Concern for people 438 U2 AOS1: Interpersonal behaviour Team leadership style Country club style Middle-of-the-road style Impoverished style Authoritarian style Concern for productivity Figure 15.4 Blake and Mouton’s managerial grid. Research suggests that groups working with an authoritarian style of management are less happy and productive than those working with a democratic team approach. Country club leadership (high people/low productivity) O C N U Authoritarian leadership (high productivity/low people) S F O RO P E G Description A P This style shows most concern about the needs and feelings of members D of the team. TEManagers assume that as long as team members are happy C andE secure then productivity will follow. This creates a work environment R that R is fun and relaxed, but production suffers because of lack of direction Table 15.3 The five major leadership styles Leadership style As predicted, the democratic group was happier and more productive than the other two. The authoritarian group was productive, but only when the leader was actively managing the process. The laissez-faire group was low in productivity and its members were less happy than members of either of the other groups. Another study, by Blake and Mouton (1964), described a ‘managerial grid’ with two dimensions: concern for people’s well-being and concern for productivity. This enabled the researchers to identify five major styles of leadership and management. and control. This style is also known as ‘produce or perish’ leadership. Managers believe that employees are just a means to an end. Employee needs are less important than the need for an efficient and productive workplace. Authoritarian leaders are very strict, have rigid work rules, policies and procedures, and use coercive power to motivate employees. Impoverished leadership (low productivity/low people) This style is ineffective. Such management results in a workplace that is disorganised, workers who are dissatisfied and a workforce lacking in harmony. Middle-of-the-road leadership (medium production/medium people) This management style is a balance between the two dimensions. It looks like a good compromise, but in fact neither well-being of people nor productivity is achieved to a satisfactory level. Leaders using this style settle for average performance. Team leadership (high production/high people) This is the ideal managerial style that values production needs and the needs of workers equally. It creates a team environment based on trust and respect, which leads to high satisfaction and motivation, and results in high productivity. Figure 15.5 Country club leadership tends to result in a workplace that is happy but has low productivity because of a lack of direction and control from the leader. 440 U2 AOS1: Interpersonal behaviour Method: participants The prison superintendent (high status) was Dr Phillip Zimbardo. The guards (high status) were given: -- military-style uniforms and reflective sunglasses to disguise their individuality and cause them to have fewer inhibitions about the way they behave (this process is called deindividuation) -- whistles and batons to emphasise their authority. The guards were instructed: -- to impose their will on the prisoners using psychological intimidation -- not to use physical violence on prisoners -- to blindfold prisoners when they needed to leave the prison to use the toilet -- that they were allowed to touch the prisoners with their batons, but not hit them. The prisoners (low status) were given: -- short, sack-like smocks to wear, with no underwear -- hats made of ladies’ stockings to cover their hair -- a prison number -- a chain around the ankles, to ensure that they could never forget their status. The prisoners were instructed: -- to obey all instructions from the guards -- to ask permission to use the toilet, smoke a cigarette or write a letter -- to refer to themselves by their prison number, not their name; together with the uniform this meant that deindividuation applied to the prisoners, with the effect of reducing their sense of self. C P E G A P D TE O C N U Figure 15.7 Student volunteers were allocated to be guards or prisoners in the experiment. E RR S F O RO CH 15: Social influences on the individual 441 Method: the setting The setting was the basement of the Psychology Department at Stanford University, which had: -- no windows -- cells, 2 m x 3 m in size, opening off a corridor which was boarded up at each end -- opposite the cells, a cupboard of 60 cm x 60 cm x 213 cm, tall enough to stand up in, used as the solitary confinement cell. Method: procedure 1 In the early hours of a Sunday morning, each prisoner was picked up at his home by genuine police officers, charged, warned of his legal rights, spread-eagled against the police car, searched and handcuffed, often in full view of amazed neighbours. 2 The police car then raced to the Palo Alto police station with sirens at full blast. 3 Each prisoner was taken inside, fingerprinted, blindfolded and left to wait. 4 The prisoners were transported in a prison van to the Stanford University ‘jail’, where they were interviewed by the ‘superintendent’ who told them of the serious nature of their offences. 5 The prisoners were stripped naked and sprayed with a de-lousing agent. 6 The prisoners were issued with prison clothing: a smock and a stocking hat. 7 The guards worked eight-hour shifts with three guards in each shift. Prisoners were three to each cell. 8 At 2.30 a.m. on the first night, and several times in each shift afterwards, the prisoners were wakened with blasting whistles and shouts to attend roll-call. 9 Guards administered punishments, such as making prisoners do push-ups and perform humiliating acts; for example, walking like Frankenstein’s monster or telling other prisoners ‘I love you’. P E G A P D TE S F O RO C What happened? U O C N E RR -- Everything went smoothly for the first day and night, as all participants adjusted to their roles. On the second day, the prisoners protested and rebelled. The guards quickly stopped the rebellion by spraying freezing carbon dioxide (dry ice) from fire extinguishers on the prisoners. -- The guards then entered each cell, stripped the prisoners of their clothes, removed the beds from the cells and forced the leaders of the rebellion into solitary confinement. From this time on, the level of harassment of the prisoners by the guards increased and psychological abuse intensified. -- One of the cells was now labelled a ‘privilege cell’, and the prisoners least involved in the rebellion were given their clothes and beds, allowed to wash and clean their teeth, and given special food to eat in front of the other prisoners who received no food. This resulted in breaking down the solidarity of the prisoner group. -- After a few hours, the guards took some of these ‘good’ prisoners and exchanged them for ‘bad’ prisoners who were put in the ‘good’ cell, which unsettled all the prisoners who now thought that some of them must be informers. The prisoners became distrustful of each other. (This tactic is used by real guards in real prisons to break prisoner solidarity.) -- The rebellion produced greater solidarity among the guards who now saw the prisoners as trouble-makers. The guards now even refused to allow prisoners to go 442 U2 AOS1: Interpersonal behaviour -- --- -- -- -- Figure 15.8 The experiment became so real that emotional distress led to it ending prematurely. to the toilet, making them use a bucket in their cell and sometimes even refusing to allow them to empty the bucket. When one prisoner began to suffer severe emotional distress less than 36 hours into the experiment, the warden and superintendent would not let him leave. Eventually he became so distressed that for a short time they realised that they had gone too far with the ‘reality’ of the prison and allowed him to withdraw. Over the next two days, two more prisoners were similarly released. One was so emotionally distressed that he developed a psychosomatic rash over his whole body. On Day 3, friends and relatives of the prisoners were allowed to visit, so the cells were cleaned and polished, and the prisoners were showered, shaved and well fed to create a good impression. Some of the parents complained, but none insisted that their son should be released. Next, there was a rumour of an attempt to escape. This never happened, but the guards were so angered by the inconvenience they suffered that they increased the abuse further, making prisoners clean out toilets with their bare hands. By Day 5, the guards had fallen into three categories; about one-third in each: -- tough but fair guards who followed prison rules -- good guys who did little favours for the prisoners and never punished them -- aggressive and cruel guards who thought of creative ways to humiliate prisoners. On Day 6, the project was abandoned, less than half-way through the proposed 14day experiment. This occurred for two reasons: -- Zimbardo and his colleagues realised, through watching closed-circuit television footage, that the guards were increasing their abuse of prisoners during the night when they thought no one was watching. -- Christina Maslach, a recent doctoral graduate from Stanford who was dating Phillip Zimbardo at the time, came to the ‘prison’. She took one look at the degraded prisoners and soundly abused Zimbardo, even telling him that if this was a true indication of the sort of person he was, there would be no hope for their relationship. (They have now been married for over 39 years.) P E G A P D TE S F O RO C O C N E RR U Criticisms of the experiment -- Violation of the no harm principle: Although counselling was offered to participants, some were still reporting negative effects 40 years later. -- Violation of withdrawal rights: Note the pressure that was put on the ‘inmate’ who became distressed and wished to withdraw. Some good did come out of this experiment, however: it caused a government review of the way in which youth detention was carried out in the United States. Further, one of the participants was so concerned with what he had seen happen that he changed his university course and became a forensic psychologist. He has spent the last 35 years working to improve conditions for prisoners in United States jails. 444 U2 AOS1: Interpersonal behaviour Figure 15.9 Nazi Party rallies demonstrated obedience to authority. In 1961, an American social psychologist at Yale University, Stanley Milgram, set out to discover whether there was a special characteristic that made Germans more obedient to authority than other races. This was a popular theory in America and Europe in the years after the Second World War. S F O RO P E Milgram’s experimentAG P D Aim TE C The aim was to discover E whether participants would obey an authority figure and R carry out actions that R caused severe pain to another person. O C Background N U Participants were told that there had been little research on the use of punishment as a method of teaching and that this was to be a valuable scientific experiment that could be of great benefit to society. Method: participants The participants were 40 male volunteers, aged 20 to 50 years. Their occupations ranged from company owners to truck drivers to students and the unemployed. Milgram paid them US$4.50 for the hour of participation. This may not seem much, but in 1961 this amount would have covered the week’s grocery bill for a family in Australia. Participants were told that they would keep the money even if they did not complete the experiment. Method: materials The ‘teacher’ sat at a control panel with 30 switches and several imposing dials and lights. The switches were labelled with their power output: from 15 to 450 volts in 15-volt increments. The ‘learner’, in another room but within earshot of the teacher, was strapped into a chair with his arms bound to the arms of the chair and electrodes strapped to his bare wrists. CH 15: Social influences on the individual 445 Method: procedure The ‘learner’ was a confederate of Milgram, an accountant named Mr Wallace, who had agreed to play the part of the learner. When Mr Wallace and the participant entered the room they were introduced to each other as participants and then drew a piece of paper from a hat to see who would be the learner and who would be the teacher. The draw was fixed so that the participant always became the teacher. The ‘experimenter’ (Mr Williams, a biology teacher who had volunteered for the job) then took the teacher and Mr Wallace to another room, strapped Mr Wallace into the chair and showed the teacher what the shocks would be like by giving him a 45-volt genuine shock from another machine. The shock stung quite a bit! Next, the teacher returned to the desk with the shock machine and asked Mr Wallace a series of questions. When Mr Wallace (deliberately) gave the wrong answer, the teacher was required to say, ‘That is incorrect, the correct answer is … This will be at 15 volts’, then press the switch to give the shock. Each wrong answer, or refusal to answer, earned another shock at the next higher level of intensity. Williams, the experimenter, used some carefully worded prods to make the teacher continue whenever he or she wanted to stop. These were used one at a time in the following sequence: -- Prod 1: ‘Please continue, please go on!’ -- Prod 2: ‘The experiment requires that you continue!’ -- Prod 3: ‘It is absolutely essential that you continue!’ -- Prod 4: ‘You have no other choice, you must go on!’ If the teacher asked at any stage if the shocks could harm the learner, the researcher said: -- Prod 5: ‘Although the shocks may be painful, there is no permanent tissue damage so please go on.’ If the teacher said that the learner did not want to go on: -- Prod 6: ‘Whether the learner likes it or not, you must go on until he has learned all the word pairs correctly, so please go on!’ In the first experiment, the learner did not make any complaints until the 300volt level and at that stage he simply pounded his fist on the wall. After this he did not give any answers and pounded the wall again at 315 volts, after which he gave no answers at all and remained silent. C P E G A P D TE U O C N E RR S F O RO Figure 15.10 Stanley Milgram with his experimental ‘teaching machine’ 452 U2 AOS1: Interpersonal behaviour Among the most significant moderator variables, Smith and Bond found the following: -- normative influence -- culture -- informational influence -- group size -- unanimity -- deindividuation -- social loafing. Normative influence Normative influence simply refers to a person’s tendency to go along with the group so that they will fit in and gain the approval of other group members. It has been found that if group members are very similar to each other (such as in age, race and gender), there will be greater normative influence, increasing the pressure towards conformity. Since almost all studies replicating Asch’s experiments have used college students as participants, this has been a significant factor. If other group members are people from whom the participant wants to gain approval—possibly teachers, family members or friends—the tendency towards conformity is further increased. S F O RO P E Gthe level of conformity has been found to In the United States, A Psince the 1950s. In studies undertaken in Asian be decreasing D E the level of conformity has been found by some countries, T C Estudies to be significantly higher than in Western cultures. Culture R R O C N U Figure 15.16 Conformity is higher in a collectivist culture where the individual is less important than the group. It has been argued that the reason for this lies in the differences between individualist and collectivist cultures. -- An individualist culture is one in which individuality and independence are highly valued, and it is considered important for people to achieve their individual goals. -- A collectivist culture is one in which the individual’s goals are less important than group goals, and each person is encouraged to consider the needs of the group to be more important than their individual requirements. Certainly there is a tendency towards individualist culture in Western societies and collectivist culture in Eastern societies, but there could be other explanations for the difference in the level of conformity. For example, in Eastern cultures it is considered impolite to point out another person’s mistakes, even indirectly. Therefore there is a tendency to conform in order for the other group members to save face (Takano & Osaka 1999). Informational influence Informational influence refers to the increased tendency to conform when the participant wants to provide a correct response but is not certain of their ability to do so, believing that others are more capable of making accurate judgments. This is a very common cause of conformity. For example, if you are cooking a new dish for CH 15: Social influences on the individual 453 dinner and you are not certain which spices to use, you could ask a friend’s opinion. If you believe your friend has good judgment in this area, you conform and follow his or her advice. Group size Asch varied the number of confederates from one to 15. He found that conformity rates increased to a maximum with three or four confederates, and then remained approximately the same. 40 Percentage of trials 35 30 25 20 15 10 5 1 P E G A P D TE 2 3 4-9 Number of confederates in group Unanimity Figure 15.17 The percentage of trials where participants conformed with unanimous majorities (various sizes); conformity reached a maximum with a group size of four, then remained fairly constant. 10-15 S F O RO C E RR If one of the confederates did not conform with the group, but gave the correct answer, conformity rate dropped from the average of four out of 12 to one out of 12. Asch believed that there are three factors influencing this significant difference: 1 The participant observed that the majority did not criticise or make fun of the dissenting confederate. 2 There was now social pressure in two directions—from the dissenter as well as the majority. 3 The dissenter increased the participant’s belief that the majority was wrong. O C N U Deindividuation We came across deindividuation earlier in this chapter when Phillip Zimbardo deliberately removed the individual identifying characteristics of the Stanford prison guards and prisoners. A form of deindividuation, in which the participant cannot be identified by other group members, has also been used in variations of the Asch experiment, when groups of participants were placed in individual booths and given false feedback about the responses of the other group members. There is evidence that the level of conformity is higher in face-to-face groups than in simulated groups (Bond & Smith 1996). Figure 15.18 The deindividuation of Ku Klux Klan members is complete when they don their threatening costume. CH 15: Social influences on the individual 455 Groups influencing others Peer pressure We saw, at the beginning of this chapter, that a group is two or more people who have a common goal and who interact and influence each other. In the sections on status, power, leadership, obedience and conformity, we have seen many ways in which this influence may occur. Of great interest to social psychologists, and often reported in the media, is the influence of the peer group—a person’s friends and acquaintances of similar age, interests and social standing—on the behaviour of individuals. Peer pressure is often discussed in terms of the way it applies to people in their teenage years. This is where it becomes most apparent and powerful, as the influence of friends and acquaintances strengthens; but peer pressure really continues to apply to us all throughout our lifetime. Some of the ways in which peer pressure may have influence are: -- Direct pressure: Someone—or more than one person—tells you what you should be doing. -- Indirect pressure: Peer pressure is not always obvious. It is common for a group of friends to have particular ways of behaving when they are together. When you are with different groups of friends, you possibly do different things; for example, you may only play video games when you are with certain friends, or you may be more likely to study with other friends. -- Individual pressure: This is the pressure coming from inside yourself to avoid feeling different from the group. It means that sometimes you may do things to make sure you feel like the rest of the group. The more a person lacks self-confidence, the more likely they are to feel the effects of peer pressure. Much of the attention given to adolescent peer groups appears to assume that peer pressure will push a young person to behave in a fashion contrary to the wishes of their parents. In fact, Noller and Callan (1991) reviewed all available research and discovered that most adolescents had ‘harmonious relationships with their parents’, and also that when there were significant parent–adolescent problems, these had usually been present well before the child reached adolescence; or that the parenting style or parental attitudes were equally significant in contributing to the relationship breakdown. Researchers generally agree that peer pressure contributes to behaviour in four main areas: -- social activities—parties, concerts, sporting activities -- relationships—relations with parents and whether an individual has a boyfriend or girlfriend -- group norms—extra-curricular activities, style of dress, Figure 15.20 Sky-diving is risk-taking behaviour language used (especially slang terms) more common in younger people and early -- risk-taking behaviours. P E G A P D TE S F O RO C O C N E RR U adulthood than in older people.