FACTORS DRIVING STUDENT MOTIVATION Beth Castiglia, Felician College Abstract Strong study habits are key to student academic success, yet an understanding of the underlying motivations for student studying eludes most faculty members. This study, conducted at Felician College in Lodi, New Jersey, was designed to explore the factors that drive students to study, to identify differences between successful and less-successful student in terms of their study behaviors, and to connect student study motivation to the larger realm of workplace motivation theory. INTRODUCTION What makes students study? This question, usually expressed with frustration, is heard over and over again whenever college faculty assemble. “Students are motivated by fear,” some faculty members might claim. “Give them tough classes and they’ll pull through.” Other faculty members might plea the opposite case, arguing that students thrive on positive feedback. Still other teaching faculty might argue that students work to satisfy their own personal achievement goals, or that students work when they believe that there will be direct payback – in future pay or opportunities – for the efforts expended today. In the end, if a dozen faculty members were asked “What makes students study?” it is likely that a dozen different opinions would be advanced. The disagreement about the sources of student motivation makes the question of academic motivation sound a great deal like that of workplace motivation. Management theory abounds with contradictory theories about what makes employees work. Maslow (1968) argued that needs drive behavior, and that workplace (and, presumably, academic) motivation is determined by the level of the needs hierarchy at which the individual is operating. McClelland (1966) claimed instead that motivation is driven by an innate need to achieve, and Herzberg (1959) proposed that the content of the jobs themselves was the source of the elusive employee motivation. The purpose of this research was to align explanations for student motivation with classical workplace motivation theories. Are students driven by needs, confirming Maslow’s (1968) theory? Are they instead motivated by innate needs to achieve, aligning with McClelland’s work? Or are they, perhaps, motivated to study only when the content of their courses inspires them, as seems to be suggested by Herzberg (1959)? Once college faculty are aware of the sources of students’ motivation, they will be better equipped to develop classroom strategies that capitalize on them. This study was conducted within the Division of Business and Management Sciences at Felician College, in Lodi, New Jersey. One hundred seven students (of a total student population of 178 within the division) voluntarily completed a survey on the sources of student motivation (reproduced in Appendix A). The survey and the research design were approved by the Felician College Institutional Review Board. The questions on the 7-scale Likert survey (written by the researchers) included questions such as I study more when the class is difficult and I’m afraid of failing, I am motivated to study because I want to graduate with honors, and I don’t study because I have no time due to work and family obligations. The questions were broad enough to cover need-based motivation (I am motivated to study because studying makes me feel good and I am motivated to study because I want to outdo my classmates and friends), achievementbased motivation (I am motivated to study because I want to make the Dean’s list) and content-based motivation (I study more when I find the material interesting and practical). The student participants were drawn from two very distinct populations at Felician College. The traditional, day-time students (of whom 75 participated) were enrolled predominantly full-time, were mostly less than 25 years of age, and were approximately half male and half female. Most worked part-time, and over one-third of them participated in college athletics. The second population was comprised of 32 students in Felician College’s accelerated degree completion program. These students were all over 25 years of age, were predominantly female, and were all employed full time during the day. At the outset of the research, it seemed logical that the factors that motivated this population might be very different from those which motivated the traditional students. THE PROBLEM Studying plays an integral role in the success of college students. Faculty members, however, are limited in their abilities to compel students to study. The motivation to study (and, ultimately, the responsibility for success or failure) must rest with the students. Davis and Murrell (1993) summarized, “While faculty members can serve as academic helmsmen, the collective energy of all students at the oars actually moves the boat through the water” (p. 11). Nevertheless, teaching faculty measure their successes or failures in terms of student learning – and student learning is closely connected to student study behavior. Faculty’s inability to harness the sources of student motivation has become a more serious concern over the past decade. According to the AAC&U (2002), the United States is approaching an era of near-universal college attendance. College is no longer the province of only the wealthy, but is seen as the ticket to upward mobility and professional life across class lines (Davis & Murrel, 1993). While the diversity of students reflected in college enrollment now reflects that of the nation as a whole, the same cannot yet be said about the diversity of the population of students who complete their studies and graduate. According to the Department of Education (2004), less than half of the lowest income students enrolled in college graduate within five years. If the attainment of a college degree is to be seen as the great equalizer in the United States, it is important to understand why some students succeed in an academic environment and others fail. Motivation may prove to be a major determinant of which students (from which socioeconomic environments) succeed and which fail. 2 THE SETTING Felician College is a small private Catholic College located on two campuses in Lodi and Rutherford, New Jersey. The mission of the college historically was to serve first-generation college students from immigrant families. The traditional mission has not changed very much in the past 40 years: Felician still draws primarily first-generation college students who are uncertain of their academic preparation for college. The president of the college likes to say, “The measure of a college should not be measured by the caliber of the students it admits, but rather in the quality of the students it graduates.” Because of this philosophy, Felician College has a liberal admissions policy (the average SAT score for the class of 2005 was approximately 1,000). Demographically, the students enrolled represent the diversity of the region in which the college is located. While Felician’s retention rate is admirable (the college ranks second in the state of New Jersey in student retention), it is mixed across programs and college populations. Very few students from the accelerated program fail to complete their degrees, and the students most at risk are young traditional minority students. Students who withdraw from the college generally do so because of poor academic performance. It is rare to have a student in good academic standing fail to complete a degree program. RESEARCH QUESTIONS College is seen as the “great equalizer,” but in order for it to perform this role students need to achieve academic success within its walls. Academic success, in part, derives from students’ study habits – which, in turn, are driven by students’ motivations. If college faculty members and administrators understood students’ motivations to study, they might be better able to help the at-risk students succeed. This study was organized around the following research questions: 1. What motivates most students to study? Are there significant differences in the motivations of adult learners and traditional students or between males and females? 2. Are there differences between the motivating factors affecting high-performing students (those with GPAs of 3.5 or above) and poorly performing students (those with GPAs below 2.5)? 3. Do the factors that motivate students to study align with the motivational theories of Abraham Maslow, Douglas McGregor, David McClelland, and/or Frederick Herzberg? RESEARCH METHODOLOGY This research was conducted on the two campuses of Felician College. The survey instrument used (Appendix A) consisted of 30 questions on motivation to study and eight questions on student background. The background information compiled included gender, age, GPA, and athletic status. The surveys were administered by the 3 researchers during the month of April, 2005 in classes that were offered in the business program during that semester. Over 95% of traditional students completed surveys. Because of logistical issues (the accelerated degree completion program runs in 5-week modules), though, less than 1/3 of the non traditional students were surveyed. The courses and students surveyed in the accelerated program were chosen by convenience (they were available during the survey period) and were not believed to be different from the adult population as a whole in any meaningful way. DATA ANALYSIS After all of the surveys were complete, the data were compiled for the 107 student participants. The surveys were scored by assigning a rank of 1 to the statement strongly agree and a score of 7 to strongly disagree. Demographic and background responses were also scored numerically (e.g., 1 = male; 2 = female). The data for each question on motivation were first correlated with the data for student GPA to seek significant relationships between motivating factors and academic performance. Spearman rank correlations were calculated between GPA ranges and responses to each of the questions on the survey. High GPAs (over 3.5) were significantly correlated (at α = .05) with the motivating factors of grades, honors, and being on the dean’s list. Low GPAs (under 2.5) were correlated significantly (at the .05 level) with concerns over “disappointing my family” “losing my scholarship.” Taken on their own, these results might imply that the high-performing students are driven by the need to achieve academic success and the lower-performing students by fear of failure. This connection might be spurious, however. Since the students responding to the questions already had either high or low GPAs, their responses to the survey questions could have been driven by their current placement in the academic pecking order – not the motivators affecting the study habits that caused the GPAs in the first place. Next, mean scores for each survey question were calculated, and the motivating factors were ordered from the lowest average score (closest to strongly agree) to the highest (closest to strongly disagree). Ranked lists were created for the student population as a whole, for traditional students alone, for accelerated students alone, for males, for females, and for low and high GPA students. Comparisons were done between traditional and accelerated, male and female, and high GPA and low GPA. Any factor with a difference of 15 basis points or more was highlighted as an important distinction between the groups. The results of this analysis are discussed below. Finally, the questions on motivation were clustered according to how well they aligned with classical motivational theory. Questions were tagged as either corresponding to McGregor, McClelland, Maslow, or Herzberg. The number of respondents either strongly agreeing or agreeing with the tagged questions was viewed as confirmation or rejection of the theory as an explanation of students’ motivation to study. The discussion of the results of this analysis appears later in this paper. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION: BY RESEARCH QUESTION 1. What motivates students to study? 4 Results. Among the top reasons for studying among all students were grades and upcoming exams. Seventy-eight percent of all students also claimed to study most when the subject is “interesting and practical” The need to “prove something to myself” was cited as a motivation to study by 68% of the population, and this percent rose to 87% when the accelerated adult students were isolated. Learning for its own sake was the third ranked reason for studying among the adult population, but came in eighth for the traditional students. The top reason cited for not studying was work and family obligations; 37% of all students claimed that obligations took away from study time. Not surprisingly, the accelerated adult learner felt this pressure more than the traditional 1821 year old student. The second greatest reason for not studying, I’d rather go out or hang out with my friends, was ranked strongly agree or agree by 31% of the traditional students, but (again, not surprisingly) only 9% of the adult learners. Fear of letting down family ranked much higher as a motivator for traditional students than accelerated students, while the desire to learn ranked higher for the adult population than the traditional one. These results are logical and were expected given the different stages of life of the members of the two groups. Less predictable were the differences between the cited motivators and study deterrents between male and female students. (Note: because the nontraditional population is heavily female, these comparisons were done both for total males vs. total females and total males vs. traditional females to avoid the confounding impact of age on the analysis.) As shown on Table 1, males were more motivated than females by an easy course, fear of disappointing family, fear of losing a scholarship, and a sense of competition. Females, on the other hand, were more motivated by the presumed applicability of the course and the possibility of making the dean’s list. 5 Table 1. Relative Importance of Motivations to Study; Males, Females, and Accelerated Per cent of students Strongly Agreeing or Agreeing Motivator Total Grades Interesting course Upcoming exams Self Application Low class average Learning Professor Fear of failing Cost Promotions Family Dean’s list Honors High class average Feel good Easy grade Maintain scholarship Competition 83% 78% 73% 68% 65% 64% 63% 54% 51% 49% 49% 47% 45% 41% 40% 33% 30% 29% 21% Traditional Accelerated Male 73% 76% 75% 60% 59% 64% 52% 52% 41% 44% 49% 56% 40% 35% 37% 23% 33% 39% 20% 97% 84% 69% 87% 81% 66% 87% 59% 56% 59% 47% 25% 56% 56% 47% 56% 22% 47% 25% 78% 82% 70% 68% 56% 58% 56% 58% 56% 44% 50% 60% 36% 36% 42% 30% 40% 48% 32% Female Traditional female 88% 75% 75% 68% 73% 70% 68% 51% 47% 53% 47% 35% 53% 46% 39% 35% 21% 12% 12% 79% 70% 79% 55% 61% 73% 58% 48% 54% 45% 49% 45% 51% 42% 36% 18% 21% 18% 9% Discussion. The differences between the motivational forces of traditional and mature students uncovered in this survey confirm the existing literature on adult learners. Tweedell (2000), for example, wrote of differences between adult learners and adolescents significant enough to require completely different learning structures between the two groups (p. 4). It is unreasonable, therefore, to presume that a factor that motivates one group would have a similar effect on the other. Table 1 shows that for the motivating factors of self, learning, and studying because it makes them “feel good,” the responses of the adult learners exceeded those of the traditional students by over 25 basis points. Erikson’s (1980) model of identity development in adulthood suggests that adults entering the stage of integrity may search for meaning in their pursuit of higher education. The need to seek truth and meaning at this stage could explain why the adults in this survey ranked learning and self second only to grades as their top motivators. The differences between male and female students in this study might have arisen from true gender differences, but could also be a function of the differences in average GPAs between the two groups at Felician College. The Spearman rho correlation between gender and GPA for students in this study was .398, which was significant at the .01 level. Because the females in the study academically outperform the males, it is 6 impossible to draw conclusions about their different motivations based on their genders alone. While it appears that the female students were motivated more by achievement and male students more by fear, these results are most likely driven by their differences in academic performance, not their different genders. Further research should be done with a larger population that could be normalized for grades before analyzed by gender. 2. Are there differences between the motivating factors of high performing and lowerperforming students? Results. As shown in Table 2, the greatest differences between high GPA and low GPA students are in the factors of learning, application, honors, dean’s list, family scholarship, and competition. Overall, the high achiever appears to thrive on the positive reinforcement that comes with his or her success, while the lower performer is driven largely by fear and competition. Again, these results might be confounded by the prevalence of females in the high GPA population. Low GPA students, expectedly, expressed much more agreement with many of the excuses not to study, as revealed in Table 3. They cited a despised topic, sports and school activities, and the presence of an easy professor with whom they can “get by” as their strongest reasons to avoid the books. Table 2. Comparison between high-GPA and low-GPA students’ motivations to study (Percent of students Strongly Agreeing or Agreeing) Motivator Grades Interesting course Upcoming exams Learning Self Application Low course average Professor Honors Dean’s list Fear of failing Cost Promotions High course average Family To feel good Easy grade Competition Maintain scholarship High GPA (n=34) 85% 82% 76% 73% 71% 71% 67% 56% 53% 53% 50% 47% 44% 44% 38% 35% 21% 18% 15% Low GPA (n=18) 83% 78% 67% 50% 78% 56% 61% 72% 28% 33% 61% 50% 44% 39% 56% 33% 33% 39% 50% 7 Table 3. Comparison between high-GPA and low-GPA students’ reasons for not studying Percent of students Strongly Agreeing or Agreeing Reason for Not Studying Obligations Useless topic Friends Can get by without Sports obligations Poor facilities Hate the topic Easy professor Boring No recognition Hopeless Noise High GPA (n=34) 41% 18% 15% 15% 12% 12% 12% 12% 12% 12% 9% 9% Low GPA (n=18) 44% 22% 22% 39% 44% 44% 50% 39% 28% 17% 17% 28% Discussion. This study showed that students with high GPAs are different in their motivations than those with low, but the conclusions offer little guidance for faculty members trying to create a full class of high achievers. None of the factors cited by high performing students seem to indicate the presence of some universal motivators that can be written into syllabi or woven into pedagogy. Instead, most of the top motivating factors (caring about grades, wanting to learn) might be established within students before they set foot into a college classroom. The conclusions are perplexing. Rewards matter: but only to the students who will tend to receive the rewards anyway. Fear motivates: but only those students who truly have something to fear. Competition spurs performance: but only for traditional-aged male students. The faculty member attempting to build motivational factors into his or her course will find himself or herself as befuddled as the manager in a business setting who attempts to motivate all of his or her employees by using the same strategies. A “one size fits all” motivational tool will fail in academia as surely as it has failed in the world of business. 4) Do the factors that motivate students to study align with the motivational theories of Abraham Maslow, Douglas McGregor, David McClelland, and/or Frederick Herzberg? McGregor’s Theory X and Theory Y McGregor’s (1985) well-known categorization of management beliefs can possibly be related to faculty’s attitudes toward students. According to McGregor, managers tend to classify their employees in one of two categories. Theory X employees find work distasteful and do all they can to avoid it. Managers who believe that their place of work is populated with this type of employee feel the need to maintain an 8 autocratic, discipline-driven workplace. Like managers, faculty form beliefs about the tendencies of their students. Are students believed to adhere to Theory X, and expected to display signs of laziness and irresponsibility whenever left unsupervised? Do they demonstrate what McGregor referred to as the “human characteristic dislike for work?” If so, the implications for faculty are similar to those for management: They should run a “tight ship,” focus on the disciplinary qualities of grading, and never expect students to do more than they are required. Of course, McGregor recognized that not all managers maintain such a dismal assessment of their employees. Managers adhering to Theory Y believe that workers have an innate tendency toward self-direction and commitment to objectives. Managers who believe that the average human being not only accepts but actively seeks responsibility provide their employees with a great deal of freedom and autonomy in their work. Faculty who subscribe to Theory Y behave similarly: They develop an open classroom environment in which students would be free to take responsibility for their own learning. For this study, several questions on the motivation survey were earmarked as “Theory X” compliant. (Theory Y was not directly measured, but was presumed instead to be confirmed by the negation of Theory X.) In the academic arena, a Theory X student would be one motivated only by the fear of failure. He or she would not care about learning for the sake of knowledge, and would only study when faced with an upcoming exam. Table 4 shows the breakdown of students by grades, gender, and program in response to questions aligning with McGregor’s Theory X. Table 4. Per cent of students agreeing or strongly agreeing with Theory X statements High avg. Low avg. Male Female Accelerated Fear 50% 61% 56% 47% 41% Scholarship* 15% 50% 48% 12% 47% Family** 38% 56% 60% 35% 25% Low avg. 67% 61% 58% 70% 66% Exams 76% 67% 70% 75% 69% * expressed as fear of losing scholarship ** expressed as fear of disappointing family _______________________________________________________________________ Although McGregor’s Theory X has fallen out of favor in management circles, the data in Table 4 imply that fear – particularly fear of failing a course in which the student currently carries a low average and fear of upcoming exams – motivates most students. The motivational force of fear is strongest among students who have low overall grade point averages. Fear, therefore, may be considered a “last resort” motivator. Students who were not previously motivated to study by loftier goals (such as love of knowledge or need to achieve) find themselves pressed to study late in the game by a fear of failing. Strong students (those with high grade point averages) are obviously motivated by grades: the presence of a low average in a particular course spurs 67% of them to study harder. Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs 9 Abraham Maslow’s (1968) hierarchy of needs is another well-known management theory of motivation. According to Maslow, motivation arises from different needs in different individuals. Needs, he argued, can be arranged in a hierarchy, and individuals progress from one level of the hierarchy to another once the needs on the lower level are satisfied. The hierarchy consists of five levels – physiological, safety, social, esteem, and self-actualization. For each (except the peak, self-actualization) satisfied needs no longer motivate. The implication of Maslow’s work for management was that there cannot be a “one size fits all” method of motivation; managers must devise motivators for each worker at the level at which he or she operates. Maslow’s model can be applied to college students and their motivations to study, with similar implications. Students might be driven to study by safety needs if they see their investment in college as a form of financial insurance in their future. They might be motivated instead by social needs if they enjoy the community of the college and wish to remain a part of their group or cohort. Perhaps esteem needs drive the competitive student (who sees his or her grades or honors as evidence of winning an academic race). Finally, some students may be driven by self-actualization, desiring to prove something to themselves through their academic successes. To identify need levels in this study the survey questions were categorized as complying with social, esteem, and self-actualization motivators. (Safety needs were not identified because they overlapped with the Theory X analysis above.) Table 5 shows that different students appear to operate at different levels of Maslow’s hierarchy. The implications of this division for faculty is the same as the implications for management: a “one size fits all” pedagogy will not motivate a mixed group of students within a class. Motivation in the classroom, like in the workplace, needs to be customized according to need levels. Table 5. Per cent of students agreeing or strongly agreeing with different need level statements High avg. Low avg. Male Female Accel Social needs Like the professor* 56% Family 38% 72% 56% 58% 60% 51% 35% 59% 25% Esteem needs Grades Promotions Honors Dean’s list Competition 85% 44% 53% 53% 18% 83% 44% 28% 33% 39% 78% 50% 36% 36% 32% 88% 47% 53% 53% 12% 97% 47% 56% 56% 25% Self-Actualization needs Feel good 35% Know material 73% Self 71% 33% 50% 78% 30% 56% 68% 35% 68% 68% 56% 87% 87% * Shortened phrases were used in the table. See Appendix A for the full survey statements. 10 The data in Table 5 suggest that students can be classified according to Maslow’s needs hierarchy. Social needs were prized most by students with low grade point averages; selfactualization needs by accelerated degree students. These tendencies may be a function of age. Traditional students overall have lower GPAs than accelerated students, so these students are over represented in the “low average” column on the table. Maslow did not argue that people move up his needs hierarchy predictably according to life stage, but it is likely that the younger traditional student would be motivated by lower level needs than the accelerated, over 25-year old student. This implies that the motivational techniques used by faculty in traditional settings should differ from those used in accelerated ones. Although the “one size fits all” approach will not always work, it would probably work better when students are separated by age than when they are in mixed classes. A faculty member would be wise to adopt a more democratic environment among the selfmotivated adults and a more autocratic one among the peer-driven traditional population. Herzberg’s Two-Factor Theory According to Frederick Herzberg (1984), the factors that motivate employees are not the opposite of those that do not. His classic separation of factors such as pay, work conditions, and supervision (hygiene factors) from work-content factors (motivators) had clear implications for the working manager. According to Herzberg, employees can only be motivated intrinsically by achievement, responsibility, and self-satisfaction. Peripheral factors, such as company policies and administration, have the potential of dissatisfying employees if they are not deemed appropriate, but can never motivate them to work harder. The insinuation of Herzberg’s work was clear: in order to motivate employees, give them interesting, fulfilling work in which they can take pride. Can Herzberg’s theory also apply in the college classroom? In an academic setting, hygiene (extrinsic) factors would include all of the issues surrounding classroom management, including grades, honors, and the professor. These factors, in two-factor theory parlance, are analogous with pay in the workplace: they are nice if they are present, but their presence is not enough to inspire students to study. If Herzberg’s theory were to hold in the college classroom, only intrinsic factors, which in an academic setting would include the content of the course and the sense of accomplishment the student would get in mastering the material, would have the power to motivate students to study. To test Herzberg’s theory in this study, survey statements were separated into hygiene factors and motivators. Table 6 displays the breakdown of students agreeing with the statements in each category. 11 Table 6. Percent of students agreeing or strongly agreeing with motivator and hygiene factor statements. High avg. Low avg. Male Female Accel Hygiene factors Professor Easy A High average Low average Midterms Price Grades Honors Deans List Scholarship 56% 21% 44% 67% 76% 47% 85% 53% 53% 15% 72% 33% 39% 61% 67% 50% 83% 28% 33% 50% 58% 40% 42% 58% 70% 44% 78% 36% 36% 48% 51% 21% 47% 70% 75% 53% 88% 46% 46% 12% 59% 22% 47% 66% 69% 59% 97% 56% 56% 47% Motivators Interesting Feel good Knowledge Self 82% 44% 73% 71% 78% 39% 50% 78% 82% 30% 56% 68% 75% 35% 68% 68% 84% 56% 87% 87% With the exception of the statement that studying makes them “feel good” (which very few students agreed with), the statements classified as motivators did show high levels of student agreement, especially among higher GPA and accelerated students. On its own, this agreement would seem to confirm Herzberg’s two-factor theory and imply that the professor who wishes to motivate students should build interesting courses that the students should want to master for the knowledge they would gain. However, the agreement with many of the statements classified as hygiene factors, particularly those related to grades, shows that students are also motivated to study by factors Herzberg claimed could not motivate. The spillover of hygiene factors into motivators encountered in this study is not necessarily a negation of Herzberg’s theory, though. Herzberg, Mausner, and Snyderman (1959) themselves conceded that deleterious context factors in the workplace, such as poor administrative practices or low job security, could bring about poor job attitudes (p. 113). This impact, in most settings, is difficult to unravel from job satisfaction emerging from the content of the job itself. Similarly, in an academic setting, it appears difficult to unravel the motivation that arises from the content of the class from the measurement of performance in that class. A student who is failing a course may find it difficult to concede that the content of the course is interesting and that the study of the subject is satisfying in its own right. 12 McClelland’s Need to Achieve David McClelland (1971) believed that human needs fall under three distinct categories: the need for achievement, the need for affiliation, and the need for power. Like Maslow (1968), McClelland believed that needs were the sources of all motivation. McClelland, unlike Maslow, argued that one level of need was preferred to all others. In his controversial article, That urge to achieve, McClelland claimed that there should be a cultural preference for the need for achievement because individuals motivated by achievement are most likely to be professionally successful. Moreover, societies with a prevalence of achievement-oriented members are more economically advanced than those societies lacking such individuals. It would appear that McClelland’s theory would be highly transferable to an academic setting. Students enrolled in colleges and university, presumably, are positioning themselves to be among the professional high achievers McClelland described in his theory. Are college students motivated by the need to achieve? To test McClelland’s theory, the researchers identified eight statements from the motivation survey representative of an achievement motivation. Table 7 shows the per cent of students in various categories agreeing or strongly agreeing with these statements. Table 7. Per cent of students agreeing or highly agreeing with achievement statements High avg Low avg Male Female Accel. Easy A High avg. Exams Grades Promotions Honors Deans List Self 21% 44% 76% 85% 44% 53% 53% 71% 33% 39% 67% 83% 44% 28% 28% 78% 40% 42% 70% 78% 50% 36% 36% 68% 21% 47% 75% 88% 47% 53% 53% 68% 22% 47% 69% 97% 47% 56% 56% 87% Table 7 shows that the accelerated degree students demonstrate a higher need for achievement than traditional students (much as they showed higher levels of selfactualization in Maslow’s schema). High GPA students also agree more with statements referring to grades and honors, but this could logically be because high grades and awards are within the reach of those students. Grades and exams, across the board, have the power to motivate, but it is impossible to say whether the need for achievement underlies their power. Grades might be seen as evidence of achievement for the high performing student, but they may also be a source of fear of punishment for the lower GPA student. Further research is necessary to unravel the achievement motivation from other sources of motivation among college students. 13 CONCLUSION AND RECOMMENDATIONS This study arose from the question “What makes students study?” Research concluded that there is not a single answer to this question and that it is difficult to neatly align student motivation with any single classic management motivation theory. The recommendation for faculty that arises from this study is that they should use multiple motivational methods in each class setting. A faculty member wishing to motivate his or her class should challenge the strong students (to appeal to their urge to succeed and to prove something to themselves), provide connections to real-world applications for those students motivated by the useable content of the course, and continue to administer regular exams since, regardless of need level, most students are motivated to study most when nervous about an upcoming test. Overall, the recommendations for faculty members are not much different than those given countless times to managers working in business settings: avoid a one-size-fits all motivational approach and recognize that students in the classroom, like employees in a corporate setting, are individuals who are motivated by various tactics. Effective teaching, like good management, hinges upon personalizing the motivation to fit the individual. 14 Appendix A Motivation Among College Students General Instructions This questionnaire asks you about your study habits and the motivations that lie behind them. Your participation in this survey is voluntary and you may withdraw at any point. Please do not write your name so that your responses will be anonymous. Thank you in advance for your participation. BACKGROUND INFORMATION (Please circle the responses that apply to you.) Gender: (a) Male (B) Female Age: (A) 16-21 yrs (B) 22-25 yrs (C) 26-30 yrs (D) Over 30 Status: (A) Freshman (B) Sophomore (C) Junior (D) Senior Enrollment (A) Full time (B) Part time GPA (A) 2.0 – 2.5 (B) 2.51 – 3.0 (C) 3.01 – 3.5 (D) 3.51 – 4.0 Do you work? (A) Yes, full time Student athlete? (A) Yes (B) Yes, part time (E) Accelerated (C) No, I do not work (B) No Do you pay for school yourself?* (A) Yes, all of it (B) Yes, some of it (C) No, I do not pay anything * This also includes loans in your name (even if the payments have been deferred until after graduation). 15 College Students’ Motivation to Study Listed below are some statements that address college students’ motivations to study. Using the scale provided below, rate these statements to the extent to which they are most applicable to you. Please circle the best answer choice for each statement. Strongly agree 1 Agree 2 Agree Somewhat 3 Not sure or N/A 4 Disagree Somewhat 5 Disagree 6 Strongly Disagree 7 I STUDY MORE… Strongly agree Strongly disagree When I find the material to be interesting and practical When I like the professor When the class is difficult and I’m afraid of failing When the class is easy and I can get an easy A When my average in the class is high When my average in the class is low When midterms and finals are approaching 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 6 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 7 I AM MOTIVATED TO STUDY… Because I have paid so much for my courses Because studying makes me feel good Because I want to get good grades Because I actually want to know the material Because I believe I can apply what I learn to my present/future job Because excelling in school can help me get a promotion at work Because I want to graduate with Honors Because I want to make the Dean’s List Because I don’t want to disappoint my family Because I don’t want to lose my athletic/academic scholarship Because I want to prove something to myself Because I want to outdo my classmates and friends MY TOP REASONS FOR NOT STUDYING ARE… I have no time to study because of work and family obligations I have no time to study because of sports and school activities I would rather go out or hang out with my friends The facilities at school are not conducive to study My house/dorm is always noisy I can get by just fine without studying I would not do well in the course anyway I’ll never even remember or use the content of the course later on I hate the course or topic My professor is “cool” or “easy” Studying bores me and is a waste of my time I never get any kind of recognition for doing well anyway 16 REFERENCES Davis, T., & Murrell, P., Turning teaching into learning: The role of student responsibility in the collegiate experience. ASHE-Eric Higher Education Report #8, 1993. Erikson, E. (1980). Identity and the life cycle. New York: W.W. Norton & Company. Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., & Snyderman, B. (2004/1959). The motivation to work. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers. Maslow, A. (1968). Toward a psychology of being. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. McClelland, D. (1966). That urge to achieve. In D.A. Kolb, J.S. Osland, & I.M. Rubing (Eds.), the organizational behavior reader. Englewood Cliffs NJ: Prentice Hall. McGregor, D. (1985). The human side of the enterprise. NY: McGraw-Hill. Tweedell, C. (2000, October). A theory of adult learning and implications for practice. Paper presented at the meeting of the Midwest Educational Research Association, Chicago, Ill. U.S. Department of Education (2004). High school and college completion in the states. Retrieved June 14, 2005 from http://nces.ed.gov. 17