personal and public achievement data were obtained 50 years later and used as criteria in analyses reported here. These showed that TTCT scores were moderately correlated with personal, but not with public, achievement. However, an interaction of intelligence and creativity was significantly related to public achievement but not to personal achievement. When a composite was formed from the 4 TTCT indexes (fluency, originality, flexibility, and elaboration), a significant quadratic trend was found with the personal achievement criterion. Three of the indicators (‘‘Love of work,’’ Tolerance of mistakes, and Minority of one) from the Beyonder instrument developed by Torrance (2003) were related to public achievement. Only one other indicator from the Beyonder measure (‘‘Well-roundedness’’) was associated with personal achievement. Men were significantly higher in public achievement than women, but there was no significant gender difference in personal achievement. Longitudinal studies may be the most useful kind of investigation for the study of creativity and its fulfillment. Obviously, they are time-consuming and potentially expensive. They are prone to sampling issues, including simple attrition and systematic biases (e.g., loss of individuals within particular segments of the sample). Yet they are ideal for tracking developmental changes and trajectories and for pinpointing the impact of experiences. They are also extremely useful in a psychometric sense, and especially for determination of predictive validity. No wonder several informative longitudinal studies have been conducted. Feist and Barron (2003), for example, reported a longitudinal investigation that began in 1950 with 80 graduate students (all male) who were at that time approximately 27 years of age and were followed up in 2003 (n ¼ 43). The predictor measures covered a fairly broad range, including Sandra Russ was the Action Editor for this article. Correspondence should be sent to Mark A. Runco, Torrance Creativity Center=Aderhold Hall, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602. E-mail: runco@uga.edu creativity, personality, and intelligence. The criterion measures, administered for the follow-up, covered the same domains as well as career data. Significantly, the personality predictors from 1950 accounted for nearly 20% of the variability in the creativity criteria at follow-up, and this is after the contributions of intelligence were statistically controlled. Albert (1969, 1978, 1980a, 1980b; Runco & Albert, 1989, 2005) conducted a longitudinal study of exceptionally gifted boys. He found quite a bit of evidence that (a) talent is dependent on family background and (b) different domains of talent differ in family background and personality. In particular, the families of mathematically talented children have several idiosyncrasies (e.g., father–son closeness). The expectations of the mothers was especially important for the divergent thinking and autonomy of the boys in the sample (Albert & Runco, 1989) and the trait flexibility stood out in the personality profiles (Runco & Albert, 2005). Helson (1996, 1999, 2000) described a longitudinal study of Mills College students and, later, alumnae. She captured the primary findings of her longitudinal research, as well as the highly significant idea of 362 RUNCO, MILLAR, ACAR, CRAMOND potential developing into actual productivity, as follows: Downloaded by [72.152.245.219] at 21:45 11 September 2011 A central finding is that varied measures of creative potential assessed openness and unconventionality, manifested in both positive and negative ways, whereas creative productivity was associated with these traits along with strong creative motivation and resilience. Creative potential was actualized in some women in self-discover through relationships rather than in careers. Creative traits showed impressive consistency over time, but the creative vitality of individuals fluctuated considerably with major changes in life situation. Creative productivity showed more relation to psychosocial development than did measures of creative potential. (Helson, 1999, p. 89) E. Paul Torrance initiated a longitudinal study while working in Minnesota in the 1950s. Torrance, himself, reported results from four follow-up assessments, and later, Cramond, Matthews-Morgan, Bandalos, and Zuo (2005) reported a 40-year follow-up. This article reports the 50-year follow-up. To put these findings in context, something should be said about development of the Torrence Test of Creative Thinking (TTCT) and the earlier assessments. Torrance’s objective was to develop a reliable and valid test of creative thinking abilities that could be administered to individuals from kindergarten through adulthood (Torrance, 2008). Torrance believed there were general mental abilities that are involved in, and predict, creative achievements. Therefore, the scales in TTCT are regarded as indicators of creative potential that increase the likelihood of creative behavior. Torrance recognized that high scores on the TTCT do not guarantee actual creatively accomplishment. Motivation and the like can make a difference. Still, he conducted extensive research and identified several candidate scales that seem to measure important aspects of creativity. He chose those with lowest intercorrelations (Torrance, 2003) so that each component of the test has a unique contribution to overall assessment. Significantly, Torrance argued that TTCT differs from the tests developed by Guilford (1967, 1970) and Wallach and Kogan (1965). The first of these was an attempt to measure factorially pure mental functioning. The latter was designed to allow ideational associations using an untimed and game-like testing environment. Activities on the TTCT aim to measure creative thinking tasks that are necessary for daily life activities and creative breakthroughs under traditional test conditions. In the 1950s and 1960s, the participants of Torrance’s longitudinal study were students in grades one through six. Slightly later, students in grades 7 through 12 received the TTCT. Seven years later, many of the students received the follow-up measures (Torrance, 1969), which allowed studies of the predictive validity of the TTCT. Torrance (1972a, 1972b) reported a second follow-up, which occurred approximately 5 years later. At this point, he used a checklist of creative achievement and accomplishment as one criterion. This checklist asked participants about several different domains, including life philosophy, invention, innovation at work, research, and the arts. Checklists like this one are often used and have been refined through the years (Holland, 1961; Runco, 1986; Wallach & Wing, 1969). The next follow-up was 22 years after the TTCT had been administered to the schoolchildren (Torrance, 1980, 1981a, 1981b). Very likely, the most significant result of the first few follow-up studies was that the TTCT was a better predictor of creative achievement and accomplishment than traditional intelligence. It was also a better predictor than achievement in high school or peer nomination. Torrance reported that the TTCT explained approximately 25% of the variance in the creative quality index of the aforementioned checklist and 21% of the variance of quantity of achievement and activity. Torrance found a hint of sex differences, at least in multivariate analyses, with a canonical predictive validity correlation of .59 for males and .46 for females. He realized that predictions of life success that depend upon external measures of achievement would necessarily discriminate against females because they had fewer opportunities than did the males for public achievements. So, for the 1980 data collection, he introduced the criterion measure called the Creative Style of Life Achievements. This allowed respondents to list achievements that were more personal in nature. Women were encouraged to explain any problems that they had had in realizing their creative potential (Millar, 2002). The Creative Style of Life questions asked about activities and experiences that are not always socially recognized. Participants were asked to respond to questions about everyday creative behavior, including things such as having organized an action-group (food cooperative, environmental pressure group, etc.), designed a house, started a new school or other educational program, had a striking religious experience, or become seriously involved in a new hobby (Torrance, 2002). The questionnaires that were sent to the participants had them answer questions to provide basic demographic data, as well as information on their public creative achievements, personal creative achievements, strengths, and weaknesses (Millar, 2002, appendix D). Each time, at least three raters, trained in creativity research and theory, looked for five things: the creativeness of a future career image, the quality of the most outstanding creative achievements, the number of creative style of life achievements, the number of creative achievements since completing high school, and the number of high Downloaded by [72.152.245.219] at 21:45 11 September 2011 TORRANCE TESTS OF CREATIVE THINKING AS PREDICTORS OF PERSONAL AND PUBLIC ACHIEVEMENT school creative achievements. With the 1998 follow-up, one index of adult creative achievement was derived from the questionnaire responses—the number of publicly acknowledged creative achievements (Millar, 2002, p. 38). Torrance (1981b) found very high predictive validity (multivariate) correlations (.63) when this new criterion measure was analyzed using TTCT scores from previous data collection dates—even when the TTCT had been administered 25 years previously. Cramond et al. (2005) reported a 40-year follow-up to the Torrance longitudinal study. Data for this study were collected starting in 1998. Of the 170 respondents who could be located, complete data were obtained for 99 of them with a fairly equal representation of females and males (45 and 54, respectively). Cramond et al. examined various predictors including the global creativity index from the initial TTCT data collection, IQ, elaboration, originality, flexibility, fluency, and some data about the experience with mentors. They used the quality and quantity of creative achievements as their criteria. Expert judgments were again used to quantify the criteria with a very high level of agreement (interrelater correlations >.78). Cramond et al. introduced a new comparison; they decided to compare the bottom third of the respondents in terms of number of public achievements with the top third on the same measure. There were no differences in the proportion of males and females in the two. A two-way ANOVA of quality, by gender by group (high vs. low creative) was statistically significant; F[1,49] ¼ 9.53, p < .01. Not surprisingly, the quality ratings for both males and females were significantly higher for the high quantity group. Plucker (1999) reported a reanalysis of Torrance’s (1980) data and described how IQ and verbal TTCT scores were both significant predictors of creative achievements. The inclusion of IQ was a surprise given earlier reports of a threshold of intelligence (Albert & Runco, 1987; Guilford, 1968; Kim, 2002) but its association may have been uncovered because the analytic approach—structural equation modeling (SEM)—had not been used previously. Most important was that Plucker (1999) found TTCT scores to be much stronger predictors of creative achievement than the childhood intelligence test scores (also see Wallach & Wing, 1969). Cramond et al. (2005) also used SEM and found that originality, fluency, and the IQ, each obtained in the initial phases of the longitudinal study, were reasonable predictors of the quantity of creative achievement 40 years later. IQ, flexibility, originality, and the global creativity index were the best predictors when quality of creative achievements was the criterion. Having a mentor was also related to the quality and quantity of creative achievements. Again, quality and quantity of achievement shared quite a bit of their variance (approximately 72%). 363 Several specific differences between men and women were found as well. Data from the women, for example, showed a strong correlation between creative achievement and IQ (rs ¼ .38 and .43 for quality and quantity, respectively). Additionally, the correlation between having a mentor and creative achievement was much more pronounced among the women. This makes sense, given the difficulties for women of the time in several career areas. The relationship between quality and quantitative of creative performance was, on the other hand, higher among the men (r ¼ .90) than the women (r ¼ .81). The general objective of this investigation was to assess the participants of the longitudinal investigation 50 years after the initial data collection. It is possible that developmental and experiential changes have occurred among Torrance’s sample in the past 10 years. Comparisons with the last follow-up, reported in Cramond et al.’s (2005) 40-year follow-up, were of special interest. Most concretely, this investigation provided a test of the predictive validity of the TTCT using criteria and data collected 50 years after the tests of creative thinking were first administered. METHOD Participants Participants of the 1998 follow-up study were contacted by mail in 2008 and invited to take part in the 50-year follow-up. Sixty participants (28 male and 32 females) responded to the invitation. This number represents more than half of the previous follow-up study in 1998 (Cramond et al. 2005; Torrance, 2002). The average age of the participants is 56 years. The average intelligence score derived in the 1958–1965 period was 126 (Range ¼ 105–176) and average TTCT score is 101 (Range ¼ 75–127). Forty percent of the participants still live in Minnesota, and the remainder lives in other states. Generally, the participants are well educated; 87% have attained some postsecondary education and 32% have earned doctoral degrees. Measures Four scores from the initial administration of the TTCT were used, namely, fluency, flexibility, originality, and elaboration. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) intelligence test scores were also available and included in several analyses. These had been collected between 1958–1964, as detailed in the introduction to this article and in Cramond et al. (2005). The creative style of life (Torrance, 2002) was one criterion. It asks about public (socially recognized) and personal achievements. As mentioned above personal achievements include action group work, designing a Downloaded by [72.152.245.219] at 21:45 11 September 2011 364 RUNCO, MILLAR, ACAR, CRAMOND house, founding an educational program, and various hobbies. Torrance (2002) reported good reliabilities for the creative style of life: interrater agreement among three experts ranged from .78 to .88 for the quantity and quality of public achievements, for example. Torrance (1980) also reported good interrater reliability for the ‘‘quality of highest creative achievements’’ (rs ¼ .86 and .83 for males and females) and ‘‘creativeness of future career image’’ (both rs ¼ .81 for men and women). Six variables from The Beyonder Checklist (Torrance, 1993, 2002) were also administered: sense of mission, love of work, delight in deep thinking, tolerance of mistakes, well-roundedness, and feeling comfortable as a minority of one. The Beyonder Checklist was created by Torrance (1993) from qualities that he had observed in 10 of the respondents from the 30-year study whose creative achievements were so much higher than the others in the group. He concluded that factors such as motivation, persistence, courage, and loving what one is doing become more important than ability (Torrance, 2003). Percentiles from the Beyonder Checklist were included in that study. The six Beyonder variables listed were dichotomous in our analyses (as described in the following). Torrance’s measure of creative achievement, modified in 1998, with quantity and quality scores and covering public and personal areas, was also administered. Procedure The TTCT scores were obtained from longitudinal records. The other measures were sent to each participant electronically. Unfortunately, one question related to quality of achievement was omitted from the electronic version of the creative achievement scale. It was sent out later, also electronically, but the response rate was low (n ¼ 44). For this reason, the analyses specifically of the quality scores used smaller samples than the other analyses. RESULTS Two statistical decisions should be explained. First, one-tailed tests were used for the TTCT results, just given, and for all regression and correlational results that follow. Second, it may not seem like much of an issue, given that the emphasis in the social sciences is on effect sizes, but it is still worth noting that probability levels (tests of significance) were calculated but not given much weight in this research. This was, after all, an investigation of predictive validity, not a test of hypotheses, and in this kind of psychometric research, probability levels are not of critical importance (Nunnally, 1978). Several sets of product moment correlations TABLE 1 Bivariate Correlations of Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking Scores and Personal and Public Achievement, and the WISC 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Fluency Flexibility .67 Originality .41 .41 Elaboration .39 .41 .57 Public ach. .15 .03 .07 .04 Personal ach. .29 .22 .20 .27 .28 WISC .16 .04 .06 .29 .07 .08 Beyounder .21 .16 .05 .01 .30 .35 .06 p < .05 (1-tailed). p < .01 (1-tailed). are reported immediately following (also see Table 1). The regression and canonical correlation results are then presented. Product moment and interitem (alpha) correlations showed moderate but statistically significant relationships between the TTCT scores and personal achievement (fluency r ¼ .29, p ¼ .014, flexibility r ¼ .22, p ¼ .05, elaboration r ¼ .27, p ¼ .02). Originality was slightly lower than all of these (r ¼ .20, p ¼ .06). None of the correlations with Public Achievement were statistically significant (rs < .15). The four TTCT test scores were moderately inter-related (.67 < rs < .39; a ¼ .77), as predicted by theories of divergent thinking (Guilford, 1968; Runco, 1999; Torrance, 1995). Given this moderate overlap and the need for parsimony, a composite was computed by converting each of the fluency, flexibility, elaboration, and originality scores into a z-score and then adding them together. This standardized composite was correlated with personal (r ¼ .31, p ¼ .009) but not public achievement (r ¼ .05, ns). The four TTCT scores were not related to the Beyonders total score but it was significantly correlated with both personal (r ¼ .35, p ¼ .04) and public achievement (R ¼ .30, p ¼ .012). A canonical analysis was computed using z scores from the fluency, flexibility, originality, and elaboration indexes of the TTCT as predictors and quantity of personal and public achievement are criteria. The resulting canonical coefficient was .34. A second canonical analysis used the same TTCT predictors but had both quantity and quality for public and personal achievements as criteria (Rc ¼ .40). When intelligence test scores were added as a predictor, the coefficient increased to .46. Although statistically nonsignificant with the probability levels used in hypothesis testing research (.05 and .01), each of these canonical coefficients offers reasonable support for the predictive validity of the TTCT. This is especially true given that the predictors and the criteria were administered 50 years apart. TORRANCE TESTS OF CREATIVE THINKING AS PREDICTORS OF PERSONAL AND PUBLIC ACHIEVEMENT Downloaded by [72.152.245.219] at 21:45 11 September 2011 Intelligence In addition to predictive validity, there was also support for the discriminant validity of the TTCT. Each of the four individual TTCT scores was negatively related to WISC scores, though only the association with elaboration reached statistical significance (r ¼ .29, p ¼ .026). The composite TTCT score, described above (a sum of four z scores), was negatively related to WISC scores, but not significantly (r ¼ .17). The next analysis tested the interaction of divergent thinking and intelligence as a predictor of achievement. This was a regression using the product term (TTCT IQ) as predictor, as described by Cohen and Cohen (1983). The variable representing the interaction (TTCT IQ) was entered into the regression in a second step, after the two main effects (TTCT and IQ). Results indicated that the interaction was related to Public (R2 change ¼ .15, p ¼ .012) but not Personal Achievement (R2 change ¼ .03, ns). Pearson correlations between the WISC and Personal and Public Achievement were both negative (rs ¼ .08 and .07) but not statistically significant. Personal and Public Achievement were only moderately related (r ¼ .28, p ¼ .019). GROUP DIFFERENCES Groups were formed from the Beyonder measure and two-tailed t-tests were used to compare them. Given the sample size, equal variances were not assumed. Results are summarized in Table 2. Note that individuals who indicated a ‘‘love of work’’ had higher public achievement; t(55) ¼ 2.19 (p ¼ .016, Ms ¼ 38.4 and 81.3, respectively; SDs ¼ 22.0 and 71.7); than those who did not. There was also a significant group difference in achievement in ‘‘tolerance of mistakes,’’ with those reporting a higher tolerance reporting higher public achievement; t(14) ¼ 2.45 (p ¼ .027, Ms ¼ 75.4 and 365 39.0, for high and low tolerance, respectively; SDs ¼ 68.4 and 27.6). Responses to the ‘‘minority of one’’ Beyonder scale indicated significantly higher public achievement scores as well; t(20) ¼ 2.55 (p ¼ .019, M ¼ 82.33 and 38, respectively; SDs ¼ 70.5 and 40.62). The only significant difference involved ‘‘well roundedness,’’ with those responding affirmatively having higher personal achievement scores than those reporting less well roundedness; t(55) ¼ 2.74 (p ¼ .008, Ms ¼ 6.08 and 3.67, respectively; SDs ¼ 2.47 and 2.77). SEX DIFFERENCES Because some estimates of creativity show differences between men and women (for a recent review, see Runco, Cramond, and Pagnani, in press) the next set of analyses compared the men and women in the sample. Results indicated that they differed significantly in their achievements. Men had higher scores than women for public achievement; t(55) ¼ 2.00, p ¼ .023, Ms ¼ 90 and 56, respectively, SDs ¼ 79 and 49). Women had higher scores than men on personal achievement, but this difference was not statistically significant (Ms ¼ 4.7 and 3.6, respectively; SDs ¼ 3.2 and 2.4). Given these differences between men and women, the primary analyses for this investigation were repeated after statistically controlling gender. These were regression analyses with the achievement criteria and testing sex in a first step, TTCT scores in a second step, and TTCT squared (for curvilinear trends) in a third step. The difference between men and women was again apparent (R2 ¼ .068, p ¼ .05) in the analysis using public achievement. More importantly, the relationship of the TTCT composite with personal achievement was still statistically significant (R2 ¼ .13, p ¼ .023) and, intriguingly, there was a quadratic trend (R2 ¼ .15, p ¼ .033). This suggests that high scores on the TTCT composite were associated with high personal TABLE 2 Group Differences in Personal and Public Achievement Public Achievement Beyonder Scale Sense of Mission Love of Work Deep Thinking Tolerance of Mistakes Well-roundedness Minority of One Personal Achievement M SD M SD t df p M SD M SD t df p 71.14 81.35 72.58 75.44 90.25 82.33 59.75 71.72 65.34 68.39 87.37 70.5 78.92 38.43 74.25 39.00 65.62 38.00 87.56 22.03 68.58 27.58 58.78 40.62 0.29 2.19 0.09 2.45 0.92 2.55 14 55 52 14 14 20 0.78 0.03 0.93 0.03 0.37 0.02 4.14 4.35 4.00 4.20 6.08 4.45 2.73 2.78 2.77 2.84 2.47 2.71 4.41 3.64 4.5 3.5 3.67 3.44 3.31 3.18 2.91 3.33 2.77 3.78 0.27 0.74 0.65 0.49 2.74 0.76 15 20 52 6 20 10 0.79 0.47 0.52 0.64 0.01 0.46 Note. Groups refer to those who possess or do not possess (presence vs. absence) the skill referred by the specific Beyonder scale. For example, those who were rated ‘‘1’’ versus those with a rating of ‘‘0’’ in the ‘‘Minority of One’’ scale is compared for personal and public achievement. The same for all other beyonder scales. Downloaded by [72.152.245.219] at 21:45 11 September 2011 366 RUNCO, MILLAR, ACAR, CRAMOND achievement, but only up to a point, and afterwards the relationship showed a decline. None of the predictors was significantly related to public achievement (all R2 changes <.07). Stepwise regression analyses (Cohen & Cohen, 1983) also indicated that there was an interaction (sex total TTCT) when Personal Achievement was the criterion. Sex, entered into the equation first, was not statistically significant. The TTCT was significant (R2 change ¼ .094, p ¼ .019). Most important from this analysis was that the sex TTCT predictor was significant (R2 change ¼ .080, p ¼ .024). Product moment coefficients help explain this: For men the relationship of the TTCT and personal achievement was .153, ns, and for women .36, p ¼ .038. The same analyses were computed using public achievement and then the Beyonder total, but no predictor there contributed a significant portion of variance (all R2 changes <.03). Threshold Theory Given the nature of the data collected for this investigation, and the oft-cited threshold theory—in which intelligence and creativity are related but only at lower levels (Kim, 2006; Runco & Albert, 1987)—one set of regressions was conducted with the composite TTCT score as criterion and WISC scores entered in a first step, and the square of the WISC scores entered in a second step (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). The same analysis was also done with each of the four individual TTCT indexes as criteria, and again with gender controlled by adding it in a first step. There was no indication of a threshold relationship (all R2 < .121, ns). Although this relationship (and all correlations) may be attenuated by restricted range of scores, the IQ range in this sample was certainly large enough. It was over 100 IQ points (71 to 176). DISCUSSION The most important finding from this investigation is that predictions from TTCT hold up even over 50 years. Indeed, the individual TTCT scores, and then a standardized composite calculated from them, were related to personal achievement. They were not related to public achievement, but this is not much of a surprise. It may be that participants are now at a stage of life where personal achievements are more important than public ones. Additionally, certain criteria are more or less fitting for certain samples (Runco, Plucker, & Lim, 2000), and it is possible that the personal achievement criterion was simply the most appropriate for this age group. Recall also that Torrance (1995) seemed to think that such personal creativity was at least as important as public achievement. More recently, Runco (1995, 1996) has developed a complete theory of personal creativity, the crux of which is that creative skills are often used such that there is no product, not social achievement, no interpersonal judgment. In this view, creativity can lead to things which are original and useful but only for the individual creator himself or herself. The results of earlier follow-up studies (Torrance, 1969) showed that TTCT scores were superior to those obtained from tests of intelligence in predicting creative achievements. This fits well with the report of Wai, Lubinski, and Benbow (2005). Theirs was a follow-up of individuals ranked in the top 1% of SAT scores. The criteria of creativity used at follow-up were the number of patents and secured tenure positions in top universities, which parallel the use of public achievement criteria in the TTCT investigations. Wai et al. compared the top quartile to the bottom quartile and found that 7.5% of the top quartile had been granted patents, versus 3.8% of the bottom quartile. In the top group, 3.2% had earned tenure, versus 0.38% of the bottom group. It would appear that, in some domains at least, there is a role of convergent thinking, or at least of the skills tapped by the SAT. Notably, Wai et al. did not compare the SAT to the TTCT. Recall here the findings of Plucker (1999) and the comparisons showing that the TTCT was not only a good predictor, but a better one that general intelligence. The relationship between TTCT indexes and personal achievement seemed to indicate an optimal level of divergent thinking, at least in the regression showing the quadratic trend. This, too, makes a great deal of sense in that thinking divergently probably could be taken too far and as such led to wild, crazy, and useless ideas. Eysenck (1997) reported that psychotics can be quite divergent in their ideation, but they are not creative because their ideas are too far out, too wild and unrealistic. Nearly all contributions to creative performances (e.g., nonconformity, autonomy, divergence) seem to have an optimal level of benefit (Runco & Sakamoto, 1999) and it should come as no surprise that this applies to divergent thinking as well. Still, the present results should be tested in future research with larger and less select samples. Perhaps an optimal level of divergent thinking could be defined in such a way that it can be targeted by educators and their students. There were notable sex differences in these results, with men having higher public achievements and women higher personal achievements. Recall here that Cramond et al. (2005) also found sex differences in their report of the 40-year follow-up data. It is difficult to say much about sex differences, given that this was a longitudinal study with participants of approximately 60 years old, or perhaps even older. Opportunities and expectations for the females who were in elementary Downloaded by [72.152.245.219] at 21:45 11 September 2011 TORRANCE TESTS OF CREATIVE THINKING AS PREDICTORS OF PERSONAL AND PUBLIC ACHIEVEMENT school in the 1950s and 1960s may not be the same as for females in elementary school now. The TTCT scores suggested good discriminant validity in low and primarily negative correlations with WISC IQ scores. There was no indication of a threshold, and thus no support for threshold theory, but that may at least in part reflect the select sample involved in this investigation. An ideal test of the threshold would include a very wide range of ability (Kim, 2006; Runco & Albert, 1986). Here it was an obvious analysis to compute, given the available data, but not a hypothesis reflecting a longitudinal concern. The four TTCT scores were moderately interrelated, but this is typical and consistent with theory. It does not, however, suggest that only one index should be used when scoring the TTCT or other test of divergent thinking. The intercorrelations do not indicate huge redundancy, for example, and earlier research has suggested that the various indexes are functionally distinct (Runco, 1995). Torrance (1995) himself suggested that all indexes be used and that a composite was only meaningful as some estimate of creative energy and not indicative of one particular cognitive process. If there is practical exigency that requires that only one index could be used, it should not be fluency. Originality is more strongly tied to creativity than is fluency (Runco et al., 2009). There are practical implications of the results reported here, in addition to the psychometric implications summarized earlier. Overall, the results underscore the value of divergent thinking and they may be taken as a reminder of how important it is as an objective for parents and teachers. Adults should encourage, model, and value the divergent thinking of their children and provide opportunities for its practice. Apparently divergent thinking plays a role in certain kinds of creative expression and achievement, and does so throughout the lifespan. REFERENCES Albert, R. S. (1969). Genius: Present-day status of the concept and its implications for the study of creativity and giftedness. American Psychologist, 24, 743–753. Albert, R. S. (1978). Observations and suggestions regarding giftedness, familial influence, and the achievement of eminence. Gifted Child Quarterly, 22, 201–211. Albert, R. S. (1980a). Exceptionally gifted boys and their parents: Basic educational, cognitive, and creative similarities. Gifted Child Quarterly, 24, 174–179. Albert, R. S. (1980b). Family positions and the attainment of eminence: A study of positions and special family experiences. Gifted Child Quarterly, 24, 87–95. Albert, R. S., & Runco, M. A. (1987). The possible different personality dispositions of scientists and nonscientists. In D. N. Jackson & 367 J. P. Rushton (Eds.), Scientific excellence: Origins and assessment (pp. 67–97). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression=correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cramond, B., Matthews-Morgan, J., Bandalos, D., & Zuo, L. (2005). A report on the 40-year follow-up of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: Alive and well in the new millennium. Gifted Child Quarterly, 49, 283–291. Eysenck, H. J. (1997). Creativity and personality. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Creativity research handbook (Vol. 1, pp. 41–66). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. Feist, G. J., & Barron, F. X. (2003). Predicting creativity from early to late adulthood: Intellect, potential, and personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 62–88. Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill. Guilford, J. P. (1968). Creativity, intelligence, and their educational implications. San Diego, CA: Robert Knapp. Guilford, J. P. (1970). Creativity tests for children. Orange, CA: Sheridan Psychological Services. Helson, R. (1996). In search of the creative personality. Creativity Research Journal, 9, 295–306. Helson, R. (1999). A longitudinal study of creative personality in women. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 89–101. Helson, R. (2000). Personality development: Adulthood and aging. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 116–120). Washington, DC: APA Books. Holland, J. L. (1961). Creative and academic performance among talented adolescents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 52, 136–147. Kim, K. H. (2005). Can only intelligent people be creative? A metaanalysis. Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 16, 57–66. Kim, K. H. (2006). Is creativity unidimensional or multidimensional? Analyses of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 251–259. Millar, G. W. (2002). The Torrance kids at mid-life. Westport, CT: Ablex. Plucker, J. A. (1999). Is the proof really in the pudding? Reanalysis of Torrance’s longitudinal data. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 103–114. Runco, M. A. (1986). Predicting children’s creative performance. Psychological Reports, 59, 1247–1254. Runco, M. A. (1995). Insight for creativity, expression for impact. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 377–390. Runco, M. A. (1996). Personal creativity: Definition and developmental issues. New Directions for Child Development, 72, 3–30. Runco, M. A. (1999). Divergent thinking. In M. A. Runco & S. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of creativity (pp. 577–582). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Runco, M. A., & Albert, R. S. (1986). The threshold hypothesis regarding creativity and intelligence: An empirical test with gifted and nongifted children. Creativity Child and Adult Quarterly, 11, 212–218. Runco, M. A., & Albert, R. S. (2005). Parents’ personality and the creative potential of exceptionally gifted boys. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 355–368. Runco, M. A., Cramond, B., & Pagnani, A. ( 2010). Sex differences in creative potential and creative performance. In D. McCreary (Ed.), Handbook of gender research in psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 343– 357). New York: Springer. Runco, M. A., & Sakamoto, S. O. (1999). Experimental research on creativity. In R. S. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 62–92). New York: Cambridge University Press. 368 RUNCO, MILLAR, ACAR, CRAMOND Downloaded by [72.152.245.219] at 21:45 11 September 2011 Runco, M. A., Plucker, J. A., & Lim, W. (2000). Development and psychometric integrity of a measure of ideational behavior. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 393–400. Runco, M. A., Nobel, E. P., Reiter-Palmon, R., Acar, S., Ritchie, T., & Yurkovich, J. M. (2009). The genetic basis of creativity and ideational fluency. Manuscript submitted for publication. Torrance, E. P. (1969). Prediction of adult creative achievement among high school seniors. Gifted Child Quarterly, 13, 223–229. Torrance, E. P. (1972a). Career patterns and peak creative achievements of creative high school students twelve years later. Gifted Child Quarterly, 16, 75–88. Torrance, E. P. (1972b). Predictive validity of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior, 6, 236–252. Torrance, E. P. (1980). Norms–technical manual: Demonstrator form for the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Unpublished manuscript, Georgia Studies of Creative Behavior, Athens, GA. Torrance, E. P. (1981a). Predicting the creativity of elementary school children (1958–80)—And the teacher who ‘‘made a difference.’’ Gifted Child Quarterly, 25, 55–62. Torrance, E. P. (1981b). Empirical validation of criterion-referenced indicators of creative ability through a longitudinal study. Creative Child and Adult Quarterly, 6, 136–140. Torrance, E. P. (1993). The Beyonder in a thirty-year longitudinal study. Roeper Review, 15, 131–135. Torrance, E. P. (1995). Why fly? Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. Torrance, E. P. (2002). The manifesto: A guide to developing a creative career. Westport, CT: Ablex. Torrance, E. P. (2003). The millennium: A time for looking forward and looking back. Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, 15, 6–12. Torrance, E. P. (2008). The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking Norms-Technical Manual Figural (Streamlined) Forms A and B. Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service. Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children: A study of the creativity–intelligence distinction. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Wallach, M. A., & Wing, C. W., Jr. (1969). The talented student: A validation of the creativity–intelligence distinction. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.