. The Ecological Turn: Affordances for Learning Research? Roy Williams Roy.williams@port.ac.uk Abstract This paper is an exploration of the contribution that ecological theory and ecologically informed research can make to learning theory and research. It builds on the contributions of Gibson and the many researchers who have subsequently critiqued and developed his ideas in ecological psychology, and it build on work in semiotics, applied linguistics, neuro-psychology, complex adaptive systems theory, knowledge management, and learning theory. The aim is to establish a rigorous ‘ecological’ framework for learning research, and to do this by clarifying and resolving a number of related issues in the fields of perception, cognition, learning theory, and the semiotics of engagement with the world. The thesis of the paper is that the notion of ‘affordances’ can be re-defined rigorously and clearly enough for it to play a central role in an ecological theory of learning, and the paper outlines the requirements for that to be the case. Keywords: affordances, ecology, semiotics, indication, prediction, identity. Introduction If we are to get to grips with the ecological turn in learning theory and research, Gibson’s work is an excellent place to start. “Gibson’s ecological approach has much to offer the study of social behaviour. Most significantly, it redirects attention away from hypothesized mental representations and steps of information processing, focusing rather on the discovery of what actually occurs when people interact” (Valenti and Good 1991: 82-83). Costall writes that it “seems appropriate to remind ourselves of what Gibson’s theory of affordances was meant to be for: … a serious attempt to put meaning back into the world, within a relational ontology” (+), (1995). It was also a serious attempt to counter the cultural and linguistic relativism of some of the social psychology of the time. Gibson is working with issues of human psychology, but he is working against a much broader ecological canvas, including the full range of animal behaviour, and across individual and social psychology. The purpose of this paper is to build on that broader perspective. This resonates with, and builds on a broad and inclusive semiotics, from the most fundamental material semiotic through to the virtual semiotics of the global internet. We need to place Gibson within his context to understand the range of issues he deals with, and the range of seemingly contradictory statements he sometimes makes as he moves across this broad spectrum. However, the issue in this paper is not Gibson per se, but rather an exploration of what can be called the ecological turn, as it applies to learning, and an attempt to understand learning both ontogenetically and phylogenetically, so that we can embed meaning and learning firmly “back into the world”. Conclusion Learning can be seen as a process of exploring and establishing affordances, which requires interaction with, negotiation with, and adaptation to physical, social and cultural ecologies, as well as the manipulation and transformation of the environment, for better or for worse. In ecological terms learning, like language, always involves ‘taking up a position’ or in fact several ‘positions’: cognitive, social, psychological, emotional, physical, cultural, and ethical within several ‘communities’, ‘discourses’ or ‘social ecologies’. Gibson defines an affordance as “something that … implies the complementarity of the animal and the environment” . An affordance therefore only contributes to intelligent behaviour as long as the relationship remains complementary; beyond that it quickly becomes counterproductive, as global energy practices demonstrate so starkly. There is ample justification for allocating a special and important role for ‘reason’ and ‘predictive hypotheses’ within formal education, but learning is based within biological and social ecologies and communities. If we respond to Derry’s aim, which is to provide a rigorous and long-overdue epistemological framework for learning, we need to ground learning in the fundamentals of semiotics, applied linguistics, ecological psychology, and acquired behaviour within complex adaptive ecologies, across the full range of linguistic and psychological affordances. We should keep the ecological thrust of Gibson’s work, namely the idea of an affordance as the product of an interaction between the organism and the environment. We can situate this within a rigorous semiotic epistemology, to arrive at a more nuanced, richer concept of an affordance as the product of the interaction between the person and the environment, in which each interaction potentially alters the knowledge, capacity and identity of the person, as well as the (micro-) environment. These interactive engagements with the world will be embedded within social and ecological contexts, and will be informed by, and even ‘infused’ in part by the most powerful semiotics, including science, the arts, religion and globalised finance, some of which are based on ‘reason’. The ‘ecological turn’, based on Gibson’s interventions, adds to and builds on the semiotic and post-modern turn in the development of social theory, and the redefined notion of affordances provides a rich framework for exploring learning across a wide spectrum of contexts. References Barthes, R. (1977) Elements of Semiotics. London, Jonathan Cape Capra, F. (2005) Complexity and Life. Theory, Culture & Society. 22: 5 pp. 33 – 44. Cetina, K.K. (2005) Complex Global Microstructures: The New Terrorist Societies. Theory, Culture and Society, 22:5. pp 213-234. Costall, A. (1989) A Closer look at ‘direct perception. Pp10-21 in Gellatly A, et al, (eds.) Cognition and Social Worlds. Oxford, Clarendon. Costall, A. (2007) The Windowless Room: ‘Mediationism’ and how to get over it. Valsiner, J and Rosa, A. (eds) Cambridge Handbook of sociocultural psychology pp 109-123. C.U. Press. Deacon, T. W. (2003). The hierarchic logic of emergence: untangling the interdependence of evolution and self-organization. In B. Weber, & D. Depew (Eds.), Evolution and learning: The Baldwin effect reconsidered. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, Chap. 14 (2004) Derry J. Epistemology and conceptual resources for the development of learning technologies. J. Computer Assisted Learning 23:6, 503-510 Eco, U. (1978) Semiotics. Bloomington, Indiana University Press. Foucault M. (1980) Power/Knowledge: Selected Interviews and Other Writings, 1972-1977. New York, Pantheon Books Gibson J. J. (1986) The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, New Jersey, U.S.A. Polanyi. M. (1983) The Tacit Dimension. New York, Peter Smith. Ramachandran V. S. (2003) The Emerging Mind: Series of 5 Reith Lectures on BBC Radio 4. http://www.bbc.co.uk/radio4/reith2003/. Last accessed 21 July 2008. Reed, E.S. (1996) Encountering the World. Towards an Ecological Psychology. New York, Oxford University Press. Saint-Onge, H. & Armstrong, C. (2004) The Conductive Organisation. Oxford, Elsevier. Williams R.T. (1983) The Impact of Informatics on Social Sign Systems. Pp 353-376 in B. Bain (ed.) The Sociogenesis of Language and Human Conduct. New York, Plenum. Williams R. T. (2005) Meta-semiotics and Practical Epistemology. Theory and Psychology, 15 (5): 719-734 Williams, R. T. (2006a) Narratives of Knowledge and Intelligence: beyond the tacit and explicit. Journal of Knowledge Management 10 (4): 80-99. Williams, R.T. (2006b) Alterity, Identity and Tarde: 11 March 2006: http://www.roys-discourse-typologies.blogspot.com/ Last accessed 21 July 2008. Withagen, R. & Michaels, C. (2005). On Ecological Conceptualizations of Perceptual Systems and Action Systems. Theory & Psychology 15(5): 603–620 Wittgenstein, L. (1973) Philosophical Investigations. Oxford, Blackwell. Worf, B.L. (1956) Language, Thought and Reality. John Wiley, New York,.