P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 C 2002) Journal of Insect Behavior, Vol. 15, No. 6, November 2002 (° Physical Restraint or Stimulation? The Function(s) of the Modified Front Legs of Male Archisepsis diversiformis (Diptera, Sepsidae) William G. Eberhard1 Accepted August 9, 2002; revised September 20, 2002 Four hypotheses that could explain the elaborate species-specific morphology of the clasping organs on the front legs of male Archisepsis diversiformis flies were tested: direct male–male combat, mechanical fit, male–female conflict of interests, and male stimulation of the female. Experimental modification of the shape of the male clasper and of the female wing base where the male clasped the female both strongly reduced the chances that a mount would result in copulation. This reduction was not predicted by the male–male combat hypothesis but was predicted by the others. Males in the field did nave to fight other males to remain mounted on females, as expected by the male– male combat hypothesis. Reduced male copulatory success was not due to inferior male ability to grasp and hold onto the female’s wings, as predicted by the mechanical fit and male–female conflict hypotheses but to a reduction in the likelihood that the female would allow intromission, as predicted by the stimulation hypothesis. By a process of elimination, and in combination with data from a previous morphological study, the data support the hypothesis that the species-specific aspects of grasping organs in these flies function to stimulate females. Further behavioral data will be needed to test alternative possibilities. KEY WORDS: sexual selection; sepsid flies; lock-and-key; male–female conflict of interests. 1Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, and Escuela de Biologı́a, Universidad de Costa Rica, Ciudad Universitaria, Costa Rica. 2To whom correspondence should be addressed at Biologı́a, Universidad de Costa Rica, Ciudad Universitaria, Costa Rica. Fax: +506 228 0001. e-mail: archisepsis@biologı́a.ucr.ac.cr. 831 C 2002 Plenum Publishing Corporation 0892-7553/02/1100-0831/0 ° P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 832 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Eberhard INTRODUCTION Nongenitalic male body parts that are modified to contact females during courtship or copulation often evolve relatively rapidly and divergently compared with other body parts (Eberhard, 1985). Such contact structures are often structurally complex and useful for distinguishing closely related species because of their species-specific forms in many animal groups, including nematodes, spiders, schizomids, mites, copepods, isopods, eubranchiopods, decapods, millipedes, collembolans, and many insects (summary by Eberhard, 1985). The portions of the male’s body that are specialized for contact vary widely and include legs, cephalothorax, chelicerae, telson, hysterosome, dorsal setae, antennae, periopods, mandibles, maxillary palps, abdominal sterna, wings, forceps, and frontal horns. This general trend to rapid divergent evolution resembles the even more widespread and general trend for male genitalia to diverge rapidly and acquire elaborate morphology (Eberhard, 1985). Just as in genitalia, one common function of nongenitalic male contact structures is to grasp the female. The elaborate forms of nongenitalic male grasping structures often seem overly complex and diverse for the seemingly simple mechanical demands of holding onto the female (e.g., Fig. 1), however, suggesting that they may also have additional functions. Several of the hypotheses that were originally proposed to explain the evolution of male genitalia could also explain the evolutionary tendency for nongenitalic male grasping structures to evolve rapidly and divergently. 1. Direct male–male combat. Male grasping organs may enable males to resist attempts by other males to displace them from females after they have mounted (Darwin, 1871). Diversity in grasping structures could result from adaptations to resist different types of displacement attempts. 2. Mechanical fit. A greater degree of physical fit between male and female structures could enable the male to hold onto the female more effectively and, thus, avoid being displaced. Two types of selection could favor especially good fits. (A) The species-specific male structures may fit only conspecific females, and be mechanically incapable of grasping females of other species, and the differences in female morphology responsible for these fits may evolve to prevent grasping by heterospecific males (Fraser, 1943; Freitag, 1974; Toro and de la Hoz, 1976; see Shapiro and Porter [1989] for similar arguments regarding genitalic structures). (B) Females may discriminate among conspecific males on the basis of mechanical fit (Eberhard, 1985). Diversity in male structures could thus result from selection on P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Function of Clasping Organs of Male Flies Fig. 1. Front femur of males of four species of Archisepsis flies, illustrating the species-specific shapes of the portions that clasp the female wing. 833 P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] 834 pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Eberhard females to avoid heterospecific pairings (A) or from sexual selection on males (B). 3. Male–female conflict. Male grasping organs may evolve as weapons in coevolutionary races with females to obtain greater control over critical reproductive processes. Diversity in male grasping structures would result from the need to overcome different female defenses that have evolved to reduce her susceptibility to being grasped. The advantage to the female of avoiding being grasped would be to avoid possible costs of being seized, such as reduced chances to feed or lay eggs or increased danger from predation (Arnquist and Rowe, 1995; Holland and Rice, 1998; see Lloyd [1979] and Alexander et al., [1997] for similar arguments regarding genitalic structures). 4. Stimulation. Species-specific male organs may stimulate the female in ways that induce the female to favor the male with cooperative responses. Selection could favor male stimulatory abilities in two contexts: (A) discrimination of conspecific versus heterospecific males and (B) discrimination of conspecific males with designs better able to elicit cooperative female responses (Robertson and Paterson, 1982; Eberhard, 1985; Battin, 1993a,b). Diversity in male structures could thus result from either selection against heterospecific pairing (A) or sexual selection on males (B). The present study of the functional significance of the specialized front legs of the sepsid fly Archisepsis diversiformis (Ozerov) tests these hypotheses. As is common in many sepsids (Parker, 1972; Ward, 1983; Schulz, 1999; Eberhard, 2000; Blankenhorn et al., 2000), males of A. diversiformis wait for females at oviposition and feeding sites (dung and carrion), mount them before and during oviposition, and copulate when oviposition is finished. Copulation lasts approximately 20–25 min (Eberhard and Huber, 1998). Typically there are many more males than females at a given site (the mean for 297 flies at eight dung pats in Costa Rica was 2.9 males for each female). Male sepsids generally use their specialized front legs to clasp the bases of the female’s wings (Šulc, 1928; Hennig, 1949; Parker, 1972; Pont, 1979; Eberhard, 2001a). The male’s front femur and tibia are often more or less elaborately sculpted and provided with spines and setae on their ventral surfaces (Figs. 1 and 2A) and generally have species-specific shapes (e.g., the male’s front femur and tibia often differ among species in the same genus [Duda, 1925, 1926; Hennig, 1949; Pont, 1979]). There are stress receptors (campaniform sensilla) in the veins of the female wing near the area where the male’s leg clasps it (Eberhard, 2001a). Males mount females prior to copulation, and the female often resists a mounted male by shaking energetically from side to side, kicking, running, and bending her abdomen P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Function of Clasping Organs of Male Flies Fig. 2. (A) Front leg of a male A diversiformis just beginning to release its grasp on the wing of a female, showing how the specialized ventral surface of the femur meshes with the dorsal surface of the base of the wing; the curved ventral surface of the anterior–ventral thumb fits against the curved dorsal surface of the stem vein. (B) Male femur with a droplet of glue on the modified ventral surface of the femur. Scale bar, 200 µm. 835 P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] 836 pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Eberhard ventrally (Parker, 1972; Ward, 1983; Ward et al., 1992; Allen and Simmons, 1996; Blanckenhorn et al., 2000; Eberhard, 2001a, 2000). Mounted males court females (Eberhard, 2001b), and intromission requires active female collaboration in which she raises her abdomen and lifts its tip (proctiger) to expose the opening of her vulva (Eberhard, 2002). Allen and Simmons (1996) showed that males which were found copulating had more symmetric foretibiae than those found riding females prior to copulation. They argued, in possible reference to mechanical fit, that this difference was due to an inferior clasping ability of less symmetric front legs. The possibility that different stimuli from more asymmetric males induced differences in female cooperation and resistance behavior was not taken into account, however, in their discussion, despite the fact that they found that the intensity of female resistance behavior varied and thus might have influenced male success. Apparently there has been only one explicit attempt to explain why the front legs of male sepsids are so elaborate. Šulc (1928) argued that the surfaces of the femur and the tibia fit tightly against each other just behind the rear margin of the female wing, forming a clamp which snaps shut. A morphological study of six species (including A. diversiformis), in which pairs were frozen while the male clamped the female’s wings (Eberhard, 2001a), showed that this idea is incorrect: in all species the specialized areas of both the femur and the tibia pressed on the female’s wing, not against each other as proposed by Šulc. The elaborate forms of the male femur and tibia meshed very precisely with the contours of the female’s wing veins. Contrary to the predictions of the mechanical fit and male–female conflict hypotheses, however, female morphology proved to be relatively constant interspecifically. Most of the differences among the designs of males of different species seem to represent alternative ways of grasping the same generalized female morphology. Female sense organs are present in the wing near where the male grasps her. The stimulation hypothesis was thus favored by these results (Eberhard, 2001a), but mostly by a process of elimination of the others. The present study tests the four hypotheses in A. diversiformis Ozerov, using field observations, and direct manipulations of the shape of the male’s femoral clamp (Fig. 2) and of the female’s wing base. All of the hypotheses except direct male–male combat predict that experimental alteration of the form of the femoral clamp should reduce male mating success when male– female pairs are isolated from other males. A lack of reduction in mating success in this context would thus constitute strong evidence favoring the direct male–male combat hypothesis over the other three. In addition, the mechanical fit and male–female conflict of interests hypotheses predict that this reduction will be due to a reduction in the male’s ability to remain mounted. In contrast, the stimulation hypothesis predicts that the reduced mating success will be due to reduced female acceptance behavior but that P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 Function of Clasping Organs of Male Flies 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 837 this reduced acceptance will not necessarily be associated with a reduction in the male’s ability to stay mounted. A lack of reduction in the modified male’s ability to stay mounted would thus constitute evidence against the mechanical fit and conflict of interests hypotheses but would be compatible with the stimulation hypothesis. METHODS Field observations were made around fresh cow dung near San Antonio de Escazu (elevation, 1350 m), San Jośe Province, Costa Rica. Experimental modifications of males and females were performed in captivity in Panama and Costa Rica under similar conditions. All experimental flies were virgin A. diversiformis that had been raised in captivity on previously frozen cow dung and separated by sex within 18 h of emergence as adults. They had emerged as adults at least 3 days before they were used in experiments (the flies are not sexually mature until 1–2 days of age). All behavioral observations were made in the afternoon, and flies were prepared for observation at least 2 h previously that same morning. Flies in Panama were raised from females collected near the dung of howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) on Barro Colorado Island, Panama (elevation, 20 m); flies in Costa Rica were raised from females collected at cow dung near San Antonio de Escazu. Both populations are classified as A. diversiformis, but some details of male behavior during copulation differ (W. Eberhard, in preparation). They also behaved differently in the experiments (below), so their results were analyzed separately. Male front leg morphology was experimentally modified without anesthesia. Each experimental (modified) male was aspirated into a plastic sack, where he was immobilized by pressing the walls of the sac gently against a flat surface. One wing was then seized with a fine forceps, and the fly was removed and grasped by another forceps whose tips were covered with soft foam rubber. This forceps was held shut by slipping a ring over the tips, and the immobilized fly was placed under a dissecting microscope, positioned so that the portion of his body to which glue was to be applied was exposed to view. Modified male flies received a drop of water-soluble, polyvinyl glue (Resistol; H. B. Fuller Co., Costa Rica) (which was nearly transparent when dry). The glue was applied with the tip of a fine pin to the ventral surface of the distal portion of each front femur (Fig. 2B). To apply the drop and allow it to dry undisturbed, it was necessary to hold the leg partly extended by seizing the tarsus with another forceps. The tips of this forceps were covered by a thin layer of foam rubber and a piece of paper towel to avoid damage to the male’s tarsi. The male’s leg was extended for approximately 30–60 s P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] 838 pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Eberhard until the glue had dried. Each control male was immobilized as above and received a drop of glue on the dorsal surface of his thorax. Experimental females were also immobilized as above and received glue on the dorsal surface of the basal portion of each wing. The drop of glue was applied to the anterior portion of the wing at the level of the anal lobe and then gently spread toward the base of the wing with the tip of the needle, taking care not to contact the wing’s articulation with the thorax. Each control female received a drop of glue on the dorsum of her thorax. After the glue had dried, each fly was immediately introduced into the mating chamber (a 5-cm-diameter clear plastic petri dish with a droplet of honey on one wall) and left there undisturbed for at least 2 h before being tested. The entire process from removing a fly from the culture to introducing it into the mating chamber usually lasted between 5 and 10 min. Flies were assigned to control and experimental categories in strict alternation as they climbed the sides of a plastic sack over the culture. The petri dishes were labeled with nonconsecutive numbers and mixed together so that any given male’s or the female’s treatment category was not known during behavioral observations. A mating trial began when a fly of the opposite sex was removed from the culture container and introduced into the mating chamber. These flies were also assigned to be paired with control and experimental flies in strict alternation as they climbed the sides of a plastic sack over the culture. Each trial lasted 1 h, unless the male had not yet attempted to mount, in which case observations continued until he did attempt to mount or another hour had elapsed. Observations were made without magnification, so that it was impossible to see the glue droplets on the legs or the thorax. Not all behavioral details were noted in all pairs, so some sample sizes varied. Several trials were run simultaneously, with new pairs being formed as the trials of other pairs ended. Petri dishes were often moved with relation to each other during this process, further guaranteeing that data were taken blindly with respect to male and female treatment. A mount was judged to be successful if the male copulated, and unsuccessful if he dismounted without copulating. Durations of successful mounts refer to the time elapsed between initiation of the mount and initiation of copulation. Males often mounted the female more than once, but because subsequent mounts are usually much shorter and did not occur in some pairs, only data from first mounts were analyzed. Each individual was used only once. Males sometimes lost glue droplets from their legs before, during, or after mating trials, so each male was checked for the presence of glue soon after the trial ended. Because the moment when the glue was lost was not known, data were discarded if the glue was missing from both legs. Data on durations were highly skewed, so medians as well as means (each followed P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 Function of Clasping Organs of Male Flies 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 839 by ±1 standard deviation) are reported. Unless noted otherwise, all tests of significance employed nonparametric Mann–Whitney U-tests. RESULTS The behavior of pairs with control males or control females differed between Panama and Costa Rica, despite the similarity of the conditions under which flies were raised and observed. In Panama, 82.4% of 74 control pairs copulated (combining data from Tables I and II), while only 34.1% of 44 copulated in Costa Rica (χ 2 = 25.0, df = 1, P ¿ 0.001). Several patterns indicate that this difference was probably due largely to differences in female receptivity. Those females which did copulate did so with fewer mounts in Panama than in Costa Rica (Table I). In 59 of 61 control pairs that copulated in Panama, copulation occurred on the first mount (Tables I and II); the corresponding value in Costa Rica was 7 of 15 (P ¿ 0.001, Fisher exact test). The duration of successful mounts (beginning of mount until intromission) was also shorter in Panama (Table I). In contrast, variables that were presumably controlled largely if not exclusively by the male differed in some respects but not others between the two sites. There were differences in the distribution of giving up times by males (e.g., duration of first mount for unsuccessful modified males in Table I), but neither the amount of time elapsed before the male’s first mounting attempt nor the number of mounts in pairs that did not copulate differed between the two sites (P = 0.59 and 0.62, respectively, for totals for control males). Because of these differences, apparently due largely to differences in female receptivity, data from the two sites are analyzed separately below. As predicted by three of the four hypotheses, the addition of glue to the male’s front femur resulted in a sharp reduction in the probability that he would succeed in copulating. Of those males that mounted the female in Panama, only 19.2% of 52 modified males copulated (including those with glue on either one or both femora at the end of the experiment), compared with 87.5% of 48 control males (χ 2 = 43.1, df = 1, P ¿ 0.001). Corresponding values in Costa Rica showed the same pattern: 0% of 34 modified males compared with 34.1% of 44 control males (χ 2 = 14.2, df = 1, P < 0.001). In addition, males in Panama that had glue on only one leg at the end of the experiment had a greater likelihood of copulating than did males that had glue on both legs (33% of 18 vs. 3% of 32; P = 0.006, Fisher exact test) (two males were discarded from this analysis because only one leg was checked for glue). Males in Costa Rica did not lose glue from their legs and, thus, could not be analyzed in this respect. Glue on the dorsal surface of the female’s wings also reduced the likelihood that copulation would occur 21 b1 9.5 53.5 29.5 b2 68 b1 258 c1 — 9 b2 c1 49.7 ± 68.1 101.5 ± 204.5 61.6 ± 54.6 75.7 ± 115.3 174 ± 316 690 ± 961 — 28.4 ± 63.0 Median — 4.0 ± 3.4 2.8 ± 2.6 3.1 ± 2.8 2.4 ± 2.1 6.8 ± 4.2 1.2 ± 0.8 3.7 ± 2.7 — 4 c3 2 c1 2 1 c2 6 c3 1 c1 c2 3.5 Median Number of mounts Mean 0 34 15 29 10 42 42 6 N — 14.3 ± 21.6 14.3 ± 21.6 3.8 ± 3.8 20.8 ± 25.8 15.2 ± 22.6 10.1 ± 6.6 13.7 ± 17.3 13.1 ± 16.0 14.1 ± 16.8 27.7 ± 29.7 15.8 ± 19.0 Mean — 7 7 2.5 12.5 5 10 6 6 8.5 19 9 Median 0 34 34 14 29 43 8 41 49 42 6 48 N Time elapsed before first mount (min) 16:10 840 U tests (an exception was the number of mounts; they could not be compared between successful and unsuccessful males, because successful males had shorter lengths of time with a virgin female). Only values followed by the same italic letter and number differ significantly (b, P < 0.01; c, P < 0.001). November 8, 2002 0 34 15 29 10 42 42 6 N pp699-joir-457250-08 a Means are given for illustrative purposes only, as the data were highly skewed. Medians in the same column were compared with Mann–Whitney Panama Control males Successful Unsuccessful Total Modified males Successful Unsuccessful Total Costa Rica Control males Successful Unsuccessful Total Modified males Successful Unsuccessful Total Mean Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] Duration of first mount (s) Table I. Durations and Numbers of Mounts by Males with Glue on Their Front Femora (Modified Males) and on the Thorax (Control Males)a P1: ZCK Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Eberhard 19 2 77 11 23.5 ± 12.7 2.9 ± 3.2 77.0 ± 94.8 117 ± 320 2 10 13 7 N 1.0 4.2 ± 3.3 1.0 3.7 ± 2.7 1 4 1 3.5 Median Number of mounts Mean 2 10 19 7 N 6.5 ± 6.4 30.8 ± 39.9 26.8 ± 37.4 23.4 ± 40.9 43.9 ± 38.0 30.6 ± 40.2 Mean 6.5 12.5 11 8 28.5 12 Median 2 10 12 13 7 20 N Time elapsed before first mount (min) November 8, 2002 U tests; no differences were significant. pp699-joir-457250-08 a Means are given for illustrative purposes only, as the data were highly skewed. Medians in the same column were compared with Mann–Whitney Control females Successful Unsuccessful Total Modified females Successful Unsuccessful Total Median Duration of first mount (s) Mean Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] Table II. Durations and Numbers of Mounts of Females in Panama with Glue on Their Wing Bases (Modified Females) and with Glue on the Thorax (Control Females)a P1: ZCK 16:10 Function of Clasping Organs of Male Flies Style file version Feb 08, 2000 841 P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] 842 pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Eberhard (16.7% of 12 modified females compared with 73.0% of 26 control females) in Panama (χ 2 = 10.6, df = 1, P = 0.0012). The reductions in copulatory success were not clearly due to modified males being less able to remain mounted on females, as predicted by the mechanical fit and male–female conflict hypotheses. In Costa Rica mount durations of unsuccessful modified males were shorter than those of unsuccessful control males as predicted, but this was not the case in other within-site comparisons for which sufficient data were available. The durations of both successful and unsuccessful mounts by modified males in Panama were not significantly different from the control values (P = 0.22 and 0.30, respectively; P = 0.26 when they were combined), and the trend was the opposite of that predicted. Taking as a reference point the median duration of a mount by a control male that ended in success in Panama (21 s before intromission), 26 of 42 modified males in Panama that failed to copulate nevertheless stayed mounted for longer than 21 s. In addition, durations of unsuccessful mounts by males in Panama that had glue on both front legs were, if anything, longer (mean = 72.1 ± 119.7 s, median = 29 s; N = 32) than those of males with glue on only one front leg (mean = 46.5 ± 79.1 s, median = 10 s; N = 18; P = 0.66). Similarly, the durations of unsuccessful mounts on modified females were also longer (mean = 117 ± 320 s, median = 11 s; N = 10) than those on control females (mean = 2.9 ± 3.2 s, median = 2 s; N = 7). In fact, longer mounts were not positively correlated with mating success, as expected. The most extreme deviation occurred in control males in Costa Rica, in which 22 of 29 failed mounts lasted longer than the median duration of 14 successful mounts. Data from modified females also showed a trend in the opposite direction from that predicted by the hypotheses: unsuccessful mounts were slightly longer than successful mounts) (P = 0.06). A second trend also argues against the mechanical fit and male–female conflict hypotheses. Mounts by modified males seldom ended with events indicating that the male had been forcibly shaken off by the female. Dismounts by modified males that occurred during shaking movements by the female (“forced dismounts”) were rare (5.8% of 121 mounts checked in Panama for this detail) and were not less common in control males (8.9% of 45; χ 2 = 0.49, df = 1, P > 0.4). The small difference that existed was the opposite of that predicted by the mechanical fit and male–female conflict hypotheses (modified males were forcibly shaken off slightly less often). In the large majority of cases, the male climbed off during a period when the female was not shaking. Video recordings confirmed that dismounts during periods of shaking by the female are also unusual in Costa Rica (Baena and Eberhard, in preparation). P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 Function of Clasping Organs of Male Flies 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 843 There was no indication that the reduced copulatory success of modified males was due to their being less motivated to mount females. The time elapsed before the male’s first mounting attempt did not differ significantly between modified males and control males (Table I; P = 0.65 and 0.65 for totals in Panama and Costa Rica). The frequency with which a male failed to attempt to mount a female at least once during the observation period was no greater in modified males (7 cases among 90 males) than in control males (8 cases among 99 males). Similarly, neither the duration of the first mount nor the delay to mount differed significantly when males paired with modified and control females were compared (Table I; P = 0.65 and 0.64, respectively). The ability of modified males to remain mounted was not due to a lack of female resistance. In fact, females in Panama were more likely to perform especially energetic rejection movements (strong shaking, often combined with leg pushes and ventral flexion of the abdomen) when they were mounted by modified males (90.7% of 43 mounts by modified males checked for this detail, 44.8% of 29 mounts by control males; χ 2 = 16.6, df = 1, P ¿ 0.001). Observations of males mounted on females in the field as the female moved over the surface of fresh cow dung showed that males did not have to battle other males to remain mounted. In more than 25 encounters between a mounted male and a solitary male, the males never struggled with each other. The most aggressive interactions involved only a single brief strike by the solitary male (possibly an aborted mounting attempt) that was immediately followed by his walking away. DISCUSSION The consistent failure of solitary males to attempt to forcefully dislodge mounted males they encountered in the field argues against the importance of direct male–male combat as an explanation of the male clasping organ. Violent male–male battles over females do occur in some sepsids (Zerbe [1993] on Sepsis punctum; Eberhard [2001a] on A. pleuralis) but are rare or have never been seen in several others (Eberhard [2001a] on A. ecalcarata, A. discolor, and the closely related Palaeosepsis pusio). An additional indication that male–male combat has not been important in the evolution of front leg clasping organs in A. diversiformis is that it does not predict the reduction in copulatory success that was observed in isolated pairs when the male’s front femur or the female’s wings were modified. This hypothesis thus seems improbable and is omitted from the following discussion. The prediction by the remaining three hypotheses, that experimental modification of the male’s clasping organ or the female wing would P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] 844 pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Eberhard reduce the male’s copulation success, was confirmed. But the prediction of the mechanical fit and male–female conflict hypotheses that this decrease in copulations would be due to a reduction in the male’s ability to hold onto the female was not confirmed. Males generally dismounted spontaneously, rather than being forcibly dislodged by the female; forced dismounts were not more common in modified males than control males, despite the fact that females resisted modified males more actively; mounts by modified males were not consistently shorter; mounts by males with both front legs modified were not shorter than those of males with only one leg modified; and mounts of females with modified wings were not consistently shorter. The expectation that males which held on longer would be more likely to obtain copulations was also contradicted. Longer mounts were also not associated with copulation success in Sepsis cynipsea (Blankenhorn et al., 2000). The significance of the short durations of mounts by modified males in Costa Rica remains uncertain. While their overall brevity fits the expectations of the mechanical fit and male–female conflict hypotheses, the distribution of mount durations, most of which were extremely brief, does not fit the expected gradual elimination of males which were less able to remain mounted. Control males from the same site that failed to copulate tended to remain mounted for relatively long periods. Reduced female cooperation after the male had mounted, with respect to both his remaining mounted and his achieving intromission, probably contributed to the lower copulation success of both modified males and males mounted on modified females, as predicted by the stimulation hypothesis. In Panama female resistance to modified males was more vigorous than to control males. Active female cooperation (lifting the abdomen and flexing the proctiger dorsally to expose the vulva) is needed for intromission to occur in this and related species (Eberhard, 2002). It might be that other stimuli from the male rather than those from his front legs were responsible for reductions in female acceptance. For example, the glue on the male’s femur or on the female’s wing may have altered the stimuli the male received when he grasped the female’s wings and reduced his tendency to perform courtship behavior or attempts to establish genitalic contact, causing him to thus fail to elicit the female cooperation that leads to copulation (Eberhard, 2002). Subtle differences in the male’s behavior after he mounted the female, rather than in the morphology of his front femora, might have been responsible for the increased female rejection behavior. The possibility that manipulations of male display traits result in subtle alterations of male behavior of this sort has been ignored in many experimental studies of female choice (Andersson, 1982; Basolo, 1990a,b; Thornhill and Sauer, 1991; Møller, 1994). But ignoring this possiblity seems unjustified, as some experimental modifications of male morphology do indeed cause P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] pp699-joir-457250-08 Function of Clasping Organs of Male Flies November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 845 changes in male behavior (Briceño and Eberhard, 2002). Further behavioral observations will thus be needed to test the possibility of such changes in A. diversiformis. Similarly, further behavioral observations of mixed-species aggregations in the field will be needed to discriminate possible selection for species isolation versus sexual selection. Because the differences in the durations of mounts in this study were generally the result of male decisions to dismount, rather than of the male being physically forced off, the related possibility that differences were due to male choice must also be considered. Male choice seems unlikely to be important in this species, at least in nature, because A. diversiformis sex ratios at mating sites are highly male-biased, and the majority of male mounting attempts in the field do not even result in the male clamping the female’s wings (Eberhard, 2001a). Interspecific male choice on the basis of mechanical stimuli from the female wing also seems improbable due to the long durations of male mounts when female wings were modified and to the high degree of similarity between the wings of females of different species in this genus (Eberhard, 2001a). With respect to the stimulation hypothesis, the sharp reduction in male copulatory success documented here is important, because if the data had failed to show a reduction, they would have justified the rejection of this hypothesis. In one sense, the experimental test was conservative, in that only virgin females were used. Females need to mate at least once to reproduce, so virgins may be less selective than nonvirgins (Jackson, 1981; Jennions and Petrie, 1997). If sperm precedence in A. diversiformis is similar to the almost-complete last male precedence in another sepsid, S. punctum (Schulz, 1999), then female A. diversiformis may be able virtually to erase the effects of early matings and may, thus, be even more disposed to accept copulation when they are virgins. Despite these reasons to expect a lack of female selectivity, females rejected many males in the experiments described here. In another sense, the test of the stimulation hypothesis was crude. The experimental modifications of male femur form were relatively large compared with the differences among closely related species (compare Figs. 1 and 2B). Similarly, experimental modification of the female wing altered both the form of the female wing and probably also the responses from the stress receptors in the area clasped by the male. Females of another species, A. armata, were apparently able to distinguish, however, mounts by males of A. diversiformis (Eberhard, 2001a), despite the fact that the male front leg morphology of this species is especially similar to that of A. diversus (Fig. 1). Virgin female A. armata vigorously resisted mounts by male A. diversiformis and failed to copulate with them but then quickly copulated when they were mounted by male A. armata (Eberhard, 2001a). P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] 846 pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Eberhard The three hypotheses can be evaluated more extensively by combining the results reported here with independent data from a detailed comparative study of male and female morphology in six species of sepsid (Eberhard, 2001a). The morphological data also contradicted predictions of both the mechanical fit and the male–female conflict hypotheses for five of the six species, because female morphology was uniform in all species except one, instead of being species-specific as predicted by these hypotheses (the possibility of species-specific female resistance behavior was not tested, however). In addition, female wings also generally lacked structures predicted by the mechanical fit and male–female conflict hypothesis that could impede clamping attempts by either heterospecific or conspecific males. The morphological data confirmed one prediction of the stimulation hypothesis: in all six species there are female sense organs in the wing near the site gripped by the male’s leg. The present study and the morphological study are thus in accord in providing data that are difficult to reconcile with the mechanical fit and male–female conflict hypotheses and that conform to some predictions of the stimulation hypothesis. They are also in accord, however, in needing further observations of behavioral details before final conclusions can be drawn. One striking result of the morphological study was that there were very detailed and precise fits between the male and the female structures in different species: female structures were relatively invariable, and males of different species grasped the relative uniform female morphology in different ways. The present study suggests the somewhat surprising conclusion that such precise fits are not necessary to allow a male to maintain his grasp on the female. Even males with gross modifications of their clamping structures (Fig. 1) and males that grasped modified female wings were able to stay mounted for long periods, despite active female resistance. The evidence against the male–female conflict hypothesis is particularly significant in these flies, because females often gave clear behavioral indications of resistance to male attempts to copulate. As in other sepsids (Parker, 1972; Ward, 1983; Ward et al., 1992; Allen and Simmons, 1996), they often resist males in nature (Eberhard, 2001a). Nevertheless, male–female conflict is not reflected in either the designs or the functional attributes of the male’s front legs and the female’s wings. Possibly female resistance behavior such as shaking functions to screen males on the basis of the resulting stimulation of her wings or to communicate to the male her unwillingness to copulate (or both) (see Linley and Adams [1974], Linley and Hinds [1975], Linley and Mooks [1975], Arnqvist [1997], Crean and Gilburn [1998], Rodriguez [1998], and Blankenhorn et al. [2000] for studies of this and other possible functions of female resistance behavior in insects). Blankenhorn et al. (2000) concluded that female shaking behavior in another sepsid, P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] pp699-joir-457250-08 Function of Clasping Organs of Male Flies November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 847 S. cynipsea, did not appear to function in assessing male ability or inclination to stay mounted; they did not evaluate the possibility that females were assessing male stimulation of their wings. There are three other experimental studies of species-specific nongenitalic structures specialized to grasp females during or immediately preceding copulation whose results can be compared directly with those of this study. Experimental disabling of the clasping notal organ of male Panorpa vulgaris scorpionflies by covering it with an adhesive resulted in changes that favored the male–female conflict hypothesis and argued against the stimulation hypothesis (Thornhill and Sauer, 1991). Panorpa females apparently determine the duration of copulation, and modified males, which could not grasp the female’s wing, copulated for shorter times and transferred smaller amounts of sperm (Thornhill and Sauer, 1991). Thornhill and Sauer rejected an alternative explanation based on the stimulation hypothesis, because when females were hungry (deprived of food for the 7–8 days of their adult life) and males were well fed and able to feed the female abundantly with saliva masses, the effect of modifying the male clasping organ was eliminated. Copulations with modified males were as long as those with control males. They argued that this trend, which was predicted by the male–female conflict hypothesis, constitutes evidence against the stimulation hypothesis because they supposed that female stimulation criteria for copulation duration are likely to be consistent under all conditions. This supposition is weak, however, in light of the substantial intraspecific variation in mating criteria in many other species (Jennions and Petrie, 1997). Their rejection of a stimulatory function was also weak because they did not consider the possibility that stimulation of the female by the notal organ is important in other contexts. For example, it might affect crucial female reproductive processes such as oviposition and remating (Eberhard, 1996). In sum, the rejection was not completely convincing. The results of two other studies were similar to those of this study, in that they favored the stimulation hypothesis over the mechanical fit and male– female conflict hypotheses. In two species of Ischnura damselflies, modification of the species-specific form of the male’s abdominal clasping organ did not reduce the male’s ability to grasp the female, but a female grasped by a modified male was much less likely to bend her abdomen forward to copulate (Kreiger and Kreiger-Loibl, 1958; Loibl, 1958). Removal of the elaborate, species-specific portion of the clasping organ on the male’s antenna in the fairy shrimp Eubranchipus serratus also reduced the likelihood of copulation but did not reduce the male’s ability to grasp females (Belk, 1984). All three studies have the shortcoming that they did not include tests for possible changes in male behavior resulting from modification of their clasping organs. P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 16:10 848 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Eberhard ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I thank A. Ozerov for kindly identifying flies, H. Herz for help with German translations, Angel Aguirre for help obtaining references, and Göran Arnqvist for criticizing an early draft. The manuscript also benefited substantially from the comments of three anonymous reviewers. As have many others before me, I profited from Stan Rand’s patient and thoughtful advice. Part of this work was carried out on Barro Colorado Island, and I thank the staff there for making it so easy and pleasant to work there. Financial support was provided by the Vicerrectorı́a de Investigación of the Universidad de Costa Rica and the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute. REFERENCES Alexander, R. D., Marshall, D., and Cooley, J. (1997). Evolutionary perspectives on insect mating. In Choe, J., and Crespi, B. (eds.), The Evolution of Mating Systems in Insects and Arachnids, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 4–31. Allen, G. R., and Simmons, L. W. (1996). Coercive mating, fluctuating asymmetry and male mating success in the dung fly, Sepsis cynipsea. Anim. Behav. 52: 737–741. Andersson, M. (1982). Female choice selects for extreme tail length in a widowbird. Nature 299: 818–820. Arnqvist, G. (1997). The evolution of water strider mating systems: Causes and consequences of sexual conflicts. In Choe, J., and Crespie, B. (eds.), The Evolution of Mating Systems in Insects and Arachnids, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Arnqvist, G., and Rowe, L. (1995). Sexual conflict and arms races between the sexes: A morphological adaptation for control of mating in a female insect. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 261: 123–127. Basolo, A. (1990a). Female preference for male sword length in the green swordtail, Xiphophorus helleri (Pisces: Poeciliidae). Anim. Behav. 40: 332–338. Basolo, A. (1990b). Female preference predates the evolution of the sword in the swordtail fish. Science 250: 808–810. Battin, T. J. (1993a). The odonate mating system, communication and sexual selection. Boll. Zool. 60: 353–360. Battin, T. J. (1993b). Revision of the puella group of the genus Coenagrion Kirby, 1890 (Odonata, Zygoptera), with emphasis on morphologies contributing to reproductive isolation. Hydrobiologia 262: 13–29. Belk, D. (1984) Antennal appendages and reproductive success in the Anostraca. J. Crust.Biol. 4: 66–71. Blankenhorn, W., Mühlhäuser, C., Morrf, C., Reusch, T., and Reuter, M. (2000). Female choice, female reluctance to mate and sexual selection on body size in the dung fly Sepsis cynipsea. Ethology 106: 1–17. Briceño, R. D., and Eberhard, W. G. (2002). Courtship in the medfly Ceratitis capitata includes probable tactile stimulation with the male’s aristae. Entomol. Exp. Appl. (in press). Crean, C. S., and Gilburn, A. S. (1998). Sexual selection as a side-effect of sexual conflict in the seaweed fly, Coelopa ursina (Diptera: Coelopidae). Anim. Behav. 56: 1405–1410. Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex, Modern Library, New York. Duda, O. (1925). Monographie der Sepsiden (Dipt.) I. Ann. Naturhist. Mus. Wien, 39: 1–153. Duda, O. (1926). Monographie der Sepsiden (Dipt.) II. Ann. Naturhist. Mus. Wien 40: 1–110. P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] pp699-joir-457250-08 Function of Clasping Organs of Male Flies November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 849 Eberhard, W. G. (1985). Sexual Selection and Animal Genitalia, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. Eberhard, W. G. (1996). Female Control: Sexual Selection by Cryptic Female Choice, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Eberhard, W. G. (2000). Behavior and reproductive status of Microsepsis armillata flies away from oviposition sites. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 93: 966–971. Eberhard, W. G. (2001a). The functional morphology of species-specific clasping structures on the front legs of male sepsid flies. Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 133: 335–368. Eberhard, W. G. (2001b). Courtship and multi-stage transfer of material to the female’s wings during copulation in Microsepsis armillata (Diptera: Sepsidae). J. Kans. Entomol. Soc. 74: 23–31. Eberhard, W. G. (2002). Female resistance or screening? Male force vs. female collaboration in intromission in sepsid flies. Rev. Biol. Trop. 50: 485–505. Eberhard, W. G., and Huber, B. A. (1998). Copulation and sperm transfer in Archisepsis flies (Diptera: Sepsidae) and the evolution of their intromittent genitalia. Stud. Dipt. 5: 217–248. Fraser, F. C. (1943). The function and comparative anatomy of the oreillets in the Odonata. Proc. Roy. Entomol. Soc. Lond. A 18: 50–56. Freitag, R. (1974). Selection for nongenitalic mating structure in female tiger beetles of the genus Cicindella (Coleoptera: Cicindelidae). Can. Entomol. 106: 561–568. Hennig, W. (1949). Sepsidae. In Linder, E. (ed.) Die Fliegen der Palaearktischen Region, D. Schweizerbart’sche Verlagsbuchhandlung (Erwin Nagele), Stuttgart. Holland, B., and Rice, W. R. (1998). Chase-away selection: Antagonistic seduction vs. resistance. Evolution 52: 1–7. Jackson, R. R. (1981). Relationship between reproductive security and intersexual selection in a jumping spider, Phidippus johnsoni (Araneae: Salticidae). Evolution 35: 601–604. Jennions, M. D., and Petrie, M. (1997). Variations in mate choice and mating preferences: A review of causes and consequences. Biol. Rev. 72: 283–327. Kreiger, F., and Kreiger-Loibl, E. (1958). Beiträge zum Verhalten von Ischnura elegans und Ischnura pumilio (Odonata). Z. Tierpsychol. 15: 82–93. Linley, J. R., and Adams, G. M. (1974). Sexual receptivity in Culicoides melleus (Diptera: Ceratopogonidae). Trans. Roy. Entomol. Soc. Lond. 126: 279–303. Linley, J. R., and Hinds, M. J. (1975). Quantity of the male ejaculate influenced by female unreceptivity in the fly Culicoides melleus. J. Insect Physiol. 21: 281–285. Linley, J. R., and Mook, M. S. (1975). Behavioural interaction between sexually experienced Culicoides melleus (Coquillett) (Diptera Ceratopogonidae). Behaviour 54: 97– 110. Lloyd, J. E. (1979). Mating behavior and natural selection. Fla. Entomol. 62: 17–23. Loibl, E. (1958). Zur Ethologie un Biologie der deutschen Lestiden (Odonata). Z. Tierpsychol. 15: 54–81. Møller, A. P. (1994). Sexual Selection and the Barn Swallow, Oxford University Press, New York. Parker, G. (1972). Reproductive behaviour of Sepsis cynipsea (L.) (Diptera: Sepsidae). I. Preliminary analysis of the reproductive strategy and its associated behaviour patterns. Behaviour 41: 172–206. Pont, A. (1979). Sepsidae. Diptera Cyclorrhapha Acalyptrata. Handbook for the Identification of British Insects, Vol. 10, 5(c), pp. 1–35. Robertson, G. G., and Paterson, H. E. H. (1982). Mate recognition and mechanical isolation in Enallagma damselflies (Odonata: Coenagrionidae). Evolution 36: 243–250. Rodriguez, R. L. (1998). Possible female choice during copulation in Ozophora baranowskii (Heteroptera: Lygaeidae): Female behavior, multiple copulations, and sperm transfer. J. Insect Behav. 11: 725–741. Shapiro, A. M., and Porter, A. H. (1989). The lock-and-key hypothesis: Evolutionary and biosystematic interpretation of insect genitalia. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 34: 231–245. Schulz, K. (1999). The Evolution of Mating Systems in Black Scavenger Flies (Diptera: Sepsidae), Ph.D. thesis, University of Arizona, Tucson. P1: ZCK Journal of Insect Behavior [joib] 850 pp699-joir-457250-08 November 8, 2002 16:10 Style file version Feb 08, 2000 Eberhard Šulc, K. (1928). Biologicky vyznam vyzbroje prednich samcich nohou u sepsid (Muscidae). Biol. Spisy Vysoke Skoly Zverolekarske Brno. 7: 181–194. Thornhill, R., and Sauer, K. P. (1991). The notal organ of the scorpionfly (Panorpa vulgaris): An adaptation to coerce mating duration. Behav. Ecol. 2: 156–164. Toro, H., and de la Hoz, E. (1976). Factores mecánicos en la aislación reproductive de Apoidea (Hymenoptera). Rev. Soc. Entomol. Arg. 35: 193–202. Ward, P. I. (1983). The effects of size on the mating behaviour of the dung fly Sepsis cynipsea. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 13: 75–80. Ward, P. I., Hemmi, H., and Roösli, T. (1992). Sexual conflict in the dung fly Sepsis cynipsea. Funct. Ecol. 6: 649–653. Zerbe, F. (1993). Innerartliche Grössenvariabilität und Paarungsverhalten bei Sepsis punctum (Fabricius, 1794) [Diptera, Sepsidae], Diplomarbeit, Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg, Würzburg.