UNIT 4, Sensation and Perception Notes, pp. 115-173 (Updated for 2011; these go with Chapters 5 and 6 in 2007 book) Prosopognosia, face blindness, page 115 What are sensation and perception? What do we mean by bottom up and top down processing? 116: Sensation is referred to as bottom up processing: from the senses up to the brain; this is when we have no prior knowledge of something and we start at the bottom. Perception is top down processing: higher level function where you draw conclusions; we apply prior knowledge to something An example is stereotyping someone. We apply what we already know about the characteristics and make a judgment even though we don’t know them. Stereotyping does help because it is a mental short cut and we need to make them every day, for certain stimuli, on a regular basis in order to navigate our world. Selective Attention, 117 How are we affected by selective attention? Selective attention you focus on only one stimulus in the environment People with ADHD have difficulty with this; they can’t tune anything out and take it all in. a. Cocktail party effect focusing on one voice in a crowd at a party, for example b. Unnoticed stimuli still has an effect on you: example, women who were played a tune that they didn’t pay attention to still preferred it later c. Cell phone drivers have more accidents. Even pedestrians on cell phones are less safe. Selective Inattention, 118 c. inattentional blindness Neisser, Becklen and Cervone in their experiment people did not notice a woman walking across a basketball court because they were directed to count the number of times black shirt players passed a ball. This kind of blindness is defined as failing to see objects when our attention is directed elsewhere. Simons and Chabris did a replication in 1999. See fig. 4.2 Other examples of inattentional blindness: Change blindness lack of attention to changing events or images in a scene; for examples see Fig 4.3 Change deafness failing to recognize a change in the way a person is speaking when the words are difficult Choice blindness not recognizing a change in the choice you have made The pop-out phenomenon when a strikingly distinct image jumps out and gathers our attention What are the absolute and difference thresholds? Do stimuli below the threshold have any influence? 120 Psychophysics: the study of the relationship between physical characteristics of stimuli and the psychological perception of them Absolute threshold: the lowest amount of a stimulus to notice it 50% of the time Gustav Fechner did early research on this topic. Ex.: turning down the radio volume so that you only notice the faint sound half the time Thresholds increase with age. Signal Detection theory: arousal, expectations, experience and motivation all affect whether or not you detect a stimulus Subliminal stimulus: any stimulus below your absolute threshold Ex: a microscopic cell that you cannot see with your naked eye We can be influenced by information just below our threshold. An image or word can prime our response to a later question. We can feel what we do not know and experience responses to imperceptibly brief stimuli. This is due to context effects. Subliminal advertisements do have an affect on you, but do not persuade you. The FCC banned them in 1974 after a James Vicary claimed to have influenced movie goers with subliminal messages in a movie. Greenwald conducted 16 studies about subliminal self-help programs and found no effect. The difference threshold is the lowest difference you can detect between two stimuli 50% of the time. 122 Aka just noticeable difference, jnd Ex: noticing the difference between 1 kg. And 1.02 kg 50% of the time Weber’s Law: two stimuli must differ in percentages or ratios or proportions for a person to notice it Sensory adaptation,123 What is the function of sensory adaptation? Lowered sensitivity due to constant pressure from a stimulus Ex: you go into someone’s house and notice a smell, but this only lasts for a while because sensory adaptation allows you to focus on other stimuli Ex: you notice your foot in contact with the floor, but then move on to other stimuli We focus on informative changes in our environment. We perceive the world as it is useful for us to perceive it. II. Vision p. 124 What is the energy we perceive as visible light? Transduction: sensory energy is converted/transformed into neural energy The Stimulus Input: Light Energy Wavelength: distance between peaks determines hue (the color we see) of light Amplitudes: Height of wave; Intensity (influences brightness) and loudness Acuity: normal, nearsightedness, farsightedness A. Parts of the eye How does the eye transform light energy into neural messages? Pupil adjusts the opening Iris muscle regulating pupil Lens structure behind pupil that changes shape to focus images on the retina Accommodation: lens changes shape to focus near or far objects on the retina Retina has 125 million rods (detect brightness) and 6 million cones (detect color) REVIEW TABLE 4.1 ON PAGE 128 Optic nerve: carries neural impulses from the eye to the brain Receptive fields: collection of rod and cone receptors that funnel signals to a particular visual cell Lateral antagonism: affect on neighboring receptive fields Acuity: sharpness of vision Nearsightedness light rays are focused in front of the retina Farsightedness light rays are focused behind the retina Path of vision information: rods/cones to bipolar cells to ganglion cells to optic nerve Ganglia: cells in the retina that generate action potentials Photoreceptors: nerve cells in the retina that code light energy into neural activity Optic disc: hole in the retina where the optic nerve exits the eye: no receptors there… Blind spot: there are no receptor cells where the optic nerve leaves the retina, but the two eyes compensate for each other’s blind spot, plus our eyes are constantly moving Fovea: light is centrally focused here where there are only cones; see figure 4.10 Light travels to the back of the retina and enters rod and cones; the next process is a chemical reaction in bipolar cells; finally, ganglion cells become active; their axons connect with the optic nerve before the information is sent to the brain for processing. B. Visual information processing p. 128 How does the brain process visual information? Hubel and Wiesel discovered Feature detectors: sensitive to specific features we see. The information is then sent to other cortical features where “supercell clusters” interpret the data. Parallel processing: processing lots of information at the same time; page 130, see fig. 4.15 Computers use serial processing Recognizing a face requires 30 percent of your cortical brain power (ten times what you need to perceive sounds) Example: seeing an orange and recognizing the color, round shape and bumpy texture Blind sight: you cannot perceive the object but you can locate it. The neural areas that bring vision into awareness are impaired. Visual information process stages: Retinal processing Feature detection Parallel processing Recognition (see fig. 4.16) C. Color vision 132 What theories help explain color vision? Chromatic v. achromatic (color v. black and white) Young-Hemholtz trichromatic theory of color vision: light is detected by three types of cones sensitive to red, blue and green Evidence for theory: color blindness: see figure 4.17, page 132 Subtractive color mixing for paints: the more colors you add, the fewer that are reflected back; red, blue and yellow equal black Addititive color mixing with paints: adding more colors leads to more light waves becoming white Opponent process theory: (Ewald Hering, Jameson, Hurvich) color is then processed by opponent colors. Example: yellow is inhibited and blue is exhibited Evidence for: negative after image; see fig. 4.18 Combined theory: color processing occurs in two stages: a. retina’s red, green, and blue cones respond to different color stimuli b. their signals are processed by the nervous system’s opponent process cells en route to the visual cortex Color constancy you perceive the color as same even as the amount of light differs III. Hearing (Audition) 134 Frequency: number of waves per second Pitch: a tone’s highness or lowness Decibels: measuring unit for sound energy Audition: sound is converted into neural impulses Outer ear: auditory canal Middle ear: ear drum; hammer, anvil, stirrup these vibrate when ear drum is stimulated Inner ear: cochlea and basilar membrane: vibrations cause the cochlea’s membrane to vibrate, jostling the fluid that fills the tube. This causes ripples in the basilar membrane, bending hair cells lining its surface. The hair cell movement triggers impulses in adjacent nerve cells, whose axons converge to form the auditory nerve. Neural messages are then sent to the temporal lobe’s auditory cortex. See fig. 4.19, page 135 Hair cell damage leads to most hearing loss. Perceiving Pitch, 137 What theories help us understand pitch perception? Hermann von Helmholtz’s Place theory: we hear different pitches because a different place in the cochlea is stimulated Frequency theory: the speed of neural impulses matches the speed of the sound wave The direction of sound: directly in front or behind sounds are more difficult to locate; we locate sounds by the timing of the arrival of the sound waves to our ears Volley principle neural cells alternate firing in rapid succession to achieve a combined frequency of 1000 times per second Place and frequency theory combine to explain intermediate pitches Locating Sounds, 138 How do we locate sounds? One ear hears the sound as more intense and sooner. Hearing loss and deaf culture 138 What the common causes of hearing loss, and why does controversy surround cochlear implants? Conduction deafness: damage to ear drums or bones causes loss of hearing Nerve deafness (sensorineural hearing losss) damage to cochlea, hair cells or basilar membrane causes loss of hearing; may lose sensitivity to different tones; the hearing loss is uneven and not total Conductive deafness: problems disrupt the conduction of sound through the outer and middle ear affecting hearing before the sound reaches the cochlea and the nerve receptors of the inner ear. Cochlear implants author. a bionic ear for nerve deafness; said to be controversial by the Sensory compensation Lacking one sensation people can compensate using other senses Example: aphasics can read lying behavior: they can’t speak but focus more on facial cues, voice and body movements IV. Other important senses p. 141 A. Touch How do we sense touch and sense our body’s position and movement? How do we experience pain? Composed of four senses: warmth, pain, cold, pressure (the only one with identifiable receptors) Warm + cold = hot Pressure + cold= wet Pressure + pain= tickling itch Hairy skin: contains hair cells which detect movement and pressure Glabrous skin: no hair cells; hands, feet, lips; more sensitive Rubber hand illusion: fig. 4.23 Kinesthesis: sense of position and movement of your body parts the sensors are in the muscles, joints and tendons Ian Waterman lost this sense. Uses his sense of vision to move; has difficulty in darkness. Vestibular sense: fluids in the semicircular canals, vestibular sacs, and cochlea detect body position, motion and balance Propioception: loss of ability to move unless you can see yourself move Phantom Limb: recepting neurons still fire resulting in “feeling” in a nonexistent limb What is Pain? Page 145 Ashlyn Blocker can’t feel pain Nociceptors: these detect harm to the body Gate control theory Robert Melzack and Patrick Wall: the spinal cord has gates that open or close to transmit pain messages Small fibers open to transmit pain; large fibers close to block pain Endorphins: natural pain killers Phantom limb sensations 7/10 amputees have feelings in missing limbs Phantom sounds, the sound of silence, tinnitus Pain physical and psychological interpretations that can be distracted; V.S. Ramachandran experiment: a variation of the rubber hand illusion Biopsychosocial model of pain, fig. 4.25: Nerve activity, brain interpretations, expectations, presence of others all influence pain Controlling pain, 145 Some believe hypnosis can reduce pain; others say it merely distracts the mind; see Unit 5 Placebos are used to great effect; example Acupuncture Exercise and electric stimulation also ease pain Fire walking: based on heat diffusion, the hot wood was a poor heat conductor Lamaze: relaxation, counter stimulation and distraction method used for child birth B. Taste Gustatory sense p. 146 How do we experience taste? Is a chemical sense of sweet, sour, salty, bitter and umami (a meaty taste) table 4.2 Bitter or sour plants are often toxic, while sweet ones are often nutritious Taste buds generate every 1-2 weeks but smoking, alcohol and drinking all lower sensitivity Supertasters (Linda Bartoshuk research) they have more fungiform papillae and feel more pain from lesions on the tongue or from other irritants; and they are better at perceiving fat in food Expectations also influence taste Sensory interaction P. 148 When you cannot distinguish apple v. potato, for example, without tasting AND smelling Also influences what we hear: McGurk Effect (p. 148) seeing someone say something, while hearing another sound, leading to the perception of a third blending of the other two Synaesthesia p. 148: one sensation produces another: “hearing” a color or “tasting” a number B. Smell or Olfaction p.148 How do we experience smell? Olfactory bulbs, olfactory cilia see figure 4.28 The chemical information goes from the nose directly to the temporal lobe. This is the only sense not first transmitted to the thalamus for processing; instead going to the olfactory brain, see figure 4.30 on page 150 Dogs have many times more olfactory receptors. Pheromones: A chemical secreted externally by an organism to send information to members of the same species. Males and females (gender) can be identified based on odor alone. People come to like scents associated with good experiences. Smell and memory: areas that process memories and smells are connected by brain circuitry. Perceptual Organization, 151 How did the Gestalt psychologists understand perceptual organization? a. Visual capture when senses compete with one another, vision captures and overwhelms the others Examples: 1. Where does sound come from in a theater v. where is it perceived? 2. A ventriloquist Gestalt is an organized whole: we have a tendency to perceive the environment of pieces in terms of the big picture Form perception, 151 How do figure-ground and grouping principles contribute to our perceptions? a. Figure is the object we focus on; Ground is the background in the environment b. Figure-ground: humans have a tendency to organize their perception so that an object of focus is separated from the background fig. 4.32 (a tessellation, also called a repeating geometric pattern) c. Grouping: these are perceptual rules we follow (see visuals on p.152) i. Proximity items close together are viewed as being together(when someone we don’t know walks with us, can make us uncomfortable) ii. Closure iii. Connectedness iv. Similarity if they look the same they are grouped together (but what about when someone you don’t know wears exactly the same outfit?) v. Continuity Depth perception, 153 How do we see the world in three dimensions? partly innate; it allows us to see 2D images in 3D Visual cliff, fig. 4.35 Gibson & Walk, 1960, placed kids on a visual cliff (age 6-14 mos.); most refuse indicating depth perception Binocular cues: you need both eyes to perceive the depth levels 1. Retinal disparity: binocular cue in which the greater the disparity the farther the image is away Another binocular cue to distance is Convergence monocular cues; the more eyes turn inward, the closer the object h. Monocular cues: distance cues that are available to either eye Gateway Arch is a horizontal vertical illusion; fig. 4.37 Examples (see page 155) Relative size if two objects seem similar in size, the smaller one is assumed by the eye to be farther away Interposition, if one object blocks our view of another we assume it is closer Relative clarity, we perceive hazier objects as farther away Relative height, objects higher in our field of vision are assumed to be farther away Relative motion or Motion parallax a depth cue whereby the difference in the apparent rate of movement of different objects gives you information about the relative distance of the objects; for example objects that would be nearer to you would speed across your visual field Texture gradient, if the texture goes from coarse to fine, distance is perceived as greater Linear perspective, the more parallel lines converge the greater the perceived distance Light and shadow nearer objects reflect more light to the eye Relative brightness Amblyopia: lazy eye Strabismus: deflected eye; crossed eyes) Motion perception, 155 How do we perceive motion? We compute motion by assuming objects moving away are getting smaller Stroboscopic movement: we view rapidly moving similar objects are representing movement Phi phenomenon perception of movement when lights blink one after another (Max Werthheimer observed this) Apparent motion the illusion of movement in a stationary object; it illustrates higher level processing in the brain; movies and TV are examples where the brain creates apparent motion from still pictures Induced motion illusion when you’re tricked into seeing movement: looking up at the sky on a cloudy night you “see” the moon move through the clouds; the frame of reference is wrong Motion after effect: occurs when you gaze at a moving object for a long period of time then look at something stationary; the new thing appears to move in the opposite direction; also called the waterfall effect; this is evidence that motion-sensitive neurons exist in the visual cortex at the back of the brain Perceptual constancy, 156 How do perceptual constancies help us organize our sensations into meaningful perceptions? Linear perspective can cause you to see one object as bigger even when they appear to be the same size Shape and size constancies Objects remain constant in shape or size even as our distance from them changes Size distance relationship Examples: Moon illusion: can look up to 50% greater on the horizon; monocular cues for distance are responsible for this; see the other examples on page. Ponzo illusion; fig. 4.42 Also see the Ames room illusion on page 157, fig. 4.43 Lightness constancy, AKA brightness constancy, 158, see figure 4.44 Depends on relative luminance, the amount of light an object reflects relative to its surroundings Color constancy We see color due to how our brain interprets the light reflected by the object relative to its surrounding objects (there is no color intrinsic to the object) IV. page 159 Perceptual Interpretation Philosopher John Locke argued that perception develops through learning from experience Infancy is a critical period of development (see Unit 9 for background) Patients who had been blind can’t recognize objects by touching them Sensory deprivation and restored vision What does research on sensory deprivation and restored vision reveal about the effects on experience on perception? Sensory restriction does no damage if it occurs later in life Perceptual adaptation the ability to adjust our vision for artificial displacement How adaptable is our ability to perceive? George Stratton wore upside down goggles for eight days before adapting. Example: the quarterback on the Sensation and Perception video c. Sperry eye experiments: he surgically turned eyes of animals; mammals can adapt/reptiles cannot Perceptual set, created by experiences, assumptions, expectations How do our expectations, contexts and emotions influence our perceptions? Example: a mental, or perceptual, set causes you to see one thing and not another; explains UFOs---seeing them and not the actual natural phenomena Also see figure 4.47 on p. 161 Perceptual sets are determined by schemas, concepts that organize information; schemas can “prime” us to see certain things and not others; Jean Piaget (see Unit 9) developed this concept. Context effects, 162 A later stimulus can determine how we perceive and earlier one Emotion and motivation Expectations influence perceptions in a top-down manner Perception is a biopsychosocial phenomenon: senses, attention, schemas, cultural assumptions, expectations all play a role Extra Sensory Perception What are the claims of ESP, and what have most research psychologists concluded after putting these claims to the test? 50% of Americans believe in The claims of ESP: the information does not come from the five senses, thus extra sensory Parapsychology the study of paranormal phenomena Most scientists are skeptical. Concepts: see figure 4.53 for relationships between the following terms: Telepathy sending thoughts Clairvoyance perceiving an event with no prior knowledge or sensory experience Precognition seeing future events PK psychokinesis moving objects with mental powers Vague predictions are often interpreted later to have meaning Correlations are often assumed to be causally connected Confirmation bias occurs when the believer only finds evidence that supports their claim and ignores all the evidence that refutes it. No reproducible experiment has supported ESP 35% of major city police departments have used psychics, but none have had success. Magician James Randi offers $1 million to anyone who can demonstrate a psychic power under scientific testing. No one has passed the preliminary test to make it to final testing. www.randi.org Other groups offered 200,000 euros, with no winner. Why do people believe in ESP, when there is no evidence? 1. Misperceptions 2. Misinterpretations of results 3. Selective recall Key terms to know on page 172 AP quiz on page 173 Perception and the Human Factor, Appendix B13-15 Human factor psychologists help design machines humans can better use How do human factors psychologists work to create user-friendly machines and work settings? Norman has a website for good design (www.jnd.org) Research on human error in aircraft accidents: distance cues confused pilots into thinking they were higher than actual; caused by night and lack of visuals in front of the land strip. Action statements work best in directing behavior Assistive listening technologies Loop systems broadcast right into the hearing aid Technology designers mistakenly assume others know enough about their device; simulating what it’s like not to know is called, the curse of knowledge.