as at 29/08/2012 The University of Edinburgh SCHOOL OF PHILOSOPHY, PSYCHOLOGY & LANGUAGE SCIENCES PSYCHOLOGY FINAL HONOURS PROJECTS 2012-13 SUGGESTED TOPICS Signing Up for Projects This list is designed to help you match your interests with a potential supervisor. You do not need to register your project formally until Week 3 of the Autumn semester 2012/13, but it is helpful to many students to have this list now, to enable them to talk to potential supervisors and agree on a project choice before the start of the next academic year. It is up to you to find a supervisor. Contact details of each supervisor are given here to allow you to email or arrange meetings. Students may work together in pairs on any project, and are encouraged to do so, but only in exceptional circumstances should this number be exceeded. In recent years almost 40% of projects have been based on the student's own idea rather than a staff member. However, as with literature reviews, make sure you are choosing a topic which a staff member is willing to supervise. If the supervisor is outwith the department, e.g. a clinical or educational psychologist, then you must have a member of staff agreeing to act as internal supervisor when you register the project at the beginning of the Autumn Term. Dr Alexa Morcom Psychology 4 Course Organiser May 2012 TABLE OF CONTENTS Alphabetical list of supervisors Page Dr Sharon Abrahams 3 Dr Elizabeth Austin 3 Professor Tim Bates 4 Professor Holly Branigan 5 Dr David Carmel 5 Dr Elena Gherri 7 Dr Karri Gillespie-Smith 8 Dr Wendy Johnson 10 Dr Peter Lamont 10 Dr Billy Lee 11 Professor Robert Logie 11 Dr Sarah MacPherson 14 Dr Andrea Martin 14 Dr Rob McIntosh 15 Dr Alexa Morcom 16 Dr Lars Penke 19 Professor Martin Pickering 19 Dr Richard Shillcock 20 Dr Patrick Sturt 22 Dr Caroline Watt 22 Dr Alex Weiss 23 Dr Sue Widdicombe 23 2 DR SHARON ABRAHAMS Email: s.abrahams@ed.ac.uk Below are some suggested research topics for dissertation projects. I am willing to discuss other areas within my discipline of clinical neuropsychology with a particular focus on executive and memory functions and the effects of aging or neurodegenerative disease. The projects listed below investigate the effect of normal aging. Anyone wishing to undertake a patient group project would need to access this group (e.g. via voluntary centres) independently although I would be happy to supervise. Speed of Processing and Dual Task in Aging The study will compare speed of processing and working memory theories of aging. A minimum of two groups of healthy adults will be tested an older and younger group. This study uses a newly developed task which measures both speed of processing using a computerised visual discrimination task combined with a dual task methodology of digit recall. The study aims to show differential effect of aging on dual task and speed of processing. Kaschel R. Logie RH. Kazen M. Della Sala S. Alzheimer's disease, but not ageing or depression, affects dual-tasking. Journal of Neurology. 256(11):1860-8, 2009 Nov. Sarah E. MacPherson, Sergio Della Sala, Robert H. Logie and Gordon K. Wilcock. Specific AD impairment in concurrent performance of two memory tasks. Cortex (2007) Volume: 43, Issue: 7, Pages: 858-865 The effect of ageing on verbal fluency Verbal fluency is a standard measure used within clinical neuropsychology and is thought to be sensitive to frontal lobe lesions. The test involves generating as many words as possible beginning with a given letter. Adaptations of this task have been used in the investigation of neurodegenerative disease (Abrahams et al. 2000) which often occurs in older adults. Dissecting verbal fluency we find the test involves initiation, strategy formation, working memory, word retrieval. What is the contribution of each of these components to verbal fluency and which are most vulnerable to the effects of age? What is the effect of adding constraints onto the fluency task and how is this affected by age? Abrahams S, Leigh PN, Harvey A, Vythelingum GN, Grise D, Goldstein LH. Verbal fluency and executive dysfunction in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS). Neuropsychologia 2000;38(6):734-747. Social Cognition and Executive Functions in Aging This study will investigate the relationship between measures of social cognition and executive functions in healthy aging. The study will use a new measure of social cognition (social scenarios test) and other measures which assess the more fundamental processes of detection of eye gaze and emotional facial expression. The latter two have been used to measure dysfunction in neurodegenerative disease (Girardi et al. 2010). The study will determine what is the relationship between these measures and whether changes in social cognition are found in parallel with changes in executive functions. Girardi, A., MacPherson, S. E, Abrahams, S. Deficits in Emotional and Social Cognition in Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis Neuropsychology. Neuropsychology 2011, Vol. 25, No. 1, 53–65 DR ELIZABETH AUSTIN Email: elizabeth.austin@ed.ac.uk Validation of a new scale for assessing the use of strategies to change another person’s mood This scale is currently being developed in a project which will be completed over the summer, so it will be available for further research in September. The scale is designed in part to improve on the emotional manipulation scale developed by Austin, Farrelly, Black, and Moore (2007) and to also broaden the coverage of mood-changing strategies by including sub-scales relating to both mood- 3 improving and mood-worsening strategies. Some preliminary data on the scale’s factor structure and personality correlates will be available; additional data will be collected in one or more projects which will further examine the associations of the use of mood-improving and mood-worsening strategies (e.g. with emotion recognition performance, Theory of Mind, emotional intelligence, social network size/quality), and validate the factor structure in a new sample. Austin, E. J., Farrelly, D., Black, C., & Moore, H. (2007). Emotional intelligence, Machiavellianism and emotional manipulation: Does EI have a dark side? Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 179-189. Personality, emotional intelligence and decision-making Both personality and emotional intelligence (EI) would be expected to be related to individual differences in decision-making. For example, the personality traits linked to the behavioural approach system (BAS) and behavioural inhibition (BIS) systems have been found to be related to choices made in gambling tasks ( Kim & Lee, 2011). EI could impinge on decision-making by a number of mechanisms, including better prediction of post-decision emotions (Dunn et al., 2007). In this project the associations of personality and EI with decision-making will be examined using straightforward choice tasks (e.g. choosing between two gambles with different risk levels and payoffs) which can be implemented in an internet survey. Dunn, E. W., Brackett, M. A., Ashton-James, C., Schneiderman, E., & Salovey, P. (2007). On emotionally intelligent time travel: Individual differences in affective forecasting ability. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 85-93. Kim, D., & Lee, J. (2011). Effects of the BAS and BIS on decision-making in a gambling task. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 1131-1135. Cross-situational personality variability This project will examine how cross-situational variability in Five Factor Model personality trait scores is related to other measures. There are a number of correlates of variability that could be examined, including self-monitoring, Machiavellianism and impression management. The study method will be similar to that described by Robinson, Wright, & Kendall (2010). PROFESSOR TIM BATES Email: tim.bates@ed.ac.uk 1. Raising Capability: Can we teach IQ, with lasting results for achievement? In this project we will conduct research on whether training can raise IQ scores. Dweck and others have suggested that simple manipulations of expectation can make lasting differences. This contrasts with very expensive studies which appeared to fail in this task. Why? Here’s a gentle if somewhat dated introduction to this idea. http://www.stanfordalumni.org/news/magazine/2007/marapr/features/dweck.html A quick look on Google will show many very recent references to this same topic. If you find the idea engaging, get in touch. 2. Is self control hard work? Since Aristotle and Galton, research has examined the role and origins of agency (e.g. Archontaki, Lewis and Bates, 2012) and willpower (for instance Baumeister 2012, Job, Dweck, & Walton, 2010). We will conduct experimental studies testing how internal resources enable our ability for self-control and the exertion of agency. You might enjoy reading: 4 http://www.heckmanequation.org/heckman-equation-slideshow 3+. I am also open to other interesting topics, including aesthetics – a collaboration with the art college or architecture school would be good – genetics: We have resources for a wide range of twin studies. PROFESSOR HOLLY BRANIGAN Email: holly.branigan@ed.ac.uk Repetition of structure in children's dialogue I am interested in how children produce and understand language in dialogue. I would happy to supervise any projects in this area, but here is one project of particular interest: Children often imitate or repeat the language they hear around them, and this is thought to play an important role in language acquisition. Recent work has shown that they sometimes repeat abstract grammatical structure, as well as particular words or phrases. This tendency towards grammatical repetition suggests that children have abstract representations of at least some aspects of grammar, in contrast to some theories of language acquisition. We have developed a dialogue 'game' that allows us to investigate children's grammatical repetition: an experimenter and a child take it in turns to describe cards to each other in a 'snap' game. The project will use this task to study the circumstances under which children repeat grammar. Reference Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M. & Shimpi, P. (2004). Syntactic priming in young children. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 182-195. Do speakers cooperatively design their sentences for their listeners? Speakers sometimes seem to ‘say the same thing’ in different ways depending on what they believe about their listeners. In this project, we’ll examine whether such ‘audience design’ effects are essentially cooperative: do speakers choose sentences that they believe their listeners will most easily understand? Reference Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, pp.1482-1493. DR DAVID CARMEL Email: dave.carmel@ed.ac.uk A. Perceptual learning: Practicing any task leads to improvement. Interestingly, this is true even for basic perceptual tasks – you can literally become better at seeing something if you train at it. Such improvements, known as perceptual learning, are usually highly specific. They are limited to the trained stimulus features and locations, and this is taken to indicate an early (retinotopic) cortical locus for the neural plasticity underlying the observed learning (Carmel & Carrasco, 2008). I will be running two projects related to perceptual learning: 1. Visual attention and perceptual learning: Directing visual attention to a particular spatial location enhances perceptual processing at that location. Relatively little is known, however, about how attention affects perceptual learning (Ahissar & Hochstein, 1993). In recent work, I have found that training with attention causes not only greater perceptual learning than training without attention, but also leads to a generalization effect: observers who trained with attention improved at untrained locations as well, whereas those who trained without attention did not (this is surprising, because attention was always directed to the trained locations). 5 In this project we will attempt to figure out how attention enhances and generalizes perceptual learning. We will test the hypothesis that attention enhances the same process that occurs without attention, by training different groups of participants with and without attention, equating learning at trained locations by varying the duration of training, and testing the amount of improvement each group shows at unattended locations. 2. Unconscious perceptual learning: Is awareness of the trained stimuli necessary in order for perceptual learning to occur? Some previous work (e.g., Seitz et al, 2009) has shown that visual performance can improve even if the stimuli themselves are suppressed from awareness during training. The mechanisms underlying such learning are poorly understood. In this project we will use continuous flash suppression (Tsuchiya & Koch, 2006) to suppress visual stimuli from awareness during training. We will also manipulate attention to specific spatial locations, where (unbeknownst to our participants) trained visual stimuli may appear. We will examine the effect of such training on perceptual learning, and whether attention modulates the amount of learning at different locations. References Carmel, D., & Carrasco, M. (2008). Perceptual learning and dynamic changes in primary visual cortex. Neuron, 57 (6), 799-801. Ahissar M, Hochstein S. (1993). Attentional control of early perceptual learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 90, 5718–5722. Seitz, A.R., Kim, D., & Watanabe, T. (2009). Rewards evoke learning of unconsciously processed visual stimuli in adult humans. Neuron, 61(5), 700-707. Tsuchiya, N., and Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nat Neurosci. 8, 1096-1101. B. Perceptual asymmetries across the visual field: We normally assume that our vision is roughly the same across the visual field, with visual acuity being best at fixation and decreasing gradually but equally in all directions as the distance from fixation increases. This is not the case. Various asymmetries have been found, including better visual performance at similar distances from fixation on the horizontal than the vertical midline, and better performance in the lower than the upper half of the vertical midline (Abrams et al, 2012). Performance has so far been found to be similar across intercardinal locations (i.e., on the diagonals). Perceptual learning is the improvement in performing a perceptual task that results from practice. In vision, perceptual learning is often highly specific to the trained locations in the visual field, indicating an early (retinotopic) cortical locus of the plasticity underlying the learning. But the way the specificity of perceptual learning is usually tested is problematic: observers are trained with stimuli that appear in one location, and improve at doing the perceptual task; then, they are tested with stimuli that appear at a different location, and are usually found to be worse. But to draw from this the conclusion that perceptual learning was location-specific, you must assume the two locations were similar to begin with. The most widely used task in perceptual learning studies is Karni & Sagi’s (1991) texture discrimination task (TDT). In almost all studies to date, a location in the top left of the visual field was trained, and then a location in the bottom right was tested. In this project we will test whether performance on the TDT is the same in different locations (spoiler: I’ve already done some work showing that it isn’t, but we need to substantiate it with further experiments). References Abrams, J., Nizam, A., & Carrasco, M. (2012). Isoeccentric locations are not equivalent: The extent of the vertical meridian asymmetry. Vision Research, 52(1), 70-78. 6 Karni, A., & Sagi, D. (1991). Where practice makes perfect in texture discrimination: evidence for primary visual cortex plasticity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 88(11), 4966-4970. DR ELENA GHERRI Email: Elena.Gherri@ed.ac.uk Perceptual load in a tactile flanker task In the tactile modality, the presence of tactile distractors can impair the detection and discrimination of tactile targets presented simultaneously (e.g. Evans & Craig, 1992; Soto-Faraco, Ronald, & Spence, 2004). In these studies, tactile stimuli (both target and distractors) were presented to both hands simultaneously. However, the mechanisms underlying within-hand and between-hand tactile selection might be different, given that a different pattern of tactile ERP modulations is obtained when the attentional selection is performed between the hands (c.f. Eimer & Forster, 2003a) or within the same hand (c.f. Eimer & Forster, 2003b). Aim of the present project is to investigate whether an analogous interference effect would emerge when target and distractors are presented to fingers of the same hand, establishing a within-hand tactile equivalent of the flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974). Crucially, by manipulating the physical difference between the target and the distractors (high and low perceptual load conditions) it will be possible to evaluate whether the interference of incompatible distractors is reduced under high perceptual load condition, as postulated by the load theory of selective attention (Lavie, Hirst, de Fockert & Viding, 2004). References: Eimer, M. & Forster, B., 2003b. The spatial distribution of attentional selectivity in touch: Evidence from somatosensory ERP components Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 1298-1306. Eimer, M., & Forster, B., 2003a. Modulations of early somatosensory ERP components by transient and sustained spatial attention. Experimental Brain Research, 151, 24-31. Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W., 1974. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143-149. Evans & Craig, 1992 Evans, P. M., & Craig, J. C., 1992. Response competition: A major source of interference in a tactile identification task. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 199–206. Lavie, N., Hirst, A., Fockert, Jan W. de & Viding, E., 2004. Load Theory of Selective Attention and Cognitive Control. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 133 (3), 339-354. Soto-Faraco, S., Ronald, A., & Spence, C., 2004. Tactile selective attention and body posture: Assessing the contribution of vision and proprioception. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 1077– 1094. Conflict monitoring across sensory modalities The cognitive adaptation phenomenon can be observed in conflict tasks (Flanker task, Simon task, Stroop task) when the sequential analysis of trials is carried out (that is when the compatibility of both current and preceding trials are considered). Typically, the compatibility effect is reduced after the consecutive presentation of two incompatible trials (e.g. Gratton, Coles, and Donchin, 1992). According to the response conflict monitoring hypothesis (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, and Cohen, 2001), the response conflict in the preceding trial elicit a stronger top-down control that improve performance in subsequent trials. The aim of this project is to test whether the mechanisms responsible for conflict monitoring operate in a supra-modal fashion, that is whether these conflictadaptation effects will still be observed when stimuli of different sensory modalities are presented on successive trials. References: Botvinick, M.M., Braver, T.S., Barch, D.M., Carter, C.S. & Cohen, J.D., 2001. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624-652. Gratton, G., Coles, M.G.H. & Donchin, E., 1992. Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 121, 480-506. 7 Audiotactile cross-modal links in endogenous spatial attention While there is now consistent evidence for the existence of crossmodal links in spatial attention between vision and audition, and between vision and touch, links between audition and touch have been scarcely investigated, and contrasting findings have emerged in the few studies conducted to date (Lloyd et al., 2003; Eimer et al., 2001; 2002; Hötting et al. 2004). One reason why crossmodal interactions in spatial attention between audition and touch might be weaker than interactions between vision and other sensory modalities is that visual information may have a privileged role in mediating spatial perception and multimodal sensory integration. The aim of this project is to find new evidence for auditory-tactile crossmodal links, and to investigate the possible roles of visual information and body posture for such links. References: Eimer M, Cockburn D, Smedley B, Driver J. Cross-modal links in endogenous spatial attention are mediated by common external locations: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Exp Brain Res 2001;139:398-411. Eimer M, Van Velzen J, Driver J. Cross-modal interactions between audition, touch, and vision in endogenous spatial attention: ERP evidence on preparatory states and sensory modulations. J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14:254-271. Hötting K, Rösler F, Röder B. Crossmodal and intermodal attention modulates event-related brain potentials to tactile and auditory stimuli. Exp Brain Res 2003;148:26–37. Lloyd DM, Merat N, McGlone F, Spence C. Crossmodal links between audition and touch in covert endogenous spatial attention. Percept Psychophys 2003;65:901–924. The effects of gaze and covert attention on tactile processing When we direct our gaze to one of our hands (even when vision of the hand is prevented), the processing of tactile stimuli presented to the gazed hand is enhanced (Forster & Eimer, 2005). The effect of gaze on tactile processing is very similar to the ERP modulations of tactile stimuli that are usually found when participants covertly attend one of their hands while maintaining their gaze on a central fixation point. Furthermore, responses to tactile stimuli presented to the gazed hand are faster than those to the same tactile stimuli when presented to the other non gazed hand (c.f. Driver & Grossenbacher, 1996; Honoré, Bourdeaud’hui & Sparrow, 1989). The aim of this project is to systematically investigate the nature of this gaze effect and its links with covert tactile attention. Is the effect of gaze on tactile processing independent from endogenous attention? What happens when gaze and attention are simultaneously directed to different hands? Is the effect of gaze dependent on the availability of visual information (what happens when the hand are not visible?). References: Driver, J., & Grossenbacher, P.J., 1996. Multimodal constraints on tactile selective attention. In T. Inui & J.L. McClelland (Eds.) Attention and Performance XVI (pp 209-235). Cambridge MA: MIT Press. Forster, B., & Eimer, M., 2005. Vision and gaze direction modulate tactile processing in somatosensory cortex: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Experimental Brain Research, 165: 8-18. Honoré, J., Bourdeaud’hui, M., & Sparrow, L., 1989. Reduction of cutaneous reaction time by direction eyes towards the source of stimulation. Neuropsychologia, 27, 367-371. DR KARRI GILLESPIE-SMITH Email: kgillespie-smith@abdn.ac.uk Exploring self-referential memory effects through ownership in children. A robust feature of adult cognition is the ‘self-reference effect’ (SRE) in memory (Rogers, Kuiper, & Kirker, 1977; Symons & Johnston, 1997). The SRE is the memory advantage for information encoded in relation to the self over material encoded about others. The scarcity of evidence for self-reference effects in early childhood may be due to the tendency for developmental researchers to focus on the 8 standard adult SRE paradigm (see Symons & Johnson, 1997). The current inquiry will examine the SRE in a child population (aged 3-4 years) by applying an ownership paradigm, (ie. encoding objects in the context of self-ownership). This study will also explore if there are associations between theory of mind abilities and the SRE in children. References: Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 371-394. Sui, J., & Zhu, Y. (2005). Five-year-olds can show the self-reference advantage. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 382-387. Ross, J., Anderson, J.R., & Campbell, R.N. (2011). I remember me: Investigating mnemonic selfreference effects in preschool children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76, 1-102. Does familiarity impact on recognition of facial expression? There is evidence within face perception literature, that processing emotion and identity of faces is interdependent. For example, Kaufmann & Schweinberger (2004) showed participants familiar and unfamiliar faces expressing different emotions and asked them to make a familiarity decision. Familiar faces were recognised quicker when they showed a happy expression. However some theorists propose that this interdependent relationship may not exist in both directions. This topic will explore what happens to expression recognition when a face is familiar. Does face familiarity impact on how quickly and accurately facial expressions are recognised? References: Calder A., J, & Young A., W. (2005). Understanding facial identity and facial expression recognition, Nature Neuroscience Reviews, 6, (8), 641-653 Hole, G., & Bourne, V. (2010). Face processing. Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Chapter 1 & 3). Does visual working memory impact on time spent averting gaze during tasks of high cognitive load? Faces present a lot of information, such as configural information, eye gaze, facial expressions, identity etc. This information must carry a high cognitive load (Doherty-Sneddon and Phelps 2005). When dealing with tasks that demand a high cognitive load, it has been proposed that attention to faces must be avoided or reduced so that more cognitive resources can be used to process the task in hand. This is known as the Cognitive Load Hypothesis of gaze aversion (Glenberg 1997). By averting their gaze and not fixating on faces people may be able to use additional cognitive resources for the task in hand and therefore improve task performance. However does this apply for all individuals? Individuals with greater working memory abilities specifically visuo-spatial memory may be able to process higher amounts of configural information, and therefore not need to avoid faces during demanding tasks. This study will explore if individuals with high visual memory abilities show reduced gaze aversion during demanding cognitive tasks. References: Doherty-Sneddon, G., Bruce, V., Bonner, L., Longbotham, S., and Doyle, C. (2002). Development of gaze aversion as disengagement from visual information. Developmental Psychology, 38, 438-445. Doherty-sneddon, G., and Phelps, F. G. (2005). Gaze aversion: A response to cognitive or social difficulty. Memory and Cognition, 33, (4), 727-733. 9 Glenberg, A. M., Schroeder, J. L., and Robertson, D. A. (1998). Averting the gaze disengages the environment and facilitates remembering. Memory and Cognition, 26, (2), 651-658. DR WENDY JOHNSON Email: wjohnson@staffmail.ed.ac.uk I am prepared to supervise Final Honours projects investigating individual differences in intelligence, achievement, personality, and health developmental processes throughout the lifespan, particularly if they involve consideration of genetic and environmental influences Interested students should contact me to discuss project ideas and design. I can be especially helpful in suggesting methodology. For conceptual background reading, see: References Johnson, W. (2007). Genetic and environmental influences on behavior: Capturing all the interplay. Psychological Review, 114, 423-440. DR PETER LAMONT Email: peter.lamont@ed.ac.uk Constructing Psychology Since the emergence of the discipline, Psychology has been many different things, in terms of its objects of study, and how it studies them. Throughout all this it has claimed to be scientific, though, as the philosophy of science shows, precisely what this means is not clear. This project will use discourse analysis of psychological texts to examine how Psychology has been constructed by psychologists, in order to go beyond simplistic textbook answers to the question ‘What is Psychology?’ References: Richards, G. (2010). Putting psychology in its place. London: Routledge. Lamont, P. (2007). Discourse analysis as method in the history of psychology. History and Philosophy of Psychology, 9(2), 34-44. Belief maintenance: a discursive approach Much research has been carried out into paranormal belief, most of which has depended upon questionnaires as measures of belief, despite the fact that psychologists have identified a number of problems with these. Meanwhile, little work has been done on how beliefs are maintained at a discursive level: how are they expressed and defended in the real world? This project would use discourse analysis of naturally occurring text in order to examine how beliefs are constructed and warranted. References: Wooffitt, R. (1997). Telling tales of the unexpected: the organization of factual discourse. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Lamont, P. (2007). Paranormal belief and the avowal of prior skepticism. Theory and Psychology, 17(5), 681-96. Explaining the unexplained: scepticism about the paranormal Psychologists have written a great deal about belief in the paranormal, yet very little has been written about disbelief. This, despite the fact that disbelief is arguably a minority position, and is problematic in a variety of ways. For example, people are often faced with evidence in favour of paranormal phenomena, yet do not have an alternative explanation. This project would use discourse analysis of naturally occurring text to identify how ‘scepticism’ towards the paranormal is justified. References: Potter, J. (1997). Representing reality: discourse, rhetoric and social construction. London: Sage Publications 10 Lamont, P., Coelho, C., & McKinlay, A. (2009). Explaining the unexplained: justifying disbelief in the paranormal. Discourse Studies, 11(5), 543-559. Debunking and the Psychology of belief Since before the emergence of Psychology as a discipline, psychological scientists have been in the business of debunking (what we now call) psychic phenomena. In the process, they have warranted the need for a scientific Psychology, presented themselves as experts on such matters, and explained the beliefs of others according to their own beliefs. This project would use discourse analysis of psychological texts to examine what has been going on in the process of debunking. References: Coon, D. (1992). Testing the limits of sense and science: American experimental psychologists combat Spiritualism, American Psychologist, 47, 2, 143-151. Lamont, P. (2010). Debunking and the psychology of error: a historical analysis of psychological matters. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 7(1), 34-44. DR BILLY LEE Email: b.lee@ed.ac.uk Below are some examples of dissertation topics. I am happy to discuss other projects to explore experience and behaviour using observational or phenomenological approaches. I am happy to discuss areas that may be unusual, different or less established. Possible areas include experiences of immigration, gender and sexuality, psychotherapy and counselling, and health, well-being and quality of life. 1. Exploring Lived Experience The aim of your project will be to explore and to understand a particular experience of your choosing. You will use IPA as a qualitative method to illuminate the phenomenology of your chosen experience. This method has been used to study health, sexuality, gender, identity etc. IPA is inductive, rather than hypothesis driven. It avoids prior assumptions and tries to get at the experience as it is lived by people and at the meanings they assign to their experiences. Reid, K., Flowers, P., Larkin, M. (2005). "Exploring lived experience." The Psychologist 18 (1): 2023. 2. Nonverbal Communication in Interpersonal Relationships This project investigates interpersonal communication via body posture, gesture, and facial expression. You will make video recordings of pairs or groups of people, partners, friends or strangers, interacting spontaneously or performing a task. The purpose of the project will be to devise ways of understanding and categorizing the behaviour observed. This analysis may be used as a basis for illuminating psychological processes such as attachment, emotional intelligence, or personality disorders. Tucker, J. S. & Anders, S. L. (1998). Adult attachment style and nonverbal closeness in dating couples. Journal of Nonverbal Behaviour, 22, 109-124. PROFESSOR ROBERT LOGIE Email: rlogie@staffmail.ed.ac.uk 1. Impact of the fMRI environment on cognitive function There has been a dramatic growth in the use of brain imaging techniques, such as fMRI to study the neuroanatomical correlates of human cognition. However, very little is known about how the physical environment of the fMRI scanner affects the cognition that is being studied, and whether the way in which participants perform cognitive tasks changes when they are tested within the scanner relative to a standard laboratory. Using a full size simulator of an fMRI scanner, this project will investigate the 11 impact on the cognitive strategies of participants of the fMRI environment relative to testing in a standard behavioural laboratory setting. References Gutchess, M.S. & Park, D.C. (2006). fMRI environment can impair memory performance in young and elderly adults. Brain Research, 1099, 133-140. Henson, R. (2005). What can functional neuroimaging tell the experimental psychologist? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 193-233. Logie, R.H., Venneri, A., Della Sala, S., Redpath, T. & Marshall, I. (2003). Brain activation and the phonological loop: The impact of rehearsal. Brain and Cognition, 53, 293-296. Logothetis, N. (2008). What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature, 453, 12th June, 869-878. Mazard, A., Mazoyer, B., Etard, O., Tzourio- Mazoyer, N., Kosslyn, S.M. & Mellet, E. (2002) Impact of fMRI Acoustic Noise on the Functional Anatomy of Visual Mental Imagery. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14 (2) p172-186 Raz, A, Lieber, B., Soliman, F., Buhle, J., Posner, J., Peterson, B.S. & Posner, M.I. (2005). Ecological nuances in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): psychological stressors, posture, and hydrostatics. NeuroImage 25, 1 – 7 Zeman, A., Della Sala, Torrens, L., Gountouna, E., McGonigle, D. & Logie, R.H. (2010). Loss of imagery phenomenology with intact visual imagery performance. Neuropsychologia, 48, 145-155. 2. Visual imagery and visual perception. Do they overlap? Visual perception involves identifying and recognising objects in the environment, allowing us to interact successfully with the environment, but also to form mental images or representations of what has been perceived. Mental images also can be formed from memories of previous experiences, with no external stimulus input. There is a widely held view that the cognitive functions, and the neural networks involved in visual perception, largely overlap with those used for imagery. An alternative view is that mental imagery involves a cognitive function that deals with the products of perception, but perception and imagery involve separate systems between which there is a flow of information. This project will assess experimentally the extent to which the processes of visual perception and of mental visual imagery might be more distinct than is generally assumed. References Kosslyn, S.M., (1994). Image and Brain. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. Logie, R.H. & van der Meulen, M. (2009). Fragmenting and integrating visuo-spatial working memory. In J.R. Brockmole (Ed.) Representing the Visual World in Memory, pp 1-32. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Saito, S., Logie, R.H., Morita, A. & Law, A. (2008). Visual and phonological similarity effects in verbal immediate serial recall: A test with kanji materials. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 117. Van der Meulen, M., Logie, R.H. & Della Sala, S. (2009). Selective interference with image retention and generation: Evidence for the workspace model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1568-1580. DOI: 10.1080/17470210802483800 3. Remembering to do things: Prospective Memory Every memory function relies on remembering to carry out our intentions, whether it is to post a letter, meet a friend, turn up for a tutorial or take medicine. This ability is known as Prospective Memory. How we remember intentions at the appropriate time is not entirely understood, and it is known to decrease with age with laboratory tasks but not when tested in the home environment. This project 12 would examine prospective memory in real and laboratory simulated prospective memory tasks, and could focus on healthy young adults, or compare different adult age groups. References Bailey, P.E, Henry, J.D., Rendell, P.G., Phillips, L.H., & Kliegel, M. (2010). Dismantling the “age– prospective memory paradox”: The classic laboratory paradigm simulated in a naturalistic setting. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Craik, F.I., & Bialystok, E. (2006). Planning and task management in older adults: cooking breakfast. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1236-1249. Law, A., Logie, R.H. & Pearson, D.G. (2006). The impact of secondary tasks on multitasking in a virtual environment. Acta Psychologica, 122, 27-44. Logie, R.H., Law, A.S., Trawley, S. & Nissan, J. (2010). Multitasking, working memory and remembering intentions. Psychologica Belgica, 50, 309-326. NB - This journal is not in the University Library. If students who are interested in this topic would like a copy of this paper, then please contact Professor Logie. Logie, R.H. & Maylor, E.A. (2009). An internet study of prospective memory across adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 24, 767–774 4. Doing two, or three or more things at once Most people can walk and talk at the same time, but would have problems trying to hold a conversation while reading. One theoretical assumption (e.g. Barrouillet et al Cowan, 2005) is that we have available a single, general purpose attention system that is of limited capacity. According to this assumption, increasing the difficulty of a task should stretch this capacity to its limits and result in a breakdown in performance. Adding a second task should have an even greater effect on task performance. An alternative view (Baddeley & Logie, 1999; Logie & van der Meulen, 2009) is that we have different capacities available, and so if two tasks each use different cognitive abilities, then doing two tasks at once should be no more difficult than doing only one task at a time. References Baddeley, A.D. & Logie, R.H. (1999). Working memory: The multiple component model. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (eds.) Models of Working Memory, pp28-61. New York: Cambridge University Press. Barrouillet, P., Bernardin, S. & Camos, V. (2004) Time constraints and resource sharing in adult’s working memory spans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 83-100. Barrouillet, P. & Camos, V. (2007) The time-based resource-sharing model of working memory. In N. Osaka, R. H. Logie & M. D’Esposito (Eds) The cognitive neuroscience of working memory (pp 5980). New York: Oxford University Press Inc. Cowan, N. (2005) Working memory capacity. Hove, East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press. Logie, R.H., Cocchini, G., Della Sala, S. & Baddeley, A.D. (2004). Is there a specific executive capacity for dual task co-ordination? Evidence from Alzheimer’s Disease. Neuropsychology, 18, 504513. Logie, R.H., Della Sala, S., MacPherson, S. & Cooper, J. (2007). Dual task demands on encoding and retrieval processes: Evidence from healthy adult ageing. Cortex, 43, 159-169. Logie, R.H. & Duff, S.C. (2007). Separating processing from storage in working memory operation span. In N. Osaka, R.H. Logie, & M. D’Esposito (Eds.) The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, pp 119-135. 13 DR SARAH MACPHERSON Email: sarah.macpherson@ed.ac.uk My research interests are frontal lobe functions such as memory, executive abilities and social cognition and how they are affected by healthy adult ageing and brain damage. I am happy to discuss the supervision of other potential projects related to my research, in addition to the dissertation projects outlined below. 1. The effect of healthy adult ageing on multi-tasking There are a number of reports in the literature of patients with frontal lobe damage who are impaired in their ability to organise and structure goal-related behaviours. In everyday life, examples of such situations are those involving “multi-tasking” such as planning a dinner party or going shopping. However, despite the evidence that age effects are found on cognitive tasks sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction, little is known about the effect of healthy adult ageing on multi-tasking tasks tapping real-world executive skills. This project will investigate the effects of age on a well-known test of multitasking and more traditional tests of executive function sensitive to frontal lobe damage. Burgess, P.W., Veitch, E., de Lacy Costello, A. & Shallice, T. (2000). The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychologia, 38, 848-863. Garden, S.E., Phillips, L.H. & MacPherson, S.E. (2001). Midlife aging, open-ended planning, and laboratory measures of executive function. Neuropsychology, 15, 472-482. 2. Social cognition in later life In everyday social interactions, individuals must be able to respond to the mental states of others. This ability to attribute beliefs and desires to others in order to predict and explain behaviour is referred to as theory of mind. Recent studies have found conflicting findings as to whether there are age-related effects on theory of mind abilities. Yet, the tasks adopted often do not resemble real world situations as they are in the form of stories or cartoons rather than video clips. In this project, you will examine the effects of healthy adult ageing on more ecologically valid theory of mind type tasks. Slessor, G., Phillips, L., & Bull, R. (2007). Exploring the specificity of age related differences in theory of mind tasks. Psychology and Aging, 22, 639-643. Sullivan, S., & Ruffman, T. (2004). Social understanding: How does it fare with advancing years? British Journal of Psychology, 95, 1-18. 3. Emotional processing and healthy adult ageing The predominant finding in the emotion literature is that there is age-related decline in the ability to recognise certain negative emotions. It is argued that these age effects are due to specific neuropsychological change in the social brain. The ability to process emotions is thought to be critical for communication with others and yet older adults do not appear to demonstrate problems in their everyday social interactions. This project will investigate younger and older adults’ ability to recognise emotions in more real-life situations. Ruffman, T., Henry, J.D., Livingstone, V., & Phillips, L.H. (2008). A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: implications for neuropsychological models of aging. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 863-881. DR ANDREA MARTIN Email: amarti12@staffmail.ed.ac.uk I am interested in how memory processes and linguistic representations interact and make language use in real time possible. I am especially interested in the comprehension of sentences. I would be 14 happy to supervise any project related to sentence processing or to memory and language; here are two projects to consider: 1. Different types of cues to linguistic memory representations Often during language comprehension, speakers and listeners need to access recently processed information in memory in order to understand the linguistic input that is coming into the system in the current moment. What types of information or “cues” are used to retrieve the right information from memory during sentence comprehension? This project will focus on the role of grammatical agreement in forming long-distance linguistic dependencies. Lewis, R.L., Vasishth, S., Van Dyke, J., 2006. Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends Cognitive Sci. 10, 447–454. Martin, A. E., Nieuwland, M. S., & Carreiras, M. (2012). Event-related brain potentials index cuebased retrieval interference during sentence comprehension. NeuroImage, 59, 1859-1869. McElree, B., 2006. Accessing recent events. In: Ross, B.H. (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 46. Academic Press, San Diego. 2. Combining word-level and sentence-level information during reading During silent reading, several factors are known to affect how long it takes for a word or phrase to be read, an index of processing difficulty. However, how basic factors like lexical frequency and contextual constraint combine during online processing, and what their relative order is in function and time, is just beginning to be explored. This project will try to gain insight about the architecture of the human language processing system and its interface with the acquired skill of reading. Staub, A. (2011). Word recognition and syntactic attachment in reading: Evidence for a staged architecture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 407-433. Van Petten, C. & Kutas, M. (1990). Interactions between sentence context and word frequency in event-related brain potentials. Memory & Cognition, 18, 380-393. DR ROB McINTOSH Email: r.d.mcintosh@ed.ac.uk In addition to the following topics, I am happy to discuss students’ ideas for any projects related to motor behaviour, visual attention, or any of the ongoing projects in the Visuomotor Lab (www.psy.ed.ac.uk/people/rmcinto1/VMLprojects.html). Attention and action At any time, numerous potential actions may be available to us, but we cannot execute more than a tiny fraction of these. Coherent action is thus inherently selective. When our actions are visually guided, this means we must focus visual attention on the target object. This is directly analogous to the visual attention that we give to objects prioritised for perceptual goals, such as object recognition. A key question in contemporary cognitive science is whether the selection of visual information for perception and for action are achieved by the same or by separable attention mechanisms. This project will address this question using motion tracking and eye-tracking technologies. Smith DT & Schenk T (2012). The Premotor theory of attention: time to move on? Neuropsychologia http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.025, Cognition and action Our moment-to-moment actions proceed mostly without much thought, but certain aspects seem to require more cognitive effort. Creem & Proffitt (2001) showed that, while distracted by a difficult mental task, people will do daft things like picking up spoons by their bowl-end rather than by their handle. At the same time, the execution of those actions remained very skilful. This suggests that deciding how to pick something up may take cognitive effort, but carrying out the action once the decision has been made is more automatic. This project will extend this method to study the ‘endstate-comfort’ phenomenon, in which people choose actions that minimise postural awkwardness. We 15 would predict that this kind of action selection, like tool grasping, should require cognitive involvement, resulting in poor choices of grasp posture under dual-task conditions, but that the quality of the grasp itself will not be affected. Other aspects of action selection and guidance could also be investigated. Creem SH, Proffitt DR (2001). Grasping Objects by Their Handles: A Necessary Interaction Between Cognition and Action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27: 218-228. Physical constraints on mental imagery When we imagine moving, many of the same motor systems in our brains are active as when we make real movements. One way to probe motor imagery is to ask people to make laterality (left or right) judgements about pictures of hands and feet. People solve this task by imagining moving their own limbs to match the picture. One source of evidence for this is that the speed of laterality judgement is strongly influenced by how closely the person’s current posture matches that of the hand in the picture. This project will follow up on recent student projects in the visuomotor lab, investigating in more detail how people make laterality judgements about hands and/or feet. Parsons L (1994). Temporal and Kinematic Properties of Motor Behavior Reflected in Mentally Simulated Action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance. 20: 709730. Mirror-writing Mirror-writing, the writing of letters or words in mirror-reversed form, can arise spontaneously after brain damage, is common amongst children learning to write, and is intentionally practiced by some rare individuals. This intriguing phenomenon has received relatively little neuropsychological investigation. This project will investigate mirror-writing amongst children learning to write, or in adults deliberately attempting to mirror-write. You can read more about the phenomenon in a forthcoming review article, linked below: McIntosh RD & Della Sala S (in press). Mirror writing: spontaneous, involuntary, and deliberate. The Pssychologist. http://dl.dropbox.com/u/20090414/McIntosh_Della_Sala_MW.pdf DR ALEXA MORCOM Email: alexa.morcom@ed.ac.uk My main research interest is in memory: basic memory mechanisms, and how memory changes as we age. For more details see my website: http://www.ccns.sbms.mvm.ed.ac.uk/people/academic/morcom.html I am happy to discuss the supervision of other potential projects related to my research, as well as those outlined below. Student input into the project topic is encouraged. In both areas of interest, projects are possible examining how memory changes in ageing, or investigating the basic mechanisms of episodic memory in young adults. Ageing projects are more challenging because of the involvement of two groups. Basic mechanisms can be pilots for ageing studies, and are critical to build an understanding of the memory processes that may change in ageing. Good combinations are for 2 study partners to work together on 1 basic mechanisms project and 1 ageing project, or 2 related ageing projects, sharing data collection. Please come and see me if you are interested in doing a project with me. Topic 1. Episodic memory and episodic memory in ageing Conscious long-term memory for specific events is one of the abilities most affected by ageing. Older adult’ episodic memory is characterised by problems remembering details, context, and associations 16 between items (2-5). Events are less likely to be recollected, and a non-specific feeling of familiarity often dominates (2). Importantly, older people are prone not only to failing to remember unique events, but also to false memories, that is, mistakenly ‘remembering’ events that did not happen. We are investigating this using tests of memory for similar-appearing objects, tests of memory for words linked by meaning or by perceptual details, and tests of memory for unfamiliar faces. General references 1. Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C 2007. The medial temporal lobes and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007. 30:123–52. 2. Luo L, Craik FIM. 2008. Aging and memory: a cognitive approach. Can J Psychiatry. 53(6):34653. Current themes include: A. Ageing and false memory Why do older adults mistake new items for old ones? Is it due to difficulties with cognitive control, linked to frontal lobe damage? Or is it because of impairments in the encoding of detailed memories, linked to hippocampal damage? Some research supports the control theory: older adults are more prone to forming general or ‘gist-based’ memories, and may be more easily misled by semantic information (2). But other research also suggests that in older adults, the hippocampus is less able to ‘pattern-separate’, leading to difficulties in encoding distinct memories of different events (3). Work is ongoing investigating the contribution of these two factors and the critical role that meaning may play. 3. 4. Koutstaal W. 2006. Flexible remembering. Psychon Bull Rev. 13(1):84-91. Yassa MA, Lacy JW, Stark SM, Albert MS, Gallagher M, Stark CE. 2010. Pattern separation deficits associated with increased hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus activity in nondemented older adults. Hippocampus. [Epub ahead of print] B. ‘Environmental support’ of older adults’ memory I am also interested in how ‘environmental support’ for memory encoding can help older adults’ memory. Providing strategic support does not always reduce age-related impairments (2,5,6), but providing pre-existing meaningful links may help (5,6). The potential for support for older adults’ memory has important practical implications, but also provides opportunities to evaluate the extent to which older adults’ memory difficulties stem from problems with memory control. This project mainly involves associative memory tasks and comparing young and older adults. Castel, A. D. 2005. Memory for grocery prices in younger and older adults: the role of schematic support. Psychol Aging. 20: 718-721. 6. Naveh-Benjamin M, Hussain Z, Guez J, Bar-On M. 2003. Adult age differences in episodic memory: further support for an associative-deficit hypothesis. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 29(5), 826-837 5. C. Retrieval search and strategy in ageing A little studied aspect of episodic memory is the search process – how do people constrain memory search when they are seeking particular information? Are such strategic processes impaired in ageing? Some evidence suggests that older adults do not process items for meaning when they are trying to remember items processed for meaning, whereas young people do – and this is helpful to memory (2). The suggestion is that failure to engage a controlled search may explain the failure to recollect (7). Basic mechanisms of this need exploring in young adults, too. 7. Jacoby LL, Shimizu Y, Velanova K, Rhodes MG. 2005. Age differences in depth of retrieval: Memory for foils. J Mem Lang. 52:493-504. Topic 2. Procedural memory and procedural memory in ageing Procedural learning and memory involves learning and expressing knowledge about regularities in the environment without necessarily having explicit (declarative) knowledge of what is learned. Recent neuroimaging studies have reawakened debate about whether these automatic aspects of learning 17 change in ageing, and posed new questions about whether they are really independent (e.g. 8). But simple behavioural experiments can be very informative too. A. Learning in an uncertain world An alternative approach to assessing people’s procedural learning of a fixed ‘hidden’ sequence is a more ‘lifelike’ probabilistic approach in which the dependencies of one event on another change over time. fMRI data suggest that the hippocampus may be sensitive to variations in uncertainty about a sequential ‘implicit’ pattern (8). A basic mechanisms project would extend (8) behaviourally to determine how uncertainty is linked to procedural learning in young adults. An ageing project would use the same paradigm to explore the novel question of how uncertainty affects learning in older adults (see also 9). The task is a simple serial reaction time task. (For a student with good computational skills, the simple Bayesian learning model in (8) can be applied). Harrison LM, Duggins A, Friston KJ. 2006. Encoding uncertainty in the hippocampus. Neural Netw.19(5):535-46. B. Does it help to ‘try’ to learn? Procedural learning is traditionally regarded as independent of declarative, including episodic, memory. However, some studies suggest that different memory ‘systems’ may sometimes interact and/or compete, particularly in older adults with reduced processing resources. The evidence so far is conflicting (9,10). 8. Howard DV, Howard JH Jr. 2001. When it does hurt to try: adult age differences in the effects of instructions on implicit pattern learning. Psychon Bull Rev. 8:798-805. 10. Song S, Marks B, Howard JH, Howard DV. 2009. Evidence for parallel explicit and implicit sequence learning systems in older adults. Behav Brain Res. 196:328-322. 9. Topic 3. Scene Semantics in Visual Long Term Memory This project is supervised by Dr. Morcom but is part of a collaboration with Dr. George Malcolm at George Washington University, USA. Humans have a remarkable ability to remember scene images stored in visual long-term memory (VLTM), even when such images were only presented for a brief amount of time. Standing (12) found that after being presented with 10,000 scene images, participants recalled around ~80% correctly. One of the keys to this high performance may be how scenes are processed as members of categories. Konkle et al. (11) found evidence that VLTM representations go beyond simply remembering the category a scene belonged to (e.g., an image of a city). Scenes can be labelled at different levels of category specificity, such as the superordinate (e.g., outdoors), basic (city) and subordinate (Paris) levels (12). The proposed study seeks to further explore the extent of the semantic detail stored in VLTM, and how this impacts on episodic (recognition) memory. Depending on interest, ageing effects may be investigated – this ties in with work on false memory (topic 1A). 11. Konkle T, Brady TF, Alvarez GA, Oliva A. 2011. Scene memory is more detailed than you think: 12. 13. the role of categories in visual long-term memory. Psychol Science. 21: 1551-1556. Standing L. 1973. Learning 10,000 pictures. Q J Exp Psychol. 25, 207–222. Tversky B, Hemenway K. 1983. Categories of environmental scenes. Cognit Psychol. 15, 121– 149. A note on neuroimaging Because of the complexity of the procedures and data analysis involved, I do not usually supervise dissertation projects that use neuroimaging methods (fMRI/ ERPs). However, I will consider it on a case by case basis depending on funding and the student’s expertise (an early start would be essential, as well as previous experience and/or a particular aptitude for data processing and analysis, and excellent computer skills). 18 DR LARS PENKE Email: lars.penke@ed.ac.uk Research interests Evolutionary significance of individual differences, especially of general intelligence, personality traits, and attractiveness; genetics; cognitive aging, especially in relation to the brain’s white matter integrity; evolutionary psychology of human mate choice and mating behaviour. Projects Anthropometric correlates of interpersonal personality traits It has been hypothesised that one source of individual differences in interpersonal personality traits (like extraversion, agreeableness, narcissism, shyness or social dominance) is getting used to how other people tend to reaction towards oneself. So someone might have a tendency to act more aggressive because others are usually intimidated by his strength, or more outgoing because others tend to react positively to her beauty. If personality traits are really influenced by these physical attributes (as opposed to related behaviours), then we should be able to find correlations between anthropometric measures of body and face shape that relate to attractiveness, muscularity etc. The Body Lab allows us to quickly capture the body and face of participants with 3D scanners in great detail. Anthropometric measures derived from these scanners can be related to measures of personality. I can supervise students who want to work with me on projects in any of these or related domains. Due to the nature of the data confidence in statistical skills is an advantage. References: Brown,W. M., Price, M. E., Kang, J., Pound, N., Zhao, Y., & Yu, H. (2008). Fluctuating asymmetry and preferences for sex-typical bodily characteristics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 105, 12938–12943. Lukaszewski, A. W., & Roney, J. R. (2011). The origins of extraversion: Joint effects of facultative calibration and genetic polymorphism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 409–421. Price, M., Dunn, J., Hopkins, S., & Kang, J. (2012). Anthropometric correlates of human anger. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 33, 174-181. Stirrat, M., & Perrett, D. I. (2010). Valid facial cues to cooperation and trust: Male facial width and trustworthiness. Psychological Science, 21, 349-354. Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009). Formidability and the logic of human anger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 15073-15078. PROFESSOR MARTIN PICKERING Email: martin.pickering@ed.ac.uk 1. Cross-linguistic structural priming and bilingualism Bilinguals might store their two languages separately. However, they might find it economical to share information where possible. Recent structural priming studies suggest that some grammatical information can be shared, at least when the two languages form a particular grammatical structure in a similar way (Hartsuiker et al., 2004). In this project, you would investigate this issue by testing structural priming between any pair of languages (with one normally being English). At least one student should have good knowledge of the languages to be studied, and you would also need access to 20-30 participants. Reference Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish/English bilinguals. Psychological Science, 15, 409-414. 2. Exploring the shared Stroop effect 19 When two people "share" certain tasks, their performance interferes with each other, in a way suggesting that they co-represent the task (Sebanz et al., 2003). Our pilot studies have shown similar effects in language, using a shared Stroop task. Two participants sit side-by-side and respond to different colours, and we show that their assumptions about each other's potential responses affects their own responses. This project would apply this technique to explore the mechanisms underlying the social nature of language use. References Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others’ actions: Just like one’s own? Cognition, 88, B11–B21. 3. Language as joint action To be successful at a joint activity (such as playing a duet or using a two-handed saw), both participants need to coordinate their behaviours. Dialogue is a form of joint activity using language and interlocutors are remarkably good at it. We would investigate joint language use by "splitting" tasks between two participants. For example, one participant might describe the first part of an event (e.g., "the boy") and the other participant might complete it (e.g., "is kicking the ball"). By comparing joint and individual language use, we expect to determine the extent to which speakers represent what they think their partner is likely to say, and thus create a kind of "shared reality" for language use. For some background see: Garrod, S., & Pickering, M.J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 8-11. 4. Social manipulations of audience design Speakers clearly produce their utterances with particular addressees in mind, for example using simpler language when addressing non-experts than experts in a particular domain (Isaacs & Clark, 1987). But can they be non-consciously primed to be more or less helpful? In this study we apply techniques from social psychology to this aspect of the psychology of language. References Isaacs, E. A., & Clark, H. H. (1987). References in conversations between experts and novices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 26-37. DR RICHARD SHILLCOCK Email: r.shillcock@ed.ac.uk www: http://tinyurl.com/r-shillcock Projects in the area of reading, vision, cognitive modelling, language representation and processing. Students interested in carrying out a project in these areas should contact me to discuss the possibilities. Specific possibilities are: One or two eyes in reading We normally read with both eyes open, although there are all sorts of “noisy” oculomotor behaviours in reading. Psychologists typically undervalue such real-world aspects of reading in favour of coming up with “simple” theories and models of reading based on idealized views of the processing that happens. We will run a visual lexical decision experiment using a set of six-letter words whose processing we have studied before (using a different technique, but it means we do know some of the important dimensions of their processing). There will be two conditions used – one eye or both eyes open. We will be interested in how much the additional eye improves the speed and accuracy of visual lexical decision, and how that improvement interacts with what we know about the location of important information within the words. The student will learn to run a visual lexical decision experiment in which response times are collected, as well as other simple data (sex, handedness, eye preference) from 20 native-English speaking participants. The student will collaborate in analyzing the data to reveal the effects on word recognition. The student will explore the implications of the data for current models of word recognition. 20 Useful reading: Obregón, M. & Shillcock, R.C. (being revised). Foveational complexity in single word identification: Contralateral pathways are advantaged over ipsilateral pathways. Neurologia. Available on request. Computational modelling with TRACE The student will learn how to run the TRACE model of auditory word recognition, presenting it with idealized input (textual representations of auditory words) and recording the output from the model. TRACE is one of the most influential cognitive models in speech perception research and is still being actively researched. No computational background or programming skills are necessary, but if a student has such skills, then it will be possible to use them in testing rather different hypotheses about the model’s behaviour. The project will involve (a) coming up to speed in a qualitative understanding of how TRACE works (see the reading, below); (b) learning to run a version of TRACE on the student’s own laptop and/or on a Psychology computer; (c) understanding some of the literature on how TRACE has been used to theorize about auditory word recognition and segmentation; (d) collaborating on devising some hypotheses to test using TRACE; (e) constructing stimulus sets (i.e. list of words or nonwords that TRACE can “recognize”) that address those hypotheses; (f) running the simulations, recording and presenting the data. (There will be a premium on the careful and consistent graphical representation of the data.). Discussing the implications for spoken word recognition. It may be possible to use a new development of TRACE being worked on by RCS currently. Our default will be the standard version. The goal will be to understand more about how words are segmented from speech and identified, and to express this in computational terms. Useful reading: McClelland, J., & Elman, J. (1986a).The trace model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1–86. Or there is a book chapter in: McClelland, J.L. & Rumelhart, D.E. (1988). Explorations in Parallel Distributed Processing. MIT Press (1988). Understanding spoken word recognition from a new perspective This project would suit a student with an interest in spoken word recognition and how we theorize about it. The goal is to cash out a novel conceptual approach to theorizing about language use. An existing transcription of real speech (probably The London-Lund Corpus) will be used as data. This is real speech has been transcribed into words; an idealized phonological representation has been produced from it. The student will use these data, existing on a computer, along with an existing computerized pronunciation dictionary, to explore the structure of spoken English. In brief, the goal is to explore an alternative concept of “universal” – something that unifies many things. Instead of exclusively using abstract universals like “noun”, “prefix”, “syllable”, we will look at the implications of also using a concrete universal. The sound associated with the vowel schwa constitutes some 10% of normal English speech (e.g at the end of “panda”). We will assess quantitatively how it connects to every other type of representation in spoken English (phonological, morphological, syllabic, prosodic, lexical, syntactic, and possibly semantic). This will involve using the computerized corpus of speech and a small range of Unix tools to count things about the occurrence of schwa. The goal of the project is to end up with a new view of the structure of spoken English and how it may be approached by the developing child. 21 Useful reading: Shillcock, R.C. (submitted). The schwa sound, materialist abstraction, the concrete universal, and the study of language use. (Available on request) DR PATRICK STURT Email: patrick.sturt@ed.ac.uk In addition to the project proposals below, I am happy to supervise any other projects in the area of language comprehension, as long as I agree that the project is feasible. The cognition of structure in language and other domains of cognition (3 projects) Much of the information that we process in everyday life has an underlying abstract structure, in which a larger object (for example, a sentence, a mathematical expression, a musical phrase) can be made up of smaller parts, which may themselves be composed of smaller parts, etc. Recent work using a priming methodology has investigated the representation of structure in mathematics and language. This work shows that when people solve an equation with a particular structure, they subsequently show an increased likelihood of producing a sentence that has a similar structure (Scheepers et al, 2011). This suggests that the structures of mathematical and linguistic expressions have a common mental representation at some level. I would be happy to supervise up to three projects that would look into relations between linguistic cognition and other areas. Possible topics include looking at whether linguistic/mathematical priming is bi-directional (e.g. whether understanding a sentence with a particular structure can subsequently influence the way a mathematical expression is processed), or using eye-tracking to look for commonalities in the way mathematical and linguistic expressions are processed in real time. It is also possible to look at musical cognition and its relation to language (knowledge of musical theory would be useful in this case). Students are encouraged to discuss possible projects with me. Reference: Scheepers, C., Sturt, P., Martin, C., Myachykov, A., Teevan, K., and Viskupova, I. (2011). Structural priming across cognitive domains: From simple arithmetic to relative clause attachment. Psychological Science, 22:1319–1326. The time-course of dependency formation during language comprehension (1 project) Research on human syntactic processing has typically focused on rather simple dependencies such as verb-argument relations. However, linguistic theory shows that natural language syntax involves a wide range of different types of dependency, most of which have received very little attention from the processing point of view. This study will involve using reading time measures to study the time-course with which dependencies such as control, raising, binding, or unbounded dependencies are established. The project would be particularly suitable for a student who has done linguistics as an outside subject. Reference: Sturt, P. (2003). The time-course of the application of binding constraints in reference resolution Journal of Memory and Language 48, 542-562 DR CAROLINE WATT Email: caroline.watt@ed.ac.uk I will be supervising up to 4 students on two projects addressing the psychology of precognitive dream experiences. It has been proposed that some 'paranormal' experiences may actually arise through the processing of implicit information (Crawley, French & Yesson, 2002). The two projects will correlate belief & experience of precognitive dreams with performance on implicit awareness/learning tasks, and with associated psychological variables (e.g., transliminality and boundary thinness). 22 Project 1 will use the ‘flicker paradigm’ (Rensink, 2004), in which alternating versions of original and slightly changed pictures will be shown in rapid succession. The participants’ task is to press a button to indicate when they can sense that something about the picture has changed, and to press the button again when they can identify what it is that has changed. Project 2 will use an implicit learning task, in which participants will be presented with a sequence (e.g., of playing cards) that is associated with an outcome (e.g., reward or penalty). Through experience they may learn the 'rule' that links the sequence with the outcome, and we will measure their performance at this task. Main Hypothesis: those who believe they experience precognitive dreams will be more able to sense the change in the pictorial stimuli, and will be quicker to learn the implicit rule, compared to those who do not believe that they experience precognitive dreams. References: Crawley, SE, French, C. C., & Yesson, S. A. (2002). Evidence for transliminality from a subliminal card guessing task. Perception, 31, 887-892. Rawlings, D. (2001). An exploratory factor analysis of Hartmann's Boundary Questionnaire and an empirically-derived short version. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 21, 131-144. Rensink, D. A. (2004). Visual sensing without seeing. Psychological Science, 15, 27-32. Lange, R., Thalbourne, M. A., Houran, J., & Storm, L. (2000). The Revised Transliminality Scale: Reliability and validity data from a Rasch top-down purification procedure. Consciousness and Cognition, 9, 591-617. DR ALEXANDER WEISS Email: alex.weiss@ed.ac.uk Students choosing me as their dissertation supervisor will be required to devise their own dissertation project. I am willing to supervise feasible dissertations that focus on topics such as human or nonhuman animal personality, animal behaviour, evolutionary psychology, health psychology, and aging. Please see me before you decide to select me as it is important that we discuss the potential feasibility of a project or any pitfalls. DR SUE WIDDICOMBE Email: s.widdicombe@ed.ac.uk 1. Meanings of Debt: a discursive psychological study Student loans, economic recession, low interest rates, the availability of credit have all impacted on individual debt. Several studies show that being in debt has effects on the choice of specialism and emigration decisions among junior doctors (Moore, Gale, Dew & Simmers, 2006) and on mental and physical health (Jessop, Herberts & Solomon, 2005), although relations between debt, academic performance and mental health are complex (Ross, Cleland & MacLeod, 2006). However, attitudes towards and perceptions of debt vary, for example, according to materialism (Watson, 2003), family and school influence (Perna, 2008). There is some indication in the literature that how one perceives, accounts for, and understands debt matters (Sussman & Shafir, 2012). But most studies are correlational, and take no account of the likely variability in constructions of what it means to be in debt and accounts of debt. This interview-based study will address this absence. It will conclude by examining the implications of possible variability for the literature. References: Ross, S., Cleland, J. & MacLeod, MJ (2006). Stress, debt and undergraduate medical student performance, Medical Education, 40(6), 584-589. Jessop, D.C., Herberts, C., & Solomon, L. (2005). The impact of financial circumstances on student health, The British Journal of Health Psychology, 10, 421-439. 23 Sussman, AB & Shafir, E. (2012) On assets and debt in the psychology of perceived wealth, Psychological Science, 23(1), 101-108. For an example of how a discursive analysis may allow insight into concept such as debt (or, here, religiosity), see Widdicombe, S. (2011). ‘I am a Believer but not a Conformist’: Negotiating Claims to being Religious among Syrian Christians and Muslims, Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 21(6), 468–483. 2. Community in Practice: becoming a member in an online support group Stommel and Koole examined an online support group on eating disorders, focusing on how new members are accepted or not accepted into the forum community, how this is achieved through their interaction with established members, and how membership norms are negotiated. This project will extend their (2010) work on online support groups, by using discursive psychology and membership categorisation analysis to examine a different online support group in which identity and becoming a member may be relevant. It will discuss the implications of this approach for social psychological studies of group membership and categorisation. References: Stommel, W. & Koole, T. (2010). The online support group as a community: a micro-analysis of the interaction with a new member, Discourse Studies 12, 357-378. See also Smithson, J., Sharkey, SJ, Jones, R., Ford, T., et al. (2011). Membershipand boundary maintenance in an online self-harm forum. Qualitative Health Research, 21(11), 1567-1575. 24