Theory of mind in children with severe speech and physical

Research in Developmental Disabilities 31 (2010) 617–624
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Research in Developmental Disabilities
Theory of mind in children with severe speech and physical impairments
SvenOlof Dahlgren, Annika Dahlgren Sandberg *, Maria Larsson
Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, SE-405 30 Göteborg, Sweden
A R T I C L E I N F O
A B S T R A C T
Article history:
Received 16 November 2009
Accepted 18 December 2009
The development of a person’s ability to understand other’s thoughts and feelings, socalled ‘‘theory of mind’’ (ToM), is subject to study. Children with communicative
disabilities have exhibited problems in this respect, highlighting the role of language in the
development of ToM. In this study, ToM was studied in children with cerebral palsy and
severe speech impairments. Two tasks, differently dependent on verbal abilities, were
used. The results were compared to those of a mental age matched group. The groups
differed significantly on the verbally dependent task while difference in performance did
not reach significance on the less verbally dependent one. The results are discussed in
terms of a delayed development of ToM in children with severe speech and physical
impairments, dependent on verbal abilities.
ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords:
Cerebral palsy
Children
Severe speech and physical impairments
Theory of mind
1. Introduction
Theory of mind, i.e. an understanding that other people (and oneself) have thoughts, desires and beliefs and that these
mental states govern behaviour, is a fundamental ability to fully understand human behaviour and to take part in daily life. It
is essential to understand that people engaged in the same situation as one self can have different mental states in spite of the
same experience. In general, this ability develops by the age of four (Perner, Leekam, & Wimmer, 1987; Surian & Leslie, 1999)
and subsequently at the age of 6–7 an understanding of someone else’s thinking about a third person’s thinking develops.
However the development of theory of mind begins early in the child’s life with precursor skills that includes joint attention,
use of mental states and pretend play (Miller, 2006).
Many researchers have claimed that language plays a special role in the development of theory of mind. One suggestion is
that language influences the development of theory of mind (ToM) through the children’s exposure to talk about mental
states. This suggestion is based on the findings that mothers’ and families’ talk about mental states predicts children’s later
theory of mind performance (Dunn, Brown, Slomkowski, Tesla, & Youngblade, 1991; Ruffman, Slade, & Crowe, 2002), that
siblings promote the children’s development of theory of mind (e.g. Jenkins & Astington, 1996; Peterson, 2000) and that latesigning deaf children performed worse on theory of mind tasks in comparison to native signers (e.g. Meristo et al., 2007;
Woolfe, Want, & Siegal, 2002). The latter finding could be explained by the late signers not having as many opportunities as
native signers or hearing children to learn about mental states through conversation.
Another suggestion is that general language abilities promote the development of theory of mind (de Villiers & de Villiers,
2000). Astington and Jenkins (1999), e.g. found that scores of expressive language ability and receptive scores for syntax
predicted later performance on theory of mind tasks. Farrar and Maag (2002) also found that vocabulary and MLU predicted
performance on theory of mind tasks.
* Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Box 500, SE-405 30 Göteborg, Sweden. Tel.: +46 31 7864273;
fax: +46 31 7864628.
E-mail address: annika.dahlgren.sandberg@psy.gu.se (A. Dahlgren Sandberg).
0891-4222/$ – see front matter ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2009.12.010
618
S.O. Dahlgren et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 31 (2010) 617–624
A combination of linguistic abilities and social competence, i.e. socio-linguistic intelligence, is suggested to be important
(Shatz, 1994) for developing an understanding of the mind both of oneself and others. There are two assumptions behind this
explanation. First, young children compare themselves to others as they develop a sense of self. Second, as language develops
they start to participate in conversations giving verbal expressions to internal states, in line with Siegal, Varley, and Want (2001).
Another account of how children develop a theory of mind assigns a special role to pretend play. There are studies
showing that children who engaged more in pretend play performed better on theory of mind tasks than those who did not
(Lillard, 1994). Leslie (1987) suggested that the same cognitive structures are used in understanding other minds as in
pretend play. Others claim that similar mental representation abilities are engaged in pretend play and false belief, despite
the fact that pretend play and false belief develop at different ages (pretend play, in the second year; false belief, in the fourth
or fifth year; Forguson & Gopnik, 1988; Perner, 1988; Wimmer, Hogrefe, & Sodian, 1988).
There are also suggestions of a link between the capacity to hold in mind and development of theory of mind. Gordon and
Olson (1998) claim that ‘‘the concept of false belief itself is acquired when the child has the computational resources to
represent, that is hold in mind, a previously created representation even when a new representation is created by a new
perceptual situation’’ (1998, p. 81), a view comparable with the earlier findings of Baddeley (1986). There are studies suggesting
that working memory predicts theory of mind competence (Buitelaar, van der Wees, Swaab-Barneveld, & van der Gaag, 1999).
During the last years studies have been published, proposing a close link between executive functions and theory of mind.
There are both theoretical and empirical reasons for this proposal. One reason is the hypothetical association between an
inability to engage in goal-directed activity and pretend play in children with autism (Hughes, 1998a). Another claim is that
ToM tasks pose significant demands on executive functioning (Hughes, 1998a). Carlson and Moses (2001) suggest that their
findings of statistically significant correlations between a number of inhibitory control measures and theory of mind tasks
imply that executive functioning is centrally involved in ToM development. This suggestion is also in line with other findings
(e.g. Hughes, 1998a, 1998b; Hughes, Dunn, & White, 1998; Perner & Lang, 1999).
Due to the proposed importance of the role of language development, pretend play and working memory in the
development of theory of mind, children with cerebral palsy and severe speech impairments are of special interest to the
field. The diagnosis cerebral palsy is a non-specific diagnosis, based on clinical signs, all depending on a non-progressive
damage of the immature brain, before, during or shortly after birth, with motor disabilities. As a consequence (Hagberg &
Hagberg, 1993) the children have a severely restricted capacity for independent pretend play. In addition, the children are
often limited in their productive speech which limits their contribution to discourse in conversational situations (Hjelmquist
& Dahlgren Sandberg, 1996). In her studies of literacy abilities in children with SSPI Dahlgren Sandberg (2001) also found
specific problems with working memory. Taken together, these observations indicate possible problems in the development
of theory of mind in these children.
During the last decade a few studies have been made on children with SSPI and ToM ability. The common finding is that
the children performed worse than children matched for mental age and/or IQ on theory of mind tasks (Dahlgren, Dahlgren
Sandberg, & Hjelmquist, 2003; Falkman, Dahlgren Sandberg, & Hjelmquist, 2005). Dahlgren et al. (2003) suggested that the
poor performance on the theory of mind task could partly be explained by linguistic and communicative skills. They also
questioned the ecological validity of commonly used ToM tasks and argued that performance on false belief tasks might not
predict theory of mind performance in real life. In a longitudinal study of six children with SSPI (Falkman et al., 2005) the
authors found that the children followed a normal pattern of theory of mind development but with a severe delay compared
to children without disability.
However, the role of language abilities in the development of theory of mind abilities in children with SSPI is still unclear.
The conclusions from earlier studies have partly been based on the performance on the classical false belief task, Sally and
Ann (Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985) which requires certain verbal skills. The aim of this study was to investigate
language abilities and short-term memory in relation to performance on the classical false belief task Sally and Ann and on
the ‘‘thought picture’’ measure of theory of mind that minimizes verbal requirements.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Fourteen children with SSPI participated in this study. The mean chronological age was 9;0, ranging from 5;2 to 12;8: 4
boys (mean age 9;1) and 10 girls (mean age 8;11). Their mean nonverbal mental age was 6;1 years, and their mean linguistic
age was 7;7 years. All of the children had cerebral palsy with severe speech and physical impairments. To be included in the
study the children had been judged by a speech and language therapist to be unintelligible by people outside the family. Six
of them had no speech and the other eight were impossible to understand. Twelve of the children used Bliss as their primary
mode of communication and some of them used other graphic media, like pictures or the alphabet. In addition six of the
children tried to vocalize. According to medical information, they all had normal or corrected vision and normal hearing.
Only one of the children was able to move independently and one boy could walk short distances with support. The children
were contacted through local habilitation centres and special schools in the middle and south of Sweden.
A comparison group of 14 typically developing children, nine girls and five boys, with natural speech and without
disabilities was used, matched for mental and linguistic age. According to parents and teachers, all of the children had normal
vision and hearing and there were no indications of deviant language development. The mean chronological age in the
S.O. Dahlgren et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 31 (2010) 617–624
619
Table 1
Mean chronological and mental age, IQ and linguistic age. Children with SSPI and comparison group.
Chronological age
Mean (range)
Mental age
Mean (range)
IQ
Mean (range)
Linguistic age
Mean (range)
SSPI group
(n = 14)
Comparison group
(n = 14)
9;0
(5.2–12.7)
5;7
(4.3–7.6)
6;1
(4.0–9.5)
6;1
(3.0–9.0)
72
(36–169)
106
(57–136)
7;6
(5.0–12.9)
7;4
(4.8–11. 3)
p-Values
.000
1.000
.004
.848
comparison group was 5;8 years, mean mental age 6;1 and mean linguistic age 7;5. They were selected from among children
in schools and preschools in the neighbouring area of Göteborg. Informed consent was obtained from all parents. Group
characteristics are presented in Table 1.
2.2. Materials
Eight tests were used in this study, a nonverbal test of intellectual ability, two tests of theory of mind, two tests of memory
ability and three measures of language ability.
2.2.1. Intellectual ability
To assess mental age the Raven’s matrices (Raven, Raven, & Court, 1998) were used. This measure of nonverbal
intelligence has frequently been used with non-speaking populations and has been judged to be reasonably valid and reliable
in these circumstances. The British norms were used since there is no Swedish standardization.
2.2.2. ToM tests
2.2.2.1. Eva and Anna. Eva and Anna is a Swedish version of the Sally–Anne test (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985) and the procedure
was that of Baron-Cohen et al. (1985). The scoring was 1 point for passing one trial.
2.2.2.2. Thought pictures. The false belief conditions of the ‘‘thought picture’’ test (Woolfe et al., 2002) was used. This task
was constructed in an attempt to minimize the influence of language abilities in demonstrating theory of mind ability. The
child looked at four pictures, one at a time. In each picture the critical object was hidden from the main character by a flap.
The experimenter held the hand over the main character while the child was asked to lift the flap and look at what it revealed.
The child was then presented a new picture where the main character was pictured with a thought bubble. In the bubble
there were four pictures, one that showed what the flap actually hid, one that the main character was expected to think was
behind the flap and two distracters. The child was asked what the main character would think was behind the flap and then
what actually was there. There were four different false belief scenes and each child was presented two of these, randomly
selected among the four. Maximum 2 points could be given. The order of presentation of the pictures was random.
2.2.3. Memory tasks
2.2.3.1. Corsi blocks. The children’s visuo-spatial working memory was tested by means of the Corsi blocks (Milner, 1971). A
version of the original task was prepared, adapted to the children’s fine motor difficulties: a white plate 25 cm 21 cm of size
with nine blue quadrates (3 cm 3 cm) spaced randomly on a white board. The quadrates illustrated the nine blocks in the
original version. The digits 1–9 were printed beside the cubes, and were visible only to the experimenter. The experimenter
pointed at the quadrates in random order, commencing with two and adding one for each sequence the child managed. Each
sequence was repeated twice and the longest sequence repeated back, by pointing, to the experimenter was defined as the
child’s memory score. Both a forward and a backward condition were presented.
2.2.3.2. Digit span. The Digit span subtest from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children-III (Wechsler, 1999) was adapted
for use with the children with CCN. The figures used were printed on a chart and the experimenter pointed at them one at a time
at a pace of approximately one figure per second. The child’s task was to point at the figures in the same order. The longest
sequence repeated back was the child’s memory score. Also here both a forward and a backward conditions were used.
In both memory tasks the children in the comparison group answered by pointing.
2.2.4. Measures of language ability
2.2.4.1. PPVT. A Swedish version of Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (Dunn & Dunn, 1997) was used to get a measure of
vocabulary in Swedish. The American norms were used since there is no Swedish standardization of the PPVT. The resulting
scores were used to derive linguistic age from the norm tables.
620
S.O. Dahlgren et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 31 (2010) 617–624
2.2.4.2. TROG. The Swedish version of TROG (Bishop, 1998; Holmberg & Lundälv, 2002) was used to assess reception of
grammar. Since the Swedish norms were under revision percentage correct of total number items performed was used.
2.2.4.3. Syntactic ability. An adaptation of a test of identification of syntactic correctness constructed by Nauclér and
Magnusson (1994) was also used. It consisted of nine phrases; four with correct syntax and five with syntax intended to
approximate a 2-year-old child’s way of speaking. The phrases were presented as comments made by a mother or her child.
The child’s task was to indicate by pointing at a photograph representing a mother and a little girl in a conversational
situation, which one made the comment. Percentage correct of number tried was computed.
The measures of reception of grammar and of syntactic ability were considered interesting since earlier research has
claimed grammar and syntactic ability to be indicative of theory of mind ability.
2.3. Procedure
The data collection was part of a larger study on literacy and language abilities. All children were tested individually, in
their homes or in a secluded room in their preschools or schools, according to their parents’ preferences. The number of
testings varied from three to five depending on the child’s endurance and lasted approximately 45–60 min per test occasion.
The children were tested by the second and third authors. To reach the best understanding of the child, the child’s personal
assistant or in some cases one of the parents was present during the testing. The children with SSPI were allowed to use their
optimal response mode and in the cases were the children gave their responses by pointing also the typically developing
children used the same response mode. Each task was preceded by practice trials.
2.4. Data analyses
SPSS 15.0 was used for analyses of data. To analyse frequencies the Fisher exact test and x2 were used. Group differences
were also assessed by means of ANOVA. A composite score of theory of mind was computed defined as the sum of the theory
of mind tasks. The relationship among the indicators of theory of mind ability on the one hand and mental age, language
ability and memory indicators on the other was computed by means of the Pearson’s correlation coefficient.
3. Results
The children with cerebral palsy performed statistically significantly worse on the traditional false belief task Sally and
Anne (Fisher’s exact test, p = .020). However, neither in the ‘‘thought picture’’ task (x2 = 2.600, df = 2, p = .273) (Table 2) nor
when using the composite score the difference in performance between groups was statistically significant different
(x2 = 4.961, df = 3, p = .175) (Table 3).
Seven out of 14 of the children with SSPI failed the thought picture task, though, while only three in the comparison group
did. No child in the comparison group failed the Sally and Anne task but five in the SSPI group did.
The only difference between groups where the performance was statistically significantly different was grammatical
competence measured by TROG where the comparison group had a higher result than the children with SSPI
(F(1, 26) = 5.771, p = .024). The linguistic age, measured by PPVT, and syntactic knowledge was almost the same in the two
groups (F(1, 26) = .037, p = .848 and F(1, 26) = .258, p = .616, respectively) (Table 4).
Table 2
Results on theory of mind tasks; ‘‘Sally and Anne’’ and thought pictures. Frequency passers and failers in the SSPI and comparison groups.
Sally and Anne
SSPI group
Comparison group
p-Values
Thought pictures
Pass
Fail
Pass 2 tasks
Pass 1 task
Fail
9
14
5
0
5
7
2
4
7
3
.020
.273
Table 3
Results on theory of mind tasks; composite score. Frequency, passers and failers in the SSPI and comparison groups.
Theory of mind, composite score
SSPI group
Comparison group
p-Value
3
2
1
0
4
7
3
4
3
3
4
0
.175
S.O. Dahlgren et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 31 (2010) 617–624
621
Table 4
Verbal scores. Children with SSPI and comparison group.
SSPI group
Comparison group
p-Value
TROG, percentage correct
Mean (SD)
PPVT, linguistic age
Mean (SD)
Syntactic knowledge percentage correct
Mean (SD)
88.12
(6.95)
86.88
(6.00)
7;6
(2.6)
7;4
(2.2)
70.6
(23.3)
74.6
(17.7)
.618
.843
.616
The only significant difference in memory performance was found in Corsi block, forwards, where the children with SSPI
performed worse (F(1, 22) = 5.896, p = .024) (Table 5). In Digit span the children with SSPI performed equally well as the
children in the comparison group (Digit span total F(1, 24) = .006, p = .941; Digit span forwards, F(1, 24) = .168, p = .685; Digit
span backwards, F(1, 24) = .133, p = .718). In the other Corsi block tasks the children with SSPI performed somewhat worse
than the comparison group but not significantly so (Corsi block total, F(1, 22) = 3.859, p = .062; Corsi block backwards,
F(1, 22) = 2.010, p = .170).
The interrelationships between the performance on Sally and Anne, thought pictures and the composite score on the one
hand and chronological and mental age, IQ and the measures of linguistic abilities and memory on the other were studied by
means of the Pearson’s correlation coefficient within each of the two groups (Table 6). In the group of children with SSPI the
performance on the Sally and Anne task correlated positively with results on the TROG and with measures on memory, i.e.
the total performance on digit span and all measures on Corsi block. The performance on thought pictures correlated
positively with mental age, IQ and linguistic age. The composite score of theory of mind correlated positively with mental
age, IQ, linguistic age and the measure of syntactic knowledge.
Since all children in the comparison group passed the Sally and Anne task no correlations could be computed. However,
the performance on thought picture and the total performance correlated positively with chronological and mental age and
with percentage correct on the TROG.
4. Discussion
The result in the present study confirms earlier findings that children with cerebral palsy and severe speech impairments
perform worse on theory of mind tasks than could be expected from their mental age (Dahlgren et al., 2003; Falkman et al.,
2005). This conclusion is based on three findings. First, the children with SSPI performed significantly worse on the classical
false belief task Sally and Anne. Second, they performed worse on the thought pictures, however not significantly so. Third,
the only children who failed all theory of mind trials were four children with SSPI. These results are noteworthy taking into
consideration the difference in chronological age, with the comparison children being significantly younger.
However, it is impossible to know from these data if the low performance on the theory of mind tasks, irrespective of
whether the task required high verbal performance (Sally and Anne) or not (thought pictures), reflects a lack of theory of
mind, a delayed but normal development (as proposed by Falkman et al., 2005), a lack of proper skills for application of a
theory of mind (as proposed by Dahlgren et al., 2003) or is a consequence of the experimental design (i.e. low ecological
validity as proposed by Dahlgren et al., 2003). In earlier studies of children with SSPI (Dahlgren et al., 2003; Falkman et al.,
2005) the classical Sally and Anne task was used, a task which to a high degree depends on language abilities such as syntax,
pragmatics and overall language comprehension. A case has been made for language abilities being required to participate in
classical theory of mind tasks and that poor language abilities may lead the children to answer in a way that does not reflect
their theory of mind ability (Miller, 2006). The results from the earlier studies on children with SSPI could, as in the present
study, be explained by poor language abilities, corresponding to the latter reasoning. The difference between the two groups
in the study on the less verbally dependent task were smaller than on the verbally more demanding task, which supports this
reasoning. The fact that there are children with SSPI in this study who fail to solve the theory of mind task irrespective of
verbal requirements, together with the data from the earlier studies makes it clear, however, that there are children in this
Table 5
Memory scores. Children with SSPI and comparison group.
SSPI group
Comparison group
p-Value
DS, total
Mean (SD)
DS, forwards
Mean (SD)
DS, backwards
Mean (SD)
Corsi blocks, total
Mean (SD)
Corsi blocks, forwards
Mean (SD)
Corsi blocks, backwards
Mean (SD)
6.1
(2.6)
6.1
(1.4)
4.0
(1.7)
4.2
(1.0)
2.1
(1.4)
1.9
(0.6)
5.3
(2.1)
7.0
(2.1)
3.2
(0.9)
4.1
(0.8)
2.1
(1.4)
2.9
(1.4)
.941
.685
.718
.062
.024
.170
622
S.O. Dahlgren et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 31 (2010) 617–624
Table 6
Correlation between performance on the theory of mind tasks and chronological age, mental age, IQ, verbal scores and memory scores.
SSPI group
Chronological age
Mental age
IQ
TROG, percentage correct
PPVT, linguistic age
Syntactic knowledge
DS, total
DS, forwards
DS, backwards
Corsi blocks, total
Corsi blocks, forwards
Corsi blocks, backwards
Comparison group
Sally and Anne
Thought picture
Composite score
.024
p = .935
.429
p = .126
.372
p = .191
.572
p = .032
.503
p = .067
.427
p = .128
.590
p = .044
.447
p = .145
.554
p = .061
.771
p = .009
.651
p = .042
.683
p = .030
.145
p = .620
.874
p < .001
.610
p = .021
.366
p = .199
.662
p = .010
.530
p = .051
.178
p = .580
.166
p = .607
.132
p = .683
.342
p = .333
.267
p = .455
.316
p = .374
.103
p = .726
.852
p < .001
.624
p = .017
.516
p = .059
.717
p = .004
.584
p = .028
.377
p = .227
.311
p = .326
.326
p = .301
.607
p = .063
.494
p = .146
.549
p = .100
Sally and Anne
Thought picture
Composite score
.569
p = .034
.629
p = .016
.442
p = .114
.563
p = .036
.422
p = .133
.243
p = .403
.375
p = .107
.396
p = .161
.195
p = .505
.176
p = .546
.080
p = .785
.221
p = .447
.569
p = .034
.629
p = .016
.442
p =.114
.563
p = .036
.422
p = .133
.243
p = .403
.375
p = .107
.396
p = .161
.195
p = .505
.176
p = .546
.080
p = .785
.221
p = .447
group who have difficulties to develop theory of mind. The nature of this incompletely developed theory of mind is still
unclear. However, a late developed theory of mind also means that the overall development, including language, social
cognition and social development, could be negatively affected.
A Pearson’s correlation was computed in order to find out if IQ, mental age, language and memory could be a part of the
explanation to the children’s shortcomings in theory of mind performance. In the SSPI group mental age and IQ were
positively correlated to the thought pictures task and the composite score. If mental age and IQ could explain why one group
of children with SSPI constantly fail to solve theory of mind tasks you would expect a lower IQ and mental age in the four
children in the present study who failed all the theory of mind tasks than in those children who solved all the tasks. However,
the results are not clear cut. Two children in the ‘‘passer’’ group had a lower IQ than one of the four children in the ‘‘failer’’
group. Only three of the children in the ‘‘passer’’ group had a higher mental age than all four children in the ‘‘failer’’ group.
These results imply that mental age and IQ cannot alone explain the children’s performance.
Measures of language competence correlated with the performance on all theory of mind tasks implying that language
plays an important role in the theory of mind development. This is in line with Tager-Flusberg’s reasoning (2000).
A question that needs to be answered is if these children have been exposed to conversations about mental states to the
same degree as children in the spoken community. It has been argued that exposure to talk about mental states influences
the development of theory of mind (e.g. Dunn et al., 1991; Jenkins & Astington, 1996; Peterson, 2000; Ruffman et al., 2002). In
the deaf community it has been found that late-signing deaf children performed worse on theory of mind tasks in
comparison to native signers. The authors (Meristo et al., 2007) explained this finding saying that late signers do not have as
many opportunities as native signers or hearing children to learn about mental states through conversation. This could also
be true for children with SSPI. In the present study the children with SSPI all used some AAC mode for communication. The
children had few or no possibilities to use mental states on their own due to the design of the communication modes where
few glosses for mental states were present. Their ability to produce speech is extremely limited. Often the communication is
characterized by their interlocutors presenting an interpretation of what the child really wants to express (Hjelmquist &
Dahlgren Sandberg, 1996).
It is also still unclear if the severe restriction on interactive abilities and mobility of the participating children with SSPI
could interfere with the development of theory of mind. The development of theory of mind must be understood in the
context of interaction, including language use and the time pressure in human interaction (Leslie, 1994). These children have
extremely limited possibilities to interact independently with others, and also to engage in pretend play.
Working memory (Buitelaar et al., 1999) or the ability to hold in mind (Gordon & Olson, 1998) has been suggested to play
an important role in the development of theory of mind. Dahlgren Sandberg (2001) proposed that children with cerebral
palsy had a deficit in working memory and that this deficit could be a key factor in the children’s short comings in theory of
mind tasks (Dahlgren et al., 2003). In the present study a relation between working memory, especially measured by Corsi
blocks, and the performance on the Sally and Anne task was found. This relation, however, could not entirely explain why the
four children with SSPI failed to pass any of the theory of mind tasks, since there were children with SSPI who passed some or
S.O. Dahlgren et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 31 (2010) 617–624
623
all theory of mind tasks and still performed at the same level on all of the memory measures as the children who failed to
solve the theory of mind tasks.
In summary, there are children with cerebral palsy who fail to solve theory of mind tasks irrespectively of the verbal
loading of the tasks. It seems as if expressive language ability and deficits in working memory could partly explain the
children’s performance. However, to fully understand why some children with SSPI fail to solve the task we need to know
more of these children’s possibilities to take part in conversations of mental states, their experiences of mental states in other
peoples’ conversations and their possibility to take part in pretend play. We also have to study if the low results on theory of
mind tasks reflect a lack of theory of mind, a slow but normal development of theory of mind, a lack of proper skills for
application of a theory of mind or if it is a consequence of the experimental design. Even if we do not really know the whole
picture it is important to have the development of theory of mind in mind in the children’s early development. As the
development of theory of mind and language are closely connected it means that deficits in or an immature theory of mind
could limit a child’s language and communicative development and vice versa. By taking advantage of the growing literature
on intervention for abilities related to theory of mind as suggested by Miller (2006), clinicians, such as speech and language
pathologists, can help the child in this development.
Acknowledgment
The study was supported by a grant from the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research.
References
Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35,
1311–1320.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.
Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the children’s communication checklist (CCC): a method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment
in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879–892.
Buitelaar, J. K., van der Wees, M., Swaab-Barneveld, H., & van der Gaag, J. (1999). Verbal memory and performance IQ predict theory of mind and emotion
recognition ability in children with autism spectrum disorders and in psychiatric control children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 869–881.
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.
Dahlgren Sandberg, A. (2001). Reading and spelling, phonological awareness and working memory in children with severe expressive speech impairments – A
longitudinal study. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 17, 11–26.
Dahlgren, S. O., Dahlgren Sandberg, A., & Hjelmquist, E. (2003). The non-specificity of theory of mind deficits: Evidence from children with communicative
disabilities. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 15, 129–155.
de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (2000). Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false beliefs. In P. Mitchell & K. J. Riggs (Eds.), Children’s reasoning and
the mind (pp. 191–228). Hove, England: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis (UK).
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child
Development, 62, 1352–1366 (electronic version).
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Falkman, K. W., Dahlgren Sandberg, A., & Hjelmquist, E. (2005). Theory of mind in children with severe speech and physical impairment (SSPI): A longitudinal
study. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 52, 139–157.
Farrar, M. J., & Maag, L. (2002). Early language development and the emergence of a theory of mind. First Language, 22, 197–213.
Forguson, L., & Gopnik, A. (1988). The ontogeny of common sense. In J. W. Astington, P. L. Harris, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of mind (pp. 226–243).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Gordon, A. C. L., & Olson, D. R. (1998). The relation between acquisition of a theory of mind and the capacity to hold in mind. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 68, 70–83.
Hagberg, B., & Hagberg, G. (1993). The origins of cerebral palsy. In T. J. David (Ed.), Recent advances in paediatrics XI (pp. 67–83). Edinburgh, London: Churchill
Livingstone.
Hjelmquist, E., & Dahlgren Sandberg, A. (1996). Sounds and silence: Interaction in aided language use. In S. Von Tetzchner & M. H. Jensen (Eds.), Augmentative and
alternative communication: European perspectives (pp. 137–152). London: Whurr.
Holmberg, E., & Lundälv, E. (2002). TROG svensk manual [Swedish manual]. Umeå: Specialpedagogiska institutet.
Hughes, C. (1998a). Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 233–253.
Hughes, C. (1998b). Finding your marbles: Does preschoolers’ strategic behaviour predict later understanding of mind? Developmental Psychology, 34, 1326–1339.
Hughes, C., Dunn, J., & White, A. (1998). Trick or treat? Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive dysfunction in ‘‘hard-to-manage’’ preschoolers.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 981–994.
Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (1996). Cognitive factors and family structure associated with theory of mind development in young children. Developmental
Psychology, 32, 70–78.
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretence and representation: The origins of ‘‘theory of mind’’. Psychological Review, 94, 412–426.
Leslie, A. M. (1994). Pretending and believing: issues in the theory of ToMM. Cognition, 50, 211–238.
Lillard, A. (1994). Making sense of pretence. In C. Lewis & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children’s early understanding of mind. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.,
Publishers.
Meristo, M., Falkman, K. W., Hjelmquist, E., Surian, L., Tedoldi, M., & Siegal, M. (2007). Language access and theory of mind reasoning: Evidence from deaf children
in bilingual and oralist environments. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1156–1169.
Miller, C. (2006). Developmental relationships between language and theory of mind. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15(2), 142–154.
Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. British Medical Bulletin, 27, 272–277.
Nauclér, K., & Magnusson, E. (1994). Identification and correction of syntactically un-acceptable sentences. Working Papers 42. Lund: Lund University, Dept. of
Linguistics.
Perner, J. (1988). Developing semantics for theories of mind: From propositional attitudes to mental representation. In J. W. Astington, P. L. Harris, & D. R. Olson
(Eds.), Developing theories of mind (pp. 141–172). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Perner, J., & Lang, B. (1999). Development of theory of mind and executive control. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 337–344.
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 5, 125–137.
Peterson, C. C. (2000). Influence of siblings’ perspectives on theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 435–455.
624
S.O. Dahlgren et al. / Research in Developmental Disabilities 31 (2010) 617–624
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press Ltd.
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child
Development, 73, 734–751.
Shatz, M. (1994). Theory of mind and the development of social-linguistic intelligence in early childhood. In C. Lewis & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children’s early
understanding of mind. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd., Publishers.
Siegal, M., Varley, R., & Want, S. C. (2001). Mind over grammar: Reasoning in aphasia and development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 296–301.
Surian, L., & Leslie, A. (1999). Competence and performance in false belief understanding: A comparison of autistic and normal 3-year-old children. British Journal
of Developmental Psychology, 17, 141–155.
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2000). Language and understanding minds: Connections in autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding
other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 124–149). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press. (reprinted 2005).
Wechsler, D. (1999). Manual of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children, Swedish standardization. Stockholm, Sweden: Psykologiförlaget AB.
Wimmer, H., Hogrefe, J., & Sodian, B. (1988). A second stage in children’s conception of mental life: Understanding informational accesses as origins of knowledge
and belief. In J. W. Astington, P. L. Harris, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of mind (pp. 173–192). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Woolfe, T., Want, S., & Siegal, S. (2002). Signpost in development: Theory of mind in deaf children. Child Development, 73, 768–778.