Public Is Invited Marietta Natural History Society Fall 2005 Newsletter . All programs are in room 143, Rickey Science Center, 4th St., Marietta College campus. 7:00 PM Butterflies in Winter Thursday, October 13 Presenter: Jim Davidson Jim is a repeat presenter. He is an amateur naturalist and volunteer with Division of Natural Areas and Preserves and with Columbus MetroPark. He also is active with the Appalachian Land Alliance. Thursday, December 8 Presenter: Brian Riley Brian Riley, of the Ohio Department of Natural Resources, Division of Forestry, is Mr. Ohio Champion Tree. Even as a student he was tracking down the big ones. Now, the Emerald Ash Borer, is currently threatening many trees in Ohio and he will be able to give us an update. Flying Squirrels Thursday, November 10 Presenter: Ed Michael Ed Michel also is making a return visit. He has done research on both the southern and northern flying squirrel and will provide lots of information about these fascinating animals. He will also entertain questions about snapping turtles (about which he spoke last time) – and he may have copies of his book “A Valley Called Canaan: 1885-2002”. Page 2 Marietta Natural History Society Web Threads Fall 2005 1, 1, 2, 3, 5, 8, 13, 21 . . . A site you can count on. MathWorld is a comprehensive and interactive mathematics encyclopedia maintained by Eric W. Weisstein and intended for students, educators, math enthusiasts, and researchers. The site is continuously updated to include new material and discoveries. You can find discussions of technical areas of math such as Algebra, Calculus, Geometry, Number Theory, etc, while the Recreational Mathematics section may have widest appeal. Here you can find mathematical Games, Illusions, Art, Humor, Puzzles and Sports. If you can never have too much math, visit http://mathworld.wolfram.com/about/mathworld. html. Winter Birdfeeder Watch Begins Again in November Members of the MNHS and other residents have been monitoring local bird populations. The Winter Bird Feeder Watch is modeled on a similar program started by the West Virginia Department of Natural Resources. The feeder watch offers an opportunity for bird feeder loyalists, “armchair ornithologists” and anyone interested in birds to contribute to long term tracking of resident and migratory bird species. Participants record the species and number of birds frequenting their bird feeder(s) every other weekend from November to mid March. Even if you cannot watch each weekend, data you can collect are still valuable. The feeder watch is a great way to involve kids! Neonaturalists can fulfill school, scout and 4-H projects. Involvement in a real monitoring program sparks children’s interests and builds enthusiasm for birding and nature. With your help, children can learn to identify many of our local bird species and behaviors. If you would like to participate, a printed tally sheet is available. Other information can be recorded that can reveal important ecological information about our local bird populations. For example, how does your residential setting (suburban, urban, or agricultural), local trees or forests (conifer, mixed deciduous, or predominantly oak/hickory) and type of bird feed that you provide affect the diversity and vitality of local bird populations? Very little is presently known about these factors. Correlations with air quality measurements may also reveal valuable information about the influence of our local environmental conditions on indigenous bird species. Establishing long term records will be important to drawing meaningful conclusions. This is a great opportunity to turn your backyard bird feeder into a satellite research station. If you want to participate call either Ava Bradley (373-5790) or Bird Watchers Digest (373-5285). Tally sheets and directions are available to all participants. Recycled Paper 100% Post-Consumer Johnson Grass by Marilyn Ortt Many native grasses such as big bluestem, little bluestem, switchgrass and Indian grass are warm season grasses and, ecologically, are much preferred over fescue and several other cool season grasses. Warm season grasses are in flower now and are quite attractive. There are a few bad actors though. Chinese silk grass or eulalia (Miscanthus sinensis) has already been written about in this series and is a large threat becoming larger because it is still being sold by landscape nurseries and purchased by unaware customers. Doubters have only to look along both sides of I-77 in West Virginia after crossing the Ohio River Bridge and, if traffic allows, take a look at the open area on Buckley Island on your way back across the bridge. Another prominent grass at this time of the year is Johnson grass (Sorghum halapense). Much reviled by the agricultural community because of its persistence and aggressiveness, it is now moving into flower and vegetable gardens and wildflower meadows. An overnight eruption will occur if you recently purchased rich top soil came collected from a floodplain. Johnson grass is a coarse perennial that will grow to eight feet tall in large dense clumps. It accomplishes this by having very stout rhizomes - up to ¾" in diameter! See Johnson, page 7 Suggestions, Comments or Contributions for the MNHS Newsletter? Send them to the Editor: 625 5th St Marietta, OH 45750 374-8778 spilatrs@marietta.edu Page 3 Marietta Natural History Society Fall 2005 Good Works Department Marietta Natural History Society provided the paper for four Birding in Washington County brochures. The four driving routes are Western Washington County, Eastern Washington County, Marietta and Birding the Muskingum River. Information was compiled by Lynn Barnhart, Brad Bond, and Marilyn Ortt. Elin Jones designed the brochures. The brochures are available at the Marietta Visitors’ Center and the information center near the Bank One Drive-Through on Acme St. They will also be available on-line. Call 373-3372 for more information. Page 4 Marietta Natural History Society Fall 2005 Scandanavian Pachyderms, Italian Lagomorphs, and the Beauty of Numbers by Dr. Mark Miller, Department of Mathematics and Computer Science, Marietta College Some have posited that mathematics is a universal language and that if we were to discover intelligent life on other planets, mathematics there would be the same as mathematics here. This is a rather bold claim. Is mathematics a system of truths that hold independent of human experience? Or did mathematics simply evolve with the rest of the human condition? The answers to these questions are, respectively, “Yes” and “No – well sort of”. Consider the mathematical parlor game shown in the box. Repeated trials of this little puzzle show that the overwhelming majority of participants tend to end up with gray elephants from Denmark, although from time to time a green eel from Djibouti may appear. (I once encountered a student who ended up with a green eguana (sic) from the Dominican Republic – whether the student was displaying his wit or ignorance was unclear.) Of course the secret to this puzzle lies in a fact most grade school children learn: When a number is multiplied by 9 its digits add up to 9 or a multiple of 9. But why nine? What – if anything – makes nine so special? The answer, it ends up, is more anthropological/anatomical than it is mathematical. The anthropologists tell us that our counting systems likely developed the way they did because people found it easy to count on their ten fingers. Since we have ten digits anatomically, we developed ten digits mathematically (0,1,…,9). Thus, this “trick” works because 9 is the largest of the digits. Thus it might seem natural to conclude from this that the mathematics that we have today is the result of evolutionary forces. However, a second, deeper, look at mathematics leads to a different conclusion. It is true that if people had eight fingers instead of ten, that our arithmetic system would be different. However this is profoundly different from saying that mathematics would be different. Arithmetic is a human invention; mathematics is not. This leads us to the question, “What is mathematics?” At its most basic level, mathematics is the study of knowledge. Hence mathematicians often ask the question, “What is true?”As opposed to philosophers or theologians who might ask, “What is Truth?”, or natural scientists who might ask, “What appears to be true based on empirical data?”, those of us in the mathematical community often concern ourselves with questioning what conclusions could be true given a set of agreed upon postulates. So in this sense, mathematics is A Mathematical Parlor Game Choose your favorite whole number bigger than 1 but less than 10. Now multiply this number by 9, add up the digits of this new number, and then subtract 5 from this sum and write down the result. Once you have done this, find the letter of the alphabet that corresponds with the number you have written down (1:A, 2:B, 3:C, …). Now, write down the name of a country that starts with this letter. Move one letter forward in the alphabet and write down an animal that starts with this new letter. Finally move two more letters forward in the alphabet and write down a color that starts with this letter. a universal language insomuch as logic itself is universal. Some properties of arithmetic, on the other hand, are relative (particularly if the other hand has only 4 digits). The “gray elephant in Denmark” puzzle referenced above fails to be universal because it depends not only on properties of numbers but also on the way we represent these numbers. Mathematicians tend to be more interested in numbers themselves than in their decimal representations. In fact, many of the numbers that have intrigued mathematicians over the past three thousand years do not have a decimal representation. I will consider three such numbers which give me particular intellectual pleasure – is there any other kind? The first such number is that ubiquitous number, π. Most people will recall that if a circle’s circumference is divided by its diameter, the result is constant, i.e. the ratio is independent of the size of the circle itself. This constant, which is approximately 3.1416, is what we call π. This number, π, finds its way into all sorts of applications which on their face seem to be completely unrelated to circles. For example, start with the positive whole numbers: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, … Now square these numbers: 1, 4, 9, 16, 25, 36, 49, 64, 81, 100, 121, … Next, consider the fractions whose numerators are 1 and whose denominators are these squares: 1/1, 1/4, 1/9, 1/16, 1/25, 1/36, 1/49, 1/64, 1/81, 1/100, 1/121, … What would happen if you started adding up these numbers? Is there some sum that you would approach? Let’s see what we get: 1/1 + 1/4 =1.25 1/1 + 1/4 + 1/9 = 1.36111… 1/1 + 1/4 + 1/9 + 1/16 = 1.4236111 1/1 + 1/4 + 1/9 + 1/16 + 1/25 = 1.4636111… Cont. on page 5 Page 5 Marietta Natural History Society Numbers, Con’t from page 4 It may not seem obvious, but these sums are actually approaching some threshold. If you add the first few (say, 20,000) such fractions, you will see this sum approaching something close to 1.6449. What’s so special about that? To see, multiply it by 6; you should get 9.8694. Still not impressed? Now take the square root; your result should be approximately 3.1416. Easy as π! (To see why this works, sign up for Calculus II at your nearest liberal arts college.) In fact, the number π shows up all over the place. And while it is true that π is approximately 3.1416, this is only an approximation. There is no possible way to represent the value of π using our decimal system. Put another way, π cannot be written as a ratio of two integers. For this reason, we say that π is “irrational” - that is, not lending itself to be expressed as a ratio of whole numbers. Think for a moment about what this is saying. On the one hand, π is defined as the ratio of a circle’s circumference to its diameter. On the other hand π cannot be the ratio of two whole numbers. Ponder what this says about the existence of “perfect circles” … The second ubiquitous number to be considered here is φ. Like π, φ was first encountered by considering geometric ratios. The Greeks concerned themselves with ratios in rectangles. They observed that while some rectangles looked pleasing, others looked too “fat” or too “square”. Just what was it that made a rectangle pleasing? The Greeks settled the question this way. Call the longer side the “base (B)” and the shorter side the “height (H)”. A rectangle is pleasing if the ratio of the base to the height is the same as the ratio of the sum of the base plus the height to the base [more simply B/H should equal (B+H)/B .] Any rectangle with such ratios was called “golden”, and this ratio was called φ, the “golden ratio” or “golden mean”. This ratio ends up being approximately 1.61803. Examining architecture, one can find this golden ratio, φ, time and time again. Many artists used this ratio to give their art a “good” sense of proportion. For example, the golden ratio can be found in many of Leonardo Da Vinci’s works. Another Italian Leonardo discovered this same golden ratio in a seemingly unrelated scenario: Leonardo Fibonacci proposed the following: Suppose rabbits take one month to mature and then begin multiplying regularly each month thereafter. Further suppose that these are good moral rabbits with monogamous practices and that each litter contains two off spring – a male and a female. Finally suppose that each offspring pair imitate the practices of their parents. Leonardo then asked how many rabbits there would be at the beginning of the nth month. Since this was Fibonacci’s question, we will use Fn to indicate the population for at the beginning of month n. Some quick calculations show that F1 = 1, F2 = 1, F3 = 2, Fall 2005 F4 = 3, F5 = 5, F6 = 8, … In general, Fibonacci noticed that the population for any given month equaled the sum of the populations in the previous two months. Put more succinctly, Fn = Fn-1 + Fn-2. What does this have to do with the golden ratio? Fibonacci further observed that examining ratios of successive Fibonacci numbers gives the following ratios which we will label with Rs: R1 = 1/1, R2 =2 /1,R3 = 3/2, R4 = 5/3, R5 = 8 /5, R6 = 13/8, .... An examination of these fractions shows that these ratios are bouncing around 1.61803. R1 is smaller than φ, but R2 is larger than φ. R3 is bigger that R1 but less than φ; R4 is less that R2 but bigger than φ. In general, Rn is a fraction between Rn-2 and φ, and the larger n is, the closer Rn is to φ. So while rabbits love each other, they also seem to love the golden ratio. π and φ are not the only interesting irrational numbers; and not everyone shares my love for rational numbers. The Pythagoreans were quite disturbed to learn that irrational numbers existed. They were so disturbed that legend tells us they murdered the person who made this discovery. At the risk of upsetting any modern day Pythagoreans, I will discuss just one more irrational number – the number e. To find e, once again list out the positive whole numbers and divide these numbers into their successors: 2/1, 3/2, 4/3, 5/4, 6/5, 7/6, 8/7, 9/8, 10/9, 11/10, … Finally, raise the first fraction to the first power, the second fraction to the second power, and so on to get: (2/1)1, (3/2)2, (4/3)3, (5/4)4, (6/5)5, (7/6)6, (8/7)7, (9/8)8, (10/9)9, (11/10)10, … This list begins to level off somewhere near 2.71828. The exact value of this leveling off point is called e. Like π and φ, e is an irrational number – it has no exact decimal representation. Also like π and φ, e shows up in some rather strange places. To see just one such example, consider the “elevation-slope” relationship. Suppose you were hiking hills and that periodically there were markers that told you your elevation at given points as well as the incline at those points (incline = slope= Rise/Run). Of course, these numbers measure different things. If the elevation is 200 ft, you would be 200 ft above the base; if the slope is 200, that would mean that for every 200 feet of vertical assent, you have achieved 1 foot of horizontal distance. Again, these are two different things. But could there be a point on the hill where your incline slope number was the same as your elevation number? The answer is yes (and is not that surprising ... Why?) But now we consider a related question: Could you construct a hill so that at every point the elevation and the slope were identical? The answer to this is a somewhat surprising yes, and in fact there is only cont. on page 6 Page 6 Marietta Natural History Society Numbers, Con’t from page 5 one such type of hill: it is one whose shape is of the form Run y = ex (or Rise = e ), where e is the same e described above. Irrational numbers like e, π, and φ, show up all over the place. In fact, it ends up that there are more irrational numbers than there are rational numbers. This means that if you were to randomly select some number from all the real numbers out there, you would most likely pick a number whose value could not be expressed using our base-ten decimal system. While this may seem disconcerting at first blush, this is actually quite reassuring. What this means is that most numbers cannot be expressed with numerals – put another way, most numbers represent notions that are independent of our human experience. For me, this makes mathematics beautiful … more beautiful than that old gray elephant in Denmark. Change for the Better 1. When you have doubts about a question on a multiple choice exam, it’s best to stick with your first answer. A. This is always true B. This is generally true C. This is always false D. This is generally false Researchers have recently endeavored to answer this question empirically. They tallied the consequences of answer-switching on over 2000 exams given over a two year period in an undergraduate psychology course (J. Pers. Soc. Psychology 88, 725; 2005). Changes were detected as erasures on an answer form. They found that when students changed answers, twice as often students switched from an incorrect answer to a correct one than from a correct answer to an incorrect one. Thus, statistically, students were more likely to get a question right when they changed an answer. So why the common perception that it is better to stick with a first choice? Follow-up investigations suggested that the frustration of having thrown out a correct answer leaves a deeper psychological impact than switching to a correct one. (Is self-reprehension emotionally stronger than self-congratulation?) The effect is similar to the hesitation many people have to switching checkout lines at the grocery. Our instincts tell us not to do it because we believe that the other line will end up moving even slower than the one we’re in (probably due to Devine retribution for lack of fortitude), even though the other line is just as likely to move faster. Thus, answer ‘D’ above is the correct choice. . . . or is it ‘B’? Fall 2005 Of Migrants and Magnets The navigational skills of migratory birds allow many to travel hundreds to thousands of miles. All the more wonder that some species, such as grey-cheeked and Swainson’s thrushes do so at night! How they orient themselves has been of great interest to ornithologists (and, who knows, probably the military as well). Two common hypothesis to explain their navigational skills are that birds use a biological ‘compass’ guided by star patterns or the earth’s magnetic field. However, to maintain proper nocturnal flight over long distances, either system would require periodic recalibration, since star patterns and orientation of the magnetic flux lines vary over different regions of the earth’s surface. One way to recalibrate these compasses might be accomplished using the position of the setting sun. Using captured birds and some clever experimental manipulation, these speculations were recently sorted out. To perform the experiment the researchers captured several dozen thrushes and outfitted them with radio transmitters. Before releasing the birds into the nighttime sky, some were exposed to artificial aberrant magnetic fields rotated 80O to the east. ‘Thrush-chasers’ followed the birds in an old Oldsmobile packed with tracking equipment, and despite periodic interruptions by suspicious police officers, the flight pattern was determined over several days. They found that thrushes migrate in a generally northerly direction, but those exposed to the artificial magnetic field followed a more westward course for the entire first night. Experimentally treated birds did not return to a normal flight pattern until after sunset the following night. The results indicate that during their nocturnal flight the birds follow a magnetic compass, and that magnetic compass is calibrated to the solar azimuth (position of the setting sun). Twilight-calibration of the magnetic compass also explains one enigma of song-bird migration, that being how they can cross the magnetic equator without becoming disoriented. [The magnetic equator is where the dip or inclination of a compass needle is zero. The magnetic equator deflects above and below the geographic equator and is not fixed, slowly changing over time.] Page 7 Marietta Natural History Society Fall 2005 Smart Birds Stick Around by Jamie Tidd, Bird Watcher's Digest A study from Spain shows that birds that tough out the winter months up north may be smarter than those that go to the Bahamas for a winter getaway. Daniel Sol of the Independent University of Barcelona in Spain has found that short-distance migratory birds have smaller brains than nonmigratory birds. Long-distance migrants, in turn, have even smaller brains than short-distance migrants. Those that stay behind are also more inventive foragers. Sol and his colleagues studied previous observations of 134 bird species in Europe, Scandinavia, and western Russia. They examined data on brain size and the number of times researchers had seen birds using unique feeding techniques. One possible explanation for the size difference is that migratory birds use most of their energy to voyage south, which means less energy to build and maintain brain tissue. So migratory birds benefit from having smaller brains, but those small brains may be the reason they began migrating in the first place. With less brain tissue migrants are not smart enough to forage in harsh winter conditions. Stationary birds seem to be more innovative feeders. The blackbird, Turdus merula, has been spotted using twigs to move snow to find food. The bullfinch, Pyrrhula pyrrhula, has been seen tearing flesh from chicken and duck carcasses for nourishment. Nonmigratory birds have been seen using an average of four innovative feeding approaches, compared with three for birds that migrate short distances, and around one for species who travel below the Sahara desert. Unfortunately, Sol and his colleagues believe that migratory bird species will have more trouble adapting to future changes in environmental conditions. With humans causing climate and landscape disruptions, they will be at a greater risk for extinction than the smarter stationary species. [Thanks to Bird Watcher’s Digest for letting us use this article.] A ROAD I think that I have never knowed, a sight as lovely as a road. A road upon whose concrete tops, the flow of traffic never stops; A road that costs a lot to build, just as the City Council willed; A road the planners say we need, to get the cars to greater speed; We've let the contracts so dig in, and let the chopping now begin; Somebody else can make a tree, but roads are made by guys like me. – Mike Royko Johnson, con’t from page 2 If a stem is pulled from the ground, many fresh budded rhizomes extend in all directions from the roots. Mission: to fill all voids between this and the next clump of Johnson grass. It also spreads by seeds which ensure there are always outlier clumps to spread toward. . Leaves up to 20" long are smooth except for rough margins and have a prominent white center-vein. The coarse stems are often rusty red near the base changing to a faded pink further up. The panicles of flowers are quite large and symmetrical, loosely branched, purplish in color and have small hairs on most flower parts. Seeds are about 1/8" long and purplish also. The stems die back to the ground each winter but things are happening beneath the ground during mild weather Johnson grass is native to the Mediterranean area but now occurs in all warm-temperate regions of the world. It is especially common in cultivated floodplains including pastures, crop fields, forest edges, rights-of-way, abandoned fields, and streambanks. If succession from old-field to forest is the desired future outcome, Johnson grass will short-circuit the process. It can engulf small trees and then inhibit germination and growth of new woody plants. Johnson grass does not begin to grow until late spring but it is flowering by the first of August in Washington County. In small areas, hand pulling of smaller clumps is an option especially after a rain has softened the soil making it easier to get most of the root material. Any adventive rhizomes left in the soil will make new plants so a follow-up-pulling about a month later is necessary and then another a month after that. Farmers dislike Johnson grass because of the direct competition with row crops such as corn and soybeans. Invasion of pastures creates a problem because it contains a glucoside that yields hydrocyanic acid which can cause poisoning of livestock under certain conditions. Although it has been grown horticulturally, it is unlikely anyone would do so today. But this is just another example of why native grasses are superior to non-native species and are just as beautiful. Invite a Friend to Join the Marietta Natural History Society Wood Thrush — Individual $15 River Otter — Family $25 Monarch — Friend $50 Why not give a gift membership? Mail check to address given below Benefits of Membership L Monthly programs L Field trips L Quarterly newsletter L Educational experiences for kids and adults L Conservation Projects The MNHS Mission i To foster awareness of and sensitivity to our environment and its biodiversity i To provide a place where people with these interests can gather for information and activity i To create a presence in our community representing these ideas Marietta Natural History Society P.O. Box 1081 Marietta, Ohio 45750 (740) 373-5285