1 RESEARCH WITH THE STUDENT ADAPTATION TO COLLEGE QUESTIONNAIRE (SACQ) Robert W. Baker Clark University Posted by: Robert Shilkret Mount Holyoke College (A Note to the Reader: This is a manuscript Robert Baker was working on at the time of his death. It is current to 2002. The manuscript is quite lengthy, approximately 400 pages. The index will not work on most computers, so do not rely on the pagination suggested by the index. If there are any questions about this or the SACQ, please contact Robert Shilkret at Shilkret@mtholyoke.edu.) CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION The underlying thesis of this monograph is that student adjustment to college is a definable and measurable phenomenon, and that it has determinants that can in turn be defined and measured, including means of altering its course. There will be no attempt here to argue this position on philosophical/theoretical grounds, rather the burden of proof will be the weight of empirical evidence that has been collected by a large number of investigators employing a particular means of measuring adjustment to college. In 1984 an article appeared in a professional journal describing research with a new 2 means of measuring adjustment to college (Baker & Siryk, 1984b), an instrument subsequently expanded and named the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire, or SACQ (Baker & Siryk, 1989). In ensuing years additional reports of research with the instrument by its developers became available, but, more significantly, the SACQ also came to be used by many other researchers in a wide variety of studies. The earlier of these investigations produced a body of information that permitted detailed evaluation of the SACQ, in particular its reliability and validity, and this information was summarized in a test manual intended for SACQ users (Baker & Siryk, 1989). But the large mass of data that has accumulated from later as well as the earlier investigations also may be viewed as providing a means of gaining improved understanding of the phenomenon of adjustment to college -- its definition, its determinants, and means of facilitating it. The purpose of this monograph is to review all available data from SACQ-using research for the light they might cast on these important issues. There are of course many studies concerning adjustment to college that have not employed the SACQ, but they will not be considered here. This exclusion has the disadvantage of disregarding for the time being information that could be useful in understanding adjustment to college. Another disadvantage is that the findings of all studies included in the review will be affected by whatever limitations or flaws the SACQ may have. But there are also advantages. Findings from studies employing a variety of variables, all with the SACQ as the measure of adjustment, will be easier to interpret and compare, as will of course findings from studies that employ the same or similar variables or methodology, contributing possibly to the development of a more cohesive and coherent understanding of 3 available data. This approach may also facilitate the identification of issues that merit further exploration or clarification, and the identification of new areas for investigation. In such future research the time and thought that ordinarily would be devoted to means of measuring the construct "adjustment to college" could instead be spent on other variables or aspects of research design. While the understanding of adjustment to college that we seek to develop here will be based largely in research data, it is not our intent generally to provide detailed findings, analyses of methodology, or critique or evaluation of the studies from which data are drawn. Readers interested in the details of the studies are referred to the SACQ test manual (Baker & Siryk, 1989), to the Appendixes to this monograph which include addenda, to tables of correlations from the test manual, or to the original research reports. Neither do we intend to use the accumulated data as a basis for constructing a theory or model of adjustment to college. Rather, our interest is simply to present in one place what is known from research with the SACQ, in the hope of stimulating interest in and thought about the topic and promoting further research. Looking beyond the particular focus of this volume, and making the assumption that adjusting to college may be seen as a naturally-occurring life stressor, it is hoped that research findings to be considered here will have implications for adjustment in other stressful life situations. As for the structure of this monograph, Chapter 2 is concerned with the development of an empitrically-based definition of adjustment to college, followed in Chapter 3 by preliminary considerations regarding analysis of determinants of that adjustment. Chapters 4 through 10 deal with various kinds of characteristics of persons as determinants, and Chapters 11 through 14 with 4 environmental determinants. Facilitation of college adjustment is the topic of Chapter 16, and Chapter 17 contains an overall summary, plus implications for future investigations. The summary in Chapter 16 excludes citations for findings -- which of course are fully included in all preceding chapters -- in the interest of ease of presentation and reading. Readers wishing an overall view of the issue at hand before encounteriing detailed findings might consider proceeding to that chapter next. 5 CHAPTER 2 THE DEFINITION OF ADJUSTMENT TO COLLEGE There are two ways of using the SACQ to present a definition of adjustment to college. The first is to examine the composition and structure of the scale -- including by statistical analysis -- in order to ascertain the assumptions and conceptions that underlie it. A second approach is to review research that identifies correlates exemplifying the meaning of the construct. The Meaning of Adjustment to College as Inferred from the Composition and Structure of the SACQ The 67 verbal statement-items that comprise the SACQ each address a facet of the experience of adjusting to college and the demands that characterize it. It would be too cumbersome to try to contemplate here the contribution that each item makes to the definition of the construct being measured, and instead the reader is encouraged for that purpose to peruse the individual items. The sum of scores for the 67 items (the full scale score) provides an index of overall adjustment, but more important to the definition of the construct is the fact that 65 of the items subdivide into four subscales addressing different aspects of the adjustment. Three of the subscales may be considered as primary in the sense that they contain no overlapping items, and are intended to measure, separately, academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment. A fourth subscale contains some items exclusive to itself and other items shared with two of the primary subscales, and assesses the student's commitment to the college experience, especially attachment to the particular institution attended. 6 Factor analysis, and examination of the intercorrelations among the SACQ subscales, provide support for this view of adjustment to college as having different facets (Baker, 1993a; Baker & Siryk, 1989). Significant positive correlations of moderate magnitude among the three primary subscales -- and, to a more limited degree, the higher positive correlations between subscales that share items -- indicate that the subscales tap a common construct. On the other hand, the correlations for the most part are sufficiently low as to indicate that adjustment to college does indeed have different aspects and that there is ample room for intraindividual variation by areas -- e.g., for a given individual to have a high score in one area and low in another. The conception of adjustment to college underlying the SACQ is specified further by the identification of clusters of items within each subscale that represent different facets of the adjustment area addressed by the subscale. Four aspects of academic adjustment are thus identified: motivation for being in college and doing college work; translation of the motivation into actual academic effort; the efficacy or success of the effort expended; and satisfaction with the academic environment. Social adjustment is also seen as having four aspects: extent and success of social activities and functioning in general; involvement with other persons on campus; relocation away from home and significant persons there; and satisfaction with the social environment. Personal-emotional adjustment is conceptualized as having two aspects: sense of psychological well-being; and sense of physical well-being. Commitment to the college experience is also seen as having two facets: satisfaction with being in college in general; and satisfaction with being at the institution in which enrolled. The Meaning of Adjustment to College as Indicated by Research-Derived Behavioral Correlates 7 of the SACQ Over and above the definition of adjustment to college inherent in the composition and structure of the SACQ, research employing the instrument can provide information as to the meaning of the constructs the scale is intended to address. It can show what adjustment to college is operationally, i. e., what the behavioral correlates of the SACQ are that add body to the foregoing conceptual structure. In a real sense, all SACQ-using research – including that which will be considered in the several subsequent chapters concerning determinants of adjustment to college – clarifies the meaning of the construct(s) measured by the instrument, but the focus in this chapter will be on behavioral characteristics most directly appropriate to the particular content area measured. In attempting to fill out our definition in this manner, and because of our conception of adjustment to college as multifaceted, our primary focus will be on identifying behavioral indices of adjustment that have been found to be associated differentially in degree and sometimes selectively with the several subscales of the SACQ. Frequently we will be interested principally in the relation of behavioral indices with one of the subscales in particular, as, in the paragraph that follows, between college grade point average and the subscale that measures academic adjustment. However, this does not mean that relation between the behavioral index and the measures of other adjustment areas, or overall adjustment (i. e., the SACQ full scale score), may not be relevant to the task of defining adjustment to college. Academic adjustment to college. Unsurprisingly, many findings from SACQ-using research indicate that college adjustment is to be defined in some degree in terms of quality of academic performance. Numerous investigators have found that students indicated by the SACQ 8 as having better adjustment to college, especially better academic adjustment, have higher college grade point average or some other index of better academic performance, e.g., number of academic credits earned (Allen, 1985; Bettencourt, Charlton, Eubanks, Kernahan, & Fuller, 1999; Birnie-Lefcovitch, 1997; Baker, 1993; Baker & Siryk, 1984b, 1989; Beyers, 2001; Beyers & Goossens, 1998; 2002a, 2002b; Brower, 1990b; Carlson, 1986; Chartrand, Camp, & McFadden, 1990b; Chizhik, 1999; Coatsworth, 2001; Conti, 2000a,c; Davis, 1988; Dewein, 1994; Evans-Hughes, 1992; Foster, 1997; Gerdes, 1987; Gilkey, 1988; Gilkey, Protinsky, & Lichtman, 1989; Gold, Burrell, Haynes, & Nardecchia, 1990; Grella, 1989; Harris, 1991; Hogan, 1987/1988; Humfleet & Ribordy, 1990; Keenan, 1992; Lent, 1997; Loveland, 1992; Marcotte, 1995; Maton, 1989b; McGillin, 1986; McGowan, 1987; Montgomery, Haemmerlie, & Watkins, 2000, data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001; Napoli & Wortman, 1998; Natera, 1998; Ogden & Trice, 1986; Pfeil. 2000; Santonicola, 1989; Sennett, 2000; Sugar, 1999; Terrell, 1989; Tomlinson-Clarke, 1998; Wang & Smith, 1993; Washington, 1996; Wick & Shilkret, 1986b; Wildman, 1998; Williams, 1996; Yaffe, 1997, see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000; Young, 1994) (see Appendix A for correlations between SACQ variables and college grade point average; bolded years in foregoing references, and in subsequent references throughout this monograph, indicate studies for which correlational gpa data are included in the appendices; underlined years indicate studies for which such data are included in the SACQ test manual). Such students also have higher incidence of election to an academic honor society (Baker & Siryk, 1984b, 1989). Students indicated by the SACQ as having better adjustment to college, especially better academic adjustment, have higher predicted grade point averages at time of entrance to college, 9 with the predictions based on high school grade point average, ACT or SAT scores, and quality of high school attended (Helman, 1999). Interestingly, Young (1994) determined that the prediction of college seniors' cumulative grade point average from prematriculation measures (SAT's, high school grade point average) was considerably improved (from R=.47 to .58) by adding as a predictor the SACQ Academic Adjustment subscale administered in the senior year. Only weak to no relation was found between grade point average and SACQ variables in freshmen at a Belgian university (Beyers & Goosens, 2002a). Students in that study were administered the SACQ in November, while average grades were computed from final examinations at the end of the first semester, the end of the second semester, and “repeat” exams the following September that could be taken by students who had failed at least one of the June examinations. The only statistically significant correlations found for the five first-semester SACQ indices with grade point average at the three time periods were a modest positive one for academic adjustment in the January examinations and a modest negative one for institutional attachment in the September “repeat” examinations. The authors explained, in trying to account for the lack of greater association between academic performance and adjustment, that students at that particular university “never” took intermediate tests, exams, or quizzes during a semester, and thus lesser than usual opportunity to form a sense of their academic peformance. One investigator (Lopez, 1997) reported no relation between the SACQ Attachment subscale, the only SACQ variable used, and college grade point average; and in one other study (Mathis & Lecci, 1999) there was no relation between any SACQ variable – with the SACQ administered in the fourth week -- and first semester gpa for a freshman sample. Also unsurprising is that adjustment to college, especially academic adjustment, is 10 manifested behaviorally and attitudinally in terms of seriousness of academic purpose. Thus, students identified as better adjusted on the SACQ are more decided about a major field (Albert, 1988; Leonard, 1990; Marcy, 1996; McGowan, 1988; Pfeil. 2000,2002; Plaud, Baker, & Groccia, 1986, 1990; Smith & Baker, 1987); more satisfied with their major (Chartrand et al., 1990b); and more certain about their career plans (Carlson, 1986; Chartrand, Camp, & McFadden, 1990a; Lopez, 1989; Maton, 1989b; Maton & Weisman, 1989; Plaud et al., 1986). Other indications of seriousness of academic purpose were seen in a study with academically at-risk students. Such students with SACQ-indicated better adjustment were more likely to study alone than with others, more likely to perceive faculty as helpful and encouraging, and less likely to skip classes (Keenan, 1992). Better academic adjustment was found by Pratt (2001) to be associated with lesser self-reported skipping of classes, and, similarly, by Helman (1999) to be associated with students reporting themselves to spend more hours per week in class, and also more hours per week studying. Hurtado et al. (1996) report that academically better adjusted sophomore students were more likely to report having perceived their academic work in the first year of college as manageable, having managed their resources well (e. g., time, money), and having interacted with faculty. Social adjustment to college. Unsurprising, too, is the finding that students with higher SACQ scores, especially in social adjustment, are more involved in the life of their college. Thus, they participate more in extracurricular activities (Baker & Siryk, 1984b; Bettencourt et al., 1999; Beyers & Goossens, 2002a; Elacqua, 1992a&b; Evans-Hughes, 1992; Jackson, 1998; Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001; Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 1993; Montgomery & Howdeshell, 1993; Santonicola, 1989; Schriver, 1996; Tomlinson-Clarke & Clarke, 1994; Wick 11 & Shilkret, 1986b; Woo & Bilynsky, 1994), though Kline (1992) found no relation between any SACQ variable and number of college organizations joined. As other indications of greater involvement in the life of their college, such students describe themselves as feeling at ease in coming to know their college environment (Hurtado et al., 1996); visit home less often while college is in session (Savino, 1987); have fewer contacts with family and friends at home, as through telephone calls and letters as well as visits (Schriver, 1996), though, again, Kline (1992) found no relation between any SACQ variable and number of contacts with family per month. And, further, they are more likely to be full-time than part-time students (Burr, 1992), and to spend less time in gainful employment (Burr; Reeker, 1994). Using a variable called “non-instructional activities” that included involvement in student groups, gainful employment, and community service (a combining of activities that would seem to have both positive and negative implications for college adjustment), Helman (1999) found no relation between it and either social or overall adjustment, the only SACQ variables reported. Regarding interpersonal activities more particularly, or the formation and maintenance of relationships, SACQ-indicated better adjusted (again, especially better socially adjusted) students have more close friends (Humfleet, 1987); spend more time socializing with friends (Beyers & Goossens, 2002a; Hurtado et al.); are more likely to maintain friendships over time (Elacqua, 1992a & b); are more satisfied with their social relationships, platonic, romantic, and combined (Coatsworth, 2001); and are more likely to have romantic partners (Harris, 1988). These students see themselves as having higher quality relationships with other students (Bettencourt et al., 1999) and being more satisfied with their social life (Martin, Swartz-Kulstad, Hutz, & Fabian, 2000). And those who are also student-athletes are more likely to report having quality 12 relationships with teammates and others on campus (Ridinger, 1998). Possibly in part because of their involvement with their campus social system, and their interpersonal competence as indicated in the foregoing findings, these kinds of SACQ-indicated better adjusting students have greater likelihood of being hired if they apply for positions as dormitory assistants (Baker & Siryk, 1989). Personal-emotional adjustment to college. Higher scoring students on the SACQ, especially on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, which addresses sense of psychological well-being and sense of physical well-being, are less likely: to be known to a campus psychological services center (Baker, 1993a; Baker & Siryk, 1984b, 1989; Beyers & Goossens, 2002a); Freeman, 1987b); to describe themselves as in, or having been in, psychotherapy or counseling (Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001); to seek help from various campus resources for “emotional concerns or academic problems” (Terrell, 1989; Beyers & Goossens); to seek help from campus and local community helping resources in general (Johnson, 2001); to describe their transition from high school to college as difficult (Albert, 1988); and to report experiencing stress (Harris, 1991; Mathis & Lecci, 1999; Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001; Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 1993; Montgomery & Howdeshell, 1993) or personal difficulties (Martin, Swartz-Kulstad, & Madson, 1999; Martin et al., 2000). They are also more likely to self-report good mental health status (Mathis & Lecci. 1999). Such students are also more likely to have higher scores on test measures of general good mental health and lower scores on test measures of both general and specific aspects of mental ill-health, including depression; anxiety, both state and trait; emotional stress; dissociative experiences and symptomatology; neuroticism; and eating disorders and psychological 13 characteristics associated with them (see Chapter 4, “Person Characteristics as Determinants of Adjustment to College: Mental and Physical Health Variables” for more detailed consideration of studies reporting the foregoing findings, including citation of sources). Students scoring higher on SACQ variables, especially personal-emotional adjustment, score higher on measures of general physical health, report lesser incidence of physical symptoms, have fewer health center visits for doctor appointments, and fewer class absences due to illness (also see Chapter 4 for references to studies reporting these findings). Goal commitment and institutional attachment. Students who score higher on the SACQ, especially its measure of commitment to the college experience, including attachment to the college they are attending, are less likely to discontinue enrollment (Baker, 1993; Baker, McNeil, & Siryk, 1985; Baker & Siryk, 1984b, 1986, 1989; Beyers & Goossens, (2002a); Brower, 1990a; Brunelle-Joiner, 1999; Burr, 1992; Gerdes, 1987; Harris, 1991; Kaase, 1994; Krotseng, 1991, 1992; Napoli & Wortman, 1998; Santonicola, 1989) (see Appendix B for correlations between SACQ variables and attrition; readers are reminded that bolding of years indicates that correlational data from the relevant study are included in the appendices of this monograph; underlining of a year indicates that such data are included in the SACQ test manual). Bragg (1994a&b) devised an adaptation of the SACQ for administration in the second semester of the freshman year to assess first-semester adjustment retrospectively, and found significant differences in the expected direction on all SACQ variables between students who persisted in enrollment into the third semester and those who did not. Higher scoring students on the SACQ, again especially on the Goal Commitmen/Institutional Attachment subscale, are also less likely to describe themselves as 14 having had thoughts of leaving their college early in the college years (Harris, 1991; Sennett, 2000); less likely to indicate in pre-registration for the sophomore year that they will not be enrolled (Just, 1998); more likely to identify their college as their first choice when applying to colleges (Albert, 1988); more likely to indicate in the first week of the freshman year that they expect to graduate from the college they are attending (Wildman, 1998); more likely to report overall satisfaction with, or a positive attitude toward, the college experience (Cooler, 1995; Gerdes, 1986; Keenan, 1992; Martin, 1988; Martin et al., 1999; Martin et al, 2000; Napoli & Wortman, 1998; Ridinger, 1998) and to be satisfied with their college, with its degree of interest in and treatment of them, and with the occupational preparation it has given them (Harris). The measure of overall satisfaction used by Napoli and Wortman, cited above, was from the Student Opinion Survey (American College Testing Program, 1994), which taps attitudes towards a college’s administrative and student services, the academic and instructional environments, and the social and physical environments. The Meaning of Adjustment to College as Indicated by the SACQ’s Correlation with Other Measures of Adjustment to College Another source of research data concerning SACQ correlates that may help to define adjustment to college is studies that examine the Questionnaire's relation to other measures of adjustment to college. Perhaps more informative in this respect than the correlations themselves between the SACQ and the other measures are the particular aspects of adjustment to college that the other measures were attempting to tap. Thus, if the correlations are sufficiently high, then possibly it can be assumed that the SACQ is also tapping those aspects to some degree. Significant correlations in the expected direction, many of them quite robust, have been 15 reported between all indices of the College Inventory of Academic Adjustment (CIAA; Borow, 1949) and all SACQ indices, especially the Academic Adjustment subscale (Lapsley, 1989; Rice, Cole, & Lapsley, 1990). The aspects of adjustment that the CIAA subscales are intended to measure are: satisfaction with the college work routine and with the respondent’s particular program of courses; educational and life goals, and motivation to achieve them; efficiency in planning academic efforts and use of time; study skills; psychological stability; and social relations with instructors and peers. Beyers and Goosens (2002a) administered the Adjustment Questionnaire (Crombag, 1968) to freshmen at a Belgian university. That instrument, originally developed in Dutch, is intended to measure “adjustment to the university environment in general,” more particularly how well students feel at home at the university, enjoy being a student, and how pleased they are with their academic program and social contacts at the university. Strong correlations – especially strong with social adjustment, institutional attachment, and overall adjustment – were found with all SACQ indices. Significant correlations in the expected direction, though less robust than in the case of the CIAA and the Adjustment Questionnaire, have been found between the SACQ and most subscales of the College Life Task Assessment Instrument (CLT; Brower, 1990b). Strongest correlations were with the CLT subscale designed to measure sense of integrity of the self, or self-esteem, which may be less an aspect of adjustment than a personality determinant of adjustment. Other CLT subscales with which SACQ indices correlate in expected direction are intended to measure establishment of autonomy from family and high school friends, academic accomplishment, establishment of friendships, and maintenance of physical health. 16 Studies by Keenan (1992) and Scott (1991) using the College Student Experiences Questionnaire (Pace, 1987) have determined that students with high SACQ scores expend more effort in and become more involved in various academic, social, and recreational resources, facilities and opportunities available in the college experience. A moderately strong correlation in the expected negative direction was obtained by Schriver (1996) between the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable employed, and the College Maladjustment Scale (Kleinmuntz, 1961). The College Maladjustment Scale consists of items from the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (get ref for MMPI publisher, & add to reflist as well as here) that were found by Kleinmuntz to discriminate between students receiving and not receiving psychological services. As described by Kleinmuntz, the items dealt with problems in self-esteem, self-efficacy, and self-confidence; various other indications of depressive thinking and feeling (pessimism regarding the future, lack of interest in life, anergy); and various indications of anxiety (worrying, assorted psychosomatic symptoms, and difficulty concentrating). Napoli and Wortman (1998) investigated the relation between Pascarella and Terenzini’s Student Involvement Questionnaire (SIQ; 1980) and the SACQ. The two instruments appear at first glance to be measuring similar if not the same constructs, and even share very similar names for those constructs, but Napoli and Wortman’s results indicate that the instruments apparently are not as psychometrically alike as first glance suggests. Regarding apparent similarities, the SIQ has a two-item measure of Goal Commitment, or motivation to earn a college degree, and a three-item measure of Institutional Commitment, or intention to continue in enrollment at one’s college of present enrollment, while the SACQ’s Institutional Attachment/Goal Commitment 17 subscale combines those two aspects of college adjustment in one 15-item scale. And where the SIQ has subscales called Academic Integration and Social Integration, the SACQ has subscales named Academic Adjustment and Social Adjustment. Napoli and Wortman (1998) administered the SIQ Goal Commitment and Institutional Commitment subscales to community college students in the first and second weeks of a fall semester, and again at the end of that semester, and the SIQ Academic and Social Integration subscales at the end of the semester. The SACQ was administered at the end of the semester. The earlier administered SIQ Goal and Institututional Commitment subscales correlated positively, but only weakly to modestly, with the later administered SACQ’s subscales (except for no correlation between the former SIQ variable and personal-emotional adjustment), the highest correlation being .20 between SIQ’s Institutional Commitment and the SACQ’s Institutional Attachment/Goal Commitment subscale. The later (end of the semester, at the same time as the SACQ) administration of the SIQ Goal Commitment measure correlated modestly (with personal-emotional adjustment) to moderately (.36 to .41, with academic and social adjustment, and institutional attachment/goal commitment), but the only correlation for the SIQ Institutional Commitment variable was a weak negative one with social adjustment. The SIQ Academic Integration subscale correlated positively and weakly (with personal-emotional adjustment) to moderately (.31 to .41, with academic and social adjustment, and institutional attachment/goal commitment), strongest with academic adjustment. But, finally, the SIQ Social Integration subscale correlated only weakly with the SACQ subscales, except for no correlation with personal-emotional adjustment. The Meaning of Adjustment to College as Indicated by Behavioral Correlates of the Relation 18 between Measures of Anticipated and Actual Adjustment to College There is yet another source of information concerning the definition of adjustment to college as inferred from empirically-determined correlates of the SACQ. An adaptation of the SACQ -- the Anticipated Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (ASACQ; Baker et al., 1985; Baker & Schultz, 1992a&b, 1993) -- was developed for use prematriculation to measure expectations or level of confidence concerning adaptive capacity for the impending transition into college. When the ASACQ is employed in conjunction with postmatriculation administration of the SACQ, a measure of the degree to which expectations are fulfilled can be generated. This new measure can be viewed as yet another index of adjustment to college additional to the SACQ used by itself. In a study (Baker et al., 1985) that focused on comparison of ASACQ and SACQ full scale scores for defining degree of fulfillment of expectations concerning adaptive capacity, students whose expectations were substantially met (i. e., where a student’s SACQ scores were approximately at the same level as ASACQ scores, or higher) were compared with students apparently disillusioned regarding their adaptive capacity (i. e., where SACQ scores were significantly lower than ASACQ scores). The former students were found: to have higher grade point averages (also see Williams, 1996 and Chizhik, 1999 for similar finding) and to earn more course credits in the freshman year; to be more likely to win annual academic honors in the sophomore, junior, and senior years, and to be awarded general or departmental honors at graduation; to be less likely to be known to a psychological services center during the freshman year; to be less likely to withdraw from college before the beginning of the sophomore year; and to be more likely to graduate on time. Graduation rates for the two kinds of students six years 19 after matriculation were, respectively, 86% and 55% (Baker, 1993). Gerdes (1986) focused on comparison of ASACQ and SACQ subscale scores in defining degree of fulfillment of expectations and found that disillusionment in the academic, social, and personal-emotional areas was related to lesser overall satisfaction with the college experience (no data relevant to this comparison were reported for the Attachment subscale). Disillusionment in all adjustment areas except personal-emotional was significantly correlated with increased attrition, to the highest degree in the case of the Attachment subscale. Disillusionment in academic adjustment was also related to lower grade point average in the fall term of the freshman year. It was determined in yet another study (Baker & Schultz, 1992b) that the selfdisillusionment implied by comparison of ASACQ/SACQ scores was discernible in interview data collected at the beginning of the second semester of the freshman year, several months following administration of the two tests. That is, students could recall their prematriculation expectation levels and first semester adjustment levels, and differences between these levels. The findings were interpreted as showing that the ASACQ/SACQ score relationship is associated not only with everyday behavioral correlates, as cited above, but also in expected ways with thoughts and feelings in the experiential world of the student. A Formal Property of the Adjustment-to-College Construct as Indicated by Research Data In addition to the specific content of the definition of adjustment to college as indicated by the SACQ's composition and by its research-generated correlates, a formal or structural characteristic of the construct is illuminated by research data. Our expectation from the beginning has been that adjustment-to-college variables as measured by the SACQ are not necessarily stable 20 and enduring properties of individuals, but should be regarded as states that can vary with changes in a student's environment, life events, and personal characteristics. Data from Clark University and Holy Cross College for seven freshman samples that were administered the SACQ around the middle of both the first and second semesters (approximately 20 weeks apart) support this conception. All correlations between the two testings for each of the five SACQ variables were statistically significant, with median values ranging from .52 to .67 (Baker, 1993). Similar values were reported in another study (Bettencourt, Charlton, Eubanks, Kernahan, & Brett, 1999) employing approximately the same temporal separation of SACQ administrations (within the first and last forty-five days of an academic year) but using only the Academic and Social Adjustment subscales (correlations of .58 and .56, respectively). Thus, while there is some degree of consistency in these variables over the time span indicated, it is not enough to suggest that necessarily stable and enduring characteristics are being measured. One of the seven above-mentioned Clark and Holy Cross samples (Graham, Baker, & Wapner, 1984) was tested additionally at an intermediate time (i.e., in late fall as well as mid-fall of the first semester; students in this study were individually tested, and the average time lapse between testings was approximately seven weeks). As would be expected in the case of variables subject to change over time, the correlations for the closer testings (mid-fall to late fall, and late fall to mid-winter) were higher than those for the two most separated in time (middle of first semester to mid-winter; Baker, 1993). Several other studies have produced findings of higher correlation over shorter periods of time. Sherman (1992) had a freshman sample complete the SACQ in the 6th and again the 10th 21 weeks of the first semester and obtained correlations for the four subscales (ranging from .70 to .80; full scale score data were not reported) that were higher in each instance than the median values cited above for the seven Clark and Holy Cross samples tested in the middle of the first and second semesters. Corbett (1991) gave the SACQ twice, six weeks apart, to a sample of students from all four college year levels and obtained similarly high correlations for all five SACQ variables ranging between .63 and .86. With the SACQ Academic, Social, and PersonalEmotional Adjustment subscales administered approximately eight weeks apart in the first semester, Conti (2000a) obtained correlations ranging from .67 to .81. In Belvedere’s study (2000) the SACQ was given three months apart (approximately 12-13 weeks), yielding a correlation of .72 for the full scale score, the only SACQ variable reported. Possibly somewhat inconsistent with the foregoing findings for testings relatively closer in time, Gallant (1994) administered the SACQ twice in the first semester four weeks apart and obtained a correlation of only .56 for the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale (the only SACQ variable reported). Two studies reported data for SACQ administrations approximately two years apart, the longest time span known to date. Rice (1990/1991) had a sample complete the SACQ early in the freshman year and again in the junior year. Correlations for the Academic, Social, and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales (the Institutional Attachment subscale was not reported) ranged from .36 to .57, representing a slightly lower range of values than that cited above for an interim of several months. Shilkret and Nigrosh (1995) administered the SACQ to a small sample in the sophomore year and again in the senior year. For the Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales they found significant correlations roughly in the 22 range cited above for the Clark/Holy Cross samples tested approximately 20 weeks apart, but no significant correlation for the SACQ full scale score and close to zero values for the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales. ******* In brief summary concerning the definition of adjustment to college as inferred from research employing the SACQ, it seems feasible to regard that adjustment as a measurable characteristic of a student. The adjustment, furthermore, has different facets or aspects in which intra- as well as interindividual variation occurs; it may vary over time; and has a wide variety of discernible behavioral and experiential correlates that delineate the meaning of the construct. 23 CHAPTER 3 DETERMINANTS OF ADJUSTMENT TO COLLEGE: PRELIMINARY CONSIDERATIONS A very large amount of data from research using the SACQ provides a basis for identifying factors that influence the effectiveness of adjustment to college. These data result from the efforts of numerous investigators who have employed a wide variety of variables in conjunction with the SACQ. The primary interest for many of these investigators was in the variable(s) whose relation to the SACQ they were studying, and not in adjustment to college as such. That is, they used the SACQ as a handy source of dependent variables against which to examine the play of their independent variables of interest. But that does not prevent use of the data for a somewhat different purpose here, i.e., to analyze findings for information concerning variables that may be regarded at least provisionally as determinants of adjustment to college. In much or maybe even all of the research to be reviewed it is not possible to infer with certainty causality or direction of effect in the relation between independent variables and the SACQ. Nevertheless, we will proceed on the assumption of causality in the interest of developing a framework for conceptualizing factors that may have a determining role in adjustment to college. This framework may in turn, it is hoped, contribute to the planning of studies that are more definitive with respect to causality. Thus, the reader's forbearance is requested for references here to one variable affecting, or influencing, or determining another. Two main classes of determinants of adjustment to college, as reflected in the research to be considered here, may be identified, i.e., characteristics of the student and characteristics of the environment. We will see that it is not always possible to maintain a clear distinction between 24 these two kinds of factors, but the use of the categories will facilitate the ordering of a considerable mass of information. Further distinctions within the two main categories will be made, and will be reflected in the titles of ensuing chapters. The variables to be considered as characteristics of students will be referred to here as person characteristics, including but not restricted to personality variables. As a group, these are characteristics in terms of which individuals will differ and which students "bring with them" to the task of adjusting to college. Individual differences in these variables, it is presumed, will be associated with differences in quality or effectiveness of adjustment among students. Most of the relevant data concerning these variables were gathered by means of psychometric instruments -tests, questionnaires, inventories, and the like -- but other means of measuring or codifying person characteristics have also been employed. Some of the variables cited in the previous chapter concerning the definition of adjustment to college might just as easily be regarded as person characteristics that may be "determinants" of adjustment, depending on interpretive preference. For example, the various mental and physical health characteristics that were earlier offered as correlates or manifestations of personal-emotional adjustment, helping to flesh out the definition of that aspect of adjustment to college, could also be viewed as "causes" of quality of adjustment. That is, good mental or physical health as a person characteristic that a student brings with him or her to college may contribute to good adjustment, while impaired mental or physical health could be deleterious factors. As other examples, while degree of decidedness regarding academic major or career goal, or nature of study skills, can be seen as aspects of academic adjustment, it may be that having academic/vocational plans and good study habits and skills could be viewed instead as 25 facilitators of adjustment. This ambiguity between determinant of adjustment and aspect of adjustment will also be seen in some of the other variables to be considered. Reservations, Qualifications, and Rules of Data Presentation The findings discussed throughout this monograph come not only from a large number of investigators employing a wide variety of methodologies and variables, but also from multiple kinds of sources. These sources include published articles (from journals undoubtedly representing variation in the degree to which submissions are refereed); doctoral dissertations and master’s theses (obviously reflecting varying degrees of quantity and quality of tutorial supervision); undergraduate senior and/or honors theses and research project reports (some of uncommonly high quality); unpublished papers and research reports; and unpublished raw data from our own research and from other investigators willing to share such information. Thus, a wide net has been cast, but some of the catch will not be included in the material to be considered, and research-oriented readers are free to exercise the right of replication wherever findings seem doubtful or incomplete. As the reader has already seen in the previous chapter, a considerable amount of correlational data is discussed in the text and included in tables in the Appendixes. Correlations between potential determinants of adjustment to college and SACQ variables will be found in Appendix C (“Correlations between the SACQ and Measures of Person Characteristics”), Appendix D (“Correlations between SACQ Scores and Measures of Mental and Physical Health and Adjustment”), and Appendix E (“Correlations between SACQ Scores and Measures of Environment-Related Experience”). Most of these data are from psychometric instruments in general use, or from others that may sometime attain such use. The author has chosen to pay this 26 degree of attention to correlational data because they represent a succinct and facile expression of relation between variables. In presentation of correlational data and other forms of statistical analysis where possible, the author has taken the liberty, in his own research data as well as that of others, of employing one-tailed tests of statistical significance wherever justified by presumption of relation between variables on either theoretical or empirical grounds. Ordinarily only statistically significant findings (p<.05) are used as a basis for discussion in text, though nonsignificant correlations where available are included in Appendix tables. The reader is reminded yet again that bolding of years in references indicates presence of correlational data in the Appendixes, and underlining of years indicates that such data are included in the SACQ test manual (Baker & Siryk, 1989). Also regarding correlational data, the reader should keep in mind always the possibility or even likelihood of redundacy among the myriad independent variables to be considered. Some SACQ researchers have taken this factor into account in their statistical treatment of data, as through factor analysis and multiple regression, but because of cumbersomeness of such findings – dependent as they are on the particular mix of variables employed – no attempt will be made to present those findings. Finally, a particularly important point is made now concerning the presentation of findings in ensuing chapters. Discussion of relation between an independent variable and the SACQ will be couched in terms of all SACQ variables used and/or reported, i. e., scores for the four subscales and the full scale (the full scale often referred to as “overall adjustment”). Where an investigator has not used or reported a finding regarding one or more of the five SACQ indices, it will be so stated. Wherever such a statement of omission is not made and particular 27 SACQ variables are not mentioned, those not mentioned are to be presumed to not have a statistically significant relation with the independent variable in question. The purpose of this is avoid having to explicitly mention all nonsignificant findings. 28 CHAPTER 4 PERSON CHARACTERISTICS AS DETERMINANTS OF COLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: MENTAL AND PHYSICAL HEALTH VARIABLES As noted earlier, mental health characteristics may be viewed either as an aspect of the definition of adjustment in college or as a determinant of the adjustment. In this chapter relevant research findings will be examined from the latter point of view. The reader is reminded that for many studies cited in this monograph correlational data are available and reproduced either in the SACQ test manual (Baker & Siryk, 1989) or in this monograph’s appendixes. Underlining of the date in a particular reference indicates that correlational data may be found in the test manual; bolding indicates inclusion in this monograph’s appendixes. Almost all of the correlational data concerning mental and physical health characteristics will be found in Appendix D, with the remainder in Appendix C. Studies Employing Instruments Measuring, and Reporting, both General and Specific Aspects of Mental Health (or Ill- Health) There are several instruments used in conjunction with the SACQ that yield total or composite scores representing general aspects of mental health, and that also have subscales measuring particular – and sometimes numerous -- aspects such as depression, anxiety, or paranoid ideation. This section will focus primarily on the total or composite scores from such instruments and also provide a brief summary of subscale findings, but specific findings regarding particular aspects of mental health will be cited in later sections under appropriate headings. Significant relations were found by Flescher (1986b) between the full scale score of the 29 Mental Health Inventory (MHI; Veit & Ware, 1983) and all SACQ variables, including an especially robust correlation with personal-emotional adjustment. Furthermore, six of the seven MHI subscales (the Emotional Ties subscale being the exception) show fairly consistent patterns of significant correlation in expected direction with SACQ indices, and all seven relate significantly and most strongly with personal-emotional adjustment. Significant correlations occurred for Kenny (1994, 1995) between the total score of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL; Derogatis, 1984) and all SACQ subscales, highest with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. The HSCL subscales showed numerous significant correlations always in the expected direction with SACQ variables, highest in each instance with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale (Kenny, 1995). Shilkret and Nigrosh (1995) obtained significant correlations between three composite scores from the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983) – the Global Severity Index, the Positive Symptom Distress Index, and the Positive Symptom Total – and the SACQ Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale and full scale. Small sample size (n=11) in the Shilkret and Nigrosh study very likely worked against occurrence of relation between those composite scores and the other SACQ variables. All BSI subscales except Somatization for Shilkret and Nigrosh correlated significantly with at least one of the SACQ variables, most frequently and usually strongest with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. Significant correlations were gotten by Humfleet & Ribordy (1990) between the Total Pathology Score from the Mini-Mult (Kincannon, 1968), an abbreviated form of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (get reference for publisher of the MMPI), and the Social Adjustment, Personal-Emotional Adjustment and Attachment subscales of the SACQ (data for 30 the Academic Adjustment subscale were not reported), highest with personal-emotional adjustment. Those investigators also found significant correlations between all but one of the Mini-Mult's clinical scales and the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable employed in that part of the analysis. Studies Reporting Data for Total or Composite Scores of General Mental Health Only Leong (1999) obtained significant correlations between the three composite scores from the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983), cited above, and all SACQ variables, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. Significant correlations were found between the Global Severity Index of the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (Derogatis, 1984) and all SACQ variables except the Attachment subscale (for which data were not reported), highest with personal-emotional adjustment (Kim, Lee, & Oh, 1992). Zamostny, Slyter and Rios (1993) reported significant correlations between a total severity of psychopathology score from the Bell Global Psychopathology Scale (Schwab, Bell, Warheit, & Schwab, 1979) and all SACQ subscales, strongest in the case of personal-emotional adjustment. The Mental Health Inventory5 (Berwick, Murphy, Goldman, Ware, Barsky, & Weinstein, 1991), on which high scores indicate poorer mental health, correlated negatively for Shibazaki (1999) with all SACQ subscales, robustly with personal-emotional adjustment. Studies Reporting Data for Specific Aspects of Mental Health Depression. A number of investigators have examined the relation between depression as a particular aspect of psychopathology and the SACQ. Several have reported data for all SACQ subscales, sometimes including the full scale score, and almost always found significant correlations in the expected direction for all the variables, the highest values occurring almost 31 exclusively for personal-emotional adjustment. Thus, Merryman and Zelezny (1993), Wang and Smith (1993), Dodgen-Magee (1992), Vivona (2000b), Yaffe (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000), and Montgomery and Haemmerlie (2001) using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961); Adan and Felner (1987), M. D. Smith (1994), Shibazaki (1999), Hunsberger et al. (1996; and Hunsberger, 2000), Pratt (2001), and Beyers and Goossens (2002a) using the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) (note – there is overlap in the Smith & Hunsberger samples); Flescher (1986b) employing the Depression subscale of the Mental Health Inventory (Veit & Ware, 1983); Hutto (2001) with the Depression subscale of the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (Derogatis, 1984); and Kenny (1995) employing the Depression subscale of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist (Derogatis, 1984) all obtained significant effects for all SACQ subscales (typically strongest with personal-emotional adjustment), and the full scale when reported. One of those investigators, Dodgen-Magee, reports stronger depression/SACQ relation for women than men. Worth noting are M. D. Smith’s (1994) and Hunsberger et al.’s (1996; and Hunsberger, 2000) findings with the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (Radloff, 1977), which was administered initially a month prematriculation and again shortly after the middle of the following second semester, analyzed in relation to the SACQ administered at the latter time. While the CES-D administered prematriculation showed significant negative correlations with all SACQ variables, robustly so for personal-emotional adjustment, the values for the postmatriculation administration were considerably more substantial, as would be expected when related measures are taken more closely in time. Somewhat similarly, Yaffe (1997; see also 32 Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) administered the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1961) to his sample in the first week of the fall term and again around the middle of the second semester, and the SACQ at the latter testing time. He also found for the first BDI administration significant negative correlations with all SACQ variables, the strongest subscale value with personalemotional adjustment, and uniformly stronger correlations in the same patterning for the second BDI administration. Minor "misses" in occasional comparisons occurred in some studies where all SACQ variables were employed. Maton and Weisman (1989; see also Maton, 1989b) got significant correlations between depression as measured by the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983) -- administered both pre- and postmatriculation -- and all SACQ indices except between the prematriculation BSI score and academic adjustment. Consistent with findings described in the previous paragraph, the postmatriculation measure of depression correlated more strongly in general with the SACQ than did the prematriculation measure. Also using the BSI depression subscale, but with a very small sample (N=11), Shilkret and Nigrosh (1995) obtained significant relation only with the SACQ full scale score, though the nonsignificant correlational values with the several subscales were fairly substantial. Hogan (1987/1988), using the Depression Scale of the Psychological Distress Inventory (Lustman, Sowa, & O'Hara, 1984), had significant values in three of the five SACQ variables (i.e., not the Social Adjustment or Attachment subscales), strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. Other researchers interested in the relation between depression and adjustment to college have been selective in the use of SACQ variables. Kim et al. (1992) report data for only three of the four SACQ subscales (omitting Attachment) and found significant correlations in the 33 expected direction between the BDI (Beck et al., 1961) and all three subscales and the full scale, the strongest value again gotten with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. Rice et al. (1997) found the Kandel Depression Scale (Kandel & Davies, 1982) to be strongly correlated with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale and less so with the Social Adjustment subscale, the only two SACQ variables employed, for both Black and White students and men and women. Cooley and Carden (1992), administering the BDI (Beck et al.) both prematriculation and at the end of the first semester and the Academic Adjustment subscale at the end of the first semester, obtained significant correlations in the expected direction for both testings (again stronger in the case of postmatriculation BDI administration). Using only the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, Oliver et al. (1998; see also Reed, 1994) obtained strong correlation with the BDI (Beck et al.). Using only the SACQ full scale score, Humfleet and Ribordy (1990) found significant relation with the Depression scale from the Mini-Mult (Kincannon, 1968); as did Jampol (1988/1989) with the BDI (Beck et al.), but not Lopez, Campbell, & Watkins (1986) also using the BDI (Beck et al.). Sutton (1996) found significant correlation between the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale and a truncated, 10-item version of the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. In a study of reverse culture shock in American missionaries’ children returning from residence in foreign countries to attend college in the United States, Huff (1998) used the Homecomer Culture Shock Scale (HCSS; Fray, 1988), which has a subscale measuring grief – a form of depression -- experienced in leaving the foreign country where a considerable portion of the students’ lives had been spent. That investigator found an understandably negative 34 correlation between the Grief subscale and social adjustment to college, but, maybe oddly, a positive correlation with academic adjustment. Anxiety. A number of investigators have examined the relation between anxiety as a particular aspect of psychopathology and the SACQ. Considered first are those studies that report data for all SACQ variables, or at least all subscales. Adan and Felner (1987) and Wang and Smith (1993) found significant and substantial correlations in the expected direction between measures of both state and trait anxiety from the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, 1983) and all SACQ indices, the highest values tending to occur for personal-emotional adjustment, as did Kline (1992) using only state anxiety and Carlson (1986) trait anxiety . In Kline’s study the magnitude of relationship was very similar for both men and women. Flescher (1986b) got results consistent with those from the STAI, for all SACQ indices, using the Anxiety subscale of the Mental Health Inventory (Veit & Ware, 1983), as did Vivona (2000b) using the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988) and Hutto (2001) with the Anxiety subscale from the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (Derogatis, 1984). Shibazaki (1999) obtained negative correlations between the Costello-Comrey Anxiety Scale (Costello & Comrey, 1967) and all SACQ subscales except academic adjustment, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. Kenny (1995) found significant negative correlations with the Academic and PersonalEmotional Adjustment subscales only, highest for the latter, using the Anxiety subscale of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist (Derogatis, 1984). With a related instrument, the Brief Symptom Inventory (Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983), Shilkret and Nigrosh (1995) obtained significant 35 correlation between its Anxiety subscale and personal-emotional adjustment only; the correlational values for all other SACQ variables were substantial but nonsignificant due very likely to very small sample size. Other investigators report findings for selected SACQ variables only. Using the STAI (Spielberger, 1983), Jampol (1988/1989) obtained a substantial correlation between state anxiety and the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable reported; as did Lopez (1989) between trait anxiety and the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ subscale employed; and as did Oliver et al. (1998; see also Reed, 1994) between the Beck Anxiety Inventory and the PersonalEmotional Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. In a study focusing on a particular manifestation of anxiety, Ollendick, Lease and Cooper (1993) identified a group of students who reported current symptoms of separation anxiety disorder (as well as experience of such symptoms earlier in life), a second group who had no such symptoms currently but had had them earlier, and a group of normal controls who reported no such symptoms currently or in the past. The current symptom group had significantly lower scores on the SACQ Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales than either of the other two groups, which did not differ from each other. On the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale there were significant differences among all three groups; the current symptom group had the lowest scores and the normal controls the highest, with the other group intermediate. There were no differences among the three groups on the Academic Adjustment subscale. While the Ollendick et al. (1993) study focused on the consequences for adjustment of separation anxiety as a diagnosable disorder, other investigators used the Separation Anxiety subscale of the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence (SITA; Levine, Green, & Millon, 36 1986) to examine the effects of possibly a more benign form of that kind of anxiety that may be expected to occur more commonly in the general college population. For the SITA, separation anxiety is defined as distress resulting from threatened or actual loss of contact with an important other person, and fears of abandonment. Consistent and fairly strong negative correlations have been found between that variable and all SACQ indices, strongest for personal-emotional adjustment (Cooler, 1995; Lapsley, 1989; Rice, Cole, & Lapsley, 1990; Wang & Smith, 1993). Three studies focused on yet other particular manifestations of anxiety. Using the Phobic Anxiety subscale from the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (Derogatis, 1984), Hutto (2001) obtained significant negative correlations with all SACQ variables. Shilkret and Nigrosh (1995) found significant correlation between the Brief Symptom Inventory’s Phobic Anxiety subscale (Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983) and personal-emotional adjustment. Liter (1987) reported a weak but statistically significant correlation between the Death Anxiety Scale (Templer, 1970) and the SACQ Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. The phenomenon of worry may very well be regarded as related to anxiety. The Penn State Worry Questionnaire (Meyer, Miller, Metzger, & Borkovec, 1990), designed to measure worry as a trait, was found by Vivona (2000b) to correlate in the expected negative direction with all SACQ variables except Institutional Attachment, strongly with personal-emotional adjustment. Vivona (2000b) also employed the Worry Domains Questionnaire (WDQ; Tallis, Eysenck, & Matthews, 1992), which measures extent of worry in five content areas, two of which – Relationships and Self-Confidence – were used by Vivona. WDQ subscales for both areas correlated moderately to strongly in the expected negative direction with all SACQ variables, again strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. 37 Other disturbances of affect. While the studies considered thus far in this section have dealt specifically with depression or anxiety, several investigators have employed variables seeming to represent varieties of less precisely identified, or possibly more generic, affective or “emotional” states. Terrell (1989), with a variable called emotional lability derived by factor analysis of instruments including the Coping Response Indices Inventory (Billings & Moos, 1984), the Adult EAS Temperament Survey (Buss & Plomin, 1984), and the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965), obtained significant negative correlations with academic, social, and, especially, personal-emotional adjustment, the only SACQ variables reported. Brooks and DuBois (1995) used principal components analysis to derive a variable they called emotional stability (the opposite of emotional lability) from three instruments -- the Problem Solving Inventory (Heppner & Petersen, 1982), the Goldberg Big-Five Factor Markers (Goldberg, 1992), and the Global Self-Worth score of the Self-Perception Profile for College Students (Neemann & Harter, 1986) – and found a significant positive correlation between it and the PersonalEmotional Adjustment subscale. For Mathis and Lecci (1999), there were negative correlations between the Negative Affect Scale of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) and all SACQ variables, robustly with personal-emotional adjustment. Wintre and Sugar (2000; see also Sugar, 1997) found, for both male and female students, strong correlations between the Neuroticism variable from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, 1992), principally a measure of negative affect, and all SACQ subscales, especially personal-emotional adjustment. The Emotional Stability subscale of the “Big Five” Inventory (John, Donahue, & 38 Kentle, 1991) is somewhat equivalent to the NEO-FFI’s Neuroticism variable, though reverse keyed, and for Montgomery and Haemmerlie (2001) it correlated in the expected positive direction with all SACQ indices, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. The Adjustment Scale of the Hogan Personality Inventory (Hogan & Hogan, 1995), on which low scores are considered to be indicative of neuroticism or negative affect, was found by Montgomery, Haemmerlie, and Watkins (2000; data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) to correlate in expected positive direction with all SACQ indices, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. Oliver et al. (1998; see also Reed, 1994), using a measure of emotional stress derived from the Stress Audit (Miller, Smith, & Mehler, 1987), obtained a significant expected direction correlation with personal-emotional adjustment, the only SACQ variable employed. M. D. Smith (1994), Hunsberger et al. (1996; see also Pancer, Hunsberger, Pratt, & Alisat, 2000, and Hunsberger, 2000; and note that there is overlap between the M. D. Smith and Hunsberger et al. samples), Pratt (2001), and Yaffe (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000), using the Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983) as a measure of the degree to which a respondent is feeling generally stressed, all found strong negative correlations with all SACQ variables, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. A self-designed four-item measure of felt stress associated with adjusting to college for freshmen in their first week of the academic year was found by Wildman (1998) not to be related to any variable of the SACQ administered in the tenth week of the first semester. Interestingly, M. D. Smith (1994), Hunsberger et al. (1996; and Hunsberger, 2000) and Yaffe (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000), cited above, administered the Perceived Stress 39 Scale twice to their samples, the first testing for Smith and Hunsbrger et al. (with overlapping samples) occurring about a month prior to the start of the first semester and for Yaffe in the first week of that semester. The second PSS administration for all investigators was around or soon after the middle of the second semester, at which time the SACQ was also administered. As might be expected, Yaffe’s immediately postmatriculation-administered PSS predicted the SACQ somewhat better than Smith’s and Hunsberger et al.’s prematriculation testing, but in all instances the PSS variables from the second semester were the strongest SACQ predictors, to approximately the same degree for these different investigators. The concept of culture shock presumably can be seen as a form of general stress state, as might “reverse culture shock” as measured by the Homecomer Culture Shock Scale (HCSS; Fray, 1988). Huff (1998) used that scale in a study of American missionaries’ children returning from residence in foreign countries to attend college in the United States, and found negative correlations between its total score and the SACQ’s Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales. Dissociation. Students with high scores on a measure of dissociative experiences and symptomatology (the Dissociative Experiences Scale, or DES; Bernstein & Putnam, 1986) had significantly lower scores on all SACQ indices than students with low-to-mid range DES scores (Sandberg & Lynn, 1992). Similarly, Marcotte (1995) found modest negative correlations between the DES and all SACQ subscales except social adjustment. Eating disorders. Significant correlations in the expected direction, several moderately strong and a few even robust, were found by Kenny (1992) in all but three of 25 comparisons between the SACQ indices and 5 subscales of the Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI; Garner, 40 1991). The strongest correlations tended to be with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. Oliver et al. (1998; see also Reed, 1994) combined EDI subscales into two composite scores, eating problems and personality traits associated with eating problems, and found strong negative correlations for both with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale (the only SACQ variable reported). A somewhat different means of identifying students with eating disorders was employed by Rosko (1990). On the basis of answers to Ousley’s (1986) Weight Management, Eating, and Exercise Habits Questionnaire, analyzed in relation to formal psychiatric diagnostic criteria, students were assigned to a series of four groups presumably representing increasing presence of eating disorder: normal eaters, binge eaters, subthreshold bulimics, and clinical bulimics. The clinical bulimic category, however, is not relevant to this discussion because approximately a third of its members were graduate students. There were no significant differences among the three groups consisting of undergraduate students on the Social Adjustment subscale, and data for the Attachment subscale were not reported. But on the Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and full scale the subthreshold bulimic group had lower scores than normal eaters, and on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale and full scale the binge eater group was also lower than the normal eaters. There were no significant SACQ differences between the binger and subthreshold bulimic groups. Aspects of good mental health. Most of the mental health variables considered thus far in this chapter actually represent aspects of mental ill-health, albeit possibly varying considerably in severity. But some investigators have employed as independent variables person characteristics 41 that may be regarded as aspects of good mental health. One investigator (Addison, 1996) found strong positive relation with all SACQ subscales for a variable called existential well-being, which, when the test-items measuring it are examined, seems to tap a sense of personal well-being that is the obverse of depression or even in itself a positive mental state. That is, it is conceivable that a non-depressed person may either have or not have a sense of well-being, and the person with a sense of well-being is likely to be regarded as having good mental health. The measure employed by Addison was the Existential Well-Being subscale of the Spiritual Well-Being Scale (SWBS; Ellison, 1983). Flescher (1986b) similarly obtained significant positive correlations between the Psychological Well-Being subscale of the Mental Health Inventory (Veit & Ware, 1983) and all SACQ variables except the Attachment subscale, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. Mathis and Lecci (1999) employed the Positive Affect Scale of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988), which would seem to be closely related to sense of personal/psychological well-being, and found it to be moderately strongly to robustly correlated with all SACQ variables. Higher scores on the Well-being scale of the California Psychological Inventory-Revised (CPI; Gough, 1987) were found by Haemmerlie and Merz (1991) to be associated with higher full-scale scores on the SACQ, the only SACQ variable reported. Also apparently related to positive affect and personal/psychological well-being is the variable optimism – viewing desired outcomes as attainable -- as measured by the Life Orientation Test (LOT; Scheier & Carver, 1985). For Montgomery, Haemmerlie, & Ray (2001) the LOT correlated positively and approximately equally with all SACQ indices. With that same 42 instrument administered to entering freshmen prematriculation, and the SACQ the following March, optimism was positively correlated with all SACQ variables (Hunsberger, 2000; Pratt, 2001). Similarly, in a subsequent study at the same university, Hunsberger et al. (2000) found that entering freshmen who have higher optimism scores on the LOT in a prematriculation testing have a higher SACQ full scale score – the only SACQ variable reported – the following March. Using a revised version of the LOT (Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994), Johnson (2001) obtained correlations of approximately the same magnitude as those described above with all SACQ variables. A self-designed five-item scale for assessing positiveness of attitude toward starting college (defined in terms of optimism, enthusiasm, happiness, calmness, and confidence), administered in the first week of the freshman year, was found by Wildman (1998) to predict in expected direction institutional attachment and social and overall adjustment in the tenth week of the first semester. It seems reasonable to regard self-discipline in dealing with one’s emotions and temper as an aspect of good mental health. Higher scores on the Self-control subscale from the California Psychological Inventory-Revised (CPI; Gough, 1987), which measures that variable, was found by Haemmerlie and Merz (1991) to be associated with higher scores on the SACQ full-scale, the only SACQ variable reported. Those investigators also used another variable reflecting good mental health -- Positive Self-fulfillment (“Vector 3”; defined in terms of relative absence of neurotic trends and conflicts, maturity, stability) -- that was factor-analytically derived from the items of the CPI. Higher scores on that variable were, as in the case of the CPI Self-control subscale cited above, associated with higher scores on the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ 43 variable reported. Physical Health Characteristics. Gilkey et al. (1989; see also Gilkey, 1988) obtained significant correlations in expected direction for measures of general health symptoms from the Health Checklist (Cline & Chosy, 1972), and self-assessed health, with the SACQ full scale score (the only SACQ variable reported). Oliver et al. (1998; see also Reed, 1994) found a moderately strong negative correlation between an index of physical symptoms derived from the Stress Audit (Miller et al., 1987) and the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale (the only SACQ variable used), as did Sutton (1996) between an index of physical symptoms from a shortened version of the Strain Questionnaire (Lefebvre & Sandford, 1985) and a truncated, 10-item, version of the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. Mathis and Lecci (1999) examined the relation between a number of physical health indicators and the SACQ. Interestingly, while no relation was found between number of selfreported visits to a health center and SACQ variables, there were moderately strong negative correlations between number of “official” health center visits for doctor appointments with all SACQ variables. Also, there was negative correlation for number of class absences for illness with personal-emotional and overall adjustment, and positive correlation for self-reported good physical health with academic, personal-emotional, and overall adjustment. Friedland (1990) found no differences on any SACQ variable between physically disabled and non-disabled students. Neither were there any differences within the disabled sample on the SACQ full scale score (the only SACQ variable reported in these particular analyses) between congenitally and adventitiously, progressive and non-progressive, or aid-using and non-aid-using 44 disabled. Learning disabled students in Saracoglu's study (1987; see also Saracoglu, Minden, & Wilchesky, 1989) had lower Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale scores than non-learning disabled students, and the effects were more apparent in the upper class years than the freshman year. Analysis of the item-cluster scores for the Academic Adjustment subscale yielded findings that make good sense given the nature of learning disability and the plight of persons with such disability who are willing to place themselves in an environment that by definition is difficult for them. That is, the differences between the learning disabled and nonlearning disabled students on the Academic Adjustment subscale were due to the cluster of items related to academic performance, where difference favoring non-learning disabled students would be expected, and not to the three other clusters in the subscale where such differences would be less likely, i.e., motivation for being in college and doing academic work, translation of that motivation into effort, and satisfaction with the academic environment. Not focusing on a diagnosable condition as such, but instead on behavioral characteristics or symptoms associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), Panori (1997) administered to a general, unselected sample of college students a combination checklist/rating scale designed for use with clinical populations, but with instructions adapted for use with a nonpatient population. Thus, strong negative correlations were obtained between scores from the Patient’s (ADHD) Behavior Checklist (Barkley, 1990, p. 627) and all SACQ subscales, highest with personal-emotional and academic adjustment. 45 CHAPTER 5 PERSON CHARACTERISTICS AS DETERMINANTS OF COLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: SELF-REGARD OR SELF-APPRAISAL VARIABLES Research findings indicate that variables construable as measuring self-regard or selfappraisal may play an important role in determining effectiveness of adjustment to college, though here, too, there may be confusion as to whether such variables are determinants of adjustment or merely aspects of adjustment. Included in this category of self-regard or selfappraisal variables are such constructs as self-esteem, self-efficacy, self-confidence, and selfconcept as well as other related constructs. Pertinent to this category of variables are references made earlier in this monograph to (on p. ??) a measure of sense of integrity of the self from the College Life Task Assessment Instrument (Brower, 1990b), and (on p. ??) a measure of sense of coherence, defined in terms of confidence in meeting life challenges, from Antonovsky’s (1987) Sense of Coherence Questionnaire (Posselt, 1992). But a large number of more directly relevamt findings will be presented now. Self-Esteem, Self-Efficacy and Related Variables: Regarding Oneself in General For ease of presentation, considered first will be studies that employed either all SACQ variables or at least all SACQ subscales, followed by those that used or reported selected SACQ variables. Moderate to moderately strong correlations, sometimes even robust, have been found by a number of investigators between the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965), which yields a measure of general or overall self-esteem, and all SACQ subscales and fullscale where 46 reported (Birnie-Lefcovitch, 1997; Hertel, 1996; M. D. Smith, 1994; Hunsberger et al., 1996 and Hunsberger, 2000, samples overlapping with Smith’s; Yaffe, 1997, see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000; Hickman, Bartholomae, & McKenry, 2000; Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001; and Pappas, 2000), and by Weinstock (1995) and Napoli and Wortman (1998) with all SACQ subscales. And for Hertel there was no difference in the prediction of SACQ variables by selfesteem for first- and second-generation college attenders. In the Smith (1994) and Hunsberger et al. (1996; and Hunsberger, 2000) studies cited above, the Rosenberg scale (Rosenberg, 1965) was administered one month prematriculation and again shortly after the mid-point of the following second semester, with the SACQ administered on the latter testing occasion. Correlations between the postmatriculation self-esteem measure and the SACQ variables were somewhat larger than those for the prematriculation one. Very similarly, Yaffe (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) administered the Rosenberg to his sample in the first week of the fall term and again around the middle of the second semester, with the SACQ administered at the latter time. While correlations for the initial Rosenberg testing with the later administered SACQ were moderately strong, those for the second semester Rosenberg administration were more robust. In one study (Savino, 1987; Savino, Reuter-Krohn, & Costar, 1986b) significant and substantial positive correlations were found on two testing occasions between the Psychological Coping Resources Scale (Pearlin & Schooler, 1978), which includes all items from the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) plus other items that pertain to perceived mastery of the environment, and all SACQ indices, highest with the full scale. Another standard instrument measuring self-esteem, the Coopersmith Self-Esteem 47 Inventory (Coopersmith, 1981), has been employed by several investigators and has yielded results similar to the foregoing (Crouse, 1990; Garrett, 1994; Mooney, 1989; Mooney, Sherman, & LoPresto, 1991; and Rodriguez-Perez, 1991). Like findings have also been reported by Foster (1997) using the total self-concept score, a measure of overall level of self-esteem from the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale (Roid & Fitts, 1988), and by Hutto (2001) using the Global SelfWorth score from the Self-Perception Profile for College Students (Neemann & Harter, 1986). Two studies reporting data for all SACQ subscales and sometimes the full scale have employed measures of self-efficacy, a self-regard or self-appraisal construct probably not very different from self-esteem, and obtained findings in relation to SACQ variables that are consisitent with those already described in this section. Those studies were by Silverthorn (1993), with the Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale (Fible & Hale, 1978); and Davis (1988), who adapted three items from the Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale (Tipton & Worthington, 1984) to assess self-efficacy “as a person” in student-athletes, very likely tantamount to an index of general self-esteem. The Positive:Yourself subscale from the Personal Resilience Scale (Organizational Development Resources, 1996) is described as measuring belief in oneself as valuable and capable, which would seem to be a combination of self-esteem and self-efficacy, and was found by Brunelle-Joiner (1999) to correlate moderately strongly with all SACQ variables. Several investigators employed variables that are the obverse of self-esteem or selfefficacy. Thus, Kenny (1992) found robust but negative across-the-board relations with all SACQ variables for a measure of perceived self-ineffectiveness (the opposite of self-efficacy) from the Eating Disorder Inventory (Garner, 1991). Kenny (1995) also obtained strong 48 significant negative correlations with all SACQ subscales for the Interpersonal Sensitivity (sensitivity defined in terms that indicate the opposite of self-esteem) subscale of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist (Derogatis, 1984), as did Hutto (2001) with the same subscale from a related instrument, the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (Derogatis, 1984). And for Montgomery and Zoellner (1994) there were negative correlations with all SACQ subscales for the SelfHandicapping Scale (Jones & Rhodewalt, 1982; Berglas & Jones, 1978), which taps adoption by an individual of a preemptive face-saving attitude involving forecast of less than adequate performance, again the antithesis of the attitude of self-efficacy. In Berglas and Jones’ words, self-handicapping reflects “a basic uncertainty concerning how competent one is.” Several investigators employed standard measures of self-esteem but only selected SACQ variables, in all instances obtaining findings in similar magnitude as those described above. Thus, for the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965), correlations were gotten by Gilkey et al. (1989; see also Gilkey, 1988 and Protinsky & Gilkey, 1996) with the SACQ fullscale; by Bettencourt et al. (1999) with the Academic and Social Adjustment subscales, both the Rosenberg and the SACQ having been administered in the first half of the first semester and again in the second half of the second semester, stronger relation on the latter occasion; and by Sutton (1996) with a truncated, 10-item, version of the Academic Adjustment subscale. For the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory (Coopersmith, 1981), correlations were obtained by Frazier and Cook (1993) with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale; and by Clauss (1995) with the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales. And Caplan (1996) found relation between the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale, Form C’s total self-concept score (Roid & Fitts, 1988), a measure of overall level of self-esteem, and the SACQ full-scale score. 49 Saracoglu (1987; see also Saracoglu et al., 1989), employing a measure of self-esteem based largely but not entirely on Rosenberg's Self-Esteem Scale (described in Bachman & O'Malley, 1977), and using also the Self-Efficacy Scale (Sherer et al., 1982), obtained substantial correlations for each measure with three SACQ subscales (the Attachment subscale was not used). Changes in scores on the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965) from the beginning to the end of the academic year were found by Bettencourt et al. (1999) to be associated in the expected positive direction with changes over the same time span in the two SACQ variables employed, i. e., academic and social adjustment. Bettencourt et al. (1999), in addition to using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965), which they identify as a measure of personal self-esteem, also employed a measure of collective self-esteem, the Collectice Self-Esteem Scale (CSES; Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992). Regarded as a part of personal self-esteem, collective self-esteem is described as an individual’s positive/negative evaluation of his or her social group membership, in this instance defined in terms of membership in a college residence hall. In the first half of the first semester, positive correlation was found between the CSES total score and the SACQ Social Adjustment but not the Academic Adjustment subscale; in the last half of the second semester, there was significant correlation with both SACQ measures. Changes in scores on the Collective Self-Esteem Scale (Luhtanen & Crocker, 1992) from the beginning of the academic year to its end were found by Bettencourt et al. (1999) to be associated in the expected positive direction with changes over the same time span in SACQ Academic Adjustment subscale scores, while no relation was found with changes in Social 50 Adjustment subscale scores. Some studies have employed self-regard/self-appraisal constructs that may be seen as only a step removed from general or overall self-esteem or self-efficacy. Significant and substantial correlations were obtained by Zamostny, Slyter, & Rios (1993) between the Narcissistic Injury Scale (Slyter, 1991) -- which measures impairment to feelings about the self and about past relationships related to the development of the self -- and all SACQ subscales, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. Fuller and Heppner (1995) derived a variable they identified as “Confidence” from factor analysis of data from a number of measures of person characteristics: the Prestatie Motivatie Test, a measure of achievement motivation (PMT; Hermans, 1970); the College Self-Efficacy Instrument (CSEI; Solberg, O’Brien, Villareal, Kennel, & Davis, 1993); the Multidimensional Academic-Specific Locus of Control Scale (MASLOC; Palenzuela, 1988); and the Career Decision Scale (Osipow, 1987). That variable, Confidence, correlated moderately strongly with all SACQ subscales. Finally, Lapsley and Edgerton (1999b) used measures of two variables from the New Personal Fable Scale (NPFS; copies of the instruments available from the Scale’s author, D. K. Lapsley), sense of personal invulnerability and sense of personal omnipotence, both of which on their face would seem to be variants of feelings of general self-efficacy. And in terms of theory they are thought of as “positive illusions” about the self, which may be adaptive parts of the normal separation-individuation process in the developmental course; omnipotence itself is described as a kind of “super self-confidence.” Both variables were found by Lapsley and Edgerton to be positively correlated with the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment 51 subscales, the only two SACQ indices employed. Self-Esteem, Self-Efficacy and Related Variables: Regarding Specific Aspects of Oneself Several investigators have examined the implications for SACQ-measured adjustment to college of self-esteem, self-efficacy, self-confidence, or self-concept regarding particular areas of function, kinds of activities, or aspects of the self. Possibly the most comprehensive approaches of this sort have been made by Foster (1997) using the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale (Roid & Fitts, 1988), which has subscales measuring an individual’s evaluation of eight different aspects of one’s self (e. g., physical, moral-ethical, social, familial), and by Hutto (2001) using the SelfPerception Profile for College Students (Neemann & Harter, 1986), which has subscales assessing self-worth in twelve different aspects of oneself (e. g., intellectual ability; scholastic and athletic competence; romantic and parental relationships). Both Foster and Hutto with considerable consistency found correlations with all SACQ variables, the former investigator with samples of student-athletes and non-athletes. Self-regard/self-appraisal concerning social functioning. Several investigators, including Foster (1997) and Hutto (2001), examined self-attitude concerning social involvement in particular as it relates to adjustment to college. With the Social subscale of the Tennessee SelfConcept Scale (Roid & Fitts, 1988), which measures positive feelings about the self in generalized social situations, Foster found correlations with all SACQ variables – strongest with social adjustment – in her samples of student-athletes and non-athletes. The Self-Perception Profile for College Students (Neemann & Harter, 1986) has measures of different kinds and aspects of social involvement – romantic relationships, close friendships, relationship with parents, social acceptance, morality, appearance – and in all instances Hutto found correlation 52 with all SACQ variables. Saracoglu (1987), with samples of learning disabled and non-learning disabled students, found significant and substantial correlations between a measure of social self-efficacy from the Self-Efficacy Scale (Sherer et al., 1982) and the Social Adjustment subscale of the SACQ; lesser but still significant correlation with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale for the learning disabled students only; and no correlation with the Academic Adjustment subscale (data for the Attachment subscale were not given). With the same measure of social self-efficacy, Rice et al. (1997) got moderate to strong correlations with the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales (the only SACQ indices employed), strongest with the former subscale, for both White and Black students and men and women. Natera (1998), with a sample of Latino students, found the Social Self-Efficacy subscale from the College Self-Efficacy Instrument (CSEI; Solberg et al., 1993) to be correlated with the SACQ measures of academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment (the only SACQ variables used), strongest with social adjustment. Using the same CSEI variable with Mexican-American students, Shibazaki (1999) obtained correlations with all SACQ subscales, again strongest with social adjustment. Employing a measure of lack of social confidence from the Interpersonal Dependency Inventory (IDI; Hirschfeld et al., 1977), Caro (1985/1986) and Polewchak (1998, 1999) obtained significant negative correlations with all SACQ indices, highest with the Social and PersonalEmotional Adjustment subscales. However, when Polewchak divided her sample by gender, the correlations for males in the case of social adjustment and institutional attachment failed to reach significance. With a measure of social self-confidence (the Texas Social Behavior Inventory; Helmreich & Stapp, 1974), rather than lack of social confidence, Kenny (1995) found significant 53 positive correlations with all SACQ subscales, highest with social adjustment. Other investigators (Cooler, 1995; Lapsley, 1989) used a subscale of the SeparationIndividuation Test of Adolescence (SITA; Levine, Green, & Millon, 1986) that is intended to measure self-centeredness but may also be seen as tapping a sense of social confidence (possibly even an arrogant self-confidence). It – the Self-Centeredness or Practicing-Mirroring subscale – correlated positively with all SACQ indices, most strongly with social adjustment. Self-regard/self-appraisal concerning cognitive functioning. Several studies have focused on self-worth, self-efficacy or self-confidence concerning various kinds of cognitive activity. Using the Self-Perception Profile for College Students (Neemann & Harter, 1986) – which has separate subscales assessing self-worth regarding intellectual ability, scholastic competence, and creativity – Hutto (2001) found correlation between all three variables and all SACQ indices, typically strongest with academic and personal-emotional adjustment. A number of investigators have focused more particularly on measures of self-assessed scholastic competence, or, in other words, academic self-efficacy. With a sample of Latino students, Natera (1998) used the Course Self-Efficacy subscale from the College Self-Efficacy Instrument (CSEI; Solberg et al., 1993) as a measure of academic self-efficacy and found strong correlations with academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment (the only SACQ variables employed), robustly with academic adjustment. With Mexican-American students, Shibazaki (1999) used that same CSEI index to find correlations with all SACQ subscales, again robustly with academic adjustment. Lent et al. (1997) found a positive correlation with academic adjustment – the only SACQ variable employed -- for an instrument designed by themselves, the Self-Efficacy for Broad Academic Milestones Scale, which measures confidence regarding one’s 54 general academic capabilities. Martin et al. (1999) and Martin et al. (2000) obtained positive correlation between their own single-item rating of academic self-confidence and the SACQ fullscale score, the only SACQ variable reported. Ridinger (1998), with a sample of student-athletes and a three-item measure of academic self-efficacy designed by herself, obtained correlations with all SACQ variables except social adjustment, particularly robust with academic adjustment. Davis (1988) adapted items from the Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale (Tipton & Worthington, 1984) to assess academic self-efficacy attitudes of student-athletes and found significant and substantial correlations with the PersonalEmotional and Academic Adjustment subscales, especially the latter, and the full scale of the SACQ. Other investigators have studied self-efficacy regarding specific academic courses or academic tasks. Using the Mathematics Self-Efficacy Scale (Betz & Hackett, 1983), Lent et al. (1997) obtained a positive correlation with the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable reported, for the Mathematics Course Self-Efficacy subscale, which measures confidence in one’s ability to complete a variety of math-intensive college courses with a grade of B or better, but did not find relation for the Mathematics Problem Self-Efficacy subscale, which measures confidence in one’s ability to solve particular mathematics problems. Chartrand et al. (1990a, 1992) obtained a significant correlation between their own measure of self-efficacy regarding successful completion of psychology courses and the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. Interested in another aspect of cognive activity, Brooks and DuBois (1995) used principal components analysis to derive a measure of self-assessed problem solving effectiveness from 55 three instruments -- the Problem Solving Inventory (Heppner & Petersen, 1982); the Goldberg Big-Five Factor Markers (Goldberg, 1992); and the Global Self-Worth score of the SelfPerception Profile for College Students (Neemann & Harter, 1986) – and obtained a strong correlation in the expected direction between that variable and the SACQ Academic Adjustment subscale. Marcotte (1995) employed the Problem Solving Inventory itself and found strong negative (expected direction; low scores indicate high problem solving effectiveness) correlations with all SACQ subscales, highest for academic adjustment. Using the College Self-Efficacy Instrument’s (CSEI; Solberg et al., 1993) overall score, which assesses self-efficacy regarding both social and cognitive tasks related to “experiences in college,” Bartels (1995) obtained strong correlations with all SACQ variables, especially academic and social adjustment and the full scale, as did Fuller (2000) with all SACQ subscales. Self-regard/self-appraisal concerning emotions/affect. Natera (1998), already cited as using measures of academic and social self-efficacy from the College Self-Efficacy Instrument (CSEI; Solberg et al., 1993), devised a third CSEI-like subscale to assess what she termed “personal self-efficacy.” It followed the format of the CSEI and had content paralleling that of the SACQ’s Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, thus being intended presumably to measure degree of self-confidence regarding the control or management of the aspects of psychological and physical adjustment represented in that SACQ subscale. That new selfefficacy variable correlated moderately to strongly with academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment (the only SACQ variables employed), unsurprisingly strongest with personalemotional adjustment. A variable measuring self-worth regarding “finding humor in one’s life” from the Self- 56 Perception Profile for College Students (Neemann & Harter, 1986) was found by Hutto (2001) to correlate positively with all SACQ indices. Self-regard/self-appraisal concerning athletic ability. Interested in assessing athletic selfefficacy attitudes of student-athletes, Davis (1988) adapted items from the Generalized SelfEfficacy Scale (Tipton & Worthington, 1984) and Ridinger (1998) devised her own measure. Davis found significant relation with all SACQ variables except the Academic Adjustment subscale, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment, and Ridinger obtained significant correlations with all SACQ variables. With a self-designed measure of self-assessed athletic aptitude, very likely also tapping athletic self-efficacy, Ridinger (1998) found correlations with all SACQ variables except institutional attachment. Also possibly related, Ridinger, using a self-designed measure of student-athlete’s self-assessment of their athletic performance, found significant correlation with institutional attachment. With a general sample of undergraduate students (i. e., not selected as student-athletes), and using a measure of self-worth regarding athletic competence from the Self-Perception Profile for College Students (Neemann & Harter, 1986), Hutto (2001) found weak positive correlation with social and overall adjustment only. Self-regard/self-appraisal concerning dealing with disability. Friedland (1990) examined the relation between a particular kind of self-efficacy attitude in physically disabled students and their adjustment to college. That investigator employed the Attitudes Towards Disabled Persons Scale-Form O (ATDP; Yuker, Block & Campbell, 1960), which, when administered to disabled persons, may be seen as a measure of felt ability to cope with life’s demands (i. e., self- 57 efficaciousness) despite having a physical disability. Strong correlations were found between that measure and all SACQ variables. Self-regard/self-appraisal concerning capacity for adjusting to college. As cited earlier (pp.??-??), the Anticipated Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (ASACQ; Baker et al., 1985; Baker & Schultz, 1992a & b, 1993) has been used to provide a prematriculation measure of a student's expectations regarding the impending transition into college, in effect a measure of self-efficacy or self-confidence regarding one's capacity for adjusting to college. Correlations between the corresponding ASACQ and SACQ subscales (e.g., anticipated academic adjustment v. actual academic adjustment in the freshman year) and full scale have been found to be all positive, statistically significant, and moderately strong (Baker & Schultz, 1992a; Marcy, 1996; Williams, 1996), indicating that adjustment to college is in part a function of a student's level of confidence or sense of self-efficacy regarding the impending transition. While the ASACQ was intended primarily for use prematriculation, and was indeed employed in that manner in the studies described in the foregoing paragraph, in one study (Sears, Brewer, & Szarlan, 2002) it was administered to entering freshmen seven to ten days into the first semester. Though instructions were still phrased to elicit expectations regarding subsequent adjustment to college, the likelihood is that students’ “expectations” were to some degree influenced by very early postmatriculation experiences, thus reflecting self-assessed actual adjustment as well. Be that as it may, Sears et al. had three groups of freshmen varying in terms of prematriculation preference for dormitory assignment and subsequent actual assignment, all three of which, in addition to the ASACQ, were administered the SACQ between the ninth and eleventh weeks of the first semester. Correlations between the corresponding ASACQ and SACQ 58 subscales for all three groups were all positive, statistically significant, and moderately strong, in all instances somewhat higher – as would be expected – than the average values for those studies where the ASACQ had been administered prematriculation. Two other studies used means other than the ASACQ for assessing prematriculation expectations regarding subsequent adjustment to college. In one (Sullivan, 1991), matriculating students’ pre-enrollment ratings of how well they expected to adjust to the social life of the university correlated positively with the SACQ full scale at the end of the first quarter (no data were reported for the other SACQ variables). In the other study (Jackson, Pancer, Pratt, & Hunsberger, 2000), incoming freshmen were asked prematriculation to answer three open-ended questions regarding their expectations concerning the upcoming college experience, and, by implication at least, how effectively they would be dealing with it. Responses were analyzed into five categories: positive academic expectancies, negative academic expectancies, positive social expectancies, negative social expectancies, and positively-toned expectancies regarding adaptation/coping efforts. The SACQ was administered three times during the students’ ensuing four college years (late first, second, and fourth years), but only the full scale score was employed in analyses. Positive academic expectancies were found by Jackson et al. (2000) to be positively correlated with the adjustment measure at all three testings, and negative academic expectancies were correlated (negatively) in the late second year SACQ administration. Both positive and negative social expectancies correlated in the expected direction with the freshman year SACQ testing, but not for either variable in the two subsequent testings. Positively-toned expectations regarding adaptive efforts correlated (positively) with the SACQ full scale in the freshman year 59 but not in the two later testings. Thus, while all prematriculation expectancy measures except negative academic expectancies predicted adjustment in the freshman year, those regarding academic experience predicted the effectiveness of subsequent adjustment beyond the freshman year and even until late in the fourth year in the case of positive academic expectancies.. Jackson et al. (2000) went a step further in their analysis and identified four expectancy styles based on patterns of the above-described five expectancy components. The four styles were: (a) optimistic (a clear preponderance of positive academic and social expectancies, especially the latter, and low negative expectancies); (b) prepared (high scores on positive academic and adaptation/coping efforts but tempered by a realistic expectation of potential adjustment demands and need for coping skills); (c) fearful (high scores on negative academic and social expectancies and low scores on positive expectancies regarding academic, social and adaptation/coping efforts); and (d) complacent (low to moderate scores on all expectancy components, indicating lack of formation of clear expectancies). Reporting results for this part of their study in terms of the SACQ full scale score only, Jackson et al. (2000) found that in the first two postmatriculation testings -- late in the first and second years -- students in the fearful category had poorer adjustment than those in the other three styles. And the same pattern among the four styles – fearful students lowest, prepared or optimistic highest, and complacent intermediate – was still seen at the final, end of the fourth year, SACQ administration. Expanding the scope of interest somewhat beyond simply expectations regarding college adjustment to expectations regarding capacity to deal with obstacles or impediments in the future pursuit of educational and career goals in general, Hutz (2002a,b) employed the Coping With 60 Barriers Scale (CWB; Luzzo and McWhirter, 2001). CWB subscales concerning felt capacity to cope with each kind of barrier showed moderate positive correlations with all SACQ subscakes (except for career-related barriers in relation to academic adjustment, where there was no significant relation), somewhat stronger in the case of education-related barriers for all SACQ subscales. These differential findings seem reasonable in light of the probably more proximal relationship between college asjustment and educational barriers than more removed career barriers. Variants on Self-Regard or Self-Appraisal Variables Seemingly related to self-efficacy is the concept of primary appraisals in the coping process as described by Folkman and Lazarus (1985). Primary appraisals refer to the cognitive means by which an individual assesses or comprehends stressors, presumably in relation to one’s capacities to deal with them, and which may lead to evocation of coping strategies. So, such appraisals would seem to involve self-efficacy judgments in the process of adaptation to stressful circumstances. Jampol (1988/1989) used Folkman and Lazarus' (1985) means of measuring primary appraisals (the Primary Appraisal Emotions Scale) and found significant correlations in the expected direction between the four kinds of appraisal (of challenge, threat, harm, and benefit) and the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable reported. Appraisals of challenge and benefit (judgments of self-efficacy regarding capacity for dealing with a stressor?) are associated with better adjustment to college, and appraisals of threat and harm (questionable selfefficaciousness?) with less good adjustment. Another variant on self-regard or self-appraisal studied in relation to the SACQ is the 61 concept of realistic self-appraisal (as measured by the Superiority Scale; Robbins & Patton, 1985). That variable was seen by Robbins and Schwitzer (1988) and Schwitzer & Robbins (1986) to correlate in the expected direction with the Academic Adjustment (positively), Social Adjustment (negatively), and Personal-Emotional Adjustment (positively) subscales of the SACQ, but Dewein (1994) found only negative correlations with the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales. Engaging in rational thinking about oneself, on the other hand, as measured by the Attitudes and Beliefs Scale-II (DiGiuseppe, Leaf, Exner, & Robin, 1988), was found by Friedland (1990) to be correlated positively and moderately strongly with all SACQ variables except the Academic Adjustment subscale. 62 CHAPTER 6 PERSON CHARACTERISTICS AS DETERMINANTS OF COLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: COGNITIVE CHARACTERISTICS To be considered in this chapter are person characteristics that have been studied in relation to college adjustment as measured by the SACQ and that may be identified as cognitive in nature. Some of the mental/physical health variables already considered can be seen as at least partially involving cognition (e. g., depression, dissociative states) or even more fully so (learning disability, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder), but in those instances the health issue was regarded as pre-eminent for our purposes. The self-regard/self-appraisal variables also already discussed all involve cognition in the form of judgments made by individuals concerning their worth, competence, or capabilities, but were deemed deserving of a chapter of their own because of the extent of their shared qualities and their extensive use in relation to the SACQ. Subsequent chapters dealing with person determinants will also be seen to include occasional variables that could be categorized as cognitive but placed in those other chapters because of other transcending features. A possible characterization, then, of the variables to be reviewed here is that they are more purely in and of themselves cognitive in nature, without the kind of transcending qualities that would lead to their being placed in other categories. Cognition of Causality Possibly only a short step removed from self-regard or self-appraisal variables are constructs involving cognition of causality in the explanation of one’s behavior, in particular for purposes of this monograph that behavior being adjustment to college. A principal example of 63 such constructs would be the concept of locus of control, i. e., whether an individual sees primary responsibility for one's behavior as inhering in oneself (internality, indicating a sense of selfefficaciousness) or outside of oneself (externality, indicating a lack of sense of selfefficaciousness). Locus of control as a general characteristic. Zea et al. (1995) and Zea (1997) used Rotter’s (1966) Internal-External Locus of Control Scale with four ethnic groups of students. There were significant correlations in the expected negative direction between that measure (on which lower scores signify greater internality) and all SACQ variables for Caucasian and Latino students, some fairly robust for the latter group; with three of the five SACQ variables (not academic or personal-emotional adjustment) for African-American students; and with none of the SACQ variables for Asian-American students. For their sample as a whole there were significant correlations with all SACQ variables. With the same instrument and also using all SACQ variables, and with a sample of African-American students, Evans-Hughes (1992) found expected direction correlations with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale and full scale. Martin (1988; see also Martin & Dixon, 1989), Martin and Dixon (1994), and Kintner (1998) report that students identified as "internals" on Rotter's scale scored higher on the SACQ full scale (the only SACQ index employed) than students identified as "externals." In Kintner’s study locus of control was assessed prematriculation, and the SACQ was administered in the last week of October. Using Rotter's instrument as modified by Levenson (1973), Shilkret and Taylor (1992) found significant correlations in the expected direction with all SACQ variables except the Attachment subscale. 64 Several SACQ-using investigators have employed another measure of locus of control, the Nowicki-Strickland Internal-External Control Scale for Adults (Nowicki & Duke, 1974). Marcotte (1995) found significant correlations in the expected direction with all SACQ subscales, strongest with academic and personal-emotional adjustment, as did Montgomery and Haemmerlie (2001), strongest with academic adjustment. Cooley and Carden (1992) administered the Nowicki-Strickland both prematriculation and at the end of the first semester, and the SACQ at the latter time. They obtained significant correlations in the expected direction for both testings with academic adjustment, the only SACQ variable reported. As in earlier-cited instances of pre- and postmatriculation measurement of a person variable, the latter has the higher correlation with SACQ indices. Jampol (1988/1989) used the same measure of locus of control prematriculation and found a significant correlation in the expected direction with the full scale of the SACQ (the only SACQ variable reported) administered at the middle of the first semester. Fuller and Heppner (1995) derived a variable they called External Control (i. e., external locus of control) by factor analysis of data from several measures of person characteristics: the Prestatie Motivatie Test (PMT; Hermans, 1970); the College Self-Efficacy Instrument (CSEL; Solberg, O’Brien, Villareal, Kennel, & Davis, 1993); the Multidimensional Academic-Specific Locus of Control Scale (MASLOC; Palenzuela, 1988); and the Career Decision Scale (Osipow, 1987). That new variable correlated negatively with all SACQ indices, strongest with academic and personal-emotional adjustment. Situation- or function-specific locus of control. As in the case of the self-regard/selfappraisal variables described earlier, so, too, locus of control is sometimes defined not only in a 65 general sense but also in terms of specific kinds of situations or areas of function. In five studies, frequent and often robust correlations in the expected direction were obtained between a measure of academic locus of control (Trice, 1985) and SACQ indices, usually highest for academic adjustment and the full scale, and considerably stronger in the prediction of academic adjustment than was the general measure of locus of control (Bartels, 1995; Mooney, 1989; Mooney et al., 1991; Ogden & Trice, 1986; Weinstock, 1995). Fuller (2000) employed the Multidimensional Academic-Specific Locus of Control Scale (MASLOC; Palenzuela, 1988), which, in addition to a total score, has subscales measuring three aspects of locus of control. The total score and the Helplessness subscale correlated in the expected direction and to about the same moderately strong degree with all SACQ subscales, as did the Luck subscale to a somewhat lesser degree with academic and personal-emotional adjustment, and as did the Internality subscale – relatively weakly -- with all SACQ subscales except Personal-Emotional Adjustment. One investigator (Rines, 1998) employed a measure of internality from the Attributional Style Questionnaire (Peterson, Semmel, von Baeyer, Abramson, Metalsky, & Seligman, 1982), which can be assessed in relation to causation of life events in general or good and bad events separately. For internality in relation to life events in general, there was a weak positive correlation with the Academic Adjustment subscale. However, for internality in relation to positive life events there were slightly stronger positive correlations with all SACQ variables, and, regarding negative events, no significant relation. Jampol (1988/1989) adapted the SACQ itself to construct a measure of "situation-specific appraisal of control," i. e., whether the self or the environment was seen as the primary 66 determinant of the student's impending adjustment to college (the measure was administered during an orientation period before the start of the academic year). She found a significant correlation in the expected direction with the SACQ full scale score (the only SACQ variable reported). In this and all other of these instances concerning locus of control variables, the greater the sense of personal control over behavioral outcomes, the better the adjustment to college. Complexity of causal explanation of behavior. Another variable involving cognition of causality in the explanation of behavior was studied by M. D. Smith (1994), who expected that complexity in the causal explanation for one’s own and others’ behavior would have positive consequences for adjustment to college. Using the Attributional Complexity Scale (Fletcher, Danilovics, Fernandez, Peterson, & Reeder, 1986) to measure that variable, Smith found a weak negative correlation with personal-emotional adjustment. Then, constructing the College Attributional Complexity Scale (CACS) in order to provide for college students a more situationally specific measure of the variable, half of the items referring to social events and half to academic, Smith (1994) still found modest negative relation to personal-emotional adjustment. Interesting to note in clarification of the negative nature of the latter correlation, the CACS correlated positively for Smith -- rather than negatively as expected – with measures of depression, perceived stress, and college hassles. Other aspects of cognition of causality. Rines (1998), using the Attributional Style Questionnaire (Peterson, Semmel, von Baeyer, Abramson, Metalsky, & Seligman, 1982), found that an habitual tendency to perceive life events in general, or good life events in particular, as caused by factors that will persist unchanged over time (i. e., stable attribution of causation) has 67 no significant relation with any SACQ variable, but perceiving bad life events in such a manner correlates negatively with the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and overall adjustment. That is, perceiving negative life events as caused by relentlessly recurring factors is related to poorer adjustment to college. Rines (1998) also found that an habitual tendency to perceive life events in general as caused by factors likely to be operative in a wide variety of situations (i. e., global attribution of causation) rather than to causes specific to particular situations, is unrelated to any of the SACQ variables. However, perceiving bad life events as caused by globally operative factors is negatively correlated with personal-emotional adjustment, and perceiving good life events as caused by globally operative factors is positively correlated with academic, social, and overall adjustment to college. Ideational/Intellectual Characteristics Ideational flexibility. The Flexible:Thoughts subscale from the Personal Resilience Questionnaire (Organizational Development Resources, 1996} is described as measuring ability to view situations from multiple perspectives, to suspend judgment while considering alternative perspectives, to tolerate paradoxes and contradictions, and to think creatively and effectively. Brunelle-Joiner (1999) found that subscale to be weakly correlated with all SACQ variables to approximately the same degree. The Openness to Experience variable (i. e., receptivity to new ideas, values, and actions; creativity) from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, 1992) would seem to be related to ideational flexibility. That variable was found by Wintre and Sugar (2000; see also Sugar, 1997) to be correlated for men with academic and social adjustment and institutional 68 attachment, but not for women with any SACQ subscale (SACQ full scale scores were not reported). The Openness to Experience variable from the “Big Five” Inventory (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991), employed by Montgomery and Haemmerlie (2001), correlated with all SACQ indices, weakest with academic adjustment. The Intellectance scale of the Hogan Personality Inventory (Hogan & Hogan, 1995), considered to measure a variable similar to the NEO-FFI’s Openness to Experience (i. e., imaginative, quick-witted, analytical), was found by Montgomery, Haemmerlie, and Watkins (2000; data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) to correlate weakly with academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment. Walker (1996) employed the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale-Form A (Weissman & Beck, 1978) – which is regarded as tapping inflexible, absolutistic and perfectionistic ways of thinking, especially in relation to oneself – and found moderately strong negative correlations between it and all SACQ indices, strongest with personal-emotional and overall adjustment. Organized thinking. The Organized subscale from the Personal Resilience Questionnaire (Organizational Development Resources, 1996} is described as measuring the ability to find order in chaos and structure in ambiguity. Brunelle-Joiner (1999) found that subscale to be correlated with all SACQ variables, strongest with academic adjustment. Another possible measure of organized thinking is the Intellectual Efficiency scale from the California Psychological Inventory-Revised (CPI; Gough, 1987), higher scores on which were found by Haemmerlie and Merz (1991) to be associated with higher scores on the full-scale of the SACQ, the only SACQ variable reported. Judgment. The Prudence scale from the Hogan Personality Inventory (Hogan & Hogan, 1995) would seem to be a measure of decision-making that is characterized by circumspection, 69 self-discipline, organization, dependability, and conscientiousness, or goodness of judgment. That scale was found by Montgomery, Haemmerlie, & Watkins (2000; data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) to correlate with all SACQ variables, strongest with academic adjustment.. Ideational complexity. In a study of prematriculation expectations regarding subsequent transition to college, Pancer, Pratt, Hunsberger, and Alisat (1995; see also Pancer, Hunsberger, Pratt, & Alisat, 2000) employed a variable that can be identified as cognitive or ideational complexity. Students’ expectations were elicited by means of six openended questions and analyzed in terms of the number of different aspects of the transition considered (i. e., differentiation) and the extent to which the different aspects were interrelated (i. e., integration), the combination of which yielded a measure of the degree of complexity of a student’s expectations. Degree of complexity of expectations was found to make a difference in adjustment to college for students who prematriculation had reported experiencing high stress in their life situations as measured by the Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983), those showing high complexity having better scores on all SACQ indices than those showing low complexity. But there were no differences in adjustment between high and low complexity students who did not see their prematriculation life situations as particularly stressful. Yaffe (1997) apparently used the same technique as Pancer et al. (1995) described above – i. e., responses to six openended questions analyzed in terms of differentiation and integration of expectations regarding impending adjustment to college. He reported no significant correlation with any SACQ index for a variable he identified as integrative complexity, in this study not examined in interaction with perceived stress. 70 Scholastic aptitude. Several investigators have examined the relation between scholastic aptitude as measured by the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT) or the American College Testing Program (ACT), and the SACQ, with fragmentary and inconsistent results. Typically the findings are incidental to larger research purposes; the particular aptitude variables (e. g., whether verbal, quantitative, or a composite score) are not always specified; and more often than not only selected SACQ variables are used. Thus, in those studies that did, or probably did, use the SAT combined verbal and quantitative scores, Just (1998) found positive correlations with academic, personal-emotional and overall adjustment; Maton (1989b) with academic adjustment and institutional attachment; Liter (1987) and Conti (2000a) with personal-emotional adjustment; Chartrand et al. (1990a, 1992) with academic adjustment, the only SACQ variable used; and Williams (1996) with the full scale score, the only SACQ variable reported. But Hurtado et al. (1996) obtained no correlation between that SAT composite score and the Academic Adjustment subscale (the only SACQ variable employed) administered in the sophomore year to Latino students who had been high academic achievers in high school. Conti (2000a), reporting findings for the SAT composite score only with academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment, obtained a negative correlation with social adjustment in addition to the positive association with personal-emotional adjustment cited above, but no significant relation with academic adjustment. In a study that used the SAT verbal and quantitative scores separately, Terrell (1989) obtained positive correlations for the former and negative for the latter with the SACQ Academic and Social Adjustment subscales, and no correlation for either with personal-emotional adjustment, the only other SACQ variable employed. In addition to the findings cited above for 71 Conti, she also used the SAT verbal and quantitative scores separately, and three SACQ administrations: late September, late November, and February (Conti, 2000c). The Verbal score correlated weakly and negatively with institutional attachment only in the September testing; modestly and positively with personal-emotional adjustment only in November; and, surprisingly, negatively and moderately with academic adjustment only in February. The Quantitative score correlated weakly and negatively with social adjustment and institutional attachment in the September administration; modestly and positively with personal-emotional adjustment in November; and not with any SACQ variable in February. In the studies that used, or probably used, the ACT composite score, Dewein (1994) found positive relation with the Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales; Brooks and DuBois (1995), with the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and full scale; Marcy (1996), with academic adjustment, the only SACQ variable employed; and Schriver (1996), with the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable used. But Lent et al. (1997) found no correlation between that ACT composite score and academic adjustment, the only SACQ variable employed. Lent et al. (1997) also found no correlation between the ACT Mathematics score and academic adjustment, the only SACQ variable reported. In a study that employed an index of scholastic aptitude based on either the SAT or ACT, depending on which was available for a sample of student-athletes, Ridinger (1998) found no significant relation with any SACQ variable. Means of Reacting to and Coping with Stressors Several investigators have focused on means of reacting to and coping with stress, 72 customarily regarded as cognitive variables, as person characteristics influencing effectiveness of adjustment to college. The Hogan Personality Inventory (Hogan & Hogan, 1995) has a variable called Stress Tolerance, from its occupational scales, that measures a general ability to deal with pressure and adversity, and which was found by Montgomery, Haemmerlie, and Watkins (2000; data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) to correlate moderately to strongly with all SACQ variables, highest with personal-emotional adjustment. Posselt (1992) investigated two variables thought to be important in resistance to negative effects of stressful life events, sense of coherence and hardiness. She used Antonovsky’s (1987) Sense of Coherence Questionnaire (SOCQ; also apparently called the Orientation to Life Questionnaire), which measures confidence that the challenges one meets in the course of living are comprehensible, manageable, and even enjoyable. In more commonly used terminology, sense of coherence seems related to self-efficacy and locus of control’s internality. That variable was found to be strongly and positively related to all SACQ indices, highest with personalemotional adjustment. The second variable employed by Posselt (1992) was personality hardiness, as measured by the Hardiness Test (Campbell, Amerikaner, Swank, & Vincent, 1989), a modification of the Personal Views Survey (Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn, 1982). Personality hardiness is defined in terms of ability to establish commitments to other persons and activities (“commitment”); a sense that events in one’s life are in some important degree a consequence of one’s own input (“control”); and the capacity to respond readily, even with pleasure, to unexpected events in one’s life (“challenge”). While the Hardiness Test yields subscale scores for the three above-mentioned 73 components, there is factor analytic evidence in Kobasa et al. (1982) and Posselt (1992) that only the composite score may be psychometrically justified. In any event, of the 20 correlations reported by Posselt between the four Hardiness Test variables and the five SACQ variables, all are significant except for the challenge/academic adjustment comparison, and several are moderately strong; again, the correlations tended to be higher with personal-emotional adjustment Rather than the modification employed by Posselt (1992), Mathis and Lecci (1999) used the Personal Views Survey (PVS; Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn, 1982; Kobasa, 1985) itself to measure hardiness – slightly modified to make it appropriate for use with college students. The PVS also yields measures of commitment, control, and challenge, plus a composite score. Of the twenty correlations between the four PVS variables and five SACQ indices, all were significant except – as in Posselt’s findings – for the challenge/academic adjustment comparison. Also, as in the case of Posselt’s study, several of the values are moderately strong, but this time consistently stronger for social than for personal-emotional adjustment. Possibly related to the sense of coherence and hardiness variables discussed above, more so the former, is the Positive:World subscale of the Personal Resilience Scale (Organizational Development Resources, 1996), which is intended to measure degree of optimism in one’s attitude toward dealing with challenges and opportunities in one’s world. Brunelle-Joiner (1999) found moderately strong correlations between that subscale and all SACQ variables. Other investigators have focused more specifically on particular means of dealing with pressure and stressors. Marcotte (1995), employing the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations (CISS; Endler & Parker, 1990), found that task-oriented coping (i. e., problem-focused strategies 74 intended to deal directly with stressful situations) was strongly and positively correlated with all SACQ subscales, strongest with academic adjustment. Emotion-oriented coping (attempting to manage one’s emotional response to stressful situations at the expense of dealing more directly with the source of stress) was strongly and negatively correlated with all SACQ subscales, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. Avoidance-oriented coping (failure to confront the source of stress) was correlated in Marcotte’s study only with academic adjustment, negatively and modestly. Gallant (1994) also used the CISS but only the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale of the SACQ, the latter of which was administered twice in the first semester, a month apart. For her, task-oriented coping correlated positively, though less robustly than for Marcotte (1995), with personal-emotional adjustment in both testings. Emotion-oriented coping correlated negatively with the SACQ variable on both testings, but avoidance-oriented coping not at all. Silver (1995) found positive correlations with all SACQ variables for a measure of problem-focused coping from the Revised Ways of Coping Checklist (RWCCL; Vitaliano, 1993), and negative correlations for an index of avoidance coping with all SACQ variables except social adjustment. The RWCCL does not have a measure labelled emotion-focused coping as such, but it does have six other subscales, some of which are described as emotionfocused. Two that would seem to fall in that category, Blames Self and Blames Others, correlated negatively with several of the SACQ variables. Two of the remaining four RWCCL subscales showed positive correlations with the SACQ: Count Blessings with all SACQ variables except social adjustment, and Seeks Support with academic and overall adjustment. Two RWCCL subscales, Wishful Thinking and Religiosity, had no correlations with the SACQ. 75 Leong, Bonz, and Zachar (1997) used the COPE Scales (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989), which assess twelve different styles of dealing with stress. One style, active coping, is a task-oriented and problem-solving approach that involves formulating plans of action and implementing them. It was found to be positively but weakly related to both academic and personal-emotional adjustment. An emotion-focussed coping strategy considered to be of questionably adaptive value (see Carver et al.) – i. e., venting of emotions – was negatively related to personal-emotional adjustment. None of the COPE-measured styles were found by Leong et al. (1997) to be related to social adjustment or institutional attachment, and only two additional to active coping (i. e., positive reinterpretation and growth, and planning, both of which seem to be forms of active coping) were correlated (positively) with academic adjustment. Three other styles (mental disengagement, behavioral disengagement, and seeking social support for emotional reasons, the first two of which would seem to be forms of avoidance coping) were negatively correlated with personal-emotional adjustment (Leong, 1999). Of 60 correlations between the twelve COPE Scales and the five SACQ variables, only the eight cited above were statistically significant and those were of modest strength (Leong, 1999). Rines (1998) used an instrument for measuring two kinds of active coping (cognitive and behavioral) and avoidance-coping (Holahan & Moos, 1987) that was derived from the Health and Daily Living Form (HDL; Moos, Cronkite, Billings, & Finney, 1983). Active-behavioral coping had modest positive correlations with all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment, highest with academic adjustment, and active-cognitive coping correlated modestly with social adjustment and institutional attachment. Avoidance coping correlated negatively and more 76 robustly with all SACQ variables. Feenstra, Banyard, Rines, and Hopkins (2001), using essentially the same student sample as Rines (1998) but a composite or total score from the HDL (active-cognitive plus active-behavioral divided by the full set of coping strategies), found moderate to moderately strong correlations with all SACQ variables except Institutional Attachment, which was not reported. An index of active coping from the Behavioral Attributes of Psychosocial Competence Scale (BAPC; Tyler, 1978) correlated positively and sometimes robustly with all SACQ variables for White, African-American, Latino, and Asian-American samples separately and combined (Zea, Jarama, & Bianchi, 1995; Zea, 1997) and also for a still larger sample combining such ethnically identified students (Zea, Reisen, & Tyler, 1996). Active coping is defined by the test’s author as high initiative, ability to plan realistic goals, persistence in attaining those goals, and capacity to enjoy success and endure failure. These findings were essentially replicated, with slightly lesser correlations in most comparisons, using a shortened version of the BAPC (BAPCC; Zea et al., 1996; Zea, 1997). The BAPC items retained in the shortened version seem to tap a kind of cheery self-confidence about one’s ability to face and deal with everday life situations. Terrell (1989) identified a variable she called “activity,” or a tendency towards taking an active approach in meeting challenges, by factor analysis of data from two instruments – the Spheres of Control Battery (Paulhus, Molin, & Schuchts, 1979; Paulhus & Christie, 1981) and the Adult EAS Temperament Survey (Buss & Plomin, 1984). She found that variable to correlate fairly strongly with academic adjustment, though not social or personal-emotional adjustment. But a variable that Terrell identified simply as “coping” – derived apparently by factor analysis of data from the Coping Response Indices Inventory (Billings & Moos, 1984) and 77 consisting of items from the Logical Analysis, Problem-Solving, and Affective Regulation subscales – showed no correlation with the Academic, Social or Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales, the only SACQ variables used. Smith (1994), using the Adolescent Coping Orientation for Problem Experiences (ACOPE; Patterson & McCubbin, 1987a,b), obtained a positive correlation between a measure of problem-focused coping (defined as an active, direct approach to dealing with stressors) and the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable reported. Also using the A-COPE, Just (1998) concluded that use of more positive coping strategies was associated with higher scores on all SACQ variables except the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. Possibly viewable as a measure of coping is the Proactive subscale from the Personal Resilience Scale (Organizational Development Resources, 1996), which is described as tapping the ability to act decisively, to take risks and seek challenge. Brunelle-Joiner (1999) found that subscale to be weakly correlated with all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment Ropar (1997) was interested in “avoiding/expressing” as a means of coping with family conflict, as measured by the Structural Family Interaction Scale-Revised (Perosa & Perosa, 1990a&b), but found no relation between it and any SACQ variable. In summary, the coping style (or styles?) that has been most frequently studied in relation to the SACQ, and that has been most successful in showing relation (and positively so), is the one customarily identified as task-oriented, problem-focused, or active. Emotion-oriented and avoidant coping styles have been less investigated, but when employed characteristically show negative relation with SACQ variables. The magnitude of relation between the three basic categories of coping style (i. e., task-oriented, emotion-oriented, avoidant) and SACQ indices is 78 quite variable, due very likely to the fact that a number of different tests have been used to measure coping styles, and probable lack of commonality of construct definition. 79 CHAPTER 7 PERSON CHARACTERISTICS AS DETERMINANTS OF COLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: GOAL ORIENTATION VARIABLES A number of studies have investigated the relation between the SACQ and goal orientation characteristics defined in terms of achievement need, goal directedness, academic motivation, and vocational and educational planning. Some of these variables seem to be cognitive in nature and some motivational, but common to all is a focus on the future and, in a sense, on what one hopes or expects to be doing in that future. Achievement Need Need for achievement as measured by the Prestatie Motivatie Test (PMT; Hermans, 1970) was found by Bartels (1995) to correlate with all SACQ indices, highest and strongly with the Academic Adjustment subscale. The same variable measured by the Achieving Tendency Scale (Mehrabian & Bank, 1978) correlates modestly with the Academic and Social Adjustment subscales (Wang & Smith, 1993). Higher scores on two scales intended to measure achievement need from the California Psychological Inventory-Revised (CPI; Gough, 1987), Achievement (via Conformance) and Achievement (via Independence), were found by Haemmerlie and Merz (1991) to be associated with higher scores on the SACQ full-scale, the only SACQ variable reported. Bartels, cited above, also used a variable called Valuing of Achievement from the Life Values Inventory (Mitchell, 1984) and found it to correlate with all SACQ indices except personal-emotional adjustment, to approximately the same degree. Possibly measuring need for achievement more in the social area, as in competitiveness and drive for upward mobility, is the Ambition scale from the Hogan Personality Inventory 80 (Hogan & Hogan, 1995), which was found by Montgomery, Haemmerlie, and Watkins (2000; data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) to be positively and strongly correlated with all SACQ indices, highest with social adjustment. Still possibly measuring achievement need/ambition, but now more in the occupational area, Haemmerlie, Robinson, and Carmen (1991) employed Type A traits Competitiveness and Job Involvement from Form C of the Jenkins Activity Survey (JAS; Jenkins, Zyzanski, & Rosenman, 1979) and found that higher scores on the former variable were associated with higher SACQ full scale and Academic Adjustment subscale scores, while higher scores on the latter variable were associated with higher Social Adjustment subscale scores. The Conscientiousness variable, defined as involving achievement-striving, from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, 1992) was found by Wintre and Sugar (2000; see also Sugar, 1997) to correlate for both men and women with all SACQ subscales, strongest with academic adjustment, as was also true for Napoli and Wortman (1998) using a mixed gender sample of community college students. In the former study the correlation with academic adjustment was quite robust. Seemingly related to the construct of conscientiousness, and through it to the achievement need, is the Responsibility scale from the California Psychological Inventory-Revised (CPI; Gough, 1987), which is defined in terms of taking one’s duties seriously, presumably in the interests of achieving. Haemmerlie and Merz (1991) found higher scores on that variable to be associated with higher scores on the SACQ full-scale, the only SACQ variable reported. Those investigators also used another conscientiousness-type variable called Norm-following/Normdoubting (“Vector 2”; defined in terms of self-discipline, conventionality, dependability) that was 81 factor-analytically derived from the items of the CPI. Higher scores on that variable were, as in the case of the CPI Responsibility subscale, associated with higher scores on the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable reported. Goal Directedness Using a measure of goal directedness (i.e., the Goal Instability Scale; Robbins & Patton, 1985), Dewein (1994) found rather strong correlations in the expected direction with all SACQ subscales, especially academic and personal-emotional adjustment. With that same measure, Schwitzer and Robbins (1986), Robbins and Schwitzer (1988), and Schwitzer, Robbins and McGovern (1993) found significant relation with all SACQ indices except social adjustment, as did Robbins, Lese, and Herrick (1993) with the Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales, the only subscales for which data were given. The Focused subscale from the Personal Resilience Scale (Organizational Development Resources, 1996) -- which is designed to measure sense of purpose, priorities, and goals -- was found by Brunelle-Joiner (1999) to correlate rather strongly with all SACQ subscales, especially academic and personal-emotional adjustment. With a specially designed measure of goal setting skills, Davis (1988) obtained in student-athletes a moderately strong correlation with the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable for which data were reported. With a specially designed measure of success in attaining goals, Davis found a stronger correlation with the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable cited. Just (1998) focused on exploration of and commitment to occupational and ideological life goals as essential components of one’s identity (after Marcia, 1966, 1980), using the Revised 82 Version of the Extended Objective Measure of Ego-Identity Status (EOMEIS-2; Bennion & Adams, 1986). The EOMEIS-2 describes four identity statuses that may characterize an individual. In Achieved status, various options have been explored by the individual and selective decisions or commitments made, representing a desirable developmental outcome that was found by Just to be positively correlated with all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment. In Moratorium status, the student is currently with some difficulty exploring among life goals and not yet committed, and this variable was negatively correlated with all SACQ variables. Negatively correlated with all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment was Diffusion, wherein the student has neither explored nor committed, preferring to put off decision-making. In the fourth status, Foreclosed, the student has made commitment, but often prematurely and without adequate exploration, and this variable showed no relation to SACQ variables. Carlson (1986) employed the Extended Version of the Objective Measure of Ego Identity Status (EOM-EIS; Grotevant & Adams, 1984), the predecessor of the EOMEIS-2 (Bennion & Adams, 1986), to form groups of students defined in terms of the goal exploration/commitment variables described in the preceding paragraph. On the SACQ Academic Adjustment subscale the Achieved status group had the highest score, with no difference among the other three groups (i.e., Diffusion, Moratorium, Foreclosed). For the Social Adjustment subscale, the Institutional Attachment/Goal Commitment subscale, and full-scale, the Diffusion status group had the lowest score, no difference among the other three groups. Regarding personal-emotional adjustment, Moratorium status scored lowest, no difference among the other groups. Significant correlation in the expected direction for men but not women was obtained by 83 Schultheiss and Blustein (1994) between the Establishing & Clarifying Purpose Task subscale -which purports to measure general sense of purpose in life -- from the Student Developmental Task and Lifestyle Inventory (Winston, Miller, & Prince, 1987) and the SACQ Academic Adjustment subscale, but not for either men or women on the Personal-Emotional or Social Adjustment subscales (data for the Attachment subscale and full scale were not reported). Using another measure of sense of purpose in life, the Purpose-in-Life-Test (Crumbaugh & Mahalick, 1964), Liter (1987) obtained significant correlations with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment and Attachment subscales of the SACQ and the full scale. Academic Motivation Also relevant to the role of goal orientation in adjustment to college are studies that focus on academic motivation and commitment. The Academic Motivation Scale (Baker & Siryk, 1984a) administered prematriculation correlated positively in both semesters of the freshman year with the Academic Adjustment subscale – the only adjustment variable reported -- from an earlier, shorter version of the SACQ (Baker & Siryk, 1984b). Similar findings were obtained with a later freshman class using the current version of the SACQ (Baker & Siryk, 1989, p. 50). McGowan (1988) reported a somewhat stronger correlation between the same Academic Motivation Scale administered in the fourth week of the first semester and the Academic Adjustment subscale of the SACQ administered in the eleventh week, plus lesser though still significant correlations with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale and the full scale. Also administering the Academic Motivation Scale postmatriculation, Brett (2000) obtained correlations with all SACQ subscales. In a study with freshmen at a Belgian university Beyers and Goossens (2002a) employed 84 a measure of academic motivation taken from a larger Dutch instrument designed to assess capacity for organizing and managing course work (Depreeuw, E., & Lens, W. Study management skills: A questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, Center for Research in Motivation and Time Perspective, Catholic University, Leuven, Belgium).They found a robust correlation between that measure and the SACQ Academic Adjustment subscale as well as lesser but still significant relation with the other SACQ variables, weakest with personal-emotional and social adjustment. With his own measure of academic motivation, the Mastery Learning Scale, Lopez (1997) obtained a significant correlation with the Attachment subscale, the only SACQ variable used. Chartrand et al. (1990a, 1992) used a measure of affective and behavioral commitment to the student role (the Salience Inventory; Super & Nevill, 1985), which seems rather similar in purpose to the academic motivation scale employed in the above-mentioned studies, and found a significant correlation with the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable reported. The college student version of the Work Preference Inventory (WPI; Amabile, Hill, Hennessey, & Tighe, 1994) measures intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in the college context, the former kind of motivation referring to engagement in academic effort primarily for its own sake and the latter to engagement in such effort primarily in response to inducement apart from the work itself. Conti (2000a) found intrinsic motivation as measured by the WPI to be positively correlated with academic and personal-emotional adjustment but not social adjustment, the only three SACQ variables reported, in a late September administration of the SACQ, but not to any of those three variables in a late November administration. The only significant correlation for 85 extrinsic motivation was a negative one with personal-emotional adjustment in the September testing. Conti (2000a) also devised the College Goals Questionnaire (CGQ) as another means of assessing motivation and the degree to which it is intrinsic/extrinsic. The CGQ includes some items representing the extent to which one’s goals in attending college are chosen by oneself (Autonomy), and other items the extent to which such goals reflect expectations imposed by others (Control). A combinatory measure of motivational autonomy was achieved by subtracting the average of a student’s ratings for the latter items from the average for the former items. With the CGQ administered in the summer prematriculation, motivational autonomy correlated positively with personal-emotional adjustment in a late September SACQ administration, and with social as well as personal-emotional adjustment in a late November testing. Conti (2000b,c) also analyzed her data separately for the two component parts of the above-cited measure of motivational autonomy from the College Goals Questionnaire, i. e., goals chosen by oneself (Autonomy) and goals associated with expectations of, or imposed by, others (Control). The only Autonomy finding from the three SACQ administrations (September, November, and February) was a moderate positive correlation with the Academic Adjustment subscale in February. Conti (2000b) mentioned that there was restricted range in this variable, and that the results concerning it should be interpreted with caution. Control was a somewhat more productive variable, correlating negatively with personal-emotional and overall adjustment in September; negatively with social, personal-emotional and overall adjustment in November; and negatively with personal-emotional adjustment in February. Another variable apparently tapping intrinsic motivation is the Academic Autonomy 86 subscale from the Student Developmental Task and Lifestyle Inventory (Winston et al., 1987), intended to measure capacity to attain educational goals without direction from others. Using that variable, Schultheiss and Blustein (1994) obtained rather robust correlations in the expected direction for both men and women with the SACQ Academic Adjustment subscale. There were also substantial correlations for both sexes between academic autonomy and personal-emotional adjustment, and for women a smaller but still significant correlation with social adjustment (Schultheiss & Blustein did not report data for the Attachment subscale). Using the Intellectualism subscale – which measures the valuing of intellectual activities and pursuits, including scholarly effort – from the Personal Values Scale-Revised (Braithwaite & Scott, 1991), Hertel (1996) found robust correlations with all SACQ variables except personalemotional adjustment in first-generation college attenders, but no significant correlations in second-generation attenders. The highest subscale correlation for the former group was with academic adjustment. Lent et al. (1997) found a very strong relation (r=.77) between the Academic SelfConcept Scale (ASCS; Reynolds, 1988) and the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable reported. In their study, interestingly enough, Lent et al. regarded the latter variable as a measure of academic self-concept as much as of academic adjustment, which could be justified by the high correlation obtained. However, it also seems reasonable – judging from the factor structure of the ASCS, the nature of the variables employed in its validation, or its author’s description of it as a noncognitive affective variable (see Reynolds) – instead to see that instrument as a measure of academic motivation. Or, looking only at the factor structure and validity criteria, the ASCS might even be seen as a measure less of academic self-concept than of 87 academic adjustment. Possibly having something to do with academic aspiration/motivation would be the School Success scale from the Hogan Personality Inventory (Hogan & Hogan, 1995), which was found by Montgomery, Haemmerlie, and Watkins (2000; data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) to be weakly to modestly correlated with all SACQ variables, strongest with academic and personal-emotional adjustment. The highest degree desired by students could be regarded as an indicator of academic aspiration/motivation, but Just (1998) found no relation between it and any SACQ variable. Planning Characteristics Planning behavior in general. Avoidance of making important life decisions (i. e., not planning) or making unstable ones (not planning well), as identified by the Diffuse Orientation subscale of the Identity Style Inventory-2 (ISI-2; Berzonsky, 1992), is negatively related to academic and overall adjustment to college (Hollmann, 1995; Hollmann & Metzler, 1994). On the other hand, seeking information about oneself and using it to test out understandings about the self and changing them where appropriate in the making of life decisions (i. e., planning well), as measured by the Information Orientation subscale from the ISI-2, is positively related to academic adjustment. Trying to conform in important life decision-making to the desires and expectations of significant other persons in one’s life (Normative Orientation subscale) is positively correlated with the SACQ Attachment subscale and full scale (Hollmann). Planning in relation to vocational issues. Focusing more closely on planning in relation to vocational issues, several investigators employed a measure of clarity and stability of vocational plans (the Vocational Identity Scale of My Vocational Situation; Holland, Gottfredson, & Power, 88 1980). Fuller (2000) found expected direction correlations between that measure and all SACQ subscales, strongest with academic adjustment. Maton and Weisman (1989) and Maton (1989b) reported significant correlations in the expected direction between a postmatriculation administration of that same measure and all SACQ indices, and -- to a somewhat lesser degree as usual -- between that measure administered prematriculation and the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable reported for that administration. For Lopez (1989) there were strong correlations for both men and women between the Vocational Identity Scale and the SACQ Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. Using a self-designed measure of degree of certainty of career plans, Plaud et al. (1986) found significant positive correlation between it and the Academic Adjustment subscale. Chartrand et al. (1990a, 1992) and Camp and Chartrand (1992) found no relation between several indices of interest congruence (Johansson, 1986; i. e., congruence between an individual student's interests and the interests of members of the occupational field to which the student aspires) and the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. A significant relation in the expected negative direction was obtained by Chartrand et al. (1990a&b) between level of vocational/educational indecision as measured by the Career Decision Scale (Osipow, Carney, Winer, Yanico, & Koschier, 1976) and the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ subscale employed. Also merging vocational and educational issues, and, too, focusing on a negative aspect of planning cognition, Hutz (2002a,b) employed the Perception of Barriers Scale (POB; Luzzo and McWhirter, 2001), which contains subscales assessing expectations of encountering obstacles or impediments – both outside and within oneself – in the future pursuit of educational and occupational goals. Hutz found 89 moderately strong negative correlations between expectation of obstacles regarding educational goals and all SACQ subscales, and lesser or no correlation regarding obstacles related to career goals. Planning in relation to educational issues. Looking now more specifically at educational rather than vocational planning, a number of investigators have found a positive relation between decidedness regarding academic major and freshman adjustment to college as measured by the SACQ (Albert, 1988; Allen, 1985; Leonard, 1990; Marcy, 1996; McGowan, 1988; Plaud et al., 1986, 1990; Savino, 1987; Savino, Reuter-Krohn, & Costar, 1986b; Smith & Baker, 1987). Only Martin (1988), reporting data for the SACQ full scale only, found no such relation. The Academic Adjustment subscale is the SACQ variable most consistently and most strongly correlated with decidedness about major, while the Attachment and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales with some frequency show lesser but still significant relations. In an engineering college the effect was found in the first semester for the Social Adjustment subscale as well as for the other SACQ indices (Plaud et al., 1986, 1990), and at one liberal arts college in a second semester testing the effect occurred in social adjustment as well as academic adjustment, institutional attachment and the SACQ full scale (Marcy, 1996). There is some evidence that the relation between major decidedness and adjustment to college is seen more clearly in the second semester of the freshman year than the first (Savino, 1987; Savino et al., 1986b; Smith & Baker, 1987). Using a variable that implies decidedness about academic major, i. e., having mathematics/science-related occupational aspirations, Lent (1997) found it correlated positively with the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. 90 Plaud et al. (1990), in their above-mentioned study of engineering college freshmen, used a somewhat different index of major decidedness, i.e., stability over time of decision status regarding academic major. They obtained information about major status during an orientation period in the summer before matriculation and again during the first half of the freshman year. The lowest means on all four SACQ subscales and the full scale were for students who either had no major at both testing occasions or who changed from one major in the summer to another in the fall that was not available at the college attended. (It should be noted that in the second category of student, stability of major decision is confounded with congruence between major status and the nature of the college attended, an issue to be considered later.) In all but one subscale, the next lowest mean was obtained by the other of these two categories of students. The highest means were found in students who had the same major at both testing occasions and in those who changed from one major to another that was available at the college attended. The means for students who had no major in the summer but had decided upon one by the fall were generally intermediate between the two extreme pairs of groups. Leonard (1990) investigated the relation between decidedness regarding major and adjustment in two colleges that varied in amount of pressure exerted on students to declare a major. (Here again, as above, the question of relation between student and institutional characteristics is raised, and will be revisited later.) She interpreted her findings as indicating that differences in adjustment scores among freshmen varying in decidedness are more apparent in a college that requires declaration of major in the freshman year than in a college that requires declaration in the sophomore year. Finally, with respect to academic major status as an instance of goal orientation and 91 planning, Chartrand et al. (1990b) obtained a positive correlation between degree of satisfaction with one's major and scores on the Academic Adjustment subscale (the only SACQ index used). In Conti’s (2000a,b,c) investigation of student motivation for college described earlier, the College Goals Questionnaire (CGQ) devised by her included a measure of reflectivity or reflection, defined in terms of amount of thinking (i. e., planning) devoted by the student prematriculation to his/her academic goals in college. With the CGQ administered in the summer prematriculation, that variable correlated positively with academic and overall adjustment in September and November administrations of the SACQ, but not with any SACQ variable in a February testing. But, using a prematriculation-administered measure of how often students thought about what university life would be like, Hunsberger et al. (1996) found no correlation with the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable reported. For those same investigators, however, a prematriculation measure of how much students talked about impending university life with their parents was positively but weakly correlated with the SACQ full scale, as was a prematriculation measure of how much students similarly talked with other persons than parents. Very likely also related to motivation and planning for college, Hunsberger et al. (1996) asked students prematriculation how much information they had about the various aspects of the impending university experience, and found it to be positively but weakly related to the SACQ full scale, as was their degree of satisfaction with the information. In a study that would seem to be relevant to students’ planning-for-college characteristics, Williams (1996; see also Chizhik, 1999) was interested in the consequences for adjustment to college of college-bound high school seniors’ knowledge concerning the characteristics of 92 different kinds of colleges and universities, i. e., public and private; two year and four year; liberal arts, trade, and technical institutions. The characteristics of institutions focused on were primarily academic, social, and structural, and included educational mission, programs and degrees offered, student body size and character, campus environment, teaching or research focus, etc. Two means of measuring students’ knowledgeability in this regard were employed by Williams (1996), one an open-ended “brainstorming” technique and the other involving accuracy of matching of institutional characteristics to institutional types (see also Chizhik, 1999, for the latter method). With the former technique no relation was found between “college knowledge,” measured late in the high school senior year, and subsequent adjustment to college, but there were findings – though unexpected ones – using the latter, matching, technique. Paradoxically, for example, Williams (see also Chizhik) found that the greater the high school students’ knowledge about the general characteristics of colleges and universities, the less good was their subsequent adjustment to the college they attended, as reflected in all SACQ indices. Because Williams (1996; see also Chizhik, 1999) had administered the Anticipated Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire as well as the measure of “college knowledge” while students were still in high school, she was able to use a second index of adjustment to college, i. e., the degree of disillusionment, if any, with one’s transition into college as reflected in the difference between anticipated and actual adjustment scores (see pp. for description of this index). Here, Williams (see also Chizhik) found that the greater the students’ knowledge about the characteristics of different kinds of colleges and universities, as measured by the matching technique, the greater the disillusionment as seen in all SACQ indices. This was an 93 overall finding with a sample that included African-American, Latino, Asian-American and White students, and an exception to the full-sample finding occurred for the African-American students, who, on academic adjustment, had a negative rather than positive relation between knowledgeability about institutional characteristics and degree of disillusionment experienced in the adjustment. That is, the more knowledgeable they were, the less disillusionment they experienced. Ridinger (1998) was interested in the prediction of adjustment to college by postmatriculation measurement of the amount and accuracy of prior knowledge about and expectations concerning what life in college would be like (i. e., not expectations concerning their adjustment to college), especially for student-athletes. With such students, and using a selfdesigned eight-item instrument for assessing her independent variable, Ridinger found no significant correlations with any SACQ variable. 94 CHAPTER 8 PERSON CHARACTERISTICS AS DETERMINANTS OF COLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: PERCEIVED RELATIONSHIP WITH PARENTS AND FAMILY A number of studies have explored the consequences for adjustment to college of various aspects of perceived relationship with parents and family, especially psychological separation from and attachment to parents. These variables are treated in this chapter as person characteristics and distinguished (possibly sometimes with difficulty) from other parent/familyrelated variables in a later chapter that are conceived there as environmental factors. Psychological Separation from Parents The first measure of psychological separation from parents to be considered here is an instrument, the Psychological Separation Inventory (PSI; Hoffman, 1984), that has been studied in relation to the SACQ probably more than any other, but consistently such efforts have resulted in findings very largely disconfirming expectations. That is, with an assumption that separation from parents is a hallmark of maturation, and that the four subscales of the PSI measure different aspects of separation, investigators expected in vain that all four subscales would be positively correlated with adjustment to college (Albert, 1988; Bartels, 1995; Allen, 1985, 1986; Beyers, 2001; Beyers & Goossens, 1998, 2002b; Bobier, 1989; Caro, 1985/1986; Choi, 1999, 2000; Clauss, 1995; Dewein, 1994; Edgerton, 1997; Freeman, 1987a; Garner, 1986; Haemmerlie, Montgomery, & Consolvo, 1993; Haemmerlie, Steen, & Benedicto, 1994; Hollmann, 1995; Humfleet, 1987; Kenny, 1992; Kenny & Donaldson, 1992; Kline, 1992; Lapsley, 1989; Lapsley & Edgerton, 1999a; Lapsley, Rice, & Shadid, 1989; Levin, 1996; Lopez, 1989, 1991; Lopez, Campbell, & Watkins, 1986, 1988a, 1989; Marcotte, 1995; Montgomery & Howdeshell, 1993; 95 Morray & Shilkret, 2001; Reeker, 1994; Rice, 1990/1991, 1992; Rice et al., 1990; Rodriguez, 1994; Ropar, 1997; Schultheiss & Blustein, 1994; Schwitzer & Robbins, 1986; Sherman, 1992; Shilkret & Edwards, 1997; Silver, 1995; Silverthorn, 1993; Stoltenberg, Garner & Kell, 1986; Wang, 1993; Wang & Smith, 1993; Weinstock, 1995). However, of the four PSI subscales, only Conflictual Independence (i. e., relative absence of conflict in the relationship with the parents, implying successful separation) shows fairly consistent correlations in the expected positive direction with the SACQ, with all indices, usually highest with personal-emotional adjustment (Baker, 1990). A second subscale, Emotional Independence, has weakly consistent positive correlation with personal-emotional adjustment only, and not as large as in the case of Conflictual Independence. A third PSI subscale, Attitudinal Independence, shows either negative or no correlation with SACQ variables (and also with the Conflictual Independence subscale). The fourth subscale, Functional Independence, has no consistent statistically significant relation with the SACQ indices (and not with Conflictual Independence either). Using a composite score of the PSI Attitudinal, Functional, and Emotional Independence subscales, Rice et al. (1990) and Smith (1994) not surprisingly (given the above-described findings for those three subscales) report no significant relation between it and the SACQ full scale score (the only SACQ variable reported), while Choi (2000) found weak negative correlations between that same composite score and the SACQ full scale score (again the only SACQ variable reported) in relation to mother and father separately. Lopez (1991) focussed on the Conflictual Independence subscale of the PSI, and particularly the CI scores regarding the mother and the father in all possible combinations (i. e., 96 one parent high and the other low, both high, both low), in relation to the SACQ subscales. He found that students having relatively unconflicted relationship with both parents had: higher Academic Adjustment subscale scores than students who had relatively greater conflicted relationship with both parents; and higher Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale scores than students who had relatively greater conflicted relationship with one or the other or both parents. He regarded the various combinations of CI scores as reflecting family coalition patterns. Lopez concluded, furthermore, that conflicted parent-student relationship has a stronger effect on adjustment to college than does degree of conflict between the parents. Fairly consistent findings regarding relation between PSI and SACQ variables were obtained by Beyers and Goossens (1998) across samples of freshmen, juniors, and seniors at a Belgian university. It has been argued elsewhere (Baker, 1990) that the lack of consistent correlation in the expected direction between SACQ variables and three of the PSI subscales, especially Functional and Attitudinal Independence, is very likely due to problems in definition of psychological separation and to psychometric characteristics of the PSI. Additionally, the absence of felt conflict in relation to parents – i.e., the variable measured by the Conflictual Independence subscale -- would seem at best to be only an indirect reflection of successful separation from parents rather than a central aspect of separation itself; actually it may better be regarded either as indicating degree of amicability with or attachment to parents, a topic to be considered in the following section, or a separate variable in and of itself that may have significant consequences for college adjustment. While the three PSI subscales other than Conflictual Independence do seem to represent 97 attempts to measure different aspects of separation – i.e., attitudinal, functional, and emotional – the SACQ findings in relation to those three subscales offer very little or no support for hypothesized connection between psychological separation from parents and adjustment to college. Fortunately there is some support for that relation in other studies using other instruments. One investigator (Gilkey, 1988; see also Gilkey et al., 1989 and Protinsky & Gilkey, 1996) employed the Personal Authority in the Family System Questionnaire-Version C (PAFSQC; Bray, Williamson & Malone, 1984) to assess psychological separation from parents somewhat more broadly and at the same time more focused on the core features of separation, and, interestingly, without reference to relationship conflictedness. That instrument has five subscales that address different aspects of the student/parent relationship, some being indications of successful separation and some unsuccessful separation. Those subscales are: voluntary closeness with one’s parents while at the same time maintaining appropriate boundaries (Intimacy); ability to function autonomously from the parents (Individuation); ability to take responsibility for oneself (Personal Authority); being governed or unduly influenced by parental wishes (Intimidation); and forming close relationship and alliance with one parent in opposition to the other (Triangulation). The first three of these PAFSQ-C categories may be seen as positive forms of separation, and the latter two as negative forms. Indeed, the first variable, Intimacy, presages another important characteristic of perceived student/parent relationship to be considered shortly, viz., parental attachment. None of the five variables correlated with the SACQ Academic Adjustment subscale, but all except Triangulation did correlate in the expected direction with the other 98 SACQ variables except for social adjustment in the case of Intimacy and Intimidation, and personal-emotional adjustment in the case of Intimacy. While differences among the various correlations were not tested for significance, the higher values tended to occur in relation to the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. Other studies of psychological separation from parents deal more narrowly with variables such as autonomy, ability to take responsibility for oneself, individuation, or positive separation feelings. Yaffe (1997), for example, used a variable, the Enabling Independence subscale from the Late Adolescents’ Relationships with Parents Scale (LARP; Flanagan, Schulenberg, & Fuligni, 1993), that contains items apparently intended to tap sense of autonomy from parents or ability to take responsibility for oneself. However, it also contains items that assess the student’s perception of his/her parents’ characteristics, in this instance their fostering of autonomy, which confounds person and environment variables. Furthermore, Enabling Independence is seen as part of a larger construct in which individuation is conceived within the context of continued connection to the parent. In any event, modest positive correlations were found by Yaffe between Enabling Independence and all SACQ variables. Hunsberger et al. (1996 and Hunsberger, 2000) used what they described as a “slightly abbreviated” 20-item version of the LARP – i.e., not just the Enabling Independence subscale -- and found modest correlations between its total score and all SACQ variables except social adjustment. The Individuation score from the Separation Anxiety Test (Hansburg, 1980) has statistically significant but modest positive correlations with all SACQ variables (Lapsley, 1989; Rice et al., 1990), but Wang and Smith (1993) found no relation between the Individuation subscale of the Identity Vis-a-vis Mother Questionnaire (IVM-20; Crastnopol, 1980) and any of 99 the SACQ indices. By factor analyzing the Emotional Autonomy Scale (Steinberg & Silverberg, 1986), Beyers (2001) and Beyers and Goossens (2002b, 2002c) derived a measure of emotional separation from parents, which they regarded as a normative or healthy developmental process, but found only a weak negative correlation with academic adjustment. Smith (1994) combined the Conflictual Independence subscale from the Psychological Separation Inventory (Hoffman, 1984) and the Separation Anxiety subscale from the SeparationIndividuation Test of Adolescence (Levine et al., 1986) to achieve a measure of positive feelings regarding separation from parents, and obtained a moderately strong correlation between it and the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable employed. (It should be mentioned that while the PSI Conflictual Independence subscale items contain specific reference to parents, the SITA Separation Anxiety subscale items refer to various kinds of other persons but not parents explicitly, thus mitigating somewhat the appropriateness of the new variable as a measure of separation from parents in particular.) Lapsley and Edgerton (1999a; see also Edgerton, 1997) employed the Pathological Separation-Individuation Inventory (PSII; Christenson & Wilson, 1985), which assesses adult behavioral characteristics presumed to result from disturbances in the separation-individuation process, and found strong negative correlations with the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales, the only two SACQ subscales used. Important to note, however, only five of the 39 PSII items contain direct allusions to parents or family. In a later study employing a shorter version of the PSII, Lapsley, Aalsma, and Varshney (2001) obtained a smaller negative correlation between that measure and a shortened, 8-item version of the Social Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable reported. Also addressing the issue of disturbances in the 100 separation-individuation process, Beyers (2001) and Beyers and Goossens (2002b, 2002c) used factor analysis of the Emotional Autonomy Scale (Steinberg & Silverberg, 1986) to identify a variable they called Detachment, which they defined as a negative pattern of disengagement from parents characterized by alienation and mistrust. That variable had modest negative correlations with all SACQ indices. Differentiation (a kind of separation?) from parents as related to adjustment to college was studied by Wick and Shilkret (1986a) using the Blatt Parental Representation Scale (Blatt, Wein, Chevron, & Quinlan, 1979), but the results are ambiguous and difficult to interpret. Caro (1985/1986) found no significant relations between indices from Blatt's instrument and the SACQ. The aforementioned studies in this section, while occasionally alluding to separation or independence from “family,” focus primarily on that aspect of relationship with parents in particular. Kalsner-Silver (2000), on the other hand, was interested in the relation to college adjustment of students’ separation or independence from family and family members in general. She used the Multigenerational Interconnectedness Scale (MIS; Gavazzim, Sabatelli, & ReeseWeber, 1999), which in its test items makes no reference to parents in particular, only to “family” and “family members.” The MIS is intended to measure three kinds of connectedness (or dependence upon or lack of separation from family): financial (monetary reliance); functional (regarding daily, everyday living), and psychological or emotional (for acceptance and approval of oneself and one’s behavior). The greater the connectedness, the lesser the individuation. Relatively low negative correlations were found by Kalsner-Silver (2000) between psychological/emotional connectedness and all SACQ subscales, strongest with personal- 101 emotional adjustment. No correlations were found for financial connectedness, and there were two weak positive correlations for functional connectedness with personal-emotional adjustment and institutional attachment These findings are somewhat similar to those obtained with the Psychological Separation Inventory (Hoffman, 1984) as described earlier, especially with respect to emotional independence/dependence. Parental Attachment Kenny (1990, 1993, 1994), concerned about excessive focus on separation from parents as a mark of maturation and a consequent underemphasis on the role of continued association with parents – i. e., parental attachment -- and transformations thereof, constructed the Parental Attachment Questionnaire (PAQ). The PAQ has subscales intended to measure three aspects of attachment, the first of which – Affective Quality of the Relationship -- is reminiscent of the relationship conflictedness/unconflictedness dimension tapped by the Conflictual Independence subscale of the Psychological Separation Inventory (PSI; Hoffman, 1984) and discussed in the previous section. Indeed, when the correlations for those two subscales with the five SACQ variables are averaged and compared, the resulting values are almost identical both in magnitude and pattern, the highest values occurring for personal-emotional adjustment. Thus, the PAQ’s Affective Quality of the Relationship variable quite regularly shows correlations in the expected positve direction with the several SACQ variables (Hutto, 1998; Vivona, 2000b), including for minority students (Kenny, 1993; Pfeil, 2000) and missionary children homecomers (Huff, 1998). Amin (2000) reports that high scoring students on that PAQ subscale have better academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment than low scoring students. Clauss (1995) obtained similar findings between that PAQ variable and the Social and 102 Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales of the SACQ, the only SACQ variables employed. Using the Affective Quality of Relationship variable from the PAQ as modified by Kenny and Perez (1996) for use with different cultural groups by substituting for parents in the instructions any family member to whom the student is most likely to turn for support, Kalsner-Silver (2000) obtained values very similar to those described above. The other two PAQ scales -- Parent Viewed as Fostering Autonomy; and Parent Viewed as Providing Emotional Support – seem less related to parental attachment as a characteristic of the individual and more related to the student’s perception of characteristics of his/her family, and therefore are considered later as environmental variables. This obviously is one of those instances where a distinction between person and environmental variable is very difficult to maintain. In any event, Kenny (1993) concluded from her findings (which included all three of the PAQ’s subscales) that "…. secure parental attachment and college adjustment go hand in hand." A number of studies have employed the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA; Armsden & Greenberg, 1987) in examining the relation between parental attachment and the SACQ. The IPPA-Parents has subscales addressing three aspects of the student/parent relationship – Communication, Trust, and Alienation – which can be scored separately or combined into an overall score (Communication plus Trust minus Alienation), for mother and father separately or combined. Harste (1996), administering the IPPA-Parents at the beginning of the freshman year and the SACQ at the end of the first semester, found correlations in the expected direction (negative for Alienation and positive for Communication, Trust, and the overall score), using combined 103 parents scoring, between all IPPA scores and all SACQ variables except for Alienation/Institutional Attachment, where the correlation was not significant. That is, in general, the greater the quality of communication and trust and the lesser the alienation between student and parents, the better the adjustment to college across all areas. Correlations were strongest for all three IPPA subscales with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale (magnitudes ranging from .26 to .42), and consistently more modest with the other three SACQ subscales (magnitudes ranging from .16 to .24). For the IPPA overall score its correlations were, of course, consistent with the foregoing data, accounting for about twice as much variance in personal-emotional adjustment as in the other adjustment areas. Pappas (2000) used a timing of test administration somewhat similar to Harste’s (1996), i.e., for the IPPA-Parents either shortly prior to or soon after the start of the freshman year, and the SACQ toward the end of the first semester. Also using ratings for parents combined, but only the overall IPPA score, Pappas obtained moderate correlations with all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment. Rice (1990/1991) administered the IPPA-Parents and the SACQ toward the beginning of the freshman year, and again to the same students two years later as juniors. In his data analysis Rice used the IPPA subscales and not the overall score, for both parents combined, and just the Academic, Social, and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales of the SACQ. He found in the freshman year testing significant correlations in the expected direction for all three IPPA subscales with academic adjustment, and for the IPPA Alienation subscale only with social and personal-emotional adjustment. For the junior year testing with the same sample in Rice’s study (1990/1991), there were 104 still significant correlations between the three IPPA subscales and academic adjustment to about the same degree as in the freshman year, but now there were also significant correlations between the IPPA Trust and Communication subscales and social and personal-emotional adjustment, with some of the values being quite strong. Interestingly, Rice found some weaker but still significant correlations in the expected direction between IPPA subscales administered in the freshman year and the SACQ variables as assessed in the junior year. Lapsley, Rice, & FitzGerald (1990) found, in freshmen, significant positive relation for the IPPA-measured overall parental attachment score -- for both parents combined -- on the Academic Adjustment subscale only, and, in a separate sample comprised of juniors and seniors, on all four SACQ subscales. But also with freshman students, Just (1998), using the overall IPPA score for both parents combined, obtained moderate correlations with all SACQ variables. Rice, FitzGerald, Whaley and Gibbs (1995), also using the IPPA overall parental attachment score for both parents combined, report that, for both freshman and upper class samples, students with secure attachment to parents have higher scores on academic and personal-emotional adjustment than students with insecure attachment, and, for the upper class sample, a corresponding effect was found for social adjustment as well (data for the SACQ Attachment subscale and full scale were not reported). Those same investigators also report that students identified as securely and insecurely attached in their freshman year, when juniors, two years later, still show differences in the expected direction in academic and personal-emotional adjustment (again no Attachment subscale or full scale data reported). Thus, again, this person characteristic measured in students’ first year of college still has consequences for adjustment after two years have elapsed. 105 Schultheiss and Blustein (1994), using a sample comprised of all four college year levels, and looking at IPPA-measured overall attachment to the mother and father separately for male and female students, got significant positive correlations of roughly comparable magnitude fairly consistently with personal-emotional and social adjustment, and less consistently with academic adjustment (data for the SACQ Attachment subscale and full scale were not reported). Also using the overall parental attachment score for the parents separately, Silver (1995) found in a freshman sample that attachment to the father was correlated with all SACQ variables, and attachment to the mother with academic, personal-emotional, and overall adjustment. Levin (1996), with a sample of freshman women, found a weak but significant correlation between the IPPA overall score for attachment to mother and the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable reported (attachment to father was not examined). Dewitt-Parker (2000) employed the overall IPPA score for attachment to the mother and father separately in black male and female students separately, reporting data only for the SACQ Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and full scale. She found strong positive correlations with academic adjustment for both kinds of students in relation to both the father and mother; more moderate positive correlations with the full scale score for male students in relation to the mother; and no association with personal-emotional adjustment. Using the Relationship Questionnaire, a revised version of the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment, Rice and Whaley (1994) tested their subjects at three different times varying in presumed stress during the second semester (the 3rd, 9th and 15th weeks), the third time of testing assumed to be the most stressful because it immediately preceded final examinations. They found that, for women, attachment to mother was consistently and about equally related to 106 academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment (not reporting data for the SACQ Attachment subscale or full scale) across all three testings; and women’s attachment to both parents was about equally predictive of academic and personal-emotional adjustment; but, while attachment to the mother predicted social adjustment, attachment to the father did not. For men, attachment to mother did not predict any of the three SACQ subscales employed on any of the three testings; and attachment to father was correlated only (though strongly so) with academic and personal-emotional adjustment, on the third and presumably most stressful testing time only, strongest for personal-emotional adjustment. Vivona (2000a) employed patterns of scores among the three IPPA-Parent subscales (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987) to identify three student/parent attachment styles corresponding to the infant/parent attachment styles described by Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall (1978), i.e., Secure, Ambivalent, and Avoidant (see the following chapter for definition of, and discussion of further findings concerning, these attachment styles). Students with Secure style were found to have better academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment (the only SACQ variables reported) than students with Insecure style (i. e., Ambivalent or Avoidant). However, Vivona’s (2000a) further analysis of her data by gender revealed two interesting findings. First, the Secure/Insecure differences described above were determined almost entirely by women students, the men showing no statistically significant difference among the styles. For the women members of the sample, there were statistically significant Secure/Insecure differences in all three adjustment areas measured. Second, while in the initial analysis with the sample as a whole there were no differences between the two insecure styles on any of the three SACQ variables employed, in the subsequent analysis the pattern across all three 107 adjustment areas was for Secure women students to have the highest SACQ scores, Avoidant to have the lowest scores, with Ambivalent intermediate, but only on personal-emotional adjustment was there a significant difference between Ambivalent and Avoidant women students, the former having the higher scores. Also concerned about understanding the maturational process in terms of continued association with parents (“connectedness,” in his terminology, rather than “attachment”), as well as separation from them, Baik (1997) constructed an instrument to measure both components, i.e., the Late Adolescent Individuation Questionnaire (LAIQ). Baik then identified four categories of students based on variations in separateness and connectedness as measured by the LAIQ: individuated (high on both components); pseudoautonomous (high separateness, low connectedness); dependent, or enmeshed (low separateness, high connectedness); and ambiguous (low on both components). Individuated students were found to have significantly higher scores than dependent students on all SACQ subscales except social adjustment; higher than pseudoautonomous students on all except personal-emotional adjustment; and higher than ambiguous students on all subscales. Dependent and pseudoautonomous students were rather alike in adjustment except that the former were higher in social adjustment; and the dependent students had higher scores than the ambiguous students on all SACQ subscales. Finally, pseudoautonomous students had higher scores than ambiguous students on all SACQ subscales except social adjustment. Thus, Baik’s (1997) expectations regarding adjustment differences among the four categories of students, arrayed from greater to lesser maturity, were largely corroborated, dramatically so in terms of magnitude of SACQ score differences between the two extreme 108 groups (i.e., individuated versus ambiguous). Worth noting, however, is that while all ten items in the measure of connectedness make explicit reference to relationship with parents, only three of the nine separateness items do so, raising a question as to whether the latter can be regarded as providing a maximally suitable index of separateness from parents. Other Aspects of Relationship with Parents Reciprocity in relationship with parents. Wintre and Sugar (2000; see also Sugar, 1997, and Sugar, 1999) used a measure of students’ perceived reciprocity in current relations with parents, the Perception of Parental Reciprocity Scale (POPRS; Wintre, Yaffe, & Crowley, 1995), which yields scores regarding relationship with the mother, the father, and “parents” unspecified. Reciprocity in the parent-child relationship is seen as a more mature state of affairs than the asymmetry that characterizes earlier such relationships, and is itself characterized by the parent and child treating each other as relative equals, with open and honest communication and mutual respect. Thus, this definition of reciprocity sounds very much as though it could encompass the constructs of communication and trust that are measured by the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987). The POPRS was administered in the first week of the freshman year and again in February/March of the second semester, with the SACQ administered at the latter time. Of the 24 correlations for the three fall POPRS scores (for the mother, father, and “parents”) with the four SACQ subscales, for men and women separately, all but two were significant and positive. Corresponding findings for the POPRS administered in the second semester showed all but one of the 24 correlations to be significant. The magnitude of POPRS/SACQ correlations was roughly the same for both POPRS administrations, suggesting stability over time for perceived 109 parental reciprocity. Yaffe (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) also used the POPRS and SACQ similarly administered, the former in the beginning of the academic year and again in the middle of the second semester, and the latter at the second testing occasion. The Wintre and Sugar (2000) sample (n = 357), findings for which were discussed above, was contained in the Yaffe sample (close to 400 students), with the following differences for Yaffe in data analysis. Instead of three separate scores from the POPRS (i. e., for mother, father, and “parents”) Yaffe used a total score only; instead of POPRS data from two separate testings, he used an average of the two testings because of absence of significant differences between scores from the two testings; and he combined male and female students into one sample. He obtained significant positive correlations with all SACQ variables, in approximately the same magnitude as those reported by Wintre and Sugar (2000) and Sugar (1999). Shilkret and Taylor (1992) developed the Mother and Father Questionnaire to assess compatibility of the student-parent relationship, which would seem to be closely related to the parental reciprocity and attachment variables discussed here, and obtained significant and sometimes substantial correlations in the expected direction with all SACQ variables. Affective tone of the student/parent relationship. The reader will already have seen that some measures of both psychological separation from, and attachment to, parents include subscales that assess conflictedness/amicability of the student/parent relationship. The reader may also recall the point made earlier that, in addition to regarding affective tone as possibly a central component of the definition of parental attachment, it could also be seen as a variable standing by itself as a determinant of college adjustment. It may be worthwhile, therefore, to 110 consider briefly what empirical foundation there is in SACQ-using research for such a variable. The similarity in predicting SACQ variables between the Conflictual Independence subscale of the Psychological Separation Inventory (Hoffman, 1984) and the Affective Quality of the Relationship subscale from the Parental Attachment Questionnaire (Kenny, 1990) has already been discussed. The reader very likely noted in the presentation of data from the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment-Parents that that instrument contains a subscale assessing alienation from parents, certainly also reflecting affective tone. While there is not as much correlational data available concerning the IPPA-Alientation/SACQ relation as for those other two measures, averaging what is available reveals essential similarity, higher correlation again seen with personal-emotional adjustment. Yet another instrument used to examine student/parent relationship as a determinant of college adjustment is the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence (SITA; Levine, Green, & Millon, 1986; Levine & Saintonge, 1993). And the SITA includes two subscales that bear on the issue of affective tone of the relationship. The Engulfment Anxiety subscale, comprised of items pertaining to resentment of parental control, had low negative correlations with academic adjustment for Cooler (1995) and Lapsley (1989)/Rice et al. (1990), a somewhat stronger negative correlation with personalemotional adjustment for Cooler, and a low negative correlation with the SACQ full scale for Lapsley/Rice et al. The Nurturance Seeking subscale contains items reflecting positive feelings of closeness with and dependency on the parents, a kind of welcome lack of separation, and that variable correlated positively but weakly with academic adjustment for Lapsley/Rice et al. and negatively with social and personal-emotional adjustment for Cooler. Inclusion of the quality of 111 dependency in the nurturance seeking variable may make it different from the other affective tone variables considered here, and may account for the negative correlations with SACQ variables. There are other SITA subscales that are intended to deal with separation-individuation issues, but for interpersonal relationships in general and not directly for student/parent relationship. Findings for those subscales will be considered later where appropriate. With a sample of Arab-American students, Amin (2000) devised a measure of similarity/discrepancy between students’ self-reported acculturation orientation and that of their parents as perceived by the students, which she interpreted as an indication of conflictedness in the parent-student relationship. She found that students reporting a different accultural orientation than their parents had poorer academic and social adjustment than students having a similar orientation. Perceived Relationship with Grandparents. One investigator (Erickson, 1996) was interested in the relation between students’ perception of the satisfactoriness of their relationship with their grandparents, as measured by the Grandparent Strengths and Needs Inventory-Grandchild Form (GSNI; Strom & Strom, 1989), and their adjustment in the first year of college. Of 30 correlations between the six GSNI subscales and five SACQ variables, only one was statistically significant, fewer than would be expected by chance. As will be seen in a subsequent chapter concerning environmental determinants of adjustment to college, Erickson did find more evidence of relation between the grandparents’ assessment of their grandparental role and their grandchild’s adjustment in college. 112 CHAPTER 9 PERSON CHARACTERISTICS AS DETERMINANTS OF COLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: SOCIAL RELATIONS IN GENERAL We move now from consideration of a particular kind of social relations, i. e., with parents, to social relations in general. Social Propensity, Extraversion, and Related Variables. The Social Propensity Scale, a measure of social interests and skills developed by Baker and Siryk (1983), administered prematriculation, predicted social adjustment – the only SACQ variable reported -- in both semesters of the freshman year as measured by an earlier version of the SACQ (Baker & Siryk). Similar findings were obtained with a later freshman class using the same measure of social propensity administered prematriculation but the current version of the SACQ (Baker & Siryk, 1989). Sullivan (1991), also administering the same scale prematriculation, found comparable correlation with social adjustment – still the only SACQ variable reported --from a first semester SACQ administration. McGowan (1988) reported a much stronger correlation between the Social Propensity Scale administered in the fourth week of the first semester and the Social Adjustment subscale of the SACQ administered in the eleventh week, and also lesser but still significant correlations with the other SACQ indices. The Social Competence Scale, developed originally by Levenson and Gottman (1978) and later modified and expanded by Cohen, Sherrod, and Clark (1986), yields measures for three aspects of social competence (interpersonal assertiveness, dating skills, and social skills with same-sex others), plus a full scale score. Birnie-Lefcovitch (1997) administered that instrument to entering freshmen in July before the start of the academic year and again at the end of the first 113 semester, when the SACQ was also administered. Of 40 correlations between the four Social Competence Scale variables and five SACQ indices, all were in the positive direction and all but one were statistically significant. Though not tested for significance of differences, the correlations between the social competence variables and the SACQ subscales always indicated higher association for the former with the social aspects of college adjustment, as would be expected; and the correlations for the postmatriculation administration of the Social Competence Scale with SACQ variables were always the same (in one instance) or higher than those for the prematriculation administration of the Social Competence Scale, as also would be expected. Jagels and Burger (1993) used a person variable probably closely related to social propensity, i.e., extraversion/introversion as measured by the Eysenck Personality Inventory (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968). They found that students identified in the second week of the freshman year as extraverts, in comparison with students identified as introverts, scored higher on the Social Adjustment subscale of the SACQ administered also in the second week and again in the fifth and eighth weeks. There were no differences between the groups on the Academic or Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales, the only other SACQ indices reported. The Extraversion variable from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, 1992) was found by Wintre and Sugar (2000; see also Sugar, 1997) to correlate, for both men and women, with all SACQ subscales except academic adjustment for males, strongest with social adjustment and weakest with academic adjustment. Extraversion as measured by the “Big Five” Inventory (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991) was found by Montgomery and Haemmerlie (2001) to correlate with all SACQ variables, again strongest with social adjustment and weakest with academic adjustment. The Sociability scale from the Hogan Personality 114 Inventory (Hogan & Hogan, 1995) is said to be comparable to the NEO-FFI Extraversion variable, and was found by Montgomery, Haemmerlie, and Watkins (2000; Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) to correlate with all SACQ indices, highest with social and personalemotional adjustment. A measure of extraversion/introversion (“Vector 1”) factor-analytically derived from the items of the California Psychological Inventory-Revised (CPI; Gough, 1987) was found by Haemmerlie and Merz (1991) not to be related to the full-scale SACQ, the only SACQ variable reported. Apparently defined in terms similar to social propensity and extraversion is the Flexible:Social subscale from the Personal Resilience Questionnaire (Organizational Development Resources, 1996), which Brunelle-Joiner (1999) found to be positively correlated with all SACQ variables, strongest with social adjustment. Brooks and DuBois (1995) derived a variable they called surgency (or extraversion)intellect by means of principal components analysis of data from three instruments -- the Problem Solving Inventory (Heppner & Petersen, 1982), the Goldberg Big-Five Factor Markers (Goldberg, 1992), and the global self-worth score of the Self-Perception Profile for College Students (Neemann & Harter, 1986) – and found it to correlate significantly with the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and full scale of the SACQ. The fairly strong relation with social adjustment and absence of significant relation with academic adjustment suggests that the surgency-intellect variable is more heavily weighted toward its extraversion component than its cognitive-intellective one. A variable named social resources (or social abilities?) identified by Terrell (1989) from 115 factor analysis of a number of instruments – the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965); the Adult EAS Temperament Survey (Buss & Plomin, 1984); the Spheres of Control Battery (Paulhus, Molin, & Schuchts, 1979; Paulhus & Christie, 1981); Perceived Social Support from Friends Scale (Procidano & Heller, 1983); and the Coping Response Indices Inventory (Billings & Moos, 1984) – correlated strongly with the SACQ Social Adjustment subscale, less strongly but still significantly with personal-emotional adjustment, and not at all with academic adjustment, the only SACQ variables examined. Seemingly the obverse of extraversion is shyness. Montgomery, Haemmerlie, Pyatt, and Laycock (2000; data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) used the Cheek and Buss Shyness Scale (Cheek, 1983; Cheek & Melchior, 1990) to measure that variable and found it to be negatively correlated with all SACQ variables. Variables Concerning Focus on Self or Outside the Self Some investigators have studied, as determinants of adjustment to college, individual differences in focus of one’s attention on the self or beyond the self, which would seem to be related in some important ways to introversion/extraversion and social propensity. Focusing attention on the other person rather than on the self in interpersonal situations, as measured by the Interpersonal Orientation Scale (Swap & Rubin, 1983), is associated with higher scores on the SACQ Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales, while focusing on the self is associated with higher scores on the Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales (Savino et al., 1986b). But a seemingly related variable called self-consciousness -defined in terms of directing one’s attention inward, on the self, rather than outward towards one’s environment -- as measured by a shortened form of the Self-Consciousness Scales 116 (Fenigstein, Scheier, & Buss, 1975), was found by Sutton (1996) to be associated with lower scores on a truncated, 10-item, version of the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable used. The variable Agreeableness from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, 1992), which would seem from its definition to include qualities related to focus on others (friendliness, altruism, capacity for empathy), was found by Wintre and Sugar (2000; see also Sugar, 1997) to correlate positively for both men and women with all SACQ subscales, as was true also for Napoli and Wortman (1998) with a mixed gender sample of community college students. The Agreeableness variable from the “Big Five” Inventory (John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991), as used by Montgomery and Haemmerlie (2001), also correlated with all SACQ variables. The Likeability scale from the Hogan Personality Inventory (Hogan & Hogan, 1995) is thought to be comparable with the NEO-FFI Agreeableness variable (friendly, warm, tactful), and was found by Montgomery, Haemmerlie, and Watkins (2000; data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) to correlate with all SACQ indices. Two subscales from the California Psychological Inventory-Revised (CPI; Gough, 1987) – Psychological Mindedness (attentive to and good judge of others’ thoughts and feelings) and Tolerance (sensitive to and tolerant of others’ beliefs and values) – would seem related to the concept of agreeableness, and Haemmerlie and Merz (1991) found that higher scores on both of those variables were associated with higher scores on the SACQ full-scale, the only SACQ variable reported. In a sense the opposite of agreeableness is the concept of psychological reactance, as measured by the Therapeutic Reactance Scale (Dowd, Yesenosky, Wallbrown, & Sanders, 1993), 117 which is characterized by an individual behaving in a way that is the opposite of what is wished by significant others, being less likely to follow social rules, and more likely to be careless about meeting obligations or to care about impressions one makes on others. Haemmerlie and Montgomery (1994) found no relation between that variable and any SACQ subscale. Possibly related to focus on self or others, but with a more sociological flavor, is the notion of individualistic (i. e., focus on self, self-reliance, independence) versus collectivistic (i.e., focus beyond the self, interdependence, subordination of self to family or other groups) orientations, where the former orientation is thought to be more characteristic of western European societies and the latter of eastern Asian societies. Kusaka (1995) used the Individualism-Collectivism Scale (Hui, 1988) with international students at four American west coast universities, some from five east Asian countries and others from ten west European countries. In both kinds of students, positive correlations were found between collectivistic orientation and the SACQ Attachment subscale, and for the east Asian students with the Social Adjustment subscale and full scale as well. There were no significant differences between the two kinds of students in degree of relation between collectivistic orientation and adjustment to college. Choi (1999, 2000) in her study of Korean-American students employed the Horizontal and Vertical Individualism and Collectivism Scale (HVICS; Singelis, Triandis, Bhawuk, & Gelfand, 1995), which expands the definition of individualism and collectivism to each include two additional dimensions. The Horizontal Individualism (HI) and Vertical Individualism (VI) cultural orientations both involve independence and self-reliance, but HI stresses equality of status among individuals, a kind of benign coexistence, while VI evidences competitiveness and 118 desire to excel in relation to others. Horizontal Collectivism (HC) and Vertical Collectivism (VC) both entail interdependence and identification of one’s interests with those of one’s group(s), but HC emphasizes equality among group members while VC allows for hierarchical, unequal status of members (e. g., parent and child). The only significant correlation found by Choi (1999, 2000) for the individualistic cultural orientation was a weak negative value between Vertical Individualism and personalemotional adjustment. For the collectivistic orientation there were weak positive correlations between the vertical dimension and social and overall adjustment, but moderate to stronger values between Horizontal Collectivism and all SACQ variables, especially for social adjustment and institutional attachment as well as overall adjustment. Looking at individualism and collectivism irrespective of the horizontal/vertical dimensions, Choi (2000) found no correlation between the former cultural orientation and the SACQ full scale score (the only SACQ variable employed in this particular analysis), but moderately sized positive relation for the latter orientation. Interpersonal attachment. Several studies have examined as an aspect of “social relations in general” the establishment of interpersonal relationships, and capacity for doing same, in relation to adjustment to college. Harste (1996) employed the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment-Peers (IPPA; Armsden & Greenberg, 1987) to assess students’ attachment to college peers, reporting data for its three subscales – Alienation, Communication, and Trust – and the overall score. She found significant correlations in the expected direction (negative for Alienation and positive for the other indices) for all four IPPA variables with all SACQ variables except for the non-significant 119 Communication/Personal-Emotional Adjustment comparison. That is, the greater the communication and trust and the lesser the alienation between the student and college peers, the better the adjustment to college. The IPPA subscale correlations with the SACQ Social Adjustment and Institutional Attachment subscales were consistently strong (magnitude ranging from .30 to .43), but with the Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales more variable (from non-significant to .47) and more generally lower (mdn. r = .20). These findings are, of course, reflected in the correlations between the IPPA-Peers overall score and the SACQ variables, that score accounting for four or more times the variance in the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales as in the other two SACQ subscales. Other investigators have employed the IPPA to examine the relation between attachment to peers in general – i. e., not just to those at college – and adjustment to college. Just (1998) found correlations for the overall measure of attachment to peers with all SACQ indices, strongest with social adjustment, as did Lapsley et al. (1990) with the SACQ Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales. The Harste (1996) and Just (1998) studies cited above both report correlational data for both parent and peer attachment (see previous chapter for discussion of findings regarding parental attachment), and thus permit some parenthetical conjecturing here concerning the relative importance of the two kinds of attachment for the different areas of adjustment to college. For simplicity’s sake using only the IPPA overall score in this comparison, it appears that parental attachment may be the more important predictor of academic and personalemotional adjustment, and peer attachment the more important predictor of social adjustment and institutional attachment. 120 As would be expected given the data already cited, the IPPA Combined Parent/Peer Overall Attachment Score for Harste (1996) significantly predicts all college adjustment areas, academic adjustment lesser than the other areas. Capacity for forming social relations, as measured by the Developing Mature Interpersonal Relationships Task subscale of the Student Developmental Task and Lifestyle Inventory (Winston et al., 1987), is correlated in the expected direction for both men and women with academic and personal-emotional adjustment, and for women but oddly not for men with social adjustment (data for the SACQ Attachment subscale were not reported; Schultheiss & Blustein, 1994). The Intimacy subscale from the Inventory of Psychosocial Development (IPD; Constantinople, 1969), which is intended to measure degree of openness and comfort in the formation of interpersonal relationships, was found by Vivona (2000b) to be positively correlated with all SACQ indices except academic adjustment. The Isolation subscale from the IPD, which taps lack of involvement in relationships with others, and presumably difficulty in forming such relationships, was negatively correlated with all SACQ variables, and more strongly than in the case of the Intimacy subscale (Vivona). Fackelman and Shilkret (1994) used the Social Cognition and Object Relations Scale (SCORS; Westen, Lohr, Silk, & Goodrich, 1990) in their study of psychoanalytically conceived personality variables involved in forming and maintaining interpersonal relationships as determinants of adjustment in college. Students who see interpersonal relationships as associated with positive affects and with expectations of positive outcomes (as measured by the Affect Tone subscale), and who have capacity for emotional investment in such relationships (as measured by the Capacity for Emotional Investment subscale), have better social adjustment as measured by 121 the SACQ. Students capable of emotional investment in relationships also showed greater institutional attachment. No relation was found between SACQ variables and either complexity of representations of people or understanding of causal factors in interpersonal relationships, two other variables measured by the SCORS. Also, no relation was found by Fackelman and Shilkret between the SACQ and the Personal Sphere Model (Schmiedeck, 1974), another instrument intended to measure object relations. The Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence (SITA; Levine, Green, & Millon, 1986) has several subscales relevant to establishment of interpersonal relationships, for which several investigators (Cooler, 1995; Lapsley, 1989; Rice, Cole, & Lapsley, 1990; Wang & Smith, 1993) have reported findings. The Peer Enmeshment subscale concerns especially the formation of close relationships with age-mates, and for Cooler it correlated positively with all SACQ subscales, strongest with social adjustment, but Lapsley found no significant correlations for that variable. For the Teacher Enmeshment subscale, pertaining to close relationships formed with teachers, Cooler obtained positive correlations with the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales but a negative correlation with personal-emotional adjustment, while again Lapsley found no relation between that SITA variable and SACQ indices. The SITA Healthy Separation subscale is intended to reflect a favorable resolution of the developmental separationindividuation process resulting in a capacity to engage in and enjoy interpersonal relationships but at the same time be comfortable apart from such relationships when appropriate. Cooler found positive correlations between that variable and all SACQ subscales, strongest with social adjustment, and Wang and Smith obtained such relation only for the Social Adjustment subscale, while Rice et al. and Lapsley again got no significant correlations. The SITA Dependency Denial 122 subscale taps a kind of hostility-tinged detachment from and rejection of interpersonal relationships, for which Cooler reports moderately strong negative correlations with all SACQ subscales while Lapsley again had no significant relations. Alienation The concept of alienation (from parents or peers) has already been mentioned above as a component of the overall attachment score from the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987), and would seem to be close in meaning to the dependency denial variable from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence (Levine, Green, & Millon, 1986). Thus, alienation would seem to represent a failure of attachment, or disordered attachment, and merits separate consideration. The Alienation Scale (Baker & Siryk, 1980) – in which alienation was defined in terms of compatibility or fit (or lack of it) with one's environment, including social environment -administered prematriculation predicted the full scale score (the only variable reported) from an earlier version of the SACQ in both semesters of the freshman year (Baker & Siryk). Two other studies (Liter, 1987; McGowan, 1988) employed the same Alienation Scale, but administered postmatriculation, and obtained moderate to strong negative correlations with all indices of the later, published version of the SACQ. In a study of reverse culture shock in American missionaries’ children returning from residence in foreign countries to attend college in the United States, Huff (1998) used the Homecomer Culture Shock Scale (HCSS; Fray, 1988), which has two subscales that may be viewed as measuring variations of alienation. One, Interpersonal Distance, assesses felt distance from other persons in the now new and “strange” American environment; and the other subscale, 123 Cultural Distance, taps felt distance from American cultural beliefs, values, and customs in general. Both subscales correlated in the expected negative direction with social and personalemotional adjustment to college, and the former subscale with the SACQ full scale score as well. A variable somewhat akin to alienation, pseudoautonomy (from the Pseudoautonomy Scale; Lapan & Patton, 1986), is defined in terms of pathological independence from others and rebellious non-conformity. It was found by Dewein (1994) to correlate negatively with all SACQ subscales except, oddly, social adjustment. Three subscales from the California Psychological Inventory-Revised (CPI; Gough, 1987) that have been found by Haemmerlie and Merz (1991) to be related to the SACQ may be regarded as measuring variables that are the obverse of alienation: Communality (fitting in easily with one’s environment); Socialization (conforming readily to ordinary rules and regulations); and Good Impression (wanting to do what will please others). Higher scores on all three variables were associated with higher scores on the SACQ full-scale, the only SACQ variable reported. Dependence/Independence Excessive dependence. In some sense the opposite of variables like alienation, pseudoautonomy, or detachment from others, but still expected to be negatively related to adjustment to college because it represents another disturbance in relationship formation, is inordinate dependency on other persons. Both Caro (1985/1986) and Polewchak (1998, 1999) found significant negative correlations with all SACQ variables for the Emotional Reliance on Other Persons subscale from the Interpersonal Dependency Inventory (IDI; Hirschfeld et al., 1977), highest with personal- 124 emotional adjustment. However, when Polewchak divided her sample by gender, there was no significant relation for males between that IDI variable and either academic adjustment or institutional attachment. For another IDI subscale, Assertion of Autonomy, ostensibly the obverse of dependency, Caro (1985/1986) found no significant correlation with any SACQ variable, and Polewchak (1998, 1999) obtained a modest negative correlation with the SACQ full scale only for her female sample. Using the IDI total score, Polewchak found significant negative correlations with all SACQ variables for her total sample and female students separately, but the correlations for males reached significance only in the case of personal-emotional and overall adjustment. Dewein (1994), using another measure of excessive dependency on others, the Peer Group Dependence Scale (Lapan & Patton, 1986), obtained negative correlations with all SACQ subscales in roughly equal magnitude. Independence as self-sufficiency or self-reliance. Presumably somewhere between excessive dependency on others, on the one hand, and pseudoautonomy or alienation or detachment from others, on the other hand, would be a personally and socially advantageous autonomy in the form of self-sufficiency or self-reliance that would not preclude capacity for interdependence with others when necessary, desired, or otherwise appropriate. Yaffe (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) used the “individual adequacy” scales (SelfReliance, Identity, and Work Orientation) from the Psychosocial Maturity Inventory (Greenberger, Josselson, Knerr, & Knerr, 1975) individually and in a composite score as indices of autonomy (defined as the capacity to function effectively on one’s own). He found positive correlations, some moderately strong, between all those indices and all SACQ variables. 125 Vivona (2000b) employed the Autonomy Scale (AS; Bekker, 1993), which was designed to disconfound more adaptive independence and less adaptive detachment in interpersonal relationships, principally by inclusion of a subscale measuring capacity for initiation and maintenance of close relationships. That instrument has three subscales intended to address three aspects of healthy independence: Self-Awareness (of one’s personal desires, beliefs, and preferences); Sensitivity to Others (and to their needs and wishes); and Capacity for Managing New Situations (with openness and flexibility). Self-Awareness and Capacity to Manage New Situations correlated in the expected positive direction with all SACQ indices except institutional attachment for the former AS variable and academic adjustment for the latter, where there was no significant correlation. Sensitivity to Others showed only a weak negative correlation with personal-emotional adjustment. Examination of the Sensitivity to Others subscale’s items (see Bekker, 1993) suggests that what is being measured there is less a capacity for initiation and maintenance of close relationships than an anxious overattentiveness to and concern regarding others’ feelings, attitudes, and thoughts, especially regarding the self (implying excessive dependency on others?). The Self-Other Differentiation Scale was designed by Olver, Aries, and Batgos (1989) to measure one’s experience of a separate sense of self, which may be seen as another aspect of the concept of autonomy. Using that scale, Morray and Shilkret (2001) found moderate positive correlations with all SACQ variables. Adult Attachment Styles Closely connected with the foregoing relationship-establishment findings, especially where the nature of such relationships is thought to be determined in some degree by the quality 126 of early parent-infant experience, are studies concerning the relation between adult attachment styles and adjustment to college. It will be seen that those styles pull together in an organized way many of the concepts already considered more or less separately in the several prior sections of this chapter. Kulley (1994) used the Adult Attachment Types Questionnaire (Hazan & Shaver, 1987) and the Adult Attachment Scale (Collins & Read, 1990), especially the latter, to identify three types of attachment to significant others that had theoretical and empirical links to infant-parent attachment styles described by Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall (1978). The secure attachment style in the adult is characterized by an ability: to establish reciprocally close and interdependent relationships, while at the same time maintaining separate identity and capacity for independent action; to seek and make use of social support when needed; to deal with threats of relationship disruption without undue emotional distress; and to be effective in dealing with environmental demands. The anxious-ambivalent adult attachment style is characterized by: inordinate need to establish and maintain close relationships, and by preoccupation with the attachment figure, with the consequence of less than adequate capacity to attend to and deal with environmental demands; by need to fuse one’s identity with the attachment figure; and, most importantly, by difficulty in regulating negative emotions associated with threats of relationship disruption. The avoidant adult attachment style is characterized by relative lack of capacity for or interest in forming close relationships, with consequent detachment and isolation from social involvement and preoccupation instead with noninterpersonal activity; compulsive independence; and marked emotional inhibition, especially 127 regarding interpersonal matters. The latter two categories are regarded as “insecure” attachment styles. Kulley (1994) found that students identified as having secure attachment style had higher scores on all SACQ variables than students categorized as either anxious-ambivalent or avoidant. Kulley also made predictions as to differences between the two insecure attachment styles for particular SACQ subscales, some of which were confirmed and some not. Anxious-ambivalent students, for example, did show poorer academic adjustment than avoidant students, presumably because of the former’s preoccupation with gratification of attachment needs that could interfere with academic effort, and the latter’s inhibition of such needs in favor of involvement with nonsocial activities, like academic work. An expected difference not found was better social adjustment for anxious-ambivalent than for avoidant students. Klynn (1997) employed the Attachment Style Questionnaire (Feeney, Noller, & Hanrahan, 1994) to measure the same three attachment variables described above (secure, anxious-ambivalent, and avoidant), but as dimensions rather than types. That instrument and the SACQ were each administered twice, in September (third week) and November. On both testing occasions positive correlations were found between secure attachment and all SACQ variables, and negative correlations for anxious-ambivalent and avoidant attachment with all SACQ variables. Correlations ranged from modest to robust and tended to be stronger at the second testing. Shilkret (2000) reports data from a sizeable sample to which both the Adult Attachment Types Questionnaire (Hazan & Shaver, 1987) and Attachment Style Questionnaire (Feeney, Noller, & Hanrahan, 1994) were administered. The former instrument was modified by having 128 students rate on a seven point scale the degree to which a description of each type applied to them. Both means of identifying secure attachment correlated positively with all SACQ variables, but approximately two to three times stronger in magnitude for the Attachment Style Questionnaire. Both avoidant and anxious-ambivalent attachment as measured by the two instruments correlated negatively with all SACQ variables, the Attachment Style Questionnaire again yielding substantially stronger values. Bartels (1990), like Kulley (1994) cited above, also employed Hazan and Shaver’s (1987) Adult Attachment Types Questionnaire and Collins and Read’s (1990) Adult Attachment Scale, again especially the latter. However, instead of assigning students to one of three attachment style categories, as did Kulley (1994), she used “dimension scores” developed by Collins and Read to represent psychological characteristics underlying adult attachment styles. The characteristics “Close” (degree of comfort with closeness and intimacy) and “Depend” (capacity to trust others and depend on them to be available when needed) were found by Bartels to correlate positively with the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable employed, while the characteristic “Anxiety” (amount of anxiety in relationships, such as fear of abandonment and not being loved) correlated negatively with that SACQ variable. Lapsley and Edgerton (1999a; see also Edgerton, 1997) and Pfeil (2000) used a means of conceptualizing adult attachment styles somewhat similar to that employed by Kulley (1994), as presented above, but with four instead of three categories identified by the Close Relationships Questionnaire, also referred to as the Relationships Questionnaire (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Griffen & Bartholomew, 1994). Additional to a secure style was: a preoccupied style (characterized by need for and preoccupation with excessive closeness in personal relationships, 129 and distress when such need is not met), roughly equivalent to the anxious-ambivalent style described above; a dismissing style (characterized by a lack of interest in and avoidance of close relationships, and stressing the importance of independence from others), similar to the avoidant style seen above; and, as a new category, a fearful style (also characterized by avoidance of close relationships but here not because of lack of interest in such relationships but because of fear of rejection by others). In one mode of analysis (Edgerton, 1997; Pfeil, 2000), students were asked to identify themselves with one or another of four descriptions of attachment style. Edgerton found that those self-identified as secure and those as dismissing had better personal-emotional adjustment than those self-identified as fearful or preoccupied, but there were no differences among the four groups in social adjustment, the only other SACQ variable employed. For Pfeil, using all SACQ variables, secure students had higher scores on all SACQ variables than preoccupied students. Dismissing students had higher personal-emotional and overall adjustment scores than preoccupied; and fearful were higher than preoccupied on all SACQ variables except academic adjustment. Also using the Close Relationships Questionnaire (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), Howard, Morey, and Briancesco (2001) found that students categorized as Fearful had lower personal-emotional adjustment scores than students falling in any of the other adult attachment styles, and lower scores on academic adjustment than students categorized as Dismissing. In another mode of analysis (Lapsley and Edgerton, 1999a; see also Edgerton, 1997; Pfeil, 2000), students were asked to assign ratings of the degree to which each of the four category descriptions applied to them. Higher ratings on secure attachment were found by 130 Lapsley and Edgerton to correlate positively with both the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales, again the only two SACQ variables employed, and by Pfeil, who used all SACQ variables, with academic and social adjustment and institutional attachment as well as overall adjustment. For Lapsley and Edgerton, ratings regarding the fearful and preoccupied styles correlated negatively with both of the SACQ subscales employed; for Pfeil, preoccupied ratings correlated negatively with all SACQ variables, and fearful so correlated with academic, personal-emotional, and overall adjustment. For both Lapsley and Edgerton and for Pfeil ratings for the dismissing style did not correlate with any SACQ subscale. In a later study that employed a shortened, 8-item version of the SACQ’s Social Adjustment subscale, Lapsley et al. (2001) obtained a positive correlation of approximately the same magnitude as earlier with secure attachment, a smaller negative value than earlier with fearful attachment, and no correlation with either preoccupied or dismissing attachment. In an apparently productive attempt to identify and measure the essential variables or dimensions underlying differences in adult attachment, one that would focus more closely on characteristics in which individuals would differ instead of on types of persons, Brennan, Clark, and Shaver (1998) performed factor analyses using a very large pool of items taken from a number of instruments that had been developed by other investigators for measuring adult attachment. The resulting Experiences in Close Relationships Questionnaire consists of two scales: Anxiety (which correlates highly with other scales measuring anxiety regarding and preoccupation with attachment, jealousy, and fear of rejection), and Avoidance (which correlates highly with other scales measuring avoidance of close relationships and discomfort with closeness). Using that new instrument, Kalsner-Silver (2000) found negative correlations with all 131 SACQ subscales, stronger for the Anxiety scale than the Avoidance scale. Secure attachment in Brennan et al.’s way of thinking is indicated by low scores on both the Anxiety and Avoidance Scales. Simpson (1990) and Simpson, Rholes, and Nelligan (1992) also employed factor analysis to identify, and develop measures for, continuous variables underlying differences in adult attachment. They took the thirteen sentences from the three paragraphs in Hazan and Shaver’s (1987) Adult Attachment Types Questionnaire that described three different attachment styles, and used them as separate items in a rating scale. Factor analysis of those thirteen items resulted in two scales, named by Simpson et al. as the “avoidant/secure attachment index” and the “anxious-nonanxious attachment index.” On the former, double-edged, variable, high scores indicate avoidance attachment and lower scores indicate secure attachment. Lapsley et al. (2001) used the two Simpson indexes with a shortened, 8-item version of the SACQ Social Adjustment subscale and found a moderately strong negative correlation with the avoidant/secure index (-.41, p<.01; students showing greater avoidance attachment having poorer social adjustment) and a somewhat weaker negative value (-.30, p<.01) for the anxious attachment index. Yet a third attempt to identify continuous dimensions underlying attachment styles defined as categories was made by Griffen and Bartholomew (1994), who employed the Relationships Questionnaire (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Griffen & Bartholomew) to develop means of measuring two variables: “self model,” or positivity of self-image, and “other model,” or positivity of image of others. The former variable, which correlates well with measures of self-esteem (see Griffen & Bartholomew), was found by Pfeil (2000) to correlate moderately to moderately strongly with all SACQ indices. The latter variable, which is regarded 132 as similar to sociability, oddly shows no correlation with any SACQ variable. Interestingly, Griffen and Bartholomew recommend that researchers not abandon use of the categorical attachment patterns in favor of these new dimensional measures; indeed, they call for the development of improved means of assessing attachment patterns. Lapsley et al. (2000) used still another means of studying adult attachment characteristics, this one developed by West and Sheldon (1988) and ostensibly focusing on pathological aspects of attachment. Like the ways of conceptualizing and assessing attachment characteristics already discussed in this section, which themselves seem to consider pathology of attachment to no lesser extent, West and Sheldon’s was based essentially on the work of Bowlby (1969, 1973, 1977) and Ainsworth and Bell (1970). Four aspects of attachment were identified: compulsive self-reliance (cf. avoidant behavior as defined earlier); compulsive care-giving (a variable not seemingly represented in the other Bowlby/Ainsworth concepts already considered); compulsive careseeking (cf. the dependency aspect of the anxious/ambivalent kind of attachment discussed above); and angry withdrawal (possibly a mixture of the anxious-ambivalence and avoidance characteristics discussed above). Employing a shortened, 8-item version, of the SACQ’s Social Adjustment subscale, Lapsley et al. (2001) found weak to modest negative correlations for that variable with compulsive self-reliance, compulsive care-seeking, and angry withdrawal (the three West and Sheldon variables that seem related to the more commonly encountered attachment characteristics seen earlier in this section), but no relation with compulsive care-giving.. (add brief summary for adult attachment styles, like: “Thus, several different investigators, using several different means of measuring adult attachment, including attachment differences conceptualized both as types of persons and characteristics of 133 persons, and identifying different number of types or traits assessed, all rather consistently find etc.”) Socially-defined Feeling States Several investigators have studied, as determinants of adjustment to college, affective or feeling states that are defined in terms of social relations. Social anxiety, as measured by the Social Avoidance and Distress Scale (Watson & Friend, 1969), in one study showed significant negative correlations with all SACQ variables, highest with social adjustment (Caro, 1986), and in another study showed the same pattern of significant correlational values with all SACQ variables except the Attachment subscale, for which data were not reported (Kim et al., 1992). Need for social approval as measured by the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (Watson & Friend), very likely an aspect of social anxiety, correlates modestly in the expected direction with the SACQ Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale and full-scale (Wang & Smith, 1993). Harris (1988), Caro (1986), Pratt (2001), and Beyers and Goossens (2002a) report for freshmen quite similar pattern and magnitude of correlations, some quite robust, between loneliness as measured by the Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (Russell, Peplau, & Cutrona, 1980) and the SACQ, the strongest correlations occurring with social adjustment and weakest usually with academic adjustment. Harris (1991) subsequently obtained a similar pattern and magnitude of relations between the two instruments for college seniors. Harris (1991) also found that the loneliness measure administered in the second semester of the freshman year correlated significantly and in the expected direction with the SACQ administered three years later in the senior year, the strongest correlations still occurring with the Social Adjustment subscale. 134 Montgomery and Haemmerlie (2001), with the same instrument administered to a sample of unspecified college year level, obtained expected direction negative correlations with all SACQ variables. Finally, with regard to the Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale, Sutton (1996) found significant negative relation between it and a truncated, 10-item, version of the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. Cooley and Carden (1992) found no relation between their own measure of loneliness administered one month into the first semester and the SACQ Academic Adjustment subscale (the only subscale reported) administered at the end of the semester. Shilkret and Taylor (1992) studied the relation to adjustment to college of two other socially-defined feeling states, shame and guilt. Using the Personal Feelings Questionnaire (Harder & Zalma, 1990), those investigators obtained significant and reasonably strong correlations in the expected direction between feelings of shame and all SACQ variables except the Attachment subscale, and somewhat lesser but still significant correlations between feelings of guilt and the SACQ Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and the full scale. Significant, moderately strong, and expected-direction correlations with SACQ indices were reported by Nigrosh (1993) and Shilkret and Nigrosh (1997) using measures of guilt defined in terms of Control-Mastery Theory (Weiss, Sampson, & the Mt. Zion Psychotherapy Research Group, 1986) and applied to analysis of data from structured interviews focusing on students’college experiences, family, and childhood for the purpose of identifying vestiges of developmental experiences that may impede the realization of educational goals. Nigrosh’s (1993) subjects, sophomores at the time of her first study, two years later as 135 seniors were administered the Interpersonal Guilt Questionnaire-45 (IGQ-45; O’Connor, Berry, Weiss, Bush, & Sampson, 1997) as well as the SACQ. Significant and substantial correlations were found between several subscales of that instrument (except for Separation guilt; i.e., feelings of guilt for separating and becoming different from family members, especially parents) and all SACQ indices (Shilkret & Nigrosh, 1995). In other uses of the IGQ-45, Shilkret and Vecchiotti (1995), Shilkret and Edwards (1997), Shilkret (2000), and Morray and Shilkret (2001) obtained significant correlations between all variables from that Questionnaire and all SACQ variables, most consistently and strongest for the Self-Hate Guilt (akin to low self-esteem, occurring as an effect of certain kinds of parenting) and Interpersonal Guilt scales from the IGQ-45 and for the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale from the SACQ. Bailey and Shilkret (2000b) report significant correlations for the IGQ-45 SelfHate Guilt subscale with all SACQ variables; for the Survivor Guilt subscale (feelings of advancing one’s own interests at the expense of, and surpassing by comparison, family members, especially parents), with all SACQ variables except the Attachment subscale; for the Omnipotence Guilt subscale (an exaggerated sense of responsibility and concern for the happiness and well-being of others) and total IGQ score with the Academic and PersonalEmotional Adjustment subscales and full scale; but no significant correlations for the Separation Guilt subscale with any of the SACQ indices; . Measures of shame and guilt from the Test of Self-Conscious Affect (TOSCA; Tangney, Wagner, & Gramzow, 1989) were found to correlate significantly with the SACQ Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales, and shame with the full scale as well (Shilkret & Nigrosh, 1995). Sutton (1996) devised measures of shame and guilt by merging items from the Personal 136 Feelings Questionnaire (Harder & Zalma, 1990), the Affect Intensity Measure (Diener, Sandvik, & Larsen, 1985), and an interview questionnaire designed for her study, and correlated them with a truncated, 10-item, version of the Academic Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. She reported significant negative values for shame experienced in relation to mothers for male students and her full sample that also included female students; for shame experienced in relation to fathers for both male and female samples separately and combined; and for guilt experienced in relation to fathers for female students. A positive correlation was reported for guilt experienced in relation to fathers for her male sample. Other Person Characteristics Defined in Terms of Social Relations in General Deaf student identification with deaf- or hearing-communities. Corbett (1991) was interested in the attitude of deaf college students towards their deafness as a determinant of their adjustment at a college for deaf students, with that attitude defined in terms of a particular aspect of a student’s larger social environment with which he/she identified. She used the Deaf Identity Scale (Weinberg & Sterritt, 1986) to ascertain whether deaf students identify primarily with the hearing culture/community (hearing identity), the deaf culture/community (deaf identity), or both (dual identity). Positive correlation was found between deaf identity and the SACQ Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and the full scale; negative correlation between hearing identity and the Attachment subscale; and negative correlation between dual identity and the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. In analysis of differences among SACQ means for deaf students falling into the three identity categories, Corbett (1991) found that on social adjustment students with deaf identity scored higher than students with dual identity; and on institutional attachment and overall 137 adjustment deaf identity students were higher than either hearing or dual identity students. There was no relation in this mode of analysis between identity status and either academic or personalemotional adjustment. Corbett’s (1991) sample included fairly large numbers of both white and black deaf students, and in analysis for only the latter group she found no relation between identity status and any of the SACQ variables. Also, there was no relation for either the sample as a whole or the Black subsample between attitude toward sign language, as measured by a scale developed by Corbett for her study, and SACQ variables. Perfectionism in social contexts. Merryman and Zelezny (1993) employed a measure of perfectionism (the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale, or MPS; Hewitt & Flett, 1991) that interestingly introduces a social aspect into the definition of a person variable that may not ordinarily be regarded as having a social component. That is, one of three dimensions addressed by the MPS is perfectionistic expectations imposed on oneself by oneself (self-oriented perfectionism, perfectionism as traditionally conceived), and that kind of non-social perfectionism was found not to be related to any of the SACQ variables. A second dimension is the imposition of high standards on the behavior of others (other-oriented perfectionism), and Merryman and Zelezny obtained significant though relatively low negative (expected direction) correlations between that kind of perfectionism and the SACQ Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales. A third dimension of perfectionism involves the felt obligation to live up to standards and expectations imposed on oneself by others (socially prescribed perfectionism), and that dimension was seen to correlate significantly in the expected negative direction with all SACQ variables except the Academic Adjustment subscale, with somewhat stronger values than 138 in the other-oriented perfectionism and most strongly with the Social Adjustment subscale. Variables less directly defined in terms of social relations. Several variables, or sets of variables, may be cited here that are less directly connected with social relations than those already considered, but hopefully can be included with some stretch of definition. Montgomery, Haemmerlie, and Amsden (2001; data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) report weak positive correlations between self-rated physical attractiveness (which presumably has implications for social relationships) and all SACQ variables except institutional attachment. Also, Montgomery and Haemmerlie report modest negative correlations between height and social adjustment and institutional attachment. Temperament is another variable that seemingly has implications for social relations, and, indeed, aspects of its definition include reference to social relations. McAndrew-Miller (1989) investigated the relation to college adjustment of differences in temperament, defined as an individual’s characteristic style of approach and response to people, situations and tasks, as reflected, e. g., in emotionality (mood), movement/activity (amount, tempo, rhythmicity or regularity of same), readiness of engagement (approach/withdrawal), focus of attention (distractibility) and effort (persistence), and adaptability to change (flexibility/rigidity). The means of measuring temperament employed by McAndrew-Miller was the Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS-R; Windle & Lerner, 1986), which identifies ten such dimensions. Of forty correlations between the ten temperament dimensions and four SACQ subscales, 29 were significant and in the expected direction, occurring with all SACQ subscales but most frequently with academic and personal-emotional adjustment. The stronger correlations (.30’s and .40’s) were for: Approach/Withdrawal with social adjustment and institutional 139 attachment; Flexibility/Rigidity with social and personal-emotional adjustment and institutional attachment; Mood with social adjustment and institutional attachment; and Distractibility with academic adjustment. A set of variables at least indirectly related to social relations is use of intoxicants and recreational drugs, which probably most often occurs in social contexts. Loveland (1994) found that amount of alcohol consumed by students had small positive correlations with social adjustment in college and institutional attachment, but Montgomery and Howdeshell (1993) report modest negative correlations between frequency of drinking and the SACQ full scale score, and between frequency of getting drunk and academic, personal-emotional, and overall adjustment. For Ropar (1997), however, there was no relation between a measure of “present alcohol use” from the Young Adult Alcohol Problems Screening Test (YAAPST; Hurlbut & Sher, 1992) and any SACQ variable; for Oliver et al. (1998; see also Reed, 1994) there was no relation between a measure of alcohol use from the Core Alcohol and Drug Survey (Presley, Meilman, & Lyerla, 1993 & 1995) and the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable reported; and for Pratt (2001) there was no relation between number of alcoholic drinks consumed per week and any SACQ subcale. Oliver et al. (1998; see also Reed, 1994) also derived from the Core (Presley, Meilman, & Lyerla, 1993 & 1995) a measure of negative consequences of alcohol use, and obtained a negative correlation between that index and personal-emotional adjustment. Seemingly tapping a similar variable, Coatsworth (2001) adapted questions from an Alcoholics Anonymous website on which students could rate the extent to which substance use or abuse interfered with their academic, personal, and familial life. She found significant correlation between that variable and 140 personal-emotional and overall adjustment, greater such consequence associated with poorer adjustment. Montgomery and Howdeshell (1993) state that students with low personal-emotional adjustment scores are more likely to describe themselves as drinking to relieve boredom, to relieve emotional and academic pressure, and to feel better about oneself. For Haemmerlie, Montgomery, and Saling (1994), the later the age of first use of alcohol the higher the scores on the Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and the full scale. Montgomery and Haemmerlie (1993) looked more specifically at the type of alcohol used. They found no relation between beer drinking or getting drunk on beer and any of the SACQ variables, but did find that getting drunk on hard liquor was negatively related to both academic and personal-emotional adjustment and the full scale. Pratt (2001) found no relation between smoking of tobacco and any SACQ subscale, but there were modest positive correlations between marijuana use and social and personal-emotional adjustment. 141 CHAPTER 10 PERSON CHARACTERISTICS AS DETERMINANTS OF COLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: GROUP IDENTITY/DEMOGRAPHIC VARIABLES To be considered in this chapter are person characteristics associated with an individual’s membership, however active or passive, in a body or category of persons that may be assumed to have implications for the individual’s behavior, including adjustment to college. These include categories such as gender, sex-role orientation, race/ethnicity, foreign student status, socioeconomic status, college year level, etc. Some are more demographic or status-related than psychological in nature, and, with one or two exceptions, do not seem to be nearly as fruitful in helping to understand adjustment to college as many person variables already considered. Gender For samples tested by the SACQ's developers over 20 semesters, no statistically significant mean differences between sexes were found for either the Academic Adjustment subscale or the full scale (Baker, 1993b; Baker & Siryk, 1989). There were two significant mean differences each for the Social Adjustment and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales, women higher in the former and men higher in the latter. On the Attachment subscale, one of the 20 comparisons of means was significant, women having the higher average. At other institutions, the large majority of investigators found no SACQ differences associated with sex (Adan & Felner, 1995; Amin, 2000; Bartels, 1945; Bettencourt et al., 1999; Beyers, 2001); Brooks & DuBois, 1995; Choi, 2000; Danielson, 1995; Evans-Hughes, 1992; Flescher, Clingempeel & Stein, 1986; Fox, 2000; Friedland, 1990; Gallant, 1994; Gerdes, 1986; Harris, 1988; Hurtado et al., 1996; Jampol, 1988/1989; Just, 1998; Kaczmarek, Matlock, & 142 Franco, 1990; Kalsner-Silver, 2000; Kline, 1992; Lamothe, Currie, Alisat, Sullivan, Pratt, Pancer, & Hunsberger, 1995; Lapsley et al., 1989; Lent et al., 1997; Leong, Bonz, & Zachar, 1997; Liter, 1987; Lopez, 1997; Lopez et al., 1986; Marcy, 1996; Maton, 1989b; Maton & Weisman, 1989; Mooney, 1989; Natera, 1998; Paulshock, 1994; Pfeil, 2000; Pratt, Hunsberger, Pancer, Alisat, Bowers, Mackey, Ostaniewicz, Rog, Terzian, & Thomas, 2000; Reed, 1994; Reeker, 1994; Robbins et al., 1993; Rodriguez, 1994; Ropar, 1997; Salone, 1995; Saracoglu, 1987; Savino et al., 1986a; Schriver, 1996; Scott, 1991; Serafica, Allen, Brown, & Stewart, 1990; Silverthorn, 1993; Silverthorn & Gekoski, 1995; Terrell, 1989; Woo & Bilynsky, 1994; and Zea et al., 1995). Several investigators at other institutions obtained higher mean scores for males than for females on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale (Addison, 1996; Birnie-Lefcovitch, 1997; Burr, 1992; Frazier & Cook, 1993; Garbarino & Strange, 1993; Hogan, 1986; Jackson, 1998; Kenny & Stryker, 1996; Lopez, 1991; Lopez et al., 1988a, 1989; McAndrew-Miller, 1989; Mendelowitz, 1990/1991; Napoli & Wortman, 1998; Rice, Cunningham, & Young, 1997; Sennett, 2000; Sugar, 1997, see also Wintre & Sugar, 2000; Vivona, 2000a; and Yaffe, 1997). There have been no reports from other institutions of women having higher means than males on that subscale. On the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales Albert (1988), Napoli and Wortman (1998), McGowan (1988), Montgomery and Haemmerlie (2001), and Melendez (2001) found women to have higher mean scores than men, as did Allen (1989), Barthelemy and Fine (1995), Burr (1992), Low (1994; see also Low & Handal, 1995), McAndrew-Miller (1989), Ridinger (1998), Ross (1995), and Washington (1996) on the latter subscale. One investigator reported 143 higher social adjustment mean scores for males (Posselt, 1992). On the Academic Adjustment subscale, several investigators found higher scores for women than for men (Barthelelmy & Fine, 1995; Grella, 1989; Helman, 1999; Hutz, 2002a; Kulley, 1994; Lopez, 1989; Polewchak, 1998; Ridinger, 1998; Sennett, 2000; Napoli & Wortman, 1998; Washington, 1996; and Melendez, 2001), as did Dewitt-Parker (1999) on the SACQ full scale score. Wintre and Yaffe (2000) obtained higher SACQ full-scale scores for men than for women. Thus, overall, the most common finding is no gender differences as main effects on the SACQ. When such differences are seen, they are slight, and males have higher scores on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale while females have higher scores on the other three subscales. Not covered in this review of gender effects, the reader should be aware, are interactive effects of that variable with other determinants. Sex-role Orientation Related to gender as a group identity variable, but much less studied in relation to the SACQ than gender itself, is sex-role orientation. Students high in psychological androgyny as measured by the Personal Attributes Questionnaire (Spence, Helmreich, & Stapp, 1975) had higher scores on the SACQ Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and the full scale than students low in that characteristic (Bobier, 1989). Kim (1996) used the Bem Sex-Role Inventory-Short Form (Bem, 1974) to assess sex-role orientation in Korean-American students enrolled in American universities. Students identified as psychologically androgynous had higher adjustment scores than those identified as masculine (on all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment), or than those identified as feminine or undifferentiated (on all 144 SACQ variables). Kim did not examine SACQ differences among the three categories other than androgyny, but none were found between androgynous females or males. The better adjustment of psychologically androgynous students was predicted, on the assumption that they have a wider range of behaviors to bring to bear in meeting situational demands, providing greater flexibility across diverse circumstances. Probably less directly related to sex-role orientation but still in the same general sphere, men in traditionally female and male academic majors did not differ on any SACQ variable (Crouse, 1990). Race/Ethnicity Racial or ethnic origins might be expected to be popular group identity variables given contemporary interest in “diversity,” and they are reasonably so. However, it is difficult to see what kinds of effects should be anticipated from such variables by themselves irrespective of other variables. Indeed, many or most of the SACQ-using studies carried out so far concerning race/ethnicity are essentially concerned with minority students moving into and adapting to an envurinment that to some important degree is assumed to be “foreign” to them, their adjustment being compared with that of other groups for which the environment is either more or less “foreign.” African-origin students. A number of studies involving African-Anerican students simply compare them with white students at predominantly white American colleges and universities, environments that presumably are more foreign to the former students than the latter. The most common finding in such studies is no differences in SACQ scores. Seven studies found no differences between black and white students on any of the SACQ indices (Burr, 1992; Elacqua, 145 1992a,b; Kenny & Stryker, 1994; Maton, 1989b; McGowan, 1988; Reeker, 1994; Ridinger, 1998), as did Zea et al. (1995), and Williams (1996) using only the SACQ full scale score. Two studies (Robbins et al., 1993; Tomlinson-Clarke, 1998) report black students as having higher scores than whites on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, but Robbins et al. found no difference in academic adjustment, the only other SACQ variable employed in their study; and Tomlinson-Clarke found no difference on the three SACQ subscales other than personal-emotional adjustment. Four studies found higher scores for white students. For Adan and Felner (1995), the social adjustment and institutional attachment of white freshmen at a predominantly white university were better than that of black freshmen at the same institution. In Corbett’s (1991) study, deaf white students had higher scores than deaf black students on the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and the full scale, and white female deaf students had higher scores than black female deaf students on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. Jackson (1998) and Rice et al. (1997) found white students to have higher Social Adjustment subscale scores than blacks, and the latter investigators found no difference on the only other SACQ variable used, the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. In a study done at a South African university that historically was white but became multiracial, Sennett (2000) found that black mixed-gender freshman students had lower social adjustment scores than their white counterparts, and black female students had less good academic adjustment than white female students. But, important to note in comparing this finding with those in American colleges, so-called “coloureds” (mixed black and white sudents) were not included. 146 Additional findings regarding black student adjustment to predominantly white colleges simply as a function of racial status are reported later in this section from studies using assorted racial/ethnic samples that may include black and white groups. (may want to consider pulling black/wht cf’s from those later studies up to the foregoing paragraphs – e.g., Fox, Burr, Williams, Kenny & Stryker, Reeker) But, overall, these studies focusing simply on racial status have not produced uniformly definitive results. Adan and Felner (1995) provide yet another way of comparing black and white student adjustment that represents a considerable advance over simple comparison of such adjustment at predominantly white institutions. They found that black freshmen at a predominantly black university reported better adjustment than either black or white freshmen at a predominantly white university in both the social and academic areas as well as overall adjustment, and better institutional attachment than black freshmen at a predominantly white university. No differences were found among the three student groups in personal-emotional adjustment. Thus, very important to note, possible explanation for any findings of greater adaptive difficulty for black than for white students may to some extent lie in the nature of the students’ environment, including institutional characteristics. In an examination of black/white differences as a consequnce of another environmental variable, black and white students with high level of social support from friends showed little or no difference on the SACQ full scale score (the only SACQ variable reported), both having relatively high scores, while black students with low levels of social support had significantly lower SACQ full scale scores than white students with low levels of social support (Maton & Weisman, 1989). 147 Additional findings concerning the role of environment, in the form of interracial experience, in black student adjustment to college are presented in pages ???????. Another important move toward understanding black student adjustment to college is represented in studies that take into account differences in personal characteristics of the students. Thus, Salone (1995) made the interesting assumption that black student social adjustment (the only SACQ variable employed) to a predominantly white college would be less a function of the student's racial status than of the student's attitude concerning that status, or racial identity. She found, using the Racial Identity Attitude Scale (RIAS; Parham & Helms, 1981), that students who denigrate their racial identity, or who have had recent experiences that cause them to challenge that attitude, adjust less well socially to a predominantly white college. Also using the RIAS, in a study with deaf black students, with one kind of statistical analysis (analysis of variance) Corbett (1991) found no relation between black identity status and any of the SACQ variables, while another means of analysis (multiple regression) of the same data showed a weak negative relation between the RIAS immersion-emersion score and personalemotional adjustment. The immersion-emersion stage in the development of black identity is described as associated with increased emotional turmoil, increased hostility towards white people, and idolization of black culture. Evans-Hughes (1992) found no correlations between any of the four indices of racial identity attitudes from the RIAS and any of the SACQ variables. Quite likely related to racial identity in understanding black student adjustment to college would be the concept of cultural traditionality as measured by the African American Acculturation Scale (AAAS; Landrine & Klonoff, 1994). Cultural traditionality as defined in the AAAS is the degree to which a black person is immersed in black culture, attitudes, practices, 148 beliefs, or, conversely, acculturated to the prevailing white society. With higher AAAS scores indicating greater traditional cultural orientation in the individual, and reporting data only for the SACQ Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and full scale, Dewitt-Parker (2000) found that lesser traditional cultural orientation was associated with better personalemotional and overall adjustment to predominantly white colleges for black women students, while there were no significant findings for male students. Spanish-origin students. Albert (1988) obtained lower scores for Hispanic students than non-Hispanic (presumably predominantly white) students on the SACQ Social Adjustment subscale, but for Kaczmarek et al. (1990) there were no SACQ differences between Hispanic and White students, as was true for Zea et al. (1995) using only the SACQ full scale score). Looking only at Latino students, Hurtado et al. (1996) found that, on a testing in the sophomore year, Chicano students had less good social adjustment than other kinds of Latino students. Rodriguez (1994), however, found no difference on the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable used, among kinds of Chicano/Latino students. Just as some investigators in studies with black students were more concerned with attitude toward one’s racial identity than with racial status itself as it affects adjustment to college, there are some who evidenced similar interest in their studies with Hispanic students. Thus, no relation was seen by Solberg et al. (1994) in Hispanic students between a measure of degree of identification with one's minority culture of origin (Cuellar, Harris & Jasso, 1980) and a truncated SACQ consisting of 26 items from the Academic and Social Adjustment subscales. Rodriguez (1994), using the Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM; Phinney, 1992), found no relation between ethnic identity (ethnic pride, feeling good about one’s background, being 149 happy with one’s group membership) for Chicano-Latino men or women and the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable reported, and for Shibazaki (1999) there was no relation between that same index of ethnic identity and any SACQ subscale in Mexican-American students. But, using the Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican-Americans-II (Cuellar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995), Shibazaki (1999) did find a weak positive correlation between degree of acculturation of Mexican-American students and social adjustment in a predominantly “Anglo” university. Additional findings regarding Spanish-origin student adjustment to college are reported below from studies using assorted racial/ethnic samples that included Spanish-origin and white students. (may want to consider pulling hisp/wht cf’s from those later studies up to the foregoing paragraphs) Arab-origin students. Arab-American students were found by Amin (2000) to have lower personal-emotional adjustment than non-Arab, White, American students. As in studies described above employing African-origin and Spanish-origin American college students, Amin (2000) was interested in the role of acculturation in the adjustment to an American college of her Arab-American students, and used three different means of assessing acculturation. One technique employed Arab-American students’ratings on two items: (1) degree of importance of adopting in their own lives American values and traditions, and (2) degree of importance of preserving Arab values and traditions in their lives. Patterns of ratings for these two items (not necessarily mutually exclusive) provided the basis for constructing five categories of accultural orientation. Bicultural orientation was indicated by high endorsement of both items; assimilationist orientation by predominant preference for adopting American values; 150 traditionalist orientation by predominant preference for preservation of Arab values; marginal orientation by low endorsement of both options (“reject both options”); and neutral orientation by “neither agreeing nor disagreeing” regarding the importance of both options. Somewhat unexpectedly Amin (2000) obtained no correlations with any of the SACQ subscales for either the assimilationist or marginal orientations; a negative correlation for the bicultural orientation with personal-emotional adjustment; moderately sized negative correlations for the neutral orientation with all SACQ subscales except personal-emotional adjustment; and moderately sized positive correlations with all SACQ subscales for the traditionalist orientation.. (on 4/1/02 I e-mailed the au req’g means for the various orient’s on the SACQ var’s, with tests of sig of diff’s, & if these are forthcoming I will prob’ly substitute those new data for the r’l analyses, / I don’t find easy to interpret.) A second means of tapping acculturation employed by Amin (2000) was the Ethnic Identity Achievement subscale from the Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM; Phinney, 1992), which assesses an individual’s level of exploration of and commitment to an ethnic identity (in some sense the obverse of acculturation, and akin to the traditionalist orientation discussed above). Here, too, there were positive, moderately sized correlations with all SACQ variables. The third means of assessing acculturation employed by Amin (2000) was the MajorityMinority Relations Survey (MMRS; Sodowsky, Lai, & Plake, 1991), which measures degree of affiliation with the majority group or with one’s minority group, the latter indicating rejection of American culture and a low level of assimilation/acculturation. One MMRS subscale that gauges degree of adherence to ethnic behaviors and customs (cf. traditionalism and ethnic identity as 151 presented above) correlated positively with the SACQ Academic and Social Adjustment subscales; another MMRS subscale measuring degree of use, proficiency in, and preference for one’s ethnic language correlated positively with academic adjustment. Thus, the findings for Amin (2000) across three methi\ods of measuring acculturation consistently indicate that the lesser the degree of acculturation/assimilation of her ArabAmerican students the better their adjustment to college. Importantly, however, Amin points out that the university attended by the students in her sample has a sizeable and cohesive representation of Arab-Americans, as does the community in which it is located. She reasons that these circumstances may permit and even encourage the maintenance of ethnic identity, and contribute to more effective adjustment to college under the prevailing circumstances. She conjectures further that an Arab-American student’s willingness to assimilate into the host society may even expose him/her to greater discrimination and hostility than might otherwise be experienced, contributing to less good college adjustment. This line of thinking highlights the importance of possible interaction in this particular study between characteristics of students and characteristics of the environment both on and off-campus. Differences among assorted racial/ethnic origin groups. Burr (1992) examined differences among Hispanic, American Indian, black, white, and Asian students in a community college. Hispanic and white students scored higher than American Indian students on the Academic Adjustment and Attachment subscales, and white students were higher than American Indian students on the Social Adjustment subscale. Williams (1996) had African-American, AsianAmerican, Latino and white students in her research sample, and the only differences among the groups that she reports were on academic adjustment, where both the African-American and 152 Latin-American students scored higher than the Asian-American students. Kenny and Stryker (1994) and Reeker (1994) found no differences on any SACQ variables among white, AfroAmerican, Asian, and Latino students, as did Zea et al. (1995) reporting only the SACQ full scale score; as did Kalsner-Silver (2000, 2002) on SACQ subscales among Asian, Asian-Indian, African-American, Hispanic, and white students. Also using only the SACQ full scale score, Fox (2000) obtained no differences among white, African-American, Asian-American, and Hispanic students.(reorganize this last sentence; all have neg results, but differ in ethnic categories of Ss used and SACQ var’s reported; maybe make separate paragraph) In the Williams (1996) study cited in the preceding paragraph, the investigator used, in addition to the SACQ itself, an SACQ-related index of adjustment. That index, described in Chapter 2, is the difference between scores from the prematriculation-administered Anticipated Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (ASACQ) and scores from the postmatriculationadministered SACQ, which is interpreted as reflecting the degree to which prematriculation expectations regarding adjustment to college are fulfilled, or, conversely, degree of disillusionment with one’s expectations. One means of statistical analysis found no differences on this index among African-American, Latino, Asian-American and white students but a second mode of analysis did. With regard to social adjustment, African-American students showed greater disillusionment than either Latino or white students, and Asian-American students more disillusionment than white students. Asian-American students also showed greater disillusionment than white students on the overall adjustment measure (i. e., the ASACQ/SACQ full scale scores). Morray and Shilkret (2001) found in an all-women’s college that Asian/AsianAmerican students had lower social adjustment scores than White students. 153 Comparison of non-minority students with mixed minority samples. Young (1994) found that white students had higher scores on the SACQ Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales than did a sample comprised of African-Americans, Hispanics, and Puerto Ricans. Hutz (2002b) reported that “majority” (presumably white) students had better social adjustment and institutional attachment than did a mixed group of African-American, HispanicAmerican, and Native-American students. For Just (1998), white students had higher social adjustment scores than a sample made up of Asian-American, Latino/Hispanic, and AfricanAmerican students. Comparing white students with a similarly composed minority sample that was also identified as financially and educationally disadvantaged and enrolled in a special support program, Kenny & Stryker (1996) found the former group to have higher personalemotional adjustment scores. But Melendez (2001) reported that minority women students had better academic adjustment than non-minority women students. But in several studies no differences were obtained between non-minority and mixed minority samples on any SACQ subscales, or the full scale score where employed. These include Hutz, Fabian, and Martin (2000), between “Euro-American” (presumably white) students and a group of African-American, Hispanic-American, and Native American students; Melendex (2001), between Whites and a sample including AfricanAmerican, Asian, Latino, and “other nonWhite” students; Halloran (2000), between Caucasian and “racially diverse” (not further specified) students; Napoli and Wortman (1998), between a “non-minority” and a “minority” (not further delineated) sample in a community college; and Birnie-Lefcovitch (1997), among unspecified racial groups at a Canadian university. For Helman (1999) there were no differences on academic, social, and overall adjustment – the only SACQ variables reported – between non- 154 minority students and a sample including African-American, Asian-American, Hispamic, and Native American students, and for Fox (2000) there was no difference on the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable reported, between white students and a group that included African-American, Asian-American, and Hispanic students. Racial/ethnic identity as a general characteristic of students. Kalsner-Silver (2000), using a racially-ethnically combined sample (Asian-origin, Asian-Indian-origin, African-American, Hispanic, and White students), found almost no relation between the three aspects of racial identity measured by the Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM; Phinney, 1992) and SACQ subscales. Of twelve correlations generated, only one weak value reached significance, and that barely. Also using a racially-ethnically combined sample (African-American, Hispanic, Native American, and White), and a revised version of the MEIM (Roberts, Phinney, Masse, Chen, Roberts, & Romero, 1999), Hutz (2002a,b) found no relation with any of the SACQ variables. Foreign Student Status Presumably any differences found among racial/ethnic groups in adjustment to college would be attributable in some degree to culture- or subculture-induced characteristics of students in interaction with the characteristics of the host institutional environment, with congruence between the two sets of characteristics enhancing adjustment. This same reasoning, i.e., a cultural-congruence hypothesis, should be applicable in the investigation of the adjustment to college of students from foreign countries/cultures. Cooper (1991) used this kind of reasoning to study ethnic Chinese and Pacific Islander students’ adjustment at an Australian university. In particular, that investigator was interested in culture-induced value characteristics of students from a foreign country as they may differ from 155 the culture-induced value characteristics of students at the university (and country) into which the foreign students moved. Cooper used the Schwartz Value Survey-Form A (Schwartz, 1992) to assess personal values or guiding principles, and a rather complicated means of computing degree of congruence between the values of each of the two foreign groups and a sample of Australian students at the same university. She expected that the greater the value dissimilarity between each of the foreign student groups and the group of Australian students, the poorer the adjustment would be. For the Pacific Islanders, there were no significant correlations between dissimilarity with Australian student values and any SACQ variable; for the ethnic Chinese, the greater the value similarity the better the personal-emotional adjustment. In a finding difficult to interpret, the more that individual Australian students’ values differed from the average of those same students, the better was the personal-emotional adjustment. Though Cooper’s (1991) findings were probably less than what she desired, she did address an interesting issue in an interesting way. Findings from investigations of adjustment of foreign students at American universities are, of course, also relevant to understanding the role of culture-induced characteristics of students in interaction with characteristics of the host institutional environment. Kusaka (1995) reported that students from western European countries, the primary source of American cultural values, had higher scores than students from the culturally more different east Asian countries on the SACQ Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and the full scale. Abe, Talbot and Geelhoed (1998) found that Asian students had lower scores on all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment than a culturally heterogeneous group composed of students from 156 European, Latin-American, African and Middle-Eastern countries. Possibly pertinent to the culture-congruence hypothesis if one can assume differences in academic values between American and foreign student-athletes, is Ridinger’s (1998) finding that foreign student-athletes (from Canada and western Europe) at an American university had higher scores on the Academic Adjustment subscale than domestic student-athletes. In a finding suggesting that culture-induced characteristics or their effects may be mitigated, Abe et al. (1998) report that Social Adjustment and Attachment subscale scores of international students at an American university who lived in the United States prior to matriculation were higher than those of international students who had not. Rodriguez-Perez (1991) in her study of two kinds of Puerto Rican students in culturally disparate college settings – those born and raised in mainland United States attending two New Jersey colleges, and those born and raised in Puerto Rico attending a Puerto Rican college – used a self-devised measure of “connection” to the island of Puerto Rico. That measure, termed the “Puerto Rican Index,” was based on whether the student was born in Puerto Rico, graduated from a Puerto Rican high school, spoke Spanish as a main language, and had parents who were native Puerto Ricans. No significant correlations were found for either sample between connection to Puerto Rico and adjustment to college, even though a negative relation might have been expected for the students attending mainland colleges (because of cultural incongruence) and a positive one for students attending a Puerto Rican college (because of cultural congruence), or at least a lower one for the former sample than the latter. However, somewhat difficult to understand, for both samples combined there were significant negative correlations between the Index and social 157 adjustment and institutional attachment. Other findings reported by Rodriguez-Perez (1991) that do not, however, seem relevant to the issue under discussion here, involved the mainland sample having higher scores than the “Island” sample on social and overall adjustment and attachment to their respective institutions. Extending the findings cited above regarding greater adjustment difficulties of Asian international students than other foreign students is the study by Ridinger (1998). While that study had student-athletes and non-athletes as its primary focus, it employed international as well as American students in each of the two primary categories. The international non-athlete sample was comprised very largely of Asian students, and it had lower scores than all three other groups (i. e., domestic student-athletes, domestic non-athletes, and international student-athletes) on social and overall adjustment; lower than both domestic samples on institutional attachment; and lower than foreign student-athletes on academic adjustment. Important to note, it was not only the domestic samples that were racially different from the primarily Asian group, because the foreign student-athletes were mainly from Canada and western Europe. Religion and Related Variables No relation was found by Maton (1989a) between church attendance and either the Social or Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales, the only SACQ indices employed, including for either high- or low-stress subsamples. However, students in another study reporting that they were members of a formal religious organization (a church, synagogue, temple, etc.) had higher academic adjustment scores than students who reported no such affiliation (Addison, 1996). No SACQ differences were found between Catholic and non-Catholic students by Albert (1988). Kline (1992) and Hunsberger et al., 1996) obtained no relation between one-item 158 measures of the importance of religion in one’s family and the SACQ, the former study using all SACQ variables and the latter just the full scale score. Possibly on a more personal level, the latter investigators obtained a weak positive correlation between the SACQ full scale and a oneitem measure of the extent to which students described themselves as still holding the religious beliefs taught to them while growing up, though there was no relation between that same SACQ variable and single-item measures regarding how interested students were in religion currently or how religious they considered themselves to be. Yet religiousness as measured by the Personal Religiosity Inventory (Lipsmeyer, 1984) was positively associated for Low (1994; Low & Handal, 1995) with all SACQ variables, but related differently to the various SACQ indices for men and women and for different universities. Hunsberger et al. (1996) found no relation between the Religious Fundamentalism Scale (Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 1992) and any of the SACQ variables, but did obtain modest negative correlations between the Religious Doubts Scale (Altemeyer, 1988) and all SACQ variables except social adjustment. The greater the religious doubt, the poorer the adjustment. Two SACQ-using studies were less concerned with aspects of religion as traditionally conceived and more with fundamental personal values, which presumably are essential contents of religious thinking and religiousness. The Belief Systems Analysis Scale (BSAS; Montgomery, Fine, & James-Myers, 1990) was purportedly developed as a measure of Afrocentric world view, but examination of its item content suggests that it can more likely be seen as tapping fundamental personal values of a quasi-religious, quasi-“social-philosophical” nature, e. g., spirituality; non-materialism; harmony and inclusiveness in social relations, emphasizing generosity of spirit, tolerance, and forgivingness; sense of well-being and optimism. 159 Hatter and Ottens (1998) administered the BSAS to black students at a predominantly white university and found its total score to be positively correlated with all SACQ variables, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. Maybe also related to religiousness, though a little more removed than what is assessed by the Belief Systems Analysis Scale (BSAS; Montgomery, Fine, & James-Myers, 1990), is the variable life value preferences as measured by the Life Values Inventory (Mitchell, 1984). That instrument assesses basic personal values as reflected in preferred life styles, of which Bartels (1995) employed four: Conventionally Defined Success, Materialistic Orientation, Intellectual and Cultural Interests, and Valuing of Achievement. Significant positive correlations were obtained for all four lifestyles with all SACQ indices except personal-emotional adjustment, and except for the Materialistic Orientation/academic adjustment comparison. A person characteristic that this writer finds difficult to understand, but which may be related to religiousness/basic personal values, is existentially-defined world view (relationship with the world and its contents) as measured by the Scale to Assess World Views (Ibrahim & Kahn, 1987). Rodriguez (1994) found no relation between that measure and the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable employed. Political Orientation Hunsberger et al. (1996) found no relation between scores on the Right-Wing Authoritarianism Scale (Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 1992) and the SACQ full-scale score, the only SACQ variable reported. Socio-economic Status and Related Variables Findings concerning the relation between socio-economic status and adjustment to 160 college are typically incidental to larger research purposes, sparse, and inconsistent. For Maton (1989b) there was a negative correlation with personal-emotional adjustment; for Dewitt-Parker (1999) a negative correlation with the SACQ full scale score; for Panori (1997) a positive one with academic adjustment; and for Kline (1992) positive correlations with social adjustment and the SACQ full scale. But Yaffe (1997), Hurtado et al. (1996), Hutto, (1998), Napoli and Wortman (1998), Rodriguez (1994; using the SACQ full scale only), and Natera (1998; using academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment only) found no effects. For a variable related to socio-economic status, parental/family/household income, Birnie-Lefcovitch (1997) found that students with lesser income parents had lower scores on all SACQ indices than students with more affluent parents; Just (1998) obtained positive correlation with social, personal-emotional and overall adjustment; Brooks and DuBois (1995) and Natera (1998; using the Academic, Social, and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales only) with personal-emotional adjustment; and Wang and Smith (1993) and Hertel (1996) with social adjustment. Yaffe (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) and Pfeil (2000) found no correlation with any SACQ variable, and Williams (1996) with the full scale, the only SACQ variable reported. Probably also related to socio-economic level through parental or family income is students’ level of satisfaction with his/her finances, which was found by Just (1998) to be positively correlated with academic, personal-emotional and overall adjustment. Another variable related to socio-economic status is parents’ educational level. At a Canadian university, students with mothers and/or fathers having at least a college education showed better academic, social, and overall adjustment than students with parents of no college education; and mothers’ similarly measured educational level measured in that manner was also 161 similarly reflected in students’ institutional attachment (Bernie-Lefcovitch, 1997). In Halloran’s (2000) study, students whose parents had either an associate’s or bachelor’s degree had better academic and personal-emotional adjustment than students whose parents had neither degree. Just (1998) found weak positive correlation between mothers’ educational level and personalemotional and overall adjustment, and between fathers’ level and social adjustment, while Williams (1996) obtained a positive correlation between the educational level for the parents combined and the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable reported. For Yaffe (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000), Amin (2000), and Pfeil (2000) there was no effect with SACQ variables, as was also true for Rodriguez (1994) and Wildman (1998) using the fullscale score only. Looking at another facet of parents’ educational level, Hertel’s (1996) freshman secondgeneration college attenders had higher scores than first-generation attenders on the SACQ Social Adjustment subscale and full scale, but Bartels (1995; personal communication, May 21, 1999) and Jackson (1998) found no differences on any SACQ variable between those two kinds of students. Work status Work status may be less a group identity than a demographic variable. Defined in terms of number of hours per week in paid employment, this aspect of work status was found by Hertel (1996) to be unrelated in freshman students to any SACQ variable, and for Wildman (1998) that same variable was in freshmen unrelated to the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable reported. But for Ortiz (1995), it was negatively correlated with the Academic Adjustment subscale for older (age 25-45) female community college students, and not correlated with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, the only other SACQ variable used. Jackson (1998) 162 found that students who had no on- or off-campus employment had higher scores on social adjustment and institutional attachment than students who did have such employment. Marital/Parental Status of Student In Ortiz’ (1995) study involving older female community college students, married students had better personal-emotional adjustment to college than non-married students, but there was no difference between these two kinds of students in academic adjustment, the only other SACQ variable employed. Ortiz also found no correlation between a measure of a student’s parental responsibility – defined in terms of age of the student’s youngest child – and either of the SACQ variables employed. Napoli and Wortman (1998) devised a three-item measure of “external commitments” that combined community college students’ marital/parental and occupational obligations, plus a two-item measure of “external involvements” concerning amount of time spent socializing with relatives and friends outside of the college community, both measures presumably tapping distractions from the college experience. The only correlation obtained for either measure with SACQ subscales was a very weak positive one for external commitments with institutional attachment. College Year Level and Related Variables SACQ-measured adjustment associated with college year level has been studied by several investigators, some cross-sectionally and some longitudinally. It is very likely that most or all studies focusing on college year level will have that variable confounded with students’ age, and studies explicitly concerning the latter variable will be considered in the following section, where, of course, the two variables are still likely to be confounded. 163 Cross-sectional studies have compared freshmen with upper class students separately and combined. Sophomores had higher Social Adjustment subscale scores than freshmen (Albert, 1988), as did juniors and final undergraduate year students (Beyers & Goossens, 1998; Lapsley et al., 1989). Juniors and final undergraduate year students had higher academic adjustment scores than freshmen (Tomlinson-Clarke, 1998), and in another study higher overall adjustment scores, the only SACQ variable reported in that study (Beyers, 2001). Seniors had higher Attachment subscale scores than freshmen (Loveland, 1994). And a sample of mixed upper class students had higher academic and personal-emotional adjustment scores than freshmen (no data reported for the Attachment subscale; Rice et al, 1995). Looking at students across all year levels and employing correlational analysis, Loveland found small positive correlations with class status (i. e., the more advanced classes having higher scores) for all SACQ variables except the Social Adjustment subscale, as did Montgomery and Haemmerlie (2001) with the same SACQ variables for a sizeable sample of undergraduates of unspecified year level. But, still cross-sectionally, Reeker (1994) found no differences between freshmen and sophomores on any SACQ variables. Gilkey et al. (1989; see also Gilkey, 1988 and Protinsky & Gilkey, 1996) obtained no differences across the four college year levels on the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable employed, as did Salone (1995) using only the Social Adjustment subscale. Belvedere (2000) reported that juniors and seniors who were transfer students had lower social adjustment scores than freshman students; and, indeed, they had lower social adjustment scores than did “native” sophomores and juniors, i. e., students who had entered the institution as 164 freshmen. Rice (1992; see also Rice, 1990/1991) reported that students studied longitudinally, i. e., tested both as freshmen and as juniors, had higher scores on the later occasion on the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales (data concerning the Attachment subscale were not reported). Jackson et al. (2000) found that a sample of students that was tested successively in the freshman, sophomore and senior years had higher SACQ full scale scores (the only SACQ variable reported) as seniors, with no difference between the freshman and sophomore testings. However, Harris (1991) tested students both as freshmen and later as seniors, and there were no significant differences on any SACQ variable over the two occasions. Montgomery and Haemmerlie (1993) and Montgomery and Howdeshell (1993) report a small positive correlation between number of years enrolled (rather than using the customary college class designations) and the SACQ full scale, as did Panori (1997) with the Attachment subscale. Pfeil (2000) obtained positive correlation between number of semesters completed (which of course would be highly related to number of years enrolled and college year level) and social and overall adjustment. But Ridinger (1998) found no association between number of years enrolled and any of the SACQ subscales, as did Scott (1991) for the Social Adjustment subscale (the only subscale reported), Age of Students Several studies that have focused explicitly on the relation between age and college adjustment as measured by the SACQ have used samples including students from various undergraduate year levels, still confounding college year level and age. Of such studies that reported findings for all SACQ subscales (and sometimes the full scale), three found positive 165 correlations with academic adjustment (Dewein, 1994; Panori, 1997; Posselt, 1992); three, positive correlations with personal-emotional adjustment (Dewein; Loveland, 1994, in women but not men; Panori); one, modest positive relation with social adjustment (Pfeil); one, positive correlation with institutional attachment (Panori); one, no significant correlations (Amin, 2000); and two (the only ones reporting findings for the SACQ full-scale), positive correlations with that variable (Loveland; Posselt). Some studies focusing on age and including students from various college year levels reported findings only for selected SACQ variables. Natera (1998) used only the Academic, Social, and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales, finding positive correlation with academic adjustment. Three studies (Gilkey, 1988 or Gilkey et al., 1989; Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 1993; Beyers, 2001) obtained positive correlations with the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable reported, but one (Lopez, 1997) found no relation with the Attachment subscale, again the only SACQ variable employed. In two studies using freshman and sophomore students only (Albert, 1988; Reeker, 1994), there were no differences associated with age on any SACQ subscale, and also in a third (Rodriguez, 1994) that used only the SACQ full scale.. Mixed findings regarding age occur in studies using only freshman students. Napoli and Wortman (1998) reported weak positive correlations between age and all SACQ subscales for freshman community college students; Burr (1992), positive correlations with the Academic Adjustment, Personal-Emotional Adjustment, and Attachment subscales in the same kind of student population. In Sennett’s (2000) study freshman students over age 19 had a higher mean academic adjustment score than those less than 19, and Pratt, Hunsberger, Pancer, Alisat, 166 Bowers, Mackey, Ostaniewicz, Rog, Terzian, and Thomas (2000) found a positive correlation with age on the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable used), but Brooks and DuBois (1995) paradoxically got negative correlations for the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and full scale. Wang and Smith (1993) and Birnie-Lefcovitch (1997) found no differences associated with age among freshmen. In a study involving older (age 25-45) female community college students, positive correlations were found between age and the Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales, the only SACQ variables employed (Ortiz, 1995). Finally, in a study that involved large age differences between two student samples, but also various other kinds of important and uncontrolled differences, Kane, Lennon and Petrosky (1989) compared traditional-age freshmen with a group of non-traditional age (i. e., over 25) undergraduates from all college classes combined. The freshmen scored significantly lower on academic, personal-emotional, and overall adjustment but higher on social adjustment. Academic Major Three studies found no SACQ differences between students varying in kinds of academic major: Gilleylen (1994) for science versus non-science majors in African-American students; as cited earlier, Crouse (1990) for men in traditionally female and male majors; and Kusaka (1995) for social science versus natural science majors. Student-athlete Status Comparing student-athletes with student non-athletes, Ridinger (1998) found no differences for domestic (i. e., American) students on any SACQ variable, but did find that a small sample of foreign student-athletes had higher scores on academic, social, and overall 167 adjustment than foreign non-athlete students. However, the foreign athlete group was comprised largely of Canadian and western European students while the non-athlete group was made up largely of Asian students, so there were other variables than athletic status that could account for the observed adjustment differences. Comparing varsity student-athletes with student non-athletes at NCAA Division I and IAA universities, Melendez (2001) found a weak relation favoring the athletes on academic adjustment and institutional attachment. And, similarly, that investigator also obtained weak positive correlation between participation in “recreational,” non-varsity, sports (e.g., intramural and club sports) and social adjustment and institutional attachment. Looking only at student-athletes, and using the Athletic Identity Measurement Scale (AIMS; Brewer, Van Radte, & Linder, 1993) to measure the degree to which such students identify with the athletic role, Melendez (2001) found a modest negative correlation between that identity and personal-emotional adjustment. For non-minority student-athletes there was a moderate (-.41) correlation between identification with the athletic role and academic adjustment (the weaker the athletic identity the better the academic adjustment), but no relation between those two variables for minority student-athletes. 168 CHAPTER 11 ENVIRONMENTAL DETERMINANTS OF COLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: SIGNIFICANT AND/OR STRESSFUL LIFE EVENTS Some of the variables to be considered in this and several following chapters are environmental factors defined in terms more or less independent of the student and his or her particular characteristics, e.g., language spoken in the family, characteristics of dormitories, or types of living arrangements. Other variables to be considered, however, are defined in terms of student-reported perceptions and interpretations of their environment and environmental events, or interpersonal experiences, e.g., amount of conflict between parents, parenting styles, social support from friends, amount of prior interracial experience, romantic relationships. The focus in these latter variables is still on the environment, but the environment as perceived by the student. Thus, some of the variables may seem to shade into characteristics of persons, or at least to be influenced by characteristics of the student who is making the interpretation of, or interacting with, his/her environment. Significant and/or Stressful Life Events in General A number of different means of measuring significant and/or stressful life events in general that may be experienced by an individual have been employed in studies investigating determinants of adjustment to college. Such life events typically include a variety of changes in one’s life circumstances, as in place of residence; health of self or significant others; family composition and functioning; integrity of relationships; and educational/occupational/financial status. Studies reporting data for all SACQ variables, or all except the full scale, will be considered first, followed by those employing selected SACQ variables. 169 Using the Life Events Checklist (Johnson, 1982) with three student samples, Adan and Felner (1987) examined the association between three aspects of significant life events in general (negative impact rating, positive impact rating, and total number of significant life events in the previous two years) and all SACQ variables. Of 45 correlations generated, 12 were significant, some moderately strong, and in the expected direction (negative correlations for negative impact, positive correlations for positive impact, and negative correlations for total number), and were rather evenly distributed across the five SACQ variables and three life events indices. Also using all SACQ variables, Flescher (1986b) had somewhat stronger findings with variables defined much like Adan and Felner’s, but from the Life Experiences Survey (LES; Sarason, Johnson, & Siegel, 1978), 10 of 15 correlations reaching significance. Napoli and Wortman (1998) employed the negative impact rating from the LES, separately for life events in general and for life events in college, finding for the former measure weak to modest negative correlations with all SACQ subscales, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment, and for the latter measure much stronger correlations of similar magnitude across all SACQ subscales. Hogan (1986) employing the full scale score from the Psychological Distress Inventory (Lustman et al., 1984; a life stressor measure despite a somewhat misleading instrument name), and Adan & Felner (1987) using the Adolescent Hassles Scale (Farber & Felner, 1980), obtained findings very similar to those of Flescher (1986b; cited in previous paragraph) in terms of the frequency and magnitude of significant correlations with the several SACQ variables. Marcotte (1995) obtained fairly strong correlations between the Student-Oriented Life Events Survey (Grasley, 1992) and all SACQ subscales, as did M. D. Smith (1994) between the Brief College Student Hassles Scale (Blankstein, Flett, & Koledin, 1991) and all five SACQ variables. 170 Brooks and DuBois (1995), administering the Adolescent Perceived Events Scale (APES; Compas, Davis, Forsythe, & Wagner, 1987), obtained no correlations between SACQ variables and number of negative major life events experienced in the previous three months. However, they did get significant correlations in the expected negative direction between the occurrence of daily hassles as measured by the same instrument and the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and full scale. Posselt (1992) adapted the Social Readjustment Rating Scale (Holmes & Rahe, 1967) for use with college students, and found that the number of stressful events encountered in the previous year correlated negatively with personal-emotional adjustment. Moving now to studies employing selected SACQ indices, Natera (1998) used the Student Stress Scale (SSS; Insel & Roth, 1985), which was modeled after Holmes and Rahe’s (1967) Social Readjustment Rating Scale but did not include the component of the SSS that deals with anticipated stressors in the ensuing six months. Basically a checklist, the SSS has one score (Stress) involving the number of stressful events experienced in the past six months, and another (Perceived Stress) that involves rating of degree of stressfulness for each event experienced. Reporting data for only academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment, Natera found for the Stress score negative correlations with academic and personal-emotional adjustment, and for the Perceived Stress score stronger relation with all three SACQ variables, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. Administering both the College Student Life Events Schedule (CSLES; Sandler & Lakey, 1982) – which contains negative stressful events specific to the college experience – and the SACQ Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale in the third week of October (Time 1) and again 171 in the third week of November (Time 2), Gallant (1994) found negative correlations between the CSLES at both administrations and personal-emotional adjustment at both administrations. That is, CSLES-Time 1 predicted both the contemporaneously and subsequently administered SACQ variable, and CSLES-Time 2 predicted both the earlier and contemporaneously administered SACQ variable. Also employing a measure of negative stressful events specific to the college experience, the Brief College Student Hassles Scale (Blankstein, Flett, & Koledin, 1991), Hunsberger et al. (1996) found significant relation with the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable reported. Several studies employing only the SACQ full scale score and a stressor measure administered postmatriculation obtained significant expected direction findings. These include Maton and Weisman (1989) using their own life stress measure; Jarama Alvan, Belgrave and Zea (1996) with the overall stress score from the Life Experiences Survey (Sarason, Johnson, & Siegel, 1978), obtaining a moderately strong negative correlation for Latino students; and Bartels (1990) with the stress level scale from the Psychological Distress Inventory (Lustman et al., 1984). Overall, there seems to be ample evidence of relation in the expected direction between the reported experiencing of significant life events/stressors in general and adjustment to college, possibly seen most clearly or frequently in personal-emotional and overall adjustment. Studies to be considered next have narrowed the focus from life events/stressors in general to more specific life stressors. Relatively More Specific Life Stressors In their study of determinants of adjustment to college, Zamostny, Slyter and Rios (1993) 172 constructed the Early Trauma Checklist to identify four categories of trauma experienced in the first fifteen years of life (i. e., interpersonal loss; familial/personal/economic disruptions; parental dysfunction; and abuse), each of the four kinds of trauma represented by a subscale. Overall, significant negative correlations were found between incidence of such traumas and the SACQ subscales, most consistently and usually strongest for personal-emotional adjustment, and, among the four trauma categories, most frequently and usually strongest for abuse. Specific Early Trauma Checklist subscale findings are considered below where appropriate. Abuse. Zamostny et al.’s (1993) just-mentioned Early Trauma Checklist subscale measuring abuse experienced in the first fifteen years of life included three kinds of abuse -physical, psychological, and sexual – and it correlated negatively with all SACQ subscales. Marcotte (1995), employing the Parental Psychological Maltreatment and Parental Physical Maltreatment Scales (Briere & Runtz, 1988), found significant negative correlations with all SACQ subscales for both emotional and physical abuse experienced prior to age 16. A modification of the Parental Physical Maltreatment Scale to assess abuse inflicted by other significant figures (siblings, relatives, friends, neighbors) was found by Marcotte to yield weak but significant negative correlations with the Academic Adjustment and Attachment subscales. In Acunzo’s study (1989) freshman women who had experienced sexual abuse in childhood or adolescence scored lower than freshman women who had not experienced such abuse on all SACQ indices except the Academic Adjustment subscale, but no such differences were found at any of the upper class levels. Using the Childhood Sexual Experiences Questionnaire (Stinson & Hendrick, 1992) with an all-female sample, Marcotte (1995) obtained only three weak correlations out of twelve comparisons between the three SEQ subscales (sexual 173 experiences as a child with another child, as a child with an adult, and as a nonconsenting adolescent) and the four SACQ subscales, two of them for “as a nonconsenting adolescent” with social adjustment and institutional attachment. Perceived racial/ethnic discroinmination. Shibazaki (1999) found, with MexicanAmerican students, that perceived discrimination/racism as measured by the Racism and Life Experiences Scale-Brief Version (Harrell, 1997) was negatively related to personal-emotional adjustment. In another study, the greater the discrimination experienced in the college environment by sophomore Latino students, the lesser the institutional attachment (Hurtado et al., 1996). Relationship disruption. Zamostney et al.’s (1993) subscale cited above measuring interpersonal loss in the first fifteen years of life, from their Early Trauma Checklist, correlated negatively with the SACQ Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales. Hogan (1987/1988) used the Loss History Questionnaire – devised for her study – to measure success in having dealt with past interpersonal loss and found significant relation in the expected direction between that variable and academic, personal-emotional, and overall adjustment. The Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale of the SACQ was employed by Frazier and Cook (1993) to assess residual emotional distress in students who in the previous six months had experienced the break-up of a romantic relationship. The longer the relationship had lasted, the less was the residual distress, which seems somewhat paradoxical. Less paradoxical was the finding that the more the student still wanted to be back in the relationship, the greater was the residual distress. Cultural/geographic displacement. Huff (1998) was interested in studying the reverse 174 culture shock experienced by American missionaries’ children returning to their home country to attend college after having lived a considerable portion of their lives abroad. Compared with a control group of students similarly enrolled at two Christian colleges but who had not been living abroad, the “missionary kids” showed no difference on any of the SACQ variables. But the “missionary kids” with eleven or more transitions overseas (moves to a different dwelling place, a different country, or a boarding school) had better social adjustment to college than their counterparts with ten or fewer such transitions. Reeker (1994) obtained significant positive correlations between distance from home and social adjustment and institutional attachment, but Brooks and DuBois (1995) got a significant negative correlation between social adjustment and distance from home as estimated in miles by students. In five studies (Hogan, 1987/1988; Hurtado et al., 1996; Kline, 1992; Mooney, 1989/ Mooney et al., 1991; Schriver, 1996), however, there was no relation between distance from home and SACQ variables. Similarly, McCartney (1992) and Ridinger (1998) found no differences between in-state and out-of-state student athletes on any SACQ variable, as did Ridinger between in-state and out-of-state non-athlete students. However, Mooney (see also Mooney et al.) interestingly reports significant positive correlations, the highest with the Social Adjustment subscale, between all SACQ indices and perceived distance from home, with perceived distance rated in ascending scores from "too far" to "just right." Natural disaster. Injejikian (1995) was interested in assessing the consequences of a major earthquake for adjustment to college. Samples of students at two California State universities were administered the SACQ four months after students at one of the institutions had experienced a severe, devastating earthquake, while students at the other institution had not. The 175 only SACQ variable showing a significant difference between the two institutions was social adjustment, with earthquake-experiencing students having the lower scores. Injejikian felt that this difference might simply have been due to the physical destruction of the affected campus precluding normal, previously enjoyed, social activities. 176 CHAPTER 12 ENVIRONMENTAL DETERMINANTS OF COLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: FAMILY CHARACTERISTICS A number of investigators have examined the relation between family characteristics, as distinct from certain family events discussed in the previous chapter, and adjustment to college as measured by the SACQ. One such characteristic is family functioning in general, while others refer to specific aspects of family functioning. Family Functioning in General Hollmann and Metzler (1994)/ Hollmann (1995) report that the more positive a student's perception of his or her family’s health or functioning, defined generally in terms of fostering autonomy and intimacy as measured by the total score of the Family of Origin Scale (FOS; Hovestadt, Anderson, Piercy, Cochran, & Fine, 1985), the better the adjustment in all areas tapped by the SACQ. Roberts (1995) found similar relation between that same measure and social and personal-emotional adjustment. Using another total score reflecting perceived general effectiveness of one’s family’s functioning, based on 15 subscales measuring both positive and negative aspects of family functioning (the Family Functioning Scales; Bloom, 1985), Buelow (1990) found a positive correlation with the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable reported. The Family Health/Competence subscale of the Self-Report Family Inventory-Version II (Beavers & Hampson, 1990) was found by Hutto (1998) to correlate in the expected direction with all SACQ variables, and by Clauss (1995) with the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales, the only SACQ variables employed. The General Functioning score from 177 the McMaster Family Assessment Device (Epstein, Baldwin, & Bishop, 1983), designed to measure the ability of a family to function effectively as a unit, was found by Pappas (2000) to correlate positively with all SACQ variables, strongest among the subscales with academic adjustment. Specific Aspects of Family Functioning Instead of employing only the total score from the Family of Origin Scale (FOS; Hovestadt, Anderson, Piercy, Cochran, & Fine, 1985), as did Hollmann and Metzler (1994) and Roberts (1995) cited above, Hutto (1998) reported findings for that instrument’s ten subscales that address the effectiveness of different aspects of familiy functioning. Five of those subscales concern a family’s characteristics thought to encourage development of autonomy in its members: clarity of expression, personal responsibility, respect for other family members, openness to other family members, and dealing openly with separation and loss. The other five FOS subscales focus on characteristics thought to encourage development of intimacy: expression of feelings, warm home atmosphere, handling conflict without undue stress, promoting sensitivity in family members, and trusting in the goodness of human nature. Positive correlations were found between all ten FOS subscales and all five SACQ variables. Familial fostering of development of autonomy. As noted in the foregoing paragraph, the five subscales from the Family of Origin Scale (FOS; Hovestadt, Anderson, Piercy, Cochran, & Fine, 1985) intended to assess a family’s encouragement of development of autonomy in its members were all found by Hutto (1998) to be positively correlated with all SACQ variables. Also interested in measuring families’ encouragement of the development of independence in its children, Kenny (1990, 1993, 1994) included in her Parental Attachment 178 Questionnaire (PAQ) a subscale called Parent Viewed as Fostering Autonomy. That variable has been found to fairly regularly show correlations in the expected positve direction with the several SACQ variables (Hutto, 1998; Vivona, 2000b), including for minority students (Kenny, 1993; Pfeil, 2000) and missionary children homecomers (Huff, 1998). Amin (2000) reports that high scoring students on that PAQ variable have better academic, social, and personal-emotional adjustment than low scoring students. Clauss (1995) obtained similar findings between that same PAQ variable and the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales of the SACQ, the only SACQ variables employed. Using the Parents Viewed as Fostering Autonomy variable from the PAQ as modified by Kenny and Perez (1996) for use with different cultural groups by substituting for parents in the instructions any family member to whom the student is most likely to turn for support, Kalsner-Silver (2000) obtained values very similar to those described above. Fathers’ encouragement of their daughters’ independence as measured by the Perceived Parental Attitude Scale (Koutralakos, 1971) was found by Wang & Smith (1993) to be positively related to all SACQ indices except the Academic Adjustment subscale. Family cohesion and related variables. Another specific characteristic of family functioning that has been studied by several investigators using several different means of measurement is family cohesion. Considerable evidence has been produced indicating that the more cohesive the student's family, as perceived by the student, the better the student’s adjustment to college. Two studies (Garner, 1986/Stoltenberg et al., 1986; and Walker, 1996) using the Family Cohesion subscale from the FACES-II Scale (Olson, Sprenkle, & Russell, 1979) have found positive correlation with all SACQ indices. 179 Three studies have employed the Family Cohesion subscale from the FACES-III Scale (Olson, Portner, & Lavee, 1985). Lapsley (1989)/Rice et al. (1990) found significant correlations with all SACQ indices. Reporting data for only the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales of the SACQ, Lapsley and Edgerton (1999b) obtained a modest correlation with the former variable. In Butler and Ginsburg’s study (1989) a group of students with higher FACESIII family cohesion scores had better SACQ-measured academic, personal-emotional and overall adjustment than a group with middle-range cohesion scores, while a group with low cohesion scores was not significantly different from the other two categories of students on those same SACQ variables. Three studies have used the Family Cohesion subscale from the Family Environment Scale (FES; Moos & Moos, 1981). Positive correlations were gotten by Lapsley (1989)/ Rice et al. (1990) and Hopkins (1998) with all SACQ indices, while Caplan (1996/1997) reported positive correlation with the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable employed. Hutto (1998) obtained correlations in the expected direction between the Cohesion subscale of the Self-Report Family Inventory-Version II (Beavers & Hampson, 1990) and all SACQ variables, strongest for personal-emotional adjustment among the subscales, while for Clauss (1995) there was correlation with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale but not the Social Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variables employed. The reader will recall that Hutto (1998) also used the Family of Origin Scale (FOS; Hovestadt, Anderson, Piercy, Cochran, & Fine, 1985) which has five subscales intended to assess characteristics of families thought to promote development of intimacy in the family members. The FOS definition of the concept of intimacy and its measurement seem to be rather 180 synonymous with the concept of cohesion, and was found to be positively correlated with all SACQ variables. Kline (1992) focused upon a particular familial characteristic – i. e., engagement in ritualized, routinized, or habitual family behaviors involving considerable interaction among members -- that very likely could be related to, or contribute to, family cohesion. That characteristic, identified as family ritualization, refers to the degree to which a family participates in regularized “group” behaviors associated with dining, relaxation/vacationing, celebration of special occasions (birthdays and other anniversaries, family reunions, holidays), and religious and cultural activities, etc. These kinds of activities are thought to provide order, stability, security, and comfort to family life and to the family’s members, establishing a basis in its members for capacity to adapt to new and potentially difficult circumstances, as might be encountered for example in the college experience. The Family Ritual Questionnaire (FRQ; Fiese & Kline, 1992) was devised to assess various aspects of family ritualization which, when factor analyzed, yielded one factor, called Ritualization. The variable Ritualization was found by Kline (1992) to correlate in the expected positive direction with all SACQ indices except personal-emotional adjustment for female students but not with any SACQ variable for male students. Also possibly closely related to, or contributing to, family cohesion are two additional family social climate variables from the Family Environment Scale (FES; Moos & Moos, 1981): Activity-Recreation (participation as a family in social and recreational activities) and Organization (degree of organization of family activities). Regarding Activity-Recreation, Lapsley (1989)/ Rice et al. (1990) obtained positive correlations with social adjustment, 181 institutional attachment, and overall college adjustment, Hopkins (1998) with all SACQ variables. Regarding Organization, Lapsley/Rice at al. found positive correlations with all SACQ variables, Hopkins with academic and overall adjustment. Possibly viewable as a pathological form of family cohesion, as in excessive cohesiveness, would be family fear of separation/individuation in its members as measured by the Family Structure Survey (FSS; Lopez et al., 1988b). The FSS’ authors found that variable to be negatively correlated in separate samples of male and female students with all SACQ subscales. For Grella (1989) that same variable was negatively correlated with personalemotional and overall adjustment to college, and for Kenny (1995) with academic adjustment. Finally, concerning variables possibly related to family cohesion, Ropar (1997) found no relation with any of the SACQ indices for measures of enmeshment/disengagement and parentchild cohesion from the Structural Family Interaction Scale-Revised (Perosa & Perosa, 1990a&b). Family adaptability. Two studies (Garner, 1986/Stoltenberg et al., 1986; and Walker, 1996) used the Family Adaptability subscale from the FACES-II Scale (Olson, Sprenkle, & Russell, 1979), which measures the student-perceived capacity of his/her family to be flexible in the face of changing circumstances as in appropriately altering its structure, roles, and rules. Both studies found positive correlations with all SACQ indices in very similar magnitude as those cited above for the FACES-II Family Cohesion subscale. Three studies employed the Family Adaptability subscale from the FACES-III Scale (Olson, Portner, & Lavee, 1985), but with considerably less success than the same named subscale from FACES-II. Lapsley (1989)/Rice et al. (1990) obtained modest correlations only 182 with the Academic and Social Adjustment subscales. Reporting data just for the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales of the SACQ, Lapsley and Edgerton (1999b) found no relation with either SACQ variable. In Butler and Ginsburg’s study (1989) there were no differences on any SACQ variable among groups of students scoring high, middle-range, and low in perceived adaptability of their families. Ropar (1997), using the Structural Family Interaction Scale-Revised (Perosa & Perosa, 1990a&b), found no relation with any SACQ variable for that instrument’s subscale measuring family adaptability. Hopkins (1998) obtained negative correlations between the Family Environment Scale’s (Moos & Moos, 1981) Control subscale – measuring the extent to which fixed rules and procedures govern family life, semingly the opposite of adaptability – and personal-emotional and overall adjustment, but there were no significant correlations with any SACQ variable for Lapsley (1989)/Rice et al. (1990). Family/parental support. Using the Perceived Social Support from Family Scale (Procidano & Heller, 1983), Bartels (1995), Just (1998), and Birnie-Lefcovitch (1997) – the lastmentioned investigator administering the instrument both pre- and postmatriculation -- all obtained positive correlations with all SACQ indices, as did Maton (1989a) with the Social Adjustment subscale but not the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, the only two SACQ variables reported. In Birnie-Lefcovitch’s study the correlations with all SACQ indices were stronger for the postmatriculation than the prematriculation administrations, as would be expected. There seemed to be some slight indication in the foregoing studies of stronger association for this kind of social support with academic adjustment rather than the other aspects 183 of adjustment to college. But two investigators (Caro, 1985/1986; Hogan, 1986) found no relation between that scale and the SACQ. Kenny and Stryker (1994), with a measure of social support from family derived from Hays and Oxley (1986) and Martin and Burks (1985), obtained significant relation between it and the SACQ Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales for a sample of students of color (Afro-Americans, Asians, and Latinos) but not for White students. Jarama Alvan et al. (1996) found positive correlation for Latino students between a measure of satisfaction with support from family/kin, from the Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule (Barrera, 1981), and the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable employed. Hurtado et al. (1996) report that students who describe themselves as maintaining family relationships and obtaining support therefrom have better personal-emotional adjustment. But Humfleet (1987) found no relation between self-reported amount of contact with family and the full scale score of the SACQ, the only SACQ variable used; and Gilkey (1988; see also Protinsky & Gilkey, 1996) reported the same finding for amount of contact with family/parents. Using a measure of satisfaction with one’s relationship with family from the Social Network Questionnaire (SNQ; Hays & Oxley, 1986), an instrument intended to assess the structure and function of sources of social support, Harris (1988) found positive correlation with all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment. Interestingly enough, however, Harris and also Serafica et al. (1990), using a measure from the SNQ of the extent to which students’ social networks are composed of family/relatives, obtained negative correlations with SACQ variables; that is, the more that a student’s social network consists of family/relatives, the poorer the adjustment to college. For Serafica et al. there were significant correlations with all 184 SACQ indices except academic adjustment, and for Harris with academic, personal-emotional and overall adjustment. Moving now from perceived social support from family in general to parents in particular, Yaffe (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) used the Social Provisions Scale-Parent Version (SPS-P; Cutrona, 1989) – administered in the first week of the fall term -- and found significant positive correlations between its total score and all variables from the SACQ administered the following February and March. The Parent Viewed as Providing Emotional Support subscale from the Parental Attachment Questionnaire (PAQ; Kenny (1990, 1993, 1994) correlates fairly regularly in the expected positve direction with the several SACQ variables (Hutto, 1998; Vivona, 2000b), including for minority students (Kenny, 1993; Pfeil, 2000) and missionary children homecomers (Huff, 1998). Clauss (1995) found significant correlation for that variable with the Social but not Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales of the SACQ, the only SACQ variables employed. Using the Parent Viewed as Providing Emotional Support variable from the PAQ as modified by Kenny and Perez (1996) for use with different cultural groups by substituting for parents in the instructions any family member to whom the student is most likely to turn for support, KalsnerSilver (2000) obtained values similar to those described above. The Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI; Parker, Tupling, & Brown, 1979), designed to yield a retrospective assessment of parental contribution to the establishment of the parent-child bond, as perceived by the child, has a subscale to measure caringness (expression of warmth, affection, and empathy, as opposed to indifference/rejection) that may be seen as a particular sort of social support from parents. The measure can be taken for both parents combined or 185 separately. For parents combined, McAndrew-Miller (1989) found correlations of approximately equal magnitude between PBI-assessed caringness and all SACQ subscales. In Bailey & Shilkret’s (2000b) study, correlations of similar magnitude were obtained between mother caringness and all SACQ variables, but no significant values for father caringness. Rice, Cunningham, and Young (1997) found significant correlations for both mother and father caringness with the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales – the only SACQ variables reported – for both White and Black students and men and women. Using only the same two SACQ subscales, Lapsley and Edgerton (1999b) reported significant correlation for father caringness with personal-emotional adjustment and for neither father nor mother caringness with social adjustment. Lopez (1997) obtained a modest correlation between the mother caringness index, but not father caringness, and the Attachment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. Caro (1985/1986), however, found no relation between the PBI Care subscale, both parents combined, and any SACQ variable. Coatsworth (2001) employed self-designed measures of three kinds of student-perceived social support from parents – emotional (encouragement and sympathy), instrumental (advice), and financial – with students whose parents were divorced. Perceived instrumental and emotional support from fathers showed no relation with any of the SACQ indices of college adjustment, but financial support from the father was positively and modestly to moderately associated with all SACQ variables. For the three kinds of support provided by the mother, the only significant correlation was a positive one between receipt of instrumental support and personal-emotional adjustment. 186 Other fairly direct evidence concerning the role of parental caringness as a form of social support from within the family comes from Wang & Smith’s (1993) study that focused on the fathers' role in relation to daughters. Daughters' perceptions of their fathers' expression of affection as measured by the Father-Daughter Relationship Inventory (FDRI: Brunig, 1983) was correlated in expected direction with all SACQ indices, as was the daughters' perception of amount of time and attention devoted to her by the father, except in the case of the Academic Adjustment subscale. Using a variable possibly closely akin to the parental caringness construct, Mendelson (1987/1988) found that recollections of the parents as loving in early interactions with the student, as measured by the Love-Reject Factor score from the Parent-Child Relations Questionnaire II (Siegelman & Roe, 1979), predicted the SACQ Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. The Leadership subscale from the Self-Report Family Inventory-Version II (Beavers & Hampson, 1990) is intended to measure the strength and consistency of adult (presumably primarily parental) leadership within the family, which may be regarded as a form of parrental support. It was found by Hutto (1998) to be correlated modestly in the expected direction with all SACQ variables except academic adjustment. Several studies provide somewhat practical, non-test measured, evidences of social support from parents as playing a role in adjustment in college. In Wick and Shilkret's study (1986a), students who regarded their parents as more involved in their college careers in supportive and caring ways had higher SACQ full scale scores – the only SACQ variable employed in this analysis -- than students who described their parents as cold and distant. 187 Consistent with Wick and Shilkret’s finding, in Yaffe’s (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) study the more prematriculation discussion that students reported having had with their parents concerning various aspects of the impending college experience, the higher were the scores on all SACQ variables. Similarly, Hunsberger et al. (1996) obtained a weak positive correlation between that variable and the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable reported. Martin et al. (2000) report a positive correlation between a self-designed one-item rating of parental support and the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable employed. On the assumption that there is such a thing as excessive social support, including within the family, some research is cited here that may qualify for that category. Lopez et al. (1988b) were interested in maladaptive family interaction, for which they developed the Family Structure Survey (FSS) to measure different aspects of that kind of interaction. One FSS subscale was designed to tap parent-child overinvolvement, suggested here as a form of excessive social support. Lopez et al. found significant correlations in the expected negative direction between that subscale and all four SACQ subscales for male students and all subscales except academic adjustment for female students. Another measure of excessive social support within the family, and a variable seemingly closely related to parent-child overinvolvement, is the Protective (or overprotective, versus encouragement of autonomy and independence) subscale of the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI; Parker, Tupling, & Brown, 1979), which has been used by several investigators in relation to the SACQ. The subscale assesses excessive control and intrusiveness, and may be taken for the parents combined or separately. For parents combined, McAndrew-Miller (1989) found negative correlations with all 188 SACQ subscales. Bailey and Shilkret (2000b) obtained only one significant correlation for mother protectiveness -- with personal-emotional adjustment, negative in valence – and none for father protectiveness. For Rice, Cunningham, and Young (1997), both mother and father overprotection correlated in the expected negative direction with the Social and PersonalEmotional Adjustment subscales – the only SACQ variables employed – for white and black students and also men and women except in the case of father protectiveness with male students for social adjustment. Lapsley and Edgerton (1999b) obtained negative correlations with paternal but not maternal overprotection for both social and personal-emotional adjustment, the only SACQ variables employed. But Lopez (1997) found no relation between mother or father protectiveness as measured by the PBI and the SACQ Attachment subscale, the only SACQ variable used, and Caro (1985/1986) found no relation between the PBI combined parental protectiveness index and any SACQ variable. Using yet another variable possibly similar to parent-child overinvolvement and parental overprotectiveness, and to excessive social support as well, Wick and Shilkret (1986a) obtained a significant relation in the expected direction between perceived maternal intrusiveness (Aries, Olver, & Batgos, 1985) and scores on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. Morray and Shilkret (2001, 2002) employed the same basic measure of maternal intrusiveness as Wick and Shilkret, now slightly revised and called the Permeability of Boundaries Scale (Olver et al., 1989), and added seven college-related items of their own to augment the original instrument which had focused on the student’s experience in the family home. That new measure yielded modest negative correlations with all SACQ variables except social adjustment; the more intrusive the mother, the poorer the student’s adjustment to college. 189 Another possible manifestation of pathology regarding social support within a family could be parent-child role reversal. Lopez et al. (1988b) included in their instrument for measuring maladaptive family interaction, the Family Structure Survey, a subscale intended to assess that variable. For male students Lopez et al. found negative correlations between their Parent-Child Role-Reversal subscale and all SACQ subscales except social adjustment, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment, and for female students there was significant (negative) correlation with personal-emotional adjustment only. The Rejection Expectancy subscale from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence (SITA; Levine, Green, & Millon, 1986) includes items reflecting perception of one’s parents as rejecting, callous, indifferent, or even hostile, which could be viewed as representing the opposite of social support within the family. It has been found by Cooler (1995) and Lapsley (1989)/Rice et al. (1990) to yield consistently negative correlations of similar magnitude with all SACQ subscales (and the full scale score for Rice et al./Lapsley, which was not employed by Cooler), moderately strong for Cooler. Familial/parental discord. Degree of conflict within a student’s family has been studied by several investigators for its relation to adjustment to college. The Conflict subscale from the Family Environment Scale (Moos & Moos, 1981) was found by Rice et al. (1990)/Lapsley (1989) and Hopkins (1998; see also Feenstra, Banyard, Rines, & Hopkins, 2001) to correlate negatively with all SACQ indices, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment, while for Caplan (1996/1997) it did so with the full scale, the only SACQ variable employed. The Conflict subscale from the Self-Report Family Inventory-Version II (Beavers & Hampson, 1990), employed by Hutto (1998), correlated in the expected direction with all SACQ variables. Using 190 the Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus, 1979), Reeker (1994) found the expected negative relation between amount of family conflict and only the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. In Albert's (1988) study, students reporting that there had been little conflict in their families had significantly higher scores on all four SACQ subscales than did students indicating great family conflict. Wick and Shilkret (1986a) report that student-perceived parental and familial difficulties were less evident among students who were high on the SACQ full scale score – the only SACQ variable cited --than students who were low. Several investigators report findings from studies that focused less on the family in general than on the parents in particular. Using the Marital Conflict subscale from their Family Structure Survey (FSS), Lopez et al. (1988b) found that it correlated negatively with the SACQ Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales for male students, and with the Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales for female students, strongest with personalemotional adjustment in both instances (data for the SACQ full scale were not reported). Lopez et al. (1989) subsequently reported significant relations in the expected direction between that same FSS subscale and all four SACQ subscales, but Lopez (1991) obtained significant relation only with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. In yet another article, Lopez (1989) reported no significant correlation between the FSS marital conflict score and the Academic Adjustment subscale from the SACQ, the only SACQ variable reported in that study. Grella (1989) found the FSS Marital Conflict subscale to correlate negatively with the SACQ Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and full scale. Kenny (1995), administering the FSS in the fall and the SACQ the following spring, found correlation only with personalemotional adjustment. 191 Using the total score from the Children’s Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale (CPIC; Grych, Seid, & Finchman, 1992), Bailey and Shilkret (2000b) found no significant correlation with any SACQ variable. Ropar (1997) obtained no relation between a measure of parental conflict resolution from the Structural Family Interaction Scale-Revised (Perosa & Perosa, 1990a&b) and any SACQ variable. Relevant to the issue of familial/parental discord are studies that examine the relation between parental divorce and student adjustment to college. Of studies that simply compare students of divorced and non-divorced parents, only a minority have found differences. Allen (1989)/Allen and Stoltenberg (1990) report that students from intact families had higher scores than students of divorced parents on the Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and the full scale. Matthews (1992), giving information only for the SACQ full scale, found higher scores for students from intact families than for students whose parents were divorced for three years or more. Interestingly, while Pfeil (2000) found no SACQ differences between students of still married parents and divorced parents, students’ whose parents were separated had poorer personal-emotional and overall college adjustment than those in the other two categories. Expanding somewhat the category of family disruption but still with primary – though not exclusive -- emphasis on discord, students in Albert's (1988) study whose parents were still alive and married had higher Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale scores than students whose parents were deceased, divorced or separated. However, Allen (1985, 1986), Allen, Stoltenberg, and Rosko (1990), Bailey and Shilkret (2000a), Dewein (1994), Levin (1996), Lopez et al. (1988a), Montgomery and Haemmerlie (2001), Reeker (1994), Roberts (1995), Schwitzer and Robbins (1986) and Yaffe (1997; see also 192 Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) all obtained no SACQ differences between students of divorced and nondivorced parents. Hazzard-Patterson (1999) found no differences on any SACQ subscales between students from maritally intact families and those whose parents were divorced or separated. Feenstra et al. (2001) reported no differences on any SACQ variables (excluding Institutional Attachment, which was not employed) between students whose parents were married (and not separated) and those whose parents were divorced. Bailey and Shilkret’s study included the finding of no SACQ differences between female students from non-divorce families and those from post-divorce families in either maternal or joint parental custody. Among students of divorced parents, Allen (1989) found that the older the student at the time of the divorce, the better the social adjustment in college. But Dewein (1994) found no relation between students’ age at the time of parental divorce and any of the SACQ subscales, as did Levin (1996) for the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable reported. As another kind of difference among students of divorced parents, two studies have focused on post-divorce family structure. Flescher et al. (1986) found no differential effects in SACQ scores between students whose mother remained single and those whose mother remarried, as did Bailey and Shilkret (2000a) between students in maternal or joint parental custody. The quantity and quality of postdivorce parent-student relationships have been examined. Allen (1989) found no significant relation between SACQ variables and amount of time spent with either the custodial or noncustodial parent for female students, or with the custodial parent for male students, but for male students the more the contact with the noncustodial parent the 193 better the social adjustment and institutional attachment in college. Then, looking at quality rather than amount of time spent with parents postdivorce, Allen found, for female students, no significant correlations between that variable and any SACQ indices in the case of the noncustodial parent, but in the case of the custodial parent the more highly rated the quality of time spent the better the institutional attachment and social and overall adjustment. For male students, the more highly rated the quality of time spent with both the custodial and the noncustodial parent, the better the social adjustment and institutional attachment in college. Flescher et al. (1986) describe somewhat complicated findings concerning quality of postdivorce parent-child relationship as a function of both post-divorce family structure and sex of the student. Specifically, for male students from mother-remarried families, the quality of the mother-son relationship was positively correlated with the Attachment and Academic Adjustment subscales. Flescher (1986b) also investigated the interplay of social network characteristics, postdivorce family structure, and sex of student in relation to adjustment to college. As a principal finding, he identifies a different pattern of correlations between proportion of kin in social networks and the SACQ Attachment subscale for male and female students from singleparent, mother-custody families. A positive association is reported for females, a negative one for males. Family’s means of coping with familial discord/distress. The Family Crisis Oriented Personal Evaluation Scales (F-Copes; McCubbin, Larsen, & Olson, 1985) aim at measuring a student’s assessment of his/her family’s problem-solving attitudes and skills in dealing with discord/distress that it experiences. It has five subscales, each identifying a means of coping. 194 Hopkins (1998) found that Reframing, or redefining stressful events to make them more manageable, correlated positively with all SACQ variables; and Passive Appraisal, defined as accepting problematic issues and thus minimizing reactivity, correlated positively with academic, personal-emotional, and overall adjustment. Acquiring Social Support (as from relatives, friends, neighbors) had modest correlations in the expected positive direction with the SACQ Social Adjustment and Institutional Attachment subscales and full scale, and Acquiring Help from Community Resources also correlated with social adjustment and institutional attachment, but Seeking Spiritual Support showed no relation with any SACQ variable. The total score from the F-COPES correlated with all SACQ variables, strongest with the Social Adjustment and Institutional Attachment subscales and the full scale (see also Feenstra et al., 2001, for findings with the F-Copes total score). Hopkins concluded that families with more effective problemsolving skills produce students who are better equipped to adjust well to college. Parenting styles. Parenting styles as measured by the Parental Authority Questionnaire (PAQ; Buri, 1991), through students’ retrospective reports of their growing-up years, were examined in relation to adjustment to college by Shilkret and Vecchiotti (1995, 1997), Shilkret and Edwards (1997), and Shilkret (2000) using female samples in all three studies (reporting data for all SACQ variables); by Wintre and Sugar (2000; see also Sugar, 1997) using separate samples of women and men (reporting data for the SACQ subscales only); and by Yaffe (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) and Pappas (2000) with combined male and female samples (reporting data for all SACQ variables). The Wintre and Sugar sample (n = 357) was contained in the Yaffe sample (close to 400 students), so the principal difference between those two studies in relation to parenting style is their treatment of men and women subjects separately or 195 combined. Mothers’ authoritative style (providing clear and firm direction with warmth, reasonableness, and flexibility) correlated positively with all SACQ variables for Shilkret and Vecchiotti (1995), Shilkret (2000), and Yaffe (1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000); with all SACQ subscales except institutional attachment in women and academic adjustment in men for Wintre and Sugar (2000; see also Sugar, 1997); and with social, personal-emotional and overall adjustment for Shilkret and Edwards (1997). Fathers’ authoritative style correlated positively with all SACQ variables for Yaffe (1997); with all SACQ variables except institutional attachment for Shilkret and Vecchiotti (1995) and Shilkret (2000); for Wintre and Sugar (2000; see also Sugar, 1997), with all SACQ subscales except institutional attachment in women, and all except academic adjustment in men; but Shilkret and Edwards (1997) found no relation between that style variable and adjustment to college. The results of these studies suggest that mother authoritativeness may be a little more strongly associated with students’ adjustment to college than father authoritativeness, and that parental authoritativeness may be less related to students’ institutional attachment than to other aspects of college adjustment. Furthermore, there is evidence from Wintre and Sugar’s study (2000; see also Sugar, 1997) that both mother and father authoritativeness predicts institutional attacment better for male than for female students, and that father authoritativeness also predicts other aspects of college adjustment better for males than females. Instead of asking students to rate parents separately for authoritativeness, Pappas (2000) had them assess the parent whom they regarded as having been the more influential in their 196 upbringing; or, if parents were viewed as having been equally influential, they should be rated as a unit. Significant positive correlations were obtained between such ratings and all SACQ variables, strongest among the subscales with academic adjustment. Mothers’ authoritarian style (highly directive with expectation of unquestioning obedience; lacking in warmth; punitive) was negatively related: to all SACQ variables except institutional attachment for Yaffe (1997); to academic, personal-emotional and overall adjustment in Shilkret and Vecchiotti’s (1995) and Shilkret’s (2000) studies; to academic and personal-emotional adjustment in women and social adjustment only in men for Wintre and Sugar (2000; also see Sugar, 1997); and to personal-emotional and overall adjustment for Shilkret and Edwards (1997). Fathers’ authoritarian style was negatively related: to all SACQ variables except institutional attachment for Yaffe (1997); to academic, personal-emotional and overall adjustment for Shilkret (2000); to social, personal-emotional and overall adjustment for Shilkret and Vecchiotti (1995); and to academic and personal-emotional adjustment for women and social and personal-emotional adjustment for men in Wintre and Sugar’s (2000; see also Sugar, 1997) study. Shilkret and Edwards (1997) found no correlations between that style variable and any of the SACQ variables. Pappas (2000), using her method decribed above for rating parents but now for authoritarianism, found no correlation between that variable and any of the SACQ indices. All studies concerning parental authoritarianism taken together indicate that its strongest relation among the SACQ subscales is with personal-emotional adjustment, but that it predicts institutional attachment not at all. Furthermore, there seems to be no appreciable difference in 197 correlation with SACQ variables between mother and father authoritarianism, and the degree of association for authoritarianism with college adjustment may be somewhat weaker than it was for authoritativeness. Regarding a third parenting style identified by the PAQ, permissiveness on the part of the mother and father, Wintre and Sugar (2000; see also Sugar, 1997) and Yaffe (1997) found no significant correlations with any SACQ variable. But Shilkret and Vecchiotti (1995), Shilkret and Edwards (1997), and Shilkret (2000) divided Buri’s (1991) permissive category into permissive-indulgent and permissive-neglectful. Permissive-indulgence (relative absence of discipline, predicated on principles of free expression and independence, with warmth and caring), on the part of either mother or father, was found in all three studies to be unrelated to any of the SACQ variables except for a weak positive correlation in relation to the father on academic adjustment in the most recent of the three studies. However, permissive-neglectfulness (relative absence of discipline due to parental uninvolvement, uncaringness and detachment) in Shilkret and Vecchiotti’s (1995) and Shilkret’s (2000) studies yielded the same pattern and approximate magnitude of correlations as in their findings regarding the authoritative style, but negative in valence. Shilkret and Edwards found significant negative correlations between mother and father permissive-neglectfulness and personal-emotional and overall adjustment only. Pappas (2000), using her method decribed above for rating parents but now for permissiveness (as traditionally defined, not divided as to type), found no correlation between that variable and any of the SACQ indices. Shilkret and Vecchiotti (1997), in a re-examination of their data, assigned students to groups on the basis of parenting style experienced, and employed analysis of variance to test for 198 differences between them. Those who had had authoritative parenting had higher scores on all SACQ variables than students who had had either authoritarian or permissive-neglectful parenting, and higher Academic Adjustment subscale scores than students experiencing permissive-indulgent parenting. Students who had had permissive-indulgent parenting reported better personal-emotional adjustment than those with authoritarian parents, and better social and overall adjustment than students with permissive-neglectful parents. Shilkret and Edwards (1997) also used analysis of variance to examine adjustment differences among students grouped according to parenting style experienced, asserting reaffirmation of the more favorable consequences of authoritative parenting style and the importance of analyzing effects in the different areas of adjustment separately for mothers and fathers. They report that students with authoritative fathers showed better academic adjustment than those with permissive-indulgent fathers, and that students with authoritative mothers had better personal-emotional adjustment than those with authoritarian, permissive-indulgent, or permissive-neglectful mothers. Beyers (2001) used a different means of identifying parenting styles for comparison with SACQ variables. Factor analyzing the Children’s Report on Parent Behavior for Older Children and Adolescents (CRPBI-30; Schludermann & Schludermann, 1988), a shorter version of a scale originally developed by Schaefer (1965), Beyers established three factor/subscales focusing on student evaluation of their parents’ parenting behaviors: Acceptance (parents responsive and involved); Firm Control (parents use direct means of control of their child’s behavior); and Psychological Control (parents employ covert psychological methods of control, like guilt induction and excessive pressure for change). Firm Control was expected to have positive 199 consequences for a child’s development and Psychological Control negative consequences. Beyers (2001) then used patterns of those subsccales’ scores (low, moderate, high) to construct four parenting style groups: authoritative, permissive (much like permissive-indulgent as described above), uninvolved (much like permissive-neglectful described above), and authoritarian. Reporting findings for the SACQ full scale only, Beyers found that students with authoritarian parents scored lower that those in the other three categories, and those with permissive (i.e., permissive-indulgent) parents scored highr than those with uninvolved (i.e., permissive-neglectful) parents. Relationship patterns within the family. Several investigators have sought connection between relationship patterns within the family and adjustment to college, singularly without success. Garner (1986)/Stoltenberg et al. (1986) found no SACQ differences between students who describe the primary relationship in the family as between themselves and a parent and those who describe the primary relationship as between the parents. No correlation was obtained by Ropar (1997) between formation of cross-generational coalitions from the Structural Family Interaction Scale-Revised (Perosa & Perosa, 1990) and any SACQ subscale. Using only the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale of the SACQ, Levin (1996) found no difference between freshman women from divorced families reporting a primary mother-daughter relationship and freshman women from intact families with a similar primary relationship. For Lopez et al. (1989) similarly there were no differences among groups of students categorized according to different forms of intra-familial coalitions constructed from student perceptions of intra-familial behaviors. 200 Family roles. Relevant to the notion of relationship patterns within the family, but with a mode of analysis that focuses as much on the behavioral characteristics of individual family members – especially the children – as on the characteristics of the family as a functioning unit, is the concept of roles engendered within the family. Cooler (1995) investigated the relation between roles played by students as members of their family and adjustment to college, employing the Family Role Behavior Inventory (FRBI; Verdiano, Peterson, & Hicks, 1990), an instrument devised to measure behaviors associated with a typology of family roles created by Wegscheider-Cruse (1989). The Family Hero role (organized, dependable, conscientious, achieving, conforming, mature, helpful, “together”) was positively correlated with academic and social adjustment and institutional attachment, strongest with academic adjustment. The Lost Child role (emotionally vulnerable, quiet, introverted, withdrawn, passive, socially isolated, low self-esteem, average to poor adjustment in general) was negatively related to all aspects of college adjustment measured by the SACQ, strongest for personal-emotional and social adjustment. There were no significant correlations for the Scapegoat role (characterized by acting-out, anti-social, defiant, nonconforming, “problemed” behaviors), for which the sample size was very small, or Mascot role (energetic, entertaining, comical, outgoing, sociable). An alternate instrument developed by Cooler to measure these family roles produced significant correlations only in the case of the Hero role for the same SACQ variables as in the case of the FRBI, but of lesser magnitude. With very similar definitions of the Wegscheider-Cruse (1989) roles as employed by Cooler (1995), but measured by the Children’s Role Inventory (Potter & Williams, 1991), Ropar (1997) found significant positive correlations with all SACQ subscales for the Hero role, still 201 strongest with academic adjustment. And there were negative correlations with personalemotional adjustment and institutional attachment for the Lost Child role; negative correlations with all SACQ subscales except social adjustment for the Scapegoat role; and no significant correlations for the Mascot role. Buelow (1990) also used the Wegscheider-Cruse (1989) typology of family roles played by students, but with an instrument – the Role Relationship Inventory (RRI) – devised by himself to assess the roles. And he had three subject samples: students from alcohol- or drug-abusing families; students from dysfunctional or disrupted, but not alcohol- or drug-abusing families; and students from normal, non-drug-abusing, non-dysfunctional or disrupted families. Of 48 possible correlations (three samples, four roles, and the four SACQ subscales), Buelow (1990) found 15 to be significant, all negative except for one weak positive one (the Mascot role on social adjustment in normal families). The negative correlations occurred with approximately equal frequency and magnitude across the Hero, Scapegoat and Sick (or Lost) Child roles, and with least frequency (the one positive correlation) and magnitude for the Mascot role. Particularly interesting, and somewhat puzzling given the origins of these presumably dysfunctional roles in analyses of alcoholic families, is the fact that Buelow (1990) found no significant correlations between role and SACQ variables for students from alcohol/drug-abusing families; and the negative correlations were found in roughly equal frequency and magnitude in the students from the otherwise dysfunctional families, and even the normal families. For his three samples combined, Buelow obtained a moderately-sized negative correlation between an overall score from the Role Relationship Inventory and the SACQ full scale score, the only 202 SACQ variable for which such correlation was reported. Comparing Buelow’s (1990) normal family students with Cooler’s (1995) and Ropar’s (1997) samples of students from families unselected as to pathology and therefore presumably normal, several points may be made. In none of the three studies was there indication of relation between the Mascot role and adjustment to college except for Buelow’s weak positive correlation regarding social adjustment. Conversely, in all three studies there were negative (expected direction) correlations with SACQ subscales for the Lost or Sick Child role. Both Ropar (1997) and Buelow (1990) found relation between the Scapegoat role and SACQ variables, while Cooler obtained no significant correlations, very possibly due to the very small number of students in her sample who fell into the Scapegoat category. A principal difference among the three studies is the positive relation found by both Cooler (1995) and Ropar (1997), and a negative one by Buelow (1990), between the Hero role and adjustment to college, but inspection of the item-content of the three different measures employed makes the obtained difference understandable. For Cooler and Ropar, the Hero role is a consistently positive one defined in terms of socially desirable personality characteristics. For Buelow, the Hero is seen clearly as a dysfunctional role, defined in terms of a driven, conscienceridden need to deal with family pathology at the expense of suppressing and not managing one’s own problems, with consequent feelings of guilt, inadequacy and identity-confusion. In any event, clarification of the definition and measurement of the Hero role would seem to be indicated. Familial/parental physical health and mental health characteristics. Freshmen who 203 considered their parents to have a drinking problem had significantly lower scores on the SACQ Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and on the full scale than freshmen who did not consider their parents to have a drinking problem (Garbarino & Strange, 1993). Similarly, Buelow (1990) reports a negative correlation between a measure of alcohol abuse in the family, adapted by him from the Brief Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (Brief MAST; Pokorny, Miller, & Kaplan, 1972), and the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable mentioned. That is, the more the alcohol abuse in the family as reported by the student, the poorer the student’s overall adjustment to college. Buelow (1990, 1995) also had students identify their families: (a) as including parents who were chemically dependent or abusive of alcohol or other drugs (CDs); (b) as being dysfunctional (divorced and disrupted) but with no parental alcohol or other drug problem (DFs); or (c) as non-drug abusive and non-dysfunctional (Ns). He in effect corroborated these student self-designations through administration of instruments measuring the students’ assessments of the quality or effectiveness of their families’ functioning (the Family Functioning Scales; Bloom, 1985) and parents’ alcohol dependence (the adaptation of the Brief MAST) and found differences in expected direction among the families. That is, Ns had scores indicating the highest level of family “health” or functioning and CDs the lowest, with DFs intermediate. The same statistically significant pattern was found by Buelow (1990) among students from the three kinds of families in personal-emotional, social, and overall adjustment to college as measured by the SACQ, though with rather small differences between group means. Interestingly, there were no differences among the three student groups in college grade point average. 204 Somewhat similarly to Buelow’s (1990, 1995) study, Ross (1995) had students identify their families: (a) as including parents with an alcohol abuse problem (alcohol dysfunctional); (b) as including parents who had died or were divorced, or had been physically or sexually abusive of the student (other dysfunctional); or (c) as without dysfunctional characteristics (normal). Students from alcohol dysfunctional families had lower scores than students from normal families on all SACQ variables, and lower than students from “other dysfunctional” families on academic, social, and overall adjustment. Interestingly, though the scores on all SACQ variables for the “other dysfunctional” group were intermediate between those for the alcohol dysfunctional and normal groups, there were no significant differences between the “other dysfunctional” and normal groups. This may have been due to the possibility that families including parental divorce or death may not be dysfunctional in the same sense or degree as families characterized by physical or sexual abuse of the children. Bailey and Shilkret (2000b) devised an instrument to assess students’ perception of their parents’ psychological health. Perceiving one’s mother as having psychological problems was found to be associated with poorer academic, personal-emotional, and overall adjustment in the student, but no relation was seen between perception of the father as having psychological problems and the students’ adjustment to college. Families’ social climate characteristics. The Family Environment Scale (FES; Moos & Moos, 1981) has been cited earlier as a source of variables describing family characteristics thought to be related to student adjustment in college, and includes several variables not yet mentioned that are concerned with the social climate of the family. Family expressiveness, defined as the extent to which family members are encouraged to act openly and express feelings 205 directly, was found by Lapsley (1989)/ Rice et al. (1990) and Hopkins (1998) to be significantly correlated with all SACQ variables, but not by Caplan (1996/1997) using only the SACQ full scale. The Emotional Expressiveness subscale from the Self-Report Family Inventory-Version II (Beavers & Hampson, 1990) was found by Hutto (1998) to be correlated in the expected direction with all SACQ variables. The extent of familial interest in political, social, intellectual, and cultural activities, as measured by the Family Environment Scale’s Intellectual-Cultural subscale (Moos & Moos, 1981), correlated positively with all SACQ indices in the Lapsley (1989)/Rice et al. (1990) study, but not with any in Hopkins’ (1998). The FES variable family independence -- i. e., the extent to which family members are assertive and self-sufficient, and make their own decisions – correlated weakly with academic, personal-emotional, and overall adjustment for Lapsley/Rice et al. but not with any SACQ variables for Hopkins. The Moral-Religious subscale of the FES (Moos & Moos, 1981), measuring degree of familial emphasis on ethical and religious values, for Lapsley (1989)/Rice et al. (1990) was found to correlate positively but weakly with social adjustment, institutional attachment, and overall adjustment, but was not related to any SACQ variable in Hopkins’ (1998) study. In neither study was relation found between family achievement orientation/competitiveness and the SACQ. Family demographic variables. In a study by Burr (1992), students from families where English was the primary language had higher scores on the Social Adjustment subscale than students from families where English was not the primary language. But with a Chicano/Latino student sample, Rodriguez (1994) found no relation between language spoken at home and the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable used. In a South African University where 206 English is the medium of instruction, students for whom English was a first language had better social adjustment than students for whom English was not the first language (Sennett, 2000). For Kline (1992) there was no relation between a one-item measure of the importance of ethnicity in one’s family and any SACQ variable. Amin (2000) found no relation between ArabAmerican students’ generation level within the family (e. g., first, second, third) and SACQ variables, as did Rodriguez (1994) between that variable for her Chicano/Latino students and the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable employed. Wintre and Yaffe (2000) obtained no relation between immigrant generational status among students at a Canadian university and the SACQ full-scale score, the only SACQ variable reported. Higher SACQ full scale scores (the only SACQ variable cited) are reported by Allen (1985) for earlier-born than later-born children, highest for only children. Kline (1992) found no relation between students’ number of siblings and any SACQ variable. Savino (1987; Savino et al., 1986b), on a first- but not second-semester testing, found that students from families residing in urban-suburban settings had higher scores on all SACQ indices than students from families residing in rural hometowns. Grandparents. Erickson (1996) used the Grandparent Strengths and Needs InventoryGrandparent Form (GSNI; Strom & Strom, 1989), administered to students’ grandparents to assess their sense of how successfully and with what degree of satisfaction they have carried out their grandparental role in relation to their student-grandchild. The grandparents’ scores on the six GSNI subscales were then correlated with the student-grandchildrens’ five SACQ variables. In four of the 30 comparisons, slightly more than would be expected by chance, statistically significant relation was found between the grandparent-generated variables and all of the 207 grandchild-students’ SACQ variables except the Social Adjustment subscale, especially the grandparents’ assessment of how successfully they have performed their role. The better the grandparental role was carried out, the better the student’s adjustment was in the first year of college. 208 CHAPTER 13 ENVIRONMENTAL DETERMINANTS OF COLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: NON-FAMILY SOCIAL SUPPORT As a reader of the previous chapter is now aware, social support as provided by the family has already been considered as one of the many characteristics of families that have been investigated as possible environmental determinants of student adjustment to college. But there is also a sizeable number of studies that have examined the role of social support from other environmental sources, and those will be discussed in this current chapter. Social Support from Friends It would be readily assumed, of course, that friends would be a significant source of social support. Just (1998), Bartels (1995), Caro (1985/1986), Hogan (1986), Maton and Weisman (1989)/Maton (1989b), Shibazaki (1999) and McAndrew-Miller (1989) all consistently obtained correlations – sometimes fairly strong -- in the expected direction between the Perceived Social Support from Friends Scale (Procidano & Heller, 1983) and all SACQ subscales (except Attachment for Hogan and Shibazaki, and academic adjustment for McAndrew-Miller), highest for social adjustment. Frazier and Cook (1993) report significant relation between that same social support variable and the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed. Thus, the greater the sense that a student has of receiving support from friends the better the adjustmen to college in all areas tapped by the SACQ. Also indicative of the supportive role of friends in a student’s college adjustment are studies that focus on the prevalence and quality of friendship sources. Using the Social Network 209 Questionnaire (SNQ; Hays & Oxley, 1986), Serafica et al. (1990) found that the more that a student’s social network is comprised of friends the better the social adjustment, institutional attachment, and overall adjustment. Harris (1988) obtained correlation between that same support variable derived from an adaptation of the SNQ, and also between network density (the degree to which a network is comprised of mutual friends), and social adjustment only. Harris also reported that the more friends a student has, as measured by the SNQ adaptation, the poorer the academic adjustment but the better the social adjustment, which seems intuitively reasonable. Hertel (1996) adapted Procidano and Heller’s (1983) Perceived Social Support from Friends Scale to assess support separately from friends in college (any college, not just the one attended by the respondent) and friends not in college. He found strong correlations for the former source of support (friends in college) with all SACQ indices in second-generation college freshmen (students with college-educated parents), but for the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales only in first-generation freshmen (students whose parents were not college-educated). Regarding social support from friends not in college, there were modest but still significant positive correlations for the second-generation freshmen with the Academic Adjustment subscale and full scale only, but none for first-generation freshmen. Kenny and Stryker (1994) found that social support from one's college friends, as measured by an instrument adapted from scales employed by Hays and Oxley (1986) and Martin and Burks (1985), was positively associated with SACQ variables (the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales) for white students but not for students of color (Afro-Americans, Asians or Latinos). In a subsequent article, Kenny and Stryker (1996) interpret their data as indicating that social support from college peers for a mixed minority sample is relatively less important 210 than support from family, while the reverse is true for European-American students. Those same investigators found that obtaining support from pre-college friends away from campus was negatively associated with institutional attachment for students of color (Kenny & Stryker, 1994) and also negatively associated with social adjustment as well as institutional attachment for European-American students (Kenny & Stryker, 1996). Jarama Alvan et al. (1996) found positive correlation for Latino students between a measure of satisfaction with support from “friend/other,” from the Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule (Barrera, 1981), and the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable employed. Using a self-designed single-item rating of perceived amount of support received from friends, Martin et al. (1999) and Martin et al. (2000) report a positive correlation with the SACQ full-scale score, the only SACQ variable employed. Social Support from Fellow Students, Roommates, and Coursemates Using the Social Network Questionnaire (SNQ; Hays & Oxley, 1986), Harris (1988) found that the more that a student’s social network is comprised of students (i. e., not necessarily friends), the better the adjustment to college as reflected in all SACQ indices except academic adjustment. Serafica et al. (1990) obtained positive correlation between that same support variable and social adjustment and institutional attachment. Polewchak (1998, 1999) employed the Multi-Dimensional Support Scale (Winefield, Winefield, & Tiggermann, 1992) to assess social support from “peers” (specifically including fellow students). She found that frequency of support from peers for her sample as a whole (i. e., both men and women) was significantly related to all SACQ variables (though weakly for academic adjustment); for men, with all SACQ variables except academic adjustment; and for 211 women it failed to correlate only with personal-emotional and academic adjustment. Satisfaction with frequency of support from peers correlated significantly in the full sample with all SACQ variables except academic adjustment, to about the same degree as did simple perceived frequency; for men it also correlated with all SACQ variables except academic adjustment; and, for women, with all SACQ variables except, oddly enough, social adjustment. Hurtado et al. (1996) found that reporting in the sophomore year having received help in adjusting as freshmen from “other freshman students” correlated negatively with academic adjustment, while having received help from peer advisors correlated negatively with social adjustment, but having received help from upper class students correlated positively with social adjustment and institutional attachment. The consequences for adjustment to college of social support from fellow students in courses (“coursemates”) were examined by Sullivan (1991) using a specially devised instrument tapping five aspects of social support, i. e., emotional, informational, social companionship, instrumental, and motivational. There were significant correlations for all five kinds of social support from coursemates with the SACQ Social Adjustment subscale, and for three of them with the full scale (data were reported for only those two SACQ variables). Social support from coursemates in freshman advisory/intervention seminars, as measured by the Affiliation subscale of the Classroom Environment Scale (Trickett & Moos, 1973), was found by Wildman (1998) to not correlate with any SACQ variable. Similarly, amount of time spent studying with fellowstudents who were enrolled in the same residential living-learning program as the respondent was not related to social or overall adjustment, the only SACQ variables reported (Helman, 1999). For Serafica et al. (1990) there was no correlation with any SACQ variable for the extent 212 to which a student’s social network is comprised of roommates, as measured by the Social Network Questionnaire (SNQ; Hays & Oxley, 1986). Reporting results for social and overall adjustment only, Helman (1999) found that having a roommate who was enrolled in the same residential living-learning program as the respondent was unrelated to either SACQ variable, but having known one’s roommate prior to coming to college was associated with better social and overall adjustment, and students who were more satisfied with their relationship with their roommate had better social adjustment than students who were less satisfied. Social Support from Faculty and Other Authority Figures The consequences for adjustment to college of social support from faculty were examined by Sullivan (1991) using the specially devised instrument described above that tapped five aspects of social support. Weak correlations were found only for the informational kind of support with the two SACQ indices employed (the Social Adjustment subscale and full scale). Helman (1999) found that students who rated themselves to be better known by their instructor in an orientation seminar had better academic and overall adjustment, the only SACQ variables reported, than students who felt less well know; but Wildman (1998), who employed the Teacher Support subscale from the Classroom Environment Scale (Trickett & Moos, 1973) to measure social support experienced from instructors of freshman advisory/intervention seminars, obtained no correlation between that variable and any SACQ variable. The amount of support that students perceived themselves as receiving from faculty, as measured by a rating on a self-designed single item, was positively correlated with the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable reported (Martin et al., 1999; Martin et al, 2000). Polewchak (1998, 1999) used the Multi-Dimensional Support Scale (Winefield, 213 Winefield, & Tiggermann (1992) to assess social support from authority figures (specifically including professors). Frequency of support from such figures correlated significantly in the total sample and the male subsample with all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment, and in the female subsample with the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales only (and considerably weaker on those two subscales than for the males). Satisfaction with frequency of support from those same authority figures correlated with all SACQ variables in the total sample; with all except institutional attachment in the female sample; and with all but academic and personal-emotional adjustment for men. Maton (1989a) examined the relation between a measure of spiritual support (from one's relationship with God, an ultimate authority figure) devised by himself and the SACQ Social and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales. He found no significant correlation with either subscale for his full sample, but did find a significant relation with personal-emotional in a subsample that had recently experienced high levels of life stress. None of the other three comparisons (for either subscale in the low life-stress subsample, or the Social Adjustment subscale in the high-stress subsample) were significant. Several studies report findings concerning frequency or quality of student relationships with faculty which, though not couched in terms of social support as such, may be viewed as reflecting a kind of such support. Students who report themselves as having frequent contact with professors have higher Social Adjustment subscale and full scale scores than students who do not report frequent contact (Elacqua, 1992a&b). Students identified by an adaptation of the Attachment Style Inventory (ASI; Sperling, Berman, & Fagen, 1991) as having secure relationship with faculty scored higher on the SACQ Attachment subscale, the only SACQ 214 variable employed, than students having insecure relationships with faculty (Lopez, 1997). Student ratings of quality of relationship with faculty correlated significantly with all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment for student-athletes, and quality of relationship with coaches (a kind of faculty?) correlated with all SACQ variables (Ridinger, 1998). To this point, regarding social support, we have mainly considered its consequences for college adjustment when originating separately from family, friends, fellow students, and professors. Taking a step away from social support obtained from separate sources and toward social support from the environment in general are two studies that combined separately identified sources. Social Support from Separately Identified Sources Combined Polewchak (1998, 1999) used measures of frequency, and satisfaction with frequency, of social support from family and close friends combined, from the Multi-Dimensional Support Scale (Winefield, Winefield, & Tiggermann, 1992). For the former measure (frequency), male students had significant correlations with all SACQ variables except institutional attachment but female students had none, while the full sample showed significant, modest correlation only with academic, personal-emotional and overall adjustment. For the latter measure (satisfaction with frequency), male students had fairly strong correlations with all SACQ variables but again female students had none, while the combined sample had significant, less strong, correlations with all SACQ variables. Thus, without benefit of statistical tests by gender, it appears that the relation with college adjustment for social support from family and close friends combined is stronger for male than for female students; indeed, that kind of social support predicted college adjustment quite well 215 for men and not at all for women. Also without benefit of statistical tests, it appears that for males satisfaction with frequency of social support from family and close friends combined is a better predictor of college adjustment than is simple perceived frequency of such support. Using total scores for frequency of social support, and satisfaction with such frequency, from all sources (family and close friends combined, peers, and authority figures), from the Multi-Dimensional Support Scale (Winefield, Winefield, & Tiggermann, 1992), Polewchak (1998,1999) found for the latter variable significant correlations with all SACQ indices for her total sample and men and women separately. For the former variable, simple perceived frequency, in the total sample there were significant correlations with all SACQ variables, though somewhat weaker for academic and personal-emotional adjustment. For males there were significant correlations for all SACQ variables except academic adjustment, and for women excepting academic and personal-emotional adjustment. Except in the case of academic adjustment, where correlations were of similar magnitude for both men and women, the latter had lower SACQ correlations with both social support variables. Napoli and Wortman (1998) employed an unnamed measure of perceived social support from members of the campus community and family members combined that was devised by Mallinckrodt (1988) for use in the latter investigator’s study. They obtained significant correlations with all SACQ subscales. Administering to student-athletes a self-designed measure of satisfaction with support from academic and athletic staff and friends combined, Ridinger (1998) found significant correlations with all SACQ variables. Support from the Social Environment in General, Unspecified as to Source 216 Moving now to support from the social environment in general -- i. e., not specified as to type(s) of support source – several different psychometric instruments have been used for measuring social support, some of these instruments used in several different studies. Savino (1987; see also Savino et al., 1986b) found moderately strong correlations between number of support persons, satisfaction with social support in general, and total score from the Social Support Questionnaire (SSQ; Sarason, Levine, Basham, & Sarason, 1983) and all SACQ variables, strongest for the Social Adjustment subscale. Gallant (1994) administered the SSQ and the SACQ in the third week of October and the latter again in the third week of November. She found that the number of support persons index from the SSQ correlated positively with the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale (the only SACQ variable reported) in both SACQ administrations, while the SSQ satisfaction index correlated with the first (contemporaneous) SACQ administration only. Huff (1998) obtained significant correlations for the SSQ satisfaction index with the SACQ Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and the full scale, but not with any SACQ variable for the Perceived Availability of Social Support subscale. Two measures of satisfaction with social support from the Social Support Inventory (SSI; Brown, Brady, Lent, Wolfert, & Hall, 1987) – one interestingly defined in terms of discrepancy between the amount of support the student felt had been needed and what had actually been received (the perceived fit score), and the other a subjective satisfaction score – were found by Fuller (2000) to be correlated with all SACQ subscales, especially personal-emotional adjustment; and by Corbett (1991) with all SACQ variables, especially the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales, in black and white deaf students. Bartels (1990) obtained strong 217 correlations for those same two indices from the SSI with the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable employed, and lesser but still significant values for measures of amount of need for support felt by the student (Need Strength) and amount of support received (Perceived Supply). Hunsberger et al. (1996; and Hunsberger, 2000) report correlation between the total score, measuring perceived availability of social support, from the Social Provisions Scale (Russell & Cutrona, 1984) administered prematriculation (in August) and all variables for the SACQ administered the subsequent March, strongest with social adjustment. For another sample of students from the same institution in a later year, that was administered the Social Provisions Scale in March concurrently with the SACQ, there were significant correlations with all of the SACQ subscales, now much stronger for social and personal-emotional adjustment and institutional attachment than the values found with the earlier sample for which the testings were temporally removed (Pratt, 2001). Solberg et al. (1994) found a moderately strong correlation for Hispanic students between the total score from the Social Provisions Scale (SPS; Russell & Cutrona, 1984) and an abbreviated SACQ comprised of 26 items from the Academic and Social Adjustment subscales. Also using the Social Provisions Scale, but with older (age 45-55) female community college students, Ortiz (1995) analyzed the relation between six SPS subscales measuring different kinds of social support (e.g., guidance, reassurance of worth, reliable alliance) as well as the total score from that instrument and two SACQ subscales, Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment. He found significant correlations between all social support variables except one (Opportunity for Nurturance, or feeling needed by others) and personal-emotional adjustment. Correlated with academic adjustment were two of the SPS’ subscales (Reassurance of Worth and Social 218 Integration, or sharing of similar interests and activities with others) as well as the total score. Using the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (ISEL; Cohen, Mermelstein, Kamarck, & Hoberman, 1985) to measure perceived availability of four kinds of social support, Kambach (1994) correlated the four subscale and total scores from the ISEL with all SACQ variables. Statistically significant values in the expected positive direction were obtained in all but three of the 25 comparisons, some values strong to robust, the strongest values occurring in relation to the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and the full scale. Robbins et al. (1993) examined the relation between the same ISEL variables and the SACQ Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales and found significance in the expected direction on nine of the ten comparisons. Kim et al. (1992), employing only the full scale score from the ISEL, got significant correlations with the four SACQ indices for which data were reported (i.e., the Attachment subscale was excluded). Using the total score from the Young Adult Social Support Inventory (Grochowski & McCubbin, 1987), designed to measure social support in the first year of college, Marcotte (1995) obtained significant correlations with all SACQ subscales. In addition to using the traditional ISEL (Cohen, Mermelstein, Kamarck, & Hoberman, 1985) as cited above, Kambach (1994) also devised a variation that relied more on graphical than verbal means of response in order to reduce common method variance. She still found correlations in the expected direction with the same three SACQ variables that had highest correlation with the traditional ISEL (see above), now strongest with the Social Adjustment subscale. Factor analyzing data from several measures of person characteristics -- the Prestatie 219 Motivatie Test, a measure of achievement motivation (PMT; Hermans, 1970); the College SelfEfficacy Instrument (CSEI; Solberg, O’Brien, Villareal, Kennel, & Davis, 1993); the Multidimensional Academic-Specific Locus of Control Scale (MASLOC; Palenzuela, 1988); and the Career Decision Scale (Osipow, 1987) -- Fuller and Heppner (1995) extracted a variable they named Perceived Support, which correlated strongly with all SACQ subscales in approximately equal magnitude. Washington (1996) obtained positive correlations with all SACQ subscales for reponses to a single item, “availability of a strong support person.” Several studies investigating the consequences of social support for adjustment to college have employed instruments focusing on the measurement of social networks. Some of these instruments include subscales or scores pertaining to support from separate sources – i. e., family, friends, students, etc. – and in those instances the findings have already been described in the sections of the text concerning those sources. The Social Network Questionnaire (SNQ; Hays & Oxley, 1986) has been used by Serafica et al. (1990), and an adaptation of the SNQ used by Harris (1988), to examine the relation to college adjustment of various aspects of the composition and function of general social networks. Network size for Harris had a small negative correlation with academic adjustment, while for Serafica et al. there was no correlation with any SACQ variable. Several of the SNQ (Hays & Oxley, 1986) variables, not all employed by both Serafica et al. (1990) and Harris (1988), concern amount and quality of interpersonal activity in the general social network. Frequency of contact was positively correlated with social adjustment, institutional attachment, and overall adjustment (Harris). Similarity of network member’s attitudes and values was positively related to social adjustment and institutional attachment for 220 Harris, but not to any SACQ variable for Serafica et al. Degree of intimacy of relationship was related for Harris to social adjustment (quite strongly), institutional attachment, and overall adjustment, but again not to any SACQ variable for Serafica et al. The occurrence of anger/conflict in one’s network was negatively related to personal-emotional and overall adjustment for Harris, but not to any SACQ variable for Serafica et al. Other SNQ (Hays & Oxley, 1986) variables are concerned with kinds of social support provided by one’s network. Task assistance correlated weakly and positively with social adjustment and negatively with personal-emotional adjustment for Harris (1988) but not with any SACQ variable for Serafica et al. (1990). Provision of information/advice for Harris was positively related to social adjustment and institutional attachment, but for Serafica et al. not to any SACQ variable. Obtaining emotional support from one’s network correlated positively for Harris with social adjustment and institutional attachment and negatively with personalemotional adjustment, but yet again not with any SACQ variable for Serafica et al. Experiencing fun and relaxation in the network had positive correlations with all SACQ variables for Serafica et al, and with social adjustment and institutional attachment for Harris. Harris (1991) subsequently administered her adaptation of the Social Network Questionnaire (Hays & Oxley, 1986) and the SACQ to seniors, and found the same approximate pattern of relations between the two kinds of variables that she had obtained from freshmen, though generally somewhat lesser in magnitude. Significant correlations in the expected direction were also reported by Harris between some social network variables from the freshman year and some indices of the SACQ administered three years later in the last year of college. Kenny (1995), used a measure of social networks derived from Hays and Oxley’s Social 221 Network Questionnaire (1986) and from an instrument designed by Martin and Burks (1985) that contained nineteen variables. For those variables correlated with each of the four SACQ subscales for five samples of students, the number of significant values obtained for each subscale only slightly exceeded the number expected by chance. Using the Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule (ASSIS; Barrera, 1981), Brooks and DuBois (1995) found no correlation for SACQ variables with network size in general or unconflicted or conflicted network sizes, and only one with satisfaction with support (for social adjustment), but did obtain significant correlations in the expected negative direction between felt need for support and all SACQ variables except the Attachment subscale. Also using the ASSIS, but only the overall satisfaction with social support variable, Zea et al. (1995) and Zea (1997) found significant correlations with all SACQ variables for white students, with all but personal-emotional adjustment for Latino students, with all but social adjustment and institutional attachment for African-American students, and with only personal-emotional adjustment for Asian-American students. For their sample as a whole there were significant correlations with all SACQ variables. Jarama Alvan et al. (1996) found for Latino students significant correlation between the overall satisfaction with social support index from the ASSIS (Barrera, 1981) and the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable used in that analysis. Those same investigators also looked at satisfaction with various kinds and sources of social support. Examining all SACQ variables in relation to emotional support and instrumental support, they found no significant correlations for the latter kind of support but, for the former, significant correlation with academic and overall adjustment. 222 For Mendelson (1987/1988) there was only one significant value out of six correlations between the SACQ's Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, the only SACQ variable employed, and three measures of social support networks (satisfaction with; multidimensionality; complexity) from the Perceived Support Network Inventory (Oritt, Paul, & Behrman, 1985), for men and women separately. While the foregoing findings presented in this section all concern direct relation between support from the social environment in general and college adjustment, there are others that represent somewhat less direct relation, sometimes with consideration of other kinds of variables. Zamostny et al. (1993) were interested in the relation to adjustment to college of sources of support considered to buffer the effects of early trauma (i. e., in the first fifteen years of life), using their own Early Resources Checklist to measure support sources. One Checklist subscale pertains to interpersonal relationships, similar to other measures of social support discussed here; a second concerns early success/achievement experiences in school, sports, or creative pursuits; and a third refers to involvement with recreational/play activities -- all occurring prior to age sixteen. Modest, sporadic correlations in the expected direction were found between one or the other of the three sources of support and all areas of adjustment to college except personalemotional, perhaps most closely with social adjustment. Schwitzer et al. (1993) investigated for its influence on college adjustment the relation between perceived social support and goal directedness as measured by the Goal Instability Scale (Robbins & Patton, 1985). They used the Affiliation and Teacher Support subscales from the Classroom Environment Scale (Trickett & Moos, 1973) to assess level of perceived social support within a freshman orientation class, and the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List 223 (Cohen et al., 1985) to assess perceived social support from the campus environment in general. For the two kinds of social support separately, Schwitzer et al. assigned students to four groups according to high and moderate goal-directedness and high and low social support. In both analyses they found that the high goal-directedness/high social support group had higher SACQ scores (on the Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment and Attachment subscales but not the Social Adjustment subscale, with the full-scale score not reported) than any of the other three groups, with no differences among the latter three groups. Finally, studies employing variables possibly related to the concepts of social support and social networks may be cited. Caro (1985/1986) found little if any significance in comparisons of any subscales from the Interview Schedule for Social Interaction (Henderson, Duncan-Jones, Byrne, & Scott, 1980) and SACQ indices except: (a) positive correlations between Availability of Social Integration and the SACQ Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and the full scale; (b) positive correlations between Perceived Adequacy of Attachment and SACQ personalemotional adjustment; and (c) negative correlations between Conflict with Attachment Figures and the SACQ Academic Adjustment and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales. Flescher (1986a) reported that the variable "relationship density" (Tolsdorf, 1976) is positively correlatedwith the Attachment subscale for females in stepfather families. 224 CHAPTER 14 ENVIRONMENTAL DETERMINANTS OF CCLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: INTERRACIAL EXPERIENCE The racial integration of American colleges and universities is a relatively recent phenomenon that has given rise to considerable interest in the adjustment of minority students to predominantly white institutions. One possible determinant of that adjustment that has received some attention in SACQ-using research is an environmental/social variable, i.e., amount and kind of interracial exposure or experience. Graham et al. (1984), Serafica et al. (1990), and Adan and Felner (1995) all found that Black freshmen with greater amount of pre-college exposure to or experience with White persons in the high school context – especially in relation to high school friends -- adjusted better to predominantly White colleges, as indicated by higher scores on the SACQ, than did Black freshmen with lesser such exposure or experience. For Graham et al. and Adan and Felner, effects were found in all SACQ variables, while Serafica et al. report data only for the Attachment subscale. Pfeil (2000), however, found no SACQ differences among AfricanAmerican students at a predominantly White university who had attended predominantly White, predominantly Black/minority, or racially/ethnically mixed high schools. But the students in Pfeil’s study, unlike those in the studies cited earlier in this paragraph, were not all freshmen, instead coming from mixed college year levels. To make her findings more comparable with those of Graham et al., Pfeil (2000) reduced her sample to only those students who had completed just one semester, and found – contrary to the findings of the previous investigators -- 225 that at the end of their first semester the students who had attended predominantly Black/minority high schools had higher scores on social, personal-emotional and overall adjustment than those who had attended predominantly White or racially/ethnically mixed high schools. Graham et al. (1984) also report that, where predominantly white samples in other studies had consistently shown decreases in SACQ scores from the first to the second semester, their black students showed increases, and this was interpreted as possibly an additional indication of beneficial effect of interracial experience, but now occurring postmatriculation in the current lives of students. Their findings showed, furthermore, a differential rate of improvement over the course of the academic year among black students varying in amount of prior interracial experience, the gain being greatest for those with the least such experience. Adan and Felner (1995) and Mosley (1990) very interestingly and importantly showed that prior interracial experience for black students attending primarily black universities was negatively related to adjustment. The inference drawn by Adan and Felner from their investigation, involving as it did not only black students at predominantly white and predominantly black universities, but also white students at a predominantly white university, with significant effects on all SACQ variables, was the importance of the relation or interaction between person and environmental variables in the determination of adjustment. Mosley’s study was a little more restricted in scope than Adan and Felner’s, using just a sample of black students at a predominantly black university, finding that such students who were graduates of predominantly black high schools had higher SACQ full scale scores than graduates of predominantly white or integrated high schools. The Academic Adjustment subscale, the only other SACQ variable for which data are reported by Mosley, showed no differences among the 226 three groups. The consequences of prior interracial experience in pre-college educational settings were studied by Sennett (2000) at a historically white South African university that became racially heterogeneous. That investigator looked for differences in adjustment to college among both black and white students who had had, or did not have, multi-racial schooling background, i.e., previous exposure to ethnocultural diversity. No SACQ differences were found, but it should be noted that the sample of black students did not include so-called “coloureds,” or mixed black and white students. Also studying the adjustment of black students to a predominantly white university, Scott (1991) focused on the relation between social adjustment and the racial composition of campus activities in which students were currently involved. The highest scores on the Social Adjustment subscale of the SACQ were obtained by black students involved with both black and white sponsored activities, next highest by those involved with predominantly white sponsored activities, next highest by those involved with predominantly black sponsored activities, and lowest by those with "little or no" involvement in campus activities. The difference between the top two categories was slight, and between the second and third categories and third and fourth categories the differences were larger and of about the same magnitude. While the interpretation of Scott's data implied here involves environmental influences, another interpretation could implicate a person variable. For example, the same person characteristic that disposes a student to participate in interracial activities (e. g., interracial ease) may also contribute to better adjustment. Hurtado et al. (1996) found no relation in Latino students between interracial/interethnic 227 relationships and SACQ variables. 228 CHAPTER 15 ENVIRONMENTAL DETERMINANTS OF COLLEGE ADJUSTMENT: INSTITUTIONAL CHARACTERISTICS Institutional Characteristics in Interaction with Decidedness Regarding Academic Major Studies already cited as pertinent to the relation between decidedness regarding major, as a person characteristic, and adjustment to college also included findings that were interpreted as possibly reflecting effects of environmental/institutional factors in interaction with decidedness. The reader may recall that the relation between major decidedness and adjustment to a liberal arts college was more clearly seen in the second semester than the first, and this led the investigators (Smith & Baker, 1987) to conjecture that a contributing factor might be increasing institutional pressure as the academic year progressed to make a decision regarding major. Engineering college students, in contrast with Smith and Baker's (1987) liberal arts students, manifested clear relation between major decidedness and SACQ scores in their first semester, and the effect involved more aspects of adjustment than was true for second semester liberal arts students (Plaud et al., 1990). This was interpreted by the investigators as possibly a consequence of difference between the two types of institutions in relative salience of major decidedness. That is, in some respects a decision regarding major is made by engineering students prior to matriculation, is closely determinative of their program of studies in the freshman year, and any lessening of decidedness regarding that important decision may be expected to have consequences for college adjustment. Additional indication of possible interactive relation between major decidedness and institutional pressure for decision regarding major field is Leonard's (1990) earlier-cited finding 229 that differences in SACQ scores between freshmen varying in decidedness of major are more apparent in a college that requires declaration of major in the freshman year than in a college that requires declaration in the sophomore year. As a final indication of relation between major decidedness and environmental/institutional characteristics, whether a major field was available or not at the institution of enrollment for students changing majors was seen to have important consequences for SACQ scores (Plaud et al., 1990). Students changing to a major available at their institution had higher scores than those changing to a major not available at their institution. Initial Institutional Impact on New Students Several investigators have studied effects of initial exposure to the college environment on the adjustment of matriculating students using a methodology that evaluated the adjustment outcome (as measured by the SACQ) in relation to prematriculation expectations regarding adaptive capacity for the transition into college (as measured by the ASACQ; see pp. 9-11 of this monograph). Baker et al. (1985), collecting data at two institutions, found substantial decline in scores from prematriculation to the first-semester testing (interpreted as disillusionment regarding adaptive capacity) at both institutions on the Academic and Social Adjustment subscales and full scale but at neither institution on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. There was also a decline at one of the institutions on the Attachment subscale. Kintner (1998) similarly found significant ASACQ-to-SACQ declines on all variables except personalemotional adjustment. At one of the institutions used in the Baker et al. (1985) study, testing was done in the second semester as well as the first and the lowered scores persisted, declining still further on the 230 Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales. Replication of this study several years later at one of the institutions with another freshman class produced essentially the same results, except that continued decline from the first to the second semester occurred only for academic adjustment (Baker & Schultz, 1992b). Gerdes (1986) and Plaud et al. (1986) obtained results very similar to the foregoing at two other institutions -- i.e., decline on the Academic and Social Adjustment subscales and full scale but not the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, and decline on the Attachment subscale at one of the institutions. Interestingly, rather than simply no effect on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale, these investigators found a reversal, i. e., an actual adjustment average significantly higher than the anticipated adjustment average. The reasons for this difference are not readily apparent and may have something to do with the fact that at both institutions students completed the anticipated adjustment questionnaire following attendance at a summer on-campus orientation program. Cooper and Robinson (1988a&b) -- using engineering and science majors, as did Plaud et al. (1986) – and Marcy (1996) found significant pre- to postmatriculation declines on all ASACQ/SACQ indices, including the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. Williams (1996) obtained significant decline for the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable reported. Without making a statistical test of the effect, Mendelowitz (1990/1991) noted that there were declines in scores from ASACQs administered to high school seniors at the end of the school year to SACQ scores obtained in November after matriculation at college. The smallest decline was, consistent with above-cited findings, on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale. 231 The foregoing results concerning relation between prematriculation expectations and postmatriculation outcome are largely main effects of conditions associated with initial exposure to the college environment. Baker et al. (1985), however, made a special point that not only were there wide individual differences in degree of post-matriculation disillusionment, but some students did not show it at all and others showed the opposite, implying an important interactive role for person characteristics. The reader will recall the earlier description of clear behavioral correlates of those individual differences regarding disillusionment (see pp. 10-11 of this monograph), correlates of sufficient importance as to underscore the desirability of research concerning the determinants of variation in relation between prematriculation expectations and postmatriculation outcome. Institutional Living Arrangements Savino et al. (1986b) found that, for a first-semester testing but not second-semester, small but statistically significant correlations were found between dormitory staff/student ratio and four of the five SACQ indices. The smaller the number of students per staff member, the better the adjustment to college. Savino et al. also found that, on second-semester testing but not first-semester, freshmen with freshman roommates obtained higher Social Adjustment and Attachment subscale scores than freshmen with upper class roommates. But Danielson (1995) found no differences on any SACQ variables between freshmen living in dormitory sections set aside for freshmen versus freshmen living in mixed-class sections. No notable effects were found by Savino et al. (1986b) for unisex versus coed dorms, high-rise versus low-rise dorms, or sylvan setting versus downtown dorms. And for Danielson (1995) there were no differences on the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable reported, 232 between freshmen in unisex versus coed dormitories, or in large versus small dormitories. Interestingly, Savino et al. list a wide range of SACQ full scale score means for freshmen in 16 dormitories, but do not report tests of significance nor any conjecture regarding possible reasons for differences (assuming significance) among the campus residences. The foregoing findings concern characteristics of dormitory living defined independently of student perception of or reaction to those characteristics. Barthelemy and Fine (1995), by contrast, were interested in dormitory characteristics as perceived or evaluated by students. They used the Residence Hall Climate Inventory (RHCI), which they adapted for their study from a measure of family climate (Kurdek & Fine, 1994), to assess four aspects of dormitory characteristics: availability of personal or social support, group cohesiveness, amount of conflict or contentiousness, and orderedness or regulation. Moderately strong positive correlations were found by Barthelemy and Fine (1995) between personal support perceived to be available in the dormitory and all SACQ indices for males, and, for females, somewhat more moderate correlations with all indices except academic adjustment. Regarding amount of conflict seen as characterizing life in the dormitory, for women there were moderately strong negative correlations with all SACQ indices, while for men this variable was thusly correlated only with the Social Adjustment subscale. There were relatively modest positive correlations for women between perceived group cohesiveness in the dormitory and all SACQ variables, and for men with the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and the full scale. For dormitory orderedness or regulation, there was only one significant finding, a positive correlation for men with the Academic Adjustment subscale. Because the Residence Hall Climate Inventory assesses dormitory characteristics as they 233 are perceived or reacted to by the student, the likelihood must be considered that it may be a measure of students’ adjustment to college, or to their dormitory in particular, as much as a measure of the dormitory. Indeed, the RHCI might be seen as an amplification of the only two SACQ items that directly implicate dormitory living arrangement. Several investigators found that students who live on-campus have higher Social Adjustment subscale scores than students who live off-campus or commute from home (Albert, 1988; Birnie-Lefcovitch, 1997; Elacqua, 1992a & b; Jackson, 1998; Loveland, 1994; Serafica et al., 1990; Yaffe, 1997, see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000). Jackson, Serafica et al., Loveland, and Yaffe also obtained higher scores from on-campus residents on the Attachment subscale, as did Elacqua and Friedland (1990) on the full scale. Low (1994; Low & Handal, 1945) compared students at three different colleges, two primarily residential and one a commuter college, and scores on the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales were lower at the commuter college than at the other two institutions. But Hurtado et al. (1996) found no effects associated with living on or off-campus, as did Dewitt-Parker (1999) using only the SACQ full scale score. In Halloran’s (2000) study, done at university that had been a primarily commuter institution but recently added a residence hall located “a short distance” from campus, there were no SACQ differences between students living in the residence hall and commuters. Possibly somewhat related to commuter/residential status, day college students had higher Social Adjustment subscale scores (the only variable employed) than students who attended evening or weekend classes (Salone, 1995). Freshman students at a Belgian university were described by Beyers and Goossens (2002a) as having two kinds of living arrangements, neither of which were on-campus: (a) 234 renting rooms in the area where the university is located and usually visiting their parental homes on weekends and holidays; and (b) commuting from home. No SACQ differences were found between students in those two kinds of living arrangements. Campus Social/Sports Organization Membership Fraternity/sorority membership was reported by Montgomery and Haemmerlie (1993; see also Montgomery and Howdeshell, 1993) to be negatively related to academic and personalemotional adjustment but positively related to social adjustment, and unrelated to institutional attachment. Montgomery and Haemmerlie (2001) found modest positive relation between fraternity membership and social and personal-emotional adjustment. In Jackson’s (1998) study, fraternity members had higher scores that non-fraternity members on the SACQ Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and the full-scale. In one study (Ratta, 1994), members of intercollegiate athletic teams had better adjustment than non-team members in all the areas tapped by the SACQ, while in another (Foster, 1997) freshman intercollegiate student-athletes had higher scores on the Academic Adjustment subscale only. Institutional Service Providers Hurtado et al. (1996) examined the adjustment consequences of service provision by various college staff persons. No such effects were obtained for faculty advisors, career counselors, personal counselors, or financial aid counselors, but positive correlations were obtained for reporting having received assistance from resident advisors (with social adjustment and institutional attachment), and from academic counselors (with academic adjustment and institutional attachment). Similarly, Helman (1999) found that students who were more satisfied 235 with their contacts with their resident assistant had better social and overall adjustment – the only SACQ variables reported – than students who were less satisfied. Ridinger (1998), with a sample of student-athletes, found significant correlation between a measure of quality of relationship with academic advisors and academic, social, and overall adjustment. But for Helman there was no relation between degree of satisfaction with one’s academic advisor and academic or overall adjustment, the only SACQ variables reported for that finding. Type of College Scott (1991) found no differences on the Social Adjustment subscale (the only SACQ variable reported) among students enrolled in five different colleges within a university (arts and sciences, engineering, business administration, health professions, and music). In a study focusing on engineering and science students, there were no differences between engineering and non-engineering students on academic, social, or overall adjustment, the only SACQ variables employed (Helman, 1999). Melendez (2001) obtained differences in students’ academic adjustment at several different, but unidentified, universities. Miscellaneous Institutional Characteristics Using a self-designed measure of perceived “cultural distance,” or cultural differences, between a student’s college environment and their hometown (i. e., ethnic composition, behavioral characteristics, values, entertainment/recreational activities), Ridinger (1998) found with a sample of foreign and domestic student-athletes that the greater the cultural difference the poorer the social and overall adjustment and institutional attachment. The relation between a variety of institutional characteristics and the adjustment of Latino students in the sophomore year was examined by Hurtado et al. (1996). Students at private 236 colleges had higher Social Adjustment and Attachment subscale scores than students at public colleges; the higher the Hispanic enrollment the better the academic adjustment; the larger the total undergraduate enrollment, the better the social adjustment and institutional attachment; the more that faculty and administrators are perceived as student-centered, the better the academic adjustment; the greater the perception of racial/ethnic tension in the college environment, the less good the adjustment in all areas tapped by the SACQ; the more discrimination experienced by the student in the college environment, the lesser the institutional attachment. No relation was found between the selectivity of the college, as measured by the average SAT scores of entering freshmen, and any of the SACQ variables. No SACQ differences were found by Kim (1996) between Korean-American students from private or public universities, or from east or west coast universities. 237 CHAPTER 16 THE FACILITATION OF ADJUSTMENT TO COLLEGE (in effect, as of Oct ’01, we have 2 “facilitation” chapters -- this one and the “integrated summary” of facilitation studies that’s given at the end of the next chapter. Maybe will want to leave the latter in Chapter 17 if decide to retain a summary chapter, or maybe incorporate that material at the end of this Chapter 16, or even – especially if decide not to use a summary chapter – make that material a new chapter that takes an integrative analysis approach to the facilitation studies rather than the serial approach taken in the current chapter. In any event, the solution can’t be one or the other -- both I think are needed.) Considered in this chapter are studies that have used the SACQ (and sometimes the Anticipated Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire, or ASACQ, as well) to evaluate the consequences of attempts to facilitate or remediate student adjustment to college. Because these attempts typically involve environmental manipulations, they actually belong in the prior chapters that reviewed environmental factors as determinants of adjustment to college, but because of their practical significance as deliberate efforts to enhance adjustment they will be treated separately. To date, approximately twenty-six SACQ (and sometimes ASACQ)-using intervention studies have been reported. These studies differ in a number of ways – i.e., the timing of the intervention; the nature of the target population; the nature of the intervention; the means of assessment of consequences; and the findings -- and within some of those ways there are very wide variations. These differences make very difficult if not impossible a conceptually organized, 238 integrated presentation of the available studies that does not destroy the character of each individual study. Therefore, before attempting such an approach each study will first be considered as a unit, ordered somewhat arbitrarily in terms of the time of occurrence of the intervention. Interventions Conducted with College-Bound High School Seniors. Two investigators describe interventions with college-bound high school students in the second half of their senior year for the purpose of facilitating the transition from high school to whatever college they would be attending in the fall, each using a different means of intervention. Mendelowitz (1990/1991) conducted weekly small group counseling sessions during the last two months of the school year, the purpose of which was to identify and discuss challenges to be met in the impending transition and ways of dealing with them. Consequences of the experience were assessed by means of the ASACQ (to provide a base measure of the student’s prematriculation degree of confidence for dealing with the impending transition) and SACQ (to measure the student’s actual subsequent adjustment). Participant and control groups both showed score declines from the ASACQ, administered at the termination of the former groups’ series of counseling sessions, to the SACQ administered the following November after matriculation at college, but the control group had the steeper declines, implying that the participants experienced lesser disillusionment regarding their adaptive capacity. This particular effect, however, was not tested for statistical significance. But the participants did have significantly higher Academic and Social Adjustment subscale scores on the November testing than the controls, suggesting that the counseling sessions did have beneficial consequences for the adjustment to college. 239 Buchanan (1991, 1993), the second investigator working with college-bound high school seniors, had three groups of students matched on SAT scores, one serving as a treatment group and the other two as different kinds of control groups, each of which had somewhat different experiences throughout the second half of the school year. The treatment group in addition to the regular high school program took a “modified University 101 course” – i. e., an orientation course ordinarily used with college freshmen but in this study adapted for high school students – designed to increase students’ knowledge about the college environment, career issues, and themselves. One control group took an introductory college lecture course in government in addition to their high school program, and the other had just the regular high school program. The SACQ was administered to all study participants while at their various colleges the following fall, and mean differences among the groups were compared (but not by inferential statistics) and interpreted as favoring the University 101 group on all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment. The principal reason given by the investigator for not running statistical tests was the small sample sizes (fewer than twenty students in each group), but there were other design problems that would have complicated any statistical interpretation, including some that could have reduced the possibility of hypothesis-confirming results. Interventions Conducted in the Summer or Immediately Prior to Matriculation Other prematriculation interventions occurred in the summer after graduation from high school or just prior to the start of the college's academic year. Keenan (1992) described a fourweek pre-freshman academic/social experience for students on special admittance status because of low standing (relative to other students in the class) on high school grades and on tests measuring scholastic aptitude and achievement. Evaluation of the program's consequences 240 included administration of the SACQ to program participants and a control group in March of the freshman year. Though there were no SACQ differences between participants and controls, participants' ratings of satisfaction with the program upon its completion (in August) correlated significantly and positively with all SACQ variables obtained the subsequent March, highest with the Attachment subscale. In a study reported by Elacqua (1992a&b), freshman students who were seriously disadvantaged academically and who had attended a six-week orientation program prematriculation had higher second semester SACQ Attachment subscale scores than less disadvantaged, regularly admitted, or especially advantaged students who did not attend such a program. Appenzeller (1998) presents the results of a six week, prematriculation, residence hallbased program focusing on enhancement of academic and social skills in economically and educationally disadvantaged entering freshmen. The students came from four successive entering classes in four successive years, but were all tested in the same second semester of one academic year, so they ranged from freshman through senior status at the time of testing. The participants were from different ethnic backgrounds (primarily Latino, some Asian-American, and smaller numbers of African-American and white students). No significant SACQ differences were found by Appenzeller (1998) between all matriculants attending the summer program and a control group that did not, but differences between attenders and non-attenders were also examined for students identified on the basis of various characteristics. Thus, there were no differences on any SACQ variables between program attenders and non-attenders for male or female students; for freshman, junior, or senior 241 students (sophomore attenders had higher scores than non-attenders on the SACQ Attachment subscale and full scale); for students who met regular admissions criteria in terms of test scores and high school grades and those who did not; for the Latino, Asian-American or white students (for black students, with small numbers in both the attender and non-attender groups, the former group had higher social adjustment scores); or students for whom English was a first or second language. Also using prematriculation intervention, but not with disadvantaged students, Martin (1988; see also Martin & Dixon, 1989) found no differences on any of the SACQ indices, obtained in the middle of the first semester, between students who had attended a two-day voluntary summer orientation program and those who had not. Subsequently the same investigators (Martin & Dixon, 1994), with a sample comprised of attenders at their voluntary freshman orientation programs in four different summers (two days long in one summer and three days in the other summers), who were administered the SACQ at various points over the four years of college but none earlier than the third semester, found no differences between them and non-attenders on any of the SACQ subscales. Jackson (1998) found no difference on any SACQ variable between students who had attended a freshman orientation program (of unmentioned length, presumably immediately prior to matriculation) and those who had not. One study (Brown, 1996, 1997) that involved prematriculation intervention, but also postmatriculation intervention, will be considered in the following section. Interventions Conducted Post-Matriculation Most studies employing the SACQ (and sometimes the ASACQ as well) in assessing consequences of interventions have made the interventions after the beginning of the academic 242 year. Several studies employed interventions that began immediately following matriculation of freshman students. Schwitzer, McGovern, and Robbins (1991) reported that students attending a ten-week freshman orientation seminar starting at the beginning of the first semester showed a significant score increase on the Social Adjustment subscale (the only SACQ variable employed) from the beginning to the end of the seminar. Significant positive correlations were found by Benson (1999) between number of sessions attended in a 15-session orientation program at the start of the freshman year and all SACQ indices except personal-emotional adjustment taken in the tenth week of the first semester, the strongest values occurring on the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales. Lamothe et al. (1995) conducted a six-week, small-group, social support-based intervention program starting in the first week of the freshman year, with one and a half hour weekly sessions focusing on presentations and discussions of particular themes (campus geography, formation and maintenance of social ties on campus, pre-university social ties, residential issues, academic issues). With the SACQ administered approximately two weeks after the final group meetings, the students experiencing the group intervention had higher scores on the Academic Adjustment subscale and full scale than a control group of students not experiencing the intervention. Though members of the control group had not been invited to join a treatment group, they had volunteered for participation in the program and had been assigned to a questionnaire-only sample rather than to a questionnaire plus discussion group (i. e., a treatment group), in this way possibly reducing the effects of self-selection for a voluntary project. 243 Subsequent to the Lamothe et al. (1995) study just described, Pratt, Hunsberger, Pancer, Alisat, Bowers, Mackey, Ostaniewicz, Rog, Terzian, and Thomas (2000) carried out a somewhat similar social support-based, small-group discussion, intervention program at the same university. From a pool of matriculating first-year students who had volunteered to participate in a “study focused on facilitating the transition into college,” some were assigned to treatment (membership in one of several small discussion groups) and some to control samples matched for several variables such as age, gender, parental education level, financial aid status, and university living arrangements. The discussion groups consisted of approximately ten freshmen each, each group having two specially-trained senior undergraduate or graduate student facilitators. The initial step in the intervention process of the Pratt et al. (2000) program was an individual interview for each freshman with one of their group’s two facilitators, followed by a first meeting of the groups in the first week of the academic year. Each group met a total of nine times, for one and a quarter to one and a half hours each session, weekly for the first four meetings, then bi-weekly until November, once in late January, and once in early March. Thus, important differences between this and the prior Lamothe et al. (1995) study were a larger number of meetings for each group (half again as many) extending over a longer period of time (even into the second semester), with more concentrated occurrence at the outset. Also, the sample sizes were almost double those of the earlier study, and there were two post-matriculation SACQ administrations (in November and March) instead of one. The group sessions followed a format described as “somewhat standardized” and consisted of “semi-structured exercises” as well as discussion focused on the same kinds of themes as employed in Lamothe et al. with addition of issues like balancing academic work and social life, and peer pressures and personal 244 values (regarding drugs, alcohol, sex). Reporting only the SACQ full scale score, Pratt et al. (2000) found no difference between the treatment and control samples in the November testing, but significantly higher scores for the former sample in the March administration. By personal communication, Pratt (2001) stated that there were no significant differences between the treatment and control groups on any of the SACQ subscales, though all differences were in the predicted direction and approached significance for academic adjustment. Savino (1987; see also Savino et al., 1986a) used the SACQ to examine the effects of a dormitory-based voluntary peer support intervention program conducted toward the beginning of the freshman year. Students in dormitories providing the program had higher full scale and Social Adjustment and Attachment subscale scores on a first-semester testing than did students in dormitories not providing such a program. But no such effects were found in a testing the following spring. Brunelle-Joiner (1999) describes findings from an extended orientation program, the First Year Experience, a two-hour credit course that involved two-hour meetings once a week for the entire first semester for students who voluntarily enrolled in the course. Compared with a control group comprised of students who were matched with the treatment group on high school grade point average, Scholastic Aptitude Test scores, and a measure of personal resiliency, but who elected not to enroll in the extended orientation program (thus raising the issue of effects of selfselection), the program participants had higher SACQ full scale scores, the only SACQ variable for which a test of significance was reported. Fox (2000) also reports use of a First Year Experience Program that apparently ran 245 through the first semester and involved voluntarily enrolled freshmen assigned to relatively small groups. But more detailed description of the effort is not provided, e.g., whether academic credit was awarded, the number and frequency of meetings, or the precise nature of the meetings’ content or methods beyond saying that the focus was on academic and personal enrichment, with an attempt to develop a sense of community, academic skills, decision-making skills, and an expanded sense of self. There was a control group consisting of randomly selected freshmen who apparently had chosen not to participate in the First Year Experience Program and were not enrolled in any other “learning community” on campus, but that group apparently was not matched with the treatment group on any specified criteria. Administration of the SACQ near the end of the first semester showed no difference between the treatment and control groups on the full scale score, the only SACQ variable reported. Halloran (2000) describes an extensive formation of a number of freshman learning communities (FLC’s) that were defined and organized in terms of themes associated with academic disciplines (e.g., natural sciences, social sciences, language studies) and career objectives (e.g., pre-medical, pre-law). Students could choose to become a member of one of these communities, in which all members would take the same five courses related to the particular community’s theme and attended only by members of that community. Additionally, such students all had an “FLC Orientation Course” that involved weekly contacts with the group’s faculty advisor in a mandatory seminar concerned with introduction to university life, carerr exploration, various group activities, participation in community service projects, etc. A control group of students who had elected not to participate in a learning community was established, and all students (i.e., both learning community participants and controls) were 246 administered the SACQ in the third (pre-test) and thirteenth (post-test) weeks of the first semester. Despite this apparently carefully and elegantly planned and implemented intervention program, no differential SACQ changes from pre-test to post-test were found between students who participated in a learning community and those who did not. Also, there were no such differences between subsamples of the treatment and control groups that might have been expected to profit from early facilitative interventions, viz., minority students and students whose parents did not have a college degree. Helman (1999) used the SACQ with science and engineering freshmen who had participated in a voluntary (“first-come first-served”) residential living-learning program lasting the entire fall semester and involving academic and social experiences intended to provide a stronger sense of community and academic support. All participants lived in the same residence hall; most had roommates who also were program participants; and most had at least one regular academic course that included fellow program members. There was a special one-credit seminar for program participants that met weekly, led by faculty and staff members, plus “co-curricular activities” including evening programs in the residence hall, all of which was intended to familarize participants with campus resources and faculty; develop academic skills and time and stress management abilities; provide opportunities for making social connections; and explore major fields and careers. Additionally there were tutoring services and periodic sessions with an academic advisor. However, the absence of a control group in Helman’s (1999) study severely reduced the possibility of employing SACQ scores to evaluate effects of participation in the program. The findings most closely akin to such use of SACQ variables involved ratings of students’ feelings 247 that the program had been helpful to them academically and socially. With findings given only for academic, social, and overall adjustment, students more inclined to assess the program as having been helpful academically reported better academic and overall adjustment than those less inclined; and those asserting beneficial social effects of the program reported better social adjustment. Helman also described other findings concerning relations between various aspects of program experience (e.g., study habits; relationships with peers, faculty, and academic support staff) and SACQ scores which are considered elsewhere in this monograph where appropriate. Schriver (1996) obtained no difference on the SACQ full scale score, the only SACQ variable used, between freshmen who had attended a voluntary orientation class in the first half of the first semester and those who did not. No differences on any SACQ variable were obtained by Zion (1990) between freshman students from two dormitories, one of which employed a special year-long, peer-advising orientation program. Kintner (1998) found no difference on the SACQ full scale, the only SACQ variable used, between participants in a first semester, ten week, freshman orientation seminar and a control group of freshmen at another, but similar, college who did not experience such a program. Davis (1988) describes a study that involved transfer as well as freshman student-athletes, where the intention was to evaluate the effectiveness of an early first semester ten-week seminar and small group discussion experience aimed at development of assorted coping skills (e. g., goal-setting ability, self-concept improvement, relaxation training, social networking). The SACQ was administered as a pre-test in the first week of the intervention, which was begun in the second week of the semester; as a post-test in the program’s next-to-last, ninth week; and again as a follow-up in the middle of the spring semester. There was no control group, 248 precluding true assessment of intervention effects, but there were some interesting if inexplicable findings. While Davis (1988) expected increase in SACQ scores from the pre- to the posttesting as a consequence of the intervention, actually the reverse was found on the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales and the full scale. Where the pre to post decreases, though statistically significant, were moderate in magnitude, and possibly attributable to an “end of the honeymoon” effect that might occur toward the end of a first semester in a new college, there were precipitous score drops from the posttest to the second semester follow-up in all SACQ variables ranging as high as 32%, for an average of almost 23% over the four subscales. Unfortunately, no evidence was provided that would account for such dramatic changes in measured adjustment. It might have been helpful if there had been not only a control group of student-athletes who did not experience the intervention, but also comparable treatment and control groups of students at the same university not athletically involved. Sullivan (1991) also got some contrary-to-expectation findings, but this time with use of a control group in a study attempting to evaluate the consequences of a full first-quarter comprehensive and intensive intervention. Freshman students who chose to participate in this special program were assigned to groups of 20 to 25 members each, each group organized around a particular academic theme. The members of each group took three courses together, at least one of which was a small class comprised only of program participants. Each group also met weekly in a discussion group led by an upperclass peer advisor, and there were opportunities arranged for faculty-student interaction outside of classrooms. Both the program participants and a control group of freshmen who had chosen not to participate in the special program were 249 administered a pre-program questionnaire, which revealed the two groups to be well matched on a number of important variables (except one important variable that came to light subsequently, to be mentioned momentarily). On the SACQ administered toward or soon after the end of the first quarter, program participants were found to score lower than nonparticipants in academic and personal-emotional adjustment, with no difference on the other SACQ indices. Sullivan (1991) appropriately pointed out that the unexpected findings could be due to some unknown person characteristics differentiating between students who did and did not elect to participate in the special program, but she also conjectured that some of the findings – especially the reversal of expected direction on two subscales – might paradoxically be attributable to the same program characteristics that were intended to facilitate adjustment to college. That is, she reasoned that the tight group involvement and cohesiveness promoted by the program could have generated competition among group members in academic performance (contributing to lowered scores on the Academic Adjustment subscale) and higher levels of anxiety (as seen in lowered scores on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale). Other possible contributing factors were pointed out by Sullivan (1991). Program participants had scored lower than nonparticipants on an “admissions index” based on high school grade point average and scholastic aptitude, though there was no difference on high school grade point average by itself. And consistent with that indication of pre-existing disadvantage for academic adjustment in the program participants, fewer of them than nonparticipants were found subsequently in a “high academic status category” at the end of the first college quarter. Finally, and very likely an important factor, the program participants had 250 been administered the SACQ in class just prior to final examinations at the end of the first quarter (presumably a relatively stressful time for students), whereas the SACQ administration to nonparticipants was less controlled, with an unspecified number not completing the Questionnaire until after the examinations (and therefore in presumably less stressful circumstances) or even after the start of the second quarter. This somewhat lengthy and detailed consideration of Sullivan’s (1991) study might be justified on the basis that it highlights important problems and pitfalls in the design and evaluation of interventions attempting to facilitate adjustment to college. Brown (1996; 1997; 1998) examined differences in SACQ variables among freshmen who had participated in three broad classes of orientation programs, some of the experiences occurring prematriculation and some postmatriculation. One class of orientation involved tenday, prematriculation, away-from-campus, “total immersion,” outdoor experiences (canoeing, cycling, sailing). A second class of program, held prematriculation and/or through the first semester, offered students an “alternative” opportunity to pursue particular interests or activities of a service or curricular nature through special projects, field trips, etc., operating from campus. The third class of orientation was a classroom-type experience held twice a week during the first five weeks of the semester with faculty/staff and peer counselors presenting and leading discussion of topics related to the transition into college. On the Social Adjustment and Attachment subscales, both the “outdoor” and “classroom” groups (which did not differ from each other) had higher scores than the “alternative” group, as did the “outdoor” group on the full scale score as well. It is not clear from this study, however, whether these effects are consequences of the different orientation experiences or of pre-existing 251 differences among students attracted to the different programs. In a pilot study done the previous year, Brown (1996) reports that freshmen who had had the “outdoor” experience had higher social adjustment, institutional attachment and full scale scores than did a group that combined the other two kinds of orientation programs. Baker and Siryk (1986) explored the practical usefulness of the SACQ in postmatriculation remedial intervention with freshman students. They employed the Questionnaire: (a) to identify students showing clear differences in effectiveness of first semester adjustment to college for assignment to matched treatment and control groups; (b) as a source of topics, especially concerning difficulties in adjustment to college, for discussion in interview with members of the treatment groups; and (c) to measure effects of intervention. On the basis of SACQ data collected in the middle of the first semester, freshmen in the extremes of score distributions (i.e., both low end, or poorer adjustment, and high end, or better adjustment) on the subscales were identified and, within each extreme, assigned to matched treatment and control groups. Members of both the low and high end treatment groups were invited for a feedback interview lasting from one to two hours, held over a period of several months from late November to mid-March. Though the primary content of the interviews was SACQ findings, also discussed where appropriate were ways of dealing with problems that the student may have been experiencing in adjustment to college. The SACQ was then readministered in the 11th week of the second semester to all members of the treatment and control groups. Baker and Siryk (1986) reported that the low-scoring students on the first SACQ administration who were interviewed showed greater improvement on the second semester 252 administration than their noninterviewed counterparts on all SACQ indices except the Academic Adjustment subscale, and their withdrawal from college by the start of the sophomore year was significantly less. No such differences were found between interviewed and noninterviewed students who had been high-scoring on the first semester SACQ adninistration, though the pattern of SACQ score changes from the first to the second semester was in the expected direction for all test indices. There was also evidence regarding institutional attachment that, for the less well-adjusted students, the earlier the intervention the greater the improvement of scores on re-test, while for well-adjusted students the later the intervention the greater the improvement of scores on re-test. Baker and Schultz (1993) described a three-part study, one part of which was a partial replication of Baker and Siryk (1986). All three parts investigated the consequences of individual interview for college adjustment in at-risk students. Each part employed the ASACQ and/or the SACQ either separately or jointly to identify a different kind of risk, and all three parts employed the SACQ by itself or jointly with the ASACQ as means of assessing the consequences of intervention. In the first part of Baker and Schultz’ (1993) study, matriculating freshmen with low scores on the prematriculated-administered Anticipated Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (presumed to indicate low expectations or self-confidence regarding one’s capacity for adjusting to the impending transition into college and thus at-risk) were assigned to matched treatment and control groups. Members of the treatment group each had an individual interview soon after the start of the first semester with the first author, a clinical psychologist and professor. The student was first provided feedback regarding his/her ASACQ results, followed 253 by discussion of relation between those findings and the student’s actual self-described adjustment to college, followed in turn by opportunity to discuss adjustment problems and ways of dealing with them. With the SACQ administered in the eighth week of the first semester, no significant differences were found between the treatment and control groups regarding pre- to postmatriculation changes on the ASACQ/SACQ subscales and full scale. That is, the expectation of more favorable (or less unfavorable) score change in the treatment group was not borne out. However, a conclusion of lack of effect of interview is tempered somewhat by the fact of small numbers of students in the treatment and control groups, and by the tantalizing (though not statistically significant) finding that the proportion of control group members withdrawing from college by the start of the sophomore year was almost twice that of the treatment group. The second part of the Baker and Schultz (1993) study focused on students who had shown substantial declines from the level of prematriculation expectations or confidence regarding capacity for dealing with the impending adjustment to college, as measured by the ASACQ, to the actual level of adjustment as indicated by the SACQ administered in the eighth week of the first semester. These students, viewed as having experienced disillusioment concerning their capacity for adjusting to college and thus at-risk, were assigned to matched treatment and control groups. The members of the treatment group had an individual interview with the study’s second author, an undergraduate senior psychology major doing honors research, held from soon after the start of the second semester through late February, followed by readministration of the SACQ in the tenth week of the second semester. 254 The content of the interview, consistent with the educational and training level of the interviewer, was not a clinical or counseling focus on the diagnosis and remediation of adjustment difficulties, which characterized the interviews in the first part of the study. Rather, it focused on oral administration of abbreviated versions of the ASACQ and SACQ used to induce recall of thoughts and feelings at the times of the original administrations of the instruments, with discussion of the relation between the ratings made by students in the interview and those made originally on the earlier ASACQ and SACQ administrations. No significant differences were found between treatment and control students in score changes from the first to the second administrations of the SACQ on any of that instrument’s subscales or full scale. But the pattern of score changes was in the expected direction. That is, for the treatment group there were score increases on all SACQ variables from the first to the second semester, while for the control group there was either a smaller increase or a decrease. The samples in this second part of the study were slightly larger than in the first part, but still relatively small. The third part of the Baker and Schultz (1993) study was a partial replication of Baker and Siryk (1986). Students with particularly low scores on the first semester administration of the SACQ were identified and assigned to matched treatment and control groups, and members of the former group invited for interview with the first author, a clinical psychologist/professor. This interview was similar to that employed in Baker and Siryk, lasted from one to two hours, was held between late January and mid-March, and consisted of feedback of information concerning the student’s first semester SCAQ scores, discussion of the degree of correspondence between the SACQ data and the student’s actual experienced adjustment, and, where desired by 255 the student, exploration of ways of dealing with any adjustment problems reported by the student. Of the 30 students in the treatment group, five had also had a prior interview with the first author in the first part of the study and ten had also had a prior interview with the second author in the second part of the study. The SACQ was readministered to members of the treatment and control groups in the tenth week of the second semester. Analysis of SACQ score changes from the first to the second semester showed greater net improvement in academic and overall adjustment for the treatment than the control group, and a similar finding narrowly missing significance for personal-emotional adjustment. Withdrawal from college by the start of the sophomore year was two and half times greater in the control group, a finding also narrowly missing statistical significance. As a final step in the Baker and Schultz (1993) study, students from the treatment and control groups in all three parts of the study were combined in order to provide more sizeable treatment and control samples for testing the consequences of intervention by interview. Withdrawal from college by the start of the sophomore year was still two and a half times greater in the now larger control group (i.e., all students identified as at-risk on the basis of ASACQ and/or SACQ scores, but without any attempt at intervention) than the now larger treatment group (i.e., all similarly identified at-risk students who were interviewed), but the finding was now statistically significant. An additional statistically significant effect favoring the treatment group was that it earned more course credits during the freshman year than did the control group. Two observations may be offered regarding the findings of the Baker and Siryk (1986) and Baker and Schultz (1993) studies. First, intervention by individual interview apparently can have beneficial consequences for students identified as at-risk on the basis of SACQ scores. 256 Second, replication of the first two parts of the Baker and Schultz study with larger samples would be desirable. Paulshock (1994), like Baker and Siryk (1986) and Baker and Schultz (1993), used the SACQ to identify less well adjusted freshmen for a first semester intervention. Two treatment groups and a control group were constructed. The former two groups undertook a five week therapeutic journal-writing program covering themes hopefully associated with facilitation of adjustment to college, but one group being given highly structured and the other less structured instructions. The control group received no special treatment. After five weeks, when the writing program was completed, the SACQ was re-administered to all participants. Using only the full scale score from the SACQ, Paulshock found no significant difference among the three groups in score change from pre- to posttest, indicating no consequences of the interventions. However, there was a positive correlation between student-reported number of journal pages written and amount of increase in SACQ full scale score from pre- to posttest. Using another kind of therapeutic intervention – i. e., reminiscence therapy focusing on learning/educational issues in four one-hour weekly group sessions with older, non-traditional age students of mixed college year levels -- Schatzman (1994) found no significant differences between the treatment and control groups in pre- to post-intervention scores on any SACQ variable. 257 CHAPTER 17 SUMMARY AND IMPLICATIONS FOR FUTURE INVESTIGATIONS (I’m writing this note as I come to the end of the writing of this chapter, (i.e., #17) the summary, which I think is terrible, doesn’t do what I intended to do for a summary, and I think that maybe the only use of the material in this chapter may be some occasional stuff that could be added to earlier chapters where appropriate.) The basic purpose of this monograph was to present in one place what is known about adjustment to college from research with the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire, in the hope of promoting further research that will advance understanding of that adjustment. Accordingly, the monograph will close with a summary of findings and with some thoughts concerning future research. Of particular importance will be the identification of issues that merit clarification or further exploration as well as new areas for investigation. As indicated earlier in Chapter 2, sources for particular findings are not cited in the present chapter, so that presentation of an overall view of the fruits of SACQ-using research is facilitated. All findings mentioned in this chapter will have been described and properly cited where considered in earlier chapters, and readers may want to use the Reference section to locate sources for particular findings of interest. The Definition of Adjustment to College Numerous studies by many investigators employing the SACQ have yielded considerable information concerning the operational meaning of adjustment to college. There is ample evidence that it is a measurable construct that has a wide variety of behavioral and experiential correlates which are readily recognizable as significant adaptational events in the everyday lives 258 of college students. There is also ample justification for conceptualizing the construct as having different facets which are differentially manifested in the wide variety of demonstrated correlates. It is clear, too, that this adjustment may vary over time, that it is not to be thought of as a necessarily stable and enduring characteristic of individuals, and that intra- as well as interindividual variation occurs in the adjustment and its several aspects. One facet of the construct, academic adjustment, is manifested in quality of academic performance (grade point average; academic honor society membership) and seriousness of academic purpose (decidedness about and satisfaction with academic major; career plan certainty; class attendance; study habits; attitude towards faculty). Another facet, social adjustment, is reflected in extent of involvement in the life of the college (participation in extracurricular activities; frequency of leaving campus to visit home, and number of other kinds of contacts with family and friends at home; full- or part-time enrollment; amount of time spent in gainful employment); in the formation and maintenance of relationships (number of close friends; length of friendships; amount of time socializing with friends; felt quality of relationships with other students; involvement in romantic relationships), and in interpersonal/occupational competence (qualifying for position as dormitory assistant). Personal-emotional adjustment is evinced in whether the student is known to campus psychological services agencies or other campus sources of support, or reports of being or having been in psychotherapy or counseling; in self-reports of feelings of strain and stress or experiencing difficulty adjusting to college; in various aspects of mental health (e.g., selfreported state of mental health, including occurrence of depression, anxiety, dissociation symptoms, eating disorders); and in various aspects of physical health (measures of general 259 physical health; number of physical symptoms reported; number of appointments with college physicians; number of class absences due to illness). The fourth aspect of adjustment identified, commitment to the college experience, including attachment to the college attended, is manifested by enrollment status (continuing or dropping-out) and by expressions of satisfaction/dissatisfaction with the overall college experience and the college attended (level of preference for the college attended in relation to other colleges; opinion concerning how a college has dealt with its students and provided occupational preparation; entertaining thoughts of leaving the college). While the meaning, or extent of the domain, of the construct "adjustment-to-college" has thus been fairly well spelled out by SACQ research conducted to date, it undoubtedly has not been exhausted and further research can provide additional clarification. Two areas, for example, that have not yet been sufficiently explored in definition of the construct are those aspects of the student's life and behavior that would reflect (1) the process of separation from family and friends in the home locale and how it is dealt with, and (2) the kind and quality of relationships that are established with professors and other college staff persons, attitudes towards those persons, and ways of dealing with them, whether academically or socially. With respect to the latter issue, the question is not so much whether close or friendly relationships are formed, but how relationships with or attitudes towards those persons are part of the adjustment experience. The reader may well have ideas regarding other facets of the construct that might be elaborated. Much of the construct content laid out thus far has been gathered from analysis of the differential behavioral and experiential correlates of the four SACQ subscales. The reader will 260 recall that within each subscale item-clusters were identified that are thought to tap different aspects of the adjustment area addressed by the subscale. A promising way to delineate further the definition of adjustment to college would be to focus attention on the differential behavioral and experiential correlates of those various item-clusters. Some of the item-clusters, it should be pointed out, contain relatively few items, and if the kind of research suggested above were to be attempted, it may be necessary to develop new means of measuring particular aspects of adjustment areas, possibly by building on the items that presently constitute the item-clusters. One area that has been explored very little is whether it is possible to identify "types" of adjustment, or types of adjusting students, in terms of patterns of behavior or experience associated with patterns of SACQ subscale (or even item-cluster) scores. Determinants of Adjustment to College As the reader must now be aware, a great deal of information is available from research with the SACQ regarding possible determinants of adjustment to college. A considerable amount is known about some of these determinants because they have been studied by multiple investigators using either different measures of a particular variable or sometimes the same measure, typically with consistent outcomes in terms of occurrence and strength of association. About many of the lesser studied variables there is promising evidence of association with adjustment to college that hopefully may be replicated and extended in new studies. The variety of variables that have been used is broad, necessitating -- as recalled from the earlier consideration of these variables -- their being sorted into a number of categories. Two major categories were (1) person characteristics and (2) environment characteristics. 261 *************** Some of the closest and most consistently obtained relations between the SACQ and person characteristics as possible determinant variables involve psychological health. Thus, total and/or composite scores of general mental health from a number of tests, in a number of studies, by a number of investigators, have regularly yielded significant correlation with all aspects of college adjustment measured by the SACQ, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. The same is true in the case of depression as a particular aspect of mental health, with even more test measures of depression being employed, two of the tests in a number of different studies. In the majority of these studies the measures of depression were administered postmatriculation, when it would be just as easy to construe such measures as assessing adjustment-to-college outcomes rather than determinants of that adjustment. However, in several instances the measures of depression were administered pre- as well as postmatriculation – or, in one instance, in the first week following matriculation -- with lesser but still significant association found for the earlier administration. An implication of this finding is that preexisting depression may profitably be regarded as a potential determinant of adjustment to be addressed remedially prematriculation or durimg the adjustment to college.. Anxiety is another particular aspect of mental health that has been investigated at some length in relation to college adjustment, with results in most studies quite similar to those found for depression. That is, a number of studies by a number of different investigators using a number of different psychometric instruments have rather consistently and similarly obtained correlation with the several SACQ variables, typically strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. 262 Unlike the studies of depression, which treated that variable as a homogeneous phenomenon, different kinds of anxiety have been examined – state, trait, separation, phobic, and death anxiety – and even worry as an aspect of anxiety. One study, instead of using a psychometric device to assess anxiety, examined adjustment differences among students varying in manifestation of symptoms of diagnosable separation anxiety disorder. But no studies reported to date used prematriculation administration of test measures of anxiety, which could be useful in clarifying the relation between anxiety and college adjustment, and would have implications for both pre- and postmatriculation remedial interventions. A number of investigators have studied the relation to college adjustment of other less precisely defined, or possibly more generic, affective or “emotional” states, variously identified as emotional lability, negative affect, neuroticism, emotional stress, perceived stress, and “reverse culture shock” (experienced by American missionaries’ children returning from residence in foreign countries to attend college in the United States). The findings in most of these instances are much like those for depression and anxiety, i. e., correlating usually with all SACQ variables, strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. As in the case of depression cited above, some investigators administered a measure of perceived stress either before or in the first week after matrriculation and again in the second semester of the freshman year, when the SACQ was also administered. Perceived stress at all three testing times predicted all SACQ variables, the closer in time the measure of stress to that of adjustment the stronger the relation. Two other kinds of problems in psychological health – dissociative experiences and symptoms, and eating disorders – have been found to have expected direction relation with 263 SACQ variables. A number of studies have dealt with person characteristics that may be viewed as aspects of good mental health, in contrast to those discussed in the several foregoing paragraphs. These include measures of psychological well-being, positive affect, optimism, positiveness of attitude, sense of self-fulfillment, and self-discipline in dealing with one’s emotions and temper. Investigators employing such variables typically report positive and sometimes strong relation with SACQ variables. A few researchers have looked at the role of general physical health (self-assessed state of health, occurrence of physical symptoms, number of visits to a campus physician, number of class absences for illness) in college adjustment. Expected direction relation with selected SACQ variables (usually personal-emotional or overall adjustment) has been reported for such variables, except that visits to a campus physician was related moderately strongly (and negatively) to all SACQ variables. Regarding more specific aspects of physical health, only one study has looked for adjustment differences between students with and without various physical disabilities, as well as among disabled students, and found none. But in another study learning disabled students had lower scores on academic and personal-emotional adjustment than non-learning disabled students. Finally, in a sample drawn from a general college population, strong negative correlations with all SACQ variables – highest with personal-emotional and academic adjustment – were found for behavioral characteristics and symptoms associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Several possible avenues of research concerning psycholgical health as a determinant of 264 college adjustment may be suggested. One, already mentioned earlier, would be more frequent use of prematriculation identification and measurement of relevant variables (e. g., depression, anxiety) because in the more common postmatriculation use such measures may be reflecting the adjustment itself. Additional studies concerning some of the less often used mental and physical health determinant variables (e.g., dissociative experiences and symptoms, separation anxiety disorder, eating disorders, learning disability, ADHD) would be desirable, as would use of forms of psychopathology not yet employed, and, especially, more extensive study of psychological characteristics indicating good mental health. Methodologically, the heavy use of tests in identification of mental health variables might be supplemented by greater use of criterion groups established on the basis of traditional clinical diagnostic procedures. That is, other means than psychometric devices could be employed for codifying these constructs in investigation of their relation to adjustment in college. *************** A second category of person variables that has shown consistent and close association with adjustment to college – typically with all areas measured by the SACQ in roughly equal magnitude, often strongly and sometimes robustly -- includes several constructs related to selfregard or self-appraisal (e. g., self-esteem, self-efficacy, self-confidence, and self-concept, plus assorted others like primary appraisals in the coping process, perceived self-effectiveness, realistic self-appraisal, rational thinking about oneself, narcissistic injury). Sometimes these variables as used in SACQ research are conceptualized in trait-like fashion as broadly applicable across different areas of function (i.e., pertaining to oneself in general), and other times they are conceived as specific to particular areas of function (i. e., pertaining to specific aspects of 265 oneself: social, cognitive, or emotional functioing; athletic performance, capacity to cope with life demands despite disability, adjusting to college). As in the case of depression, these variables have been employed by a large number of investigators in a large number of studies with a lot of different self-regard/self-appraisal measures, several of them well-established “standards” that were used in a number of the studies. As was true of the psychological health variables, most of the studies employing selfregard/self-appraisal variables measured them postmatriculation, raising the same question whether what is being assessed is a determinant of adjustment or an adjustment outcome. Fortunately two (chk #) studies assessed the determinant variable prematriculation, or very soon after the start of the students’ first semester, and obtained significant -- even reasonably substantial -- association with SACQ indices, suggesting that this may be a very fruitful source of constructs for understanding and predicting adjustment to college. Interestingly, there is some evidence that prematriculation self-efficacy-like judgments regarding one’s capacity to deal with the impending college experience predict adjustment beyond the freshman year and possibly even late into the fourth year. It has become increasingly the case in recent years that self variables are conceptualized less in terms of a unitary self than one consisting of different facets (e. g., physical, moral-ethical, intellectual, social, familial). Two studies using two different means of assessing such facets of the self reported considerably consistent relation with all SACQ variables, suggesting that additional studies of this sort may help to further our understanding of adjustment to college. In a number of studies, measures of self-efficacy or self-esteem with regard to social activity were found to correlate with all SACQ variables, typically strongest with social 266 adjustment, as one would expect. In several studies concerning such self-evaluation of cognitive functioning (regarding, for example, intellectual ability, scholastic competence, creativity; ability to deal with particular kinds of academic courses; problem-solving ability), while not infrequently correlating with other SACQ subscales in those instances where they were used, had its strongest values with academic adjustment. Undoubtedly a number of the variables in the self-regard/self-appraisal category are overlapping or redundant in meaning, and research directed toward clarification of that state of affairs could be useful. Even apart from the particular topic of this monograph, as well as in future studies of determinants of college adjustment, it would seem desirable for the plethora of self-regard/self-appraisal variables to be reduced to a more manageable few that have wellstandardized means of measurement. Research exploring relations between self-regard/self-appraisal and other variables as determinants of adjustment to college might be particularly worthwhile. For example, it has been shown that prematriculation level of confidence regarding one's capacity for adjusting to college is a predictor of the subsequent adjustment, but by no means a perfect one. Maybe it would help, in understanding those high in prematriculation self-confidence who subsequently experience serious adjustment problems, to couple the self-confidence measure with, for example, a prematriculation measure of locus of control. Is it possible that highly self-confident but subsequently poorly adjusting students are more likely to be "externals" than "internals," more subject to disturbing environmental factors than other more inner-directed students might be? Other combinations of the two predictor variables would similarly have implications for quality of adjustment. And the same kind of multivariate approach could be useful with any number of 267 other combinations of variables. *************** While acknowledging the cognitive nature of certain variables considered under the various rubrics (i.e., chapter headings) used to organize findings regarding person determinants, a separate chapter was devoted to a group of variables that might be regarded as more “purely” cognitive in nature. This third grouping included three subcategories: cognition of causality in the explanation of one’s own behavior; ideational/intellectual characteristics; and means of reacting to and coping with stressors. The concept of locus of control refers to the degree to which an individual sees the primary responsibility (causation) for his/her behavior as inhering in oneself (internality) or outside of oneself (externality). Three different means of measuring locus of control as a general trait, two of them in common usage, have been found in close to a dozen studies to correlate with all SACQ variables in expected direction (internality associated with better adjustment), strongest with academic and personal-emotional adjustment. One study that involved pre- and postmatriculation administration of the independent variable obtained significant relation for both testing times, strongest for the latter. Like the self-regard/self-appraisal variables considered earlier, locus of control can be defined not only as a general trait but also in relation to specific kinds of situations or areas of function. Thus, several studies of academic locus of control (i. e., regarding academic demands and functioning), by different investigators using two different means of measuring that variable, showed correlation frequently and sometimes robustly in the expected direction with SACQ variables, strongest among the subscales with academic adjustment and considerably stronger 268 than the correlation for the general measure of locus of control with academic adjustment. One investigator was interested in the consequences for college adjustment of locus of control for positive and negative life events separately as well as life events in general, finding some expected-direction but weak correlations with SACQ variables. Another researcher adapted the SACQ itself for use as a prematriculation measure of locus of control for adjusting to college. Moving away now from locus of control but still in the general area of cognition of causality, complexity of causality in the explanation of one’s own and others’ behavior – including with a measure situationally-specific for college students -- has been examined without notable outcome except for findings of weak to modest correlations in the direction contrary to prediction with one of the SACQ subscales. Also, the habitual tendency to perceive negative life events as caused by factors that will persist over time (i.e., stable attribution of causatio), and by factors likely to be operative in a wide variety of situations (global attribution of causality), are negatively associated with some SACQ indices. And perceiving positive life events as caused by factors operative in a wide variety of situations is positively correlated with SACQ variables. Several ideational/intellectual characteristics – very likely considerably redundant -- have been examined in relation to SACQ variables. They include: ideational flexibility (the ability to view situations from multiple perspectives), predicting all SACQ variables, but weakly; receptivity to new ideas (the “Big Five’s” Openness to Experience factor), more widely used and maybe a little more successful in showing association with the SACQ; absolutistic thinking, related moderately strongly (and negatively) with all SACQ indices; organized thinking/intellectual efficiency; predicting all SACQ variables, strongest with academic 269 adjustment; judgment, also associated with all SACQ variables, strongest with academic adjustment; and ideational complexity, by itself not predicting college adjustment, but among students reporting high stress prematriculation those characterized by high ideational complexity had higher scores on all SACQ variables than low complexity students. Also belonging among ideational/intellectual variables would be measures of academic aptitude, for which findings are fragmentary and inconsistent. For composite measures (verbal and quantitative scores combined), positive correlations are occasionaly found with the various SACQ indices, maybe more so with academic and personal-emotional adjustment, but not always. Use of the verbal and quantitative scores separately has been less frequent, and findings are even more inconsistent, with negative correlations sometimes occurring, especially for the quantitative score. Yet another set of cognitive characteristics is means of reacting to and coping with stressors. Some investigators have looked at stress tolerance and cognate variables (e.g., personality hardiness, personal resilience) as general traits, in terms of ability to deal with pressure and adversity irrespective of particular circumstances. In several studies those investigators have found such variables to correlate consistently and moderately to strongly with all SACQ variables, usually strongest with personal-emotional adjustment. A number of investigators have examined particular means of coping with pressure and stressors, using several different means of measuring -- and defining -- such variables. Effective means of coping have been variously identified as task-oriented, problem-focused, active (cognitive and behavioral), or proactive, all characterized as involving strategies intended to deal directly and decisively with sources of stress through timely formulation and persistent 270 implementation of appropriate plans. Findings from use of this kind of effective coping in relation to the SACQ cover a very broad range from, rarely, no relation, through occasional weak positive relation with one or another SACQ index, to, more commonly, stronger (sometimes even robust) positive correlations with various or even all SACQ indices, maybe strongest with academic adjustment. A less effective means of dealing with stress is identified as emotion-oriented coping, defined as attempting to manage one’s emotional response to stressful situations at the expense of dealing more directly with the source of stress, or venting of emotions. This variable correlates negatively and sometimes strongly with SACQ indices, most commonly with personalemotional adjustment. A third, and also less effective, means of dealing with stress is avoidance coping, involving failure to confront the source of stress, which has been seen to range from no correlation with SACQ indices to modest through more strongly negative relation with all those indices. *************** A fourth category of person variables that are closely relevant to the educational enterprise and shown to be associated with adjustment to college is characteristics related to goal orientation. This category includes some variables that are cognitive in nature and some that are motivational, but common to all is a focus on the future, and on what one hopes or expects to be doing in that future. These variables may be ordered in subcategories of achievement need, goal directedness, academic motivation, and vocational and educational planning. Several means of measuring need for achievemnt quite directly have been found to relate with the several SACQ variables, particularly academic adjustment. Other means of measuring 271 that need less directly – especially the conscientiousness variable from the “big five” factors – have been seen to relate to all SACQ indices, including robustly with academic adjustment. One study was concerned with need for achievement in the social area (competitiveness and drive for upward mobility), which correlated strongly with all SACQ indices, especially social adjustment. And measures of need for achievement occupationally correlated with certain SACQ variables. A number of measures of goal directedness, or sense of purpose, including goal-setting skills and success in attaining goals, have been found to relate, sometimes rather strongly, to all SACQ variables, especially academic and personal-emotional adjustment. Students who have adequately explored possible life goals and made goal commitments show good adjustment to college. Students who are experiencing difficulty in exploring possible goals and haven’t made commtiment, and those who have neither explored nor made commitment, adjust less well to college. Several measures of academic motivation have been shown to be associated positively with the various SACQ indices, particularly academic adjustment. Intrinsic or inherent academic motivation (goals in attending college chosen by oneself) similarly correlates positively with certain SACQ variables, while extrinsic academic motivation (reflecting expectations imposed by others) correlates negatively. A measure of capacity to attain academic goals without direction from others was robustly associated with academic adjustment, substantially so with personalemotional adjustment, and to a lesser degree with social adjustment. Valuing intellectual activities and pursuits, including scholarly effort, related robustly with SACQ indices, most so with academic adjustment; having a strong academic self-concept correlated strongly with 272 academic adjustment. Regarding planning behavior in general, avoidance of making important life decisions (i.e., not planning), or making unstable decisions (not planning well), were found to correlate negatively with academic and overall adjustment. Good planning behavior, defined in terms of seeking information about oneself and using it to examine and change self-understandings when appropriate in making life decisions, correlated positively with academic adjustment. Maybe paradoxically, in terms of the findings regarding extrinsic academic motivation cited above, trying to conform in important life decision-making to desires and expectations of others correlated positively with institutional attachment/goal commitment and overal college adjustment. Regarding vocational planning in particular, in several studies clarity/stability/certainty of such plans correlates with all SACQ indices, particularly academic adjustment, more strongly in a post- than prematriculation administration of the independent variable with overall college adjustment. Regarding educational planning, in a number of studies by a number of investigators degree of decidedness regarding academic major -- in freshman students particularly – was found to correlate with SACQ variables, academic adjustment especially. Some particularly interesting findings in this regard concern the interaction between academic major decidedness and time of the academic year (or point in the educational experience) in determining adjustment to college, as well as the interaction between major-decidedness and institutional variables. In one study adjustment differences among freshmen varying in decidedness were more apparent in a college requiring declaration of major in the freshman year than in a college requiring declaration in the 273 sophomore year. The association between major status and adjustment seems to be more apparent when there is pressure to make a decision regarding major, whether imposed by the self or by the institution, for example, and there is evidence that it is particularly catastrophic for adjustment for major-changing students to be in an institution not offering the major they want. The latter observation may be so obvious as to be trite, but the basic issue concerning relation between person and institutional characteristics as determinants of adjustment would seem to be a very promising area for future investigation. For example, most studies in this area to date have been done with freshmen in liberal arts colleges, where it has been found that the relation between major decidedness and adjustment is more evident in the second semester -when, presumably, the pressure to decide on a major grows -- than the first. Would the relation become even more evident in the sophomore year, when pressures for decision undoubtedly intensify? Amount of thinking or planning by a student prematriculation regarding academic goals in college, or talking with parents or other persons about the impending college experience, predicted academic and/or overall adjustment in college, as did the amount of information a student had about various aspects of the impending experience. Paradoxically, in one study the greater the high school students’ knowledge concerning the general characteristics of colleges and universities, the less good their subsequent adjustment to the college they attended. *************** A fifth class of variables, perceived relationship with parents, would seem to include person characteristics of considerable developmental importance that should have implications for a student's adjustment to college. But the instrument that has probably been most used with 274 the SACQ, and among the least fruitful in showing relation, is one that is intended to measure psychological separation from parents (i.e., independence or autonomy, a mark of maturity). That instrument purports to assess four maturationally-desirable aspects of independence from parents, i.e., attitudinal, functional, emotional, and conflictual. Yet it is only the lastmentioned aspect – absence of conflict in the relationship with parents -- that with considerable consistency yields significant expected-direction correlation with SACQ variables, mostly so with personal-emotional adjustment. It has been argued that the failure of the other three aspects of separation to show expected relation with college adjustment may be due to problems in the definition and measurement of the construct of separation represented in that particular instrument. To make matters even more problematical regarding interpretation of these findings, the absence of conflict in relation with parents doesn’t seem to be necessarily of central inportance in the definition of separation. That is, it appears possible that students who are psychologically independent of their parents could have conflicted relations with them. Thus, while we must look elsewhere for information concerning relation between psychological separation from parents and college adjustment, and may not wish to regard absence of conflict in the student/parent relation as a necessary indication of that separation, the presence/absence of conflict can still of course be an important feature of the student/parent relation and an important determinant of adjustment to college. Indeed, other studies using other means of measurement have provided additional evidence to that effect which will be considered shortly. A broader, more direct, more nuanced, and apparently more successful means of 275 assessing psychological separation from parents used by one investigator has five subscales addressing different aspects of the student/parent relationship, some indicating successful separation (voluntary closeness with parents while maintaining appropriate boundaries; ability to function autonomously from the parents and to take responsibility for oneself) and others indicationg unsuccessful separation (governed or unduly influenced by parental wishes; close relationship and alliance with one parent against the other, or triangulation). While none of the five subscales predicted academic adjustment, all except the triangulation variable correlated in expected direction with the other SACQ variables, higher values tending to occur with personalemotional adjustment. And several other means of measuring students’ autonomy from parents – that variable variously characterized as ability to take responsibility for oneself, or individuation, or positive separation feelings – used by other investigators showed positive though usually modest association with the several SACQ indices. One instrument that assesses adult behavioral characteristics presumed to result from disturbances in separation-individuation had strong negative association with SACQ variables. Thus, while there has been some considerable disappointment in the search for relation between psychological separation from parents and adjustment to college, especially in the results from widespread use of one particular test, there are also some encouraging findings. In any event, perceived separation from parents as a potential determinant of adjustment to college seems too important to be left in the present state of findings and further research is indicated, including with improved means of measuring the determinant variable. (this next paragraph is a reasonably succinct summary, & I may want to consider 276 using ir or something like it if I decide to move the above several paragraphs to the relevant chapter)Several different means of measuring perceived separation from parents have been employed, some more successfully than others in demonstrating relation with adjustment to college. With the most frequently employed instrument, results were disappointingly mixed and have led to some reinterpretation of the psychometric character of that instrument. Only one of four subscales intended to measure four different aspects of psychological separation from parents – i.e., conflictual independence, or absence of conflict in the relationship – was found to be consistently related in predicted direction to SACQ variables, most so with personalemotional adjustment. A number of investigators have focused on what at first blush appears to be the obverse of psychological separation from parents, i.e., perceived attachment to parents, and, employing several different means of measuring that variable, have been more consistently successful in finding expected positive association with adjustment to college. In part at least this research emerges from disapproval of what some consider excessive focus on separation from parents as a mark of maturation, and consequent underemphasis on the role of continued association with parents characterized by appropriate transformations therein. The means of assessing attachment to parents that has been most used in relation to the SACQ provides, in addition to an index of overall attachment, subscale measures of trust, communication, and alienation in the relationship. A half dozen or more studies reporting the overall measure found fairly consistent expected-direction (positive) correlations with the several SACQ indices, ranging from modest to robust. Studies reporting findings for the subscales also fairly consistently obtained expected-direction (negative for alienation, positive for trust and 277 communication) correlations with SACQ indices, ranging from modest to moderately strong. Interesting findings came from studies employing that same instrument to identify students varying in parental attachment style. In one study, freshman students identified as securely or insecurely attached to their parents, two years later as juniors still showed expecteddirection difference on SACQ variables. In another study where students were categorized as having secure, ambivalent, or avoidant attachment to parents (the latter two being instances of insecure attachment style), the secure students again showed better college adjustment, with the differences being attributable almost entirely to female members of the sample. One researcher developed an instrument designed to measure both separation from parents and connectedness (preferring that term to “attachment”), viewing both as desirable developmental features, and used it to construct four categories of students in terms of combinations of high and low scores on each variable. Expectations regarding adjustment differences among the four categories arrayed from greater to lesser maturity were largely corroborated. Possibly not far removed from the concept of student/parent attachment is a variable called perceived reciprocity in current relations with parents, considered to be a more mature state of affairs than the asymmetry of earlier such relationships. Here parent and student treat each other as relative equals, with open and honest communication and mutual respect, reminiscent of the above-decribed variables of communication and trust. In two studies this variable correlated with all SACQ variables, sometimes substantially. Returning now to the role of conflictedness of the student/parent relationship as a determinant of college adjustment, discussed briefly above in terms of conflictual independence, 278 there are three additional sources of relevant findings, all of them subscales from instruments measuring psychological separation from or attachment to parents. In a half dozen studies there was consistent positive correlation, sometimes moderately strong, between a measure of the affective quality of the relationahip – positively keyed -- and all SACQ variables; in several other studies there were regularly occurring and sometimes strong negative correlations between student-felt alienation from parents and SACQ variables; and in two studies a measure of resentment of parental control was negatively associated with SACQ variables. *************** We move now from a particular kind of social relations, i.e., with parents, to social relations in general as a sixth class of person characteristics as determinant variables studied for their role in the adjustment to college. Social propensity, or social interests and skills, measured both pre- and postmatriculation, predicts SACQ variables, stronger for the postmatriculation testing and especially with social adjustment. Similarly, several means of extraversion have been found to be positively related to all SACQ variables, strongest with social adjustment and weakest with academic adjustment. Shyness, conceivably the obverse of extraversion, correlated negatively with all SACQ variables. In one study, the tendency in interpersonal situations to focus one’s attention on other persons rather than on oneself, very likely related to extraversion, was associated with better social adjustment and institutional attachment, while focusing on the self was associated with better academic and personal-emotional adjustment. The definition of the “Big 5” factor of aggreeableness – including references to friendliness, altruism, capacity for empathy – would seem related to focus on others rather than the self, and in three studies correlated positively with 279 all SACQ variables, as did the cognate variable of likeability in yet another study. From a more sociological point of view, individualistic versus collectivistic orientations would seem to reflect, respectively, focus on oneself and focus on others, the latter orientation predicting all SACQ variables, especially social adjustment and institutional attachment. It seems likely that the foregoing characteristics of individuals, referring basically to selfor-other orientation and possibly to essental social skills, would culminate in outcomes in the form of interpersonal attachment, i.e., the establishment of interpersonal relationships and capacity for doing so. A measure of attachment to college peers correlated with all SACQ variables, the values regarding social adjustment and institutional attachment consistently strong, leading to the conclusion that the greater the student’s ability to communicate with and place trust in, and to avoid alienation with, fellow students (all conceived as components of attachment), the better the adjustment to college. The same instrument used to assess attachment to peers in general, not just those at college, also predicts all SACQ variables, strongest with social adjustment and institutional attachment. A combined measure of parental attachment and peer attachment correlates with all SACQ variables, least with academic adjustment. Compariog parental and peer attachment for predicting adjustment to college, the former appears to be a better predictor of academic and personal-emotional adjustment, the latter a better predictor of social adjustment and institutional attachment. Several measures more directly assessing capacity for forming social relationships show positive association with SACQ variables, especially social adjustment. The concept of alienation, cited above as a component of interpersonal attachment, as applied to interpersonal relationships in particular, represents a failure of attachment or 280 disordered attachment to others. Several measures of alienation and related variables (e.g., felt distance from other persons, pathological independence from others) have been found to correlate in expected direction and sometimes moderately strongly with SACQ variables. Excessive or inordinate dependence on other persons is in a sense the opposite of alienation and detachment from others but nevertheless represents still another kind of disturbance in relationship formation. Studies employing that variable have found negative correlations with all SACQ variables, especially personal-emotional adjustment. On the other hand, measures of healthy or adaptive forms of self-sufficiency/independence – i.e., that do not preclude ability to establish interdependent relationships with others when appropriate – are associated positively with all SACQ variables, some moderately strongly. Pulling together in an organized way several of the aspects of relationship formation already considered separately is the concept of adult attachment styles, referring to individuals’ characteristic manner of relating to significant other persons, typically presumed to be a consequnce of early infant/parent experience. Three basic styles are ordinarily identified. The “secure” and more adaptive style is characterized by ability to establish reciprocally close and interdependent relationships, while at the same time maintaining separate identity and capacity for independent action. In the “anxious-ambivalent” style there is inordinate need to establish and maintain close and dependent relationships, with difficulty in regulating negative emotions associated with threats of relationship disruption. The “avoidant” style is marked by lack of capacity for, or interest in, forming close relationships. These behavioral characteristics are sometimes used to identify types of persons, and sometimes traits or dimensions underlying the styles. Several investigators, whether using analysis by type or trait, regularly find higher SACQ 281 scores in all adjustment areas for the secure style. The social relations variables considered thus far in this section implicate interpersonal relationship directly, but some investigators have employed variables that do so only indirectly. Several studies, for example, involve feeling states that are defined in terms of social relations. Social anxiety correlated negatively with all SACQ variables, strongest with social adjustment. Fear of negative evaluation, or need for social approval, very likely an aspect of social anxiety, also correlated negatively but more nodestly with certain SACQ variables. Loneliness has been used in several studies and found to correlate with all SACQ variables, sometimes quite robustly, strongest with social adjustment and weakest with academic adjustment. In one study, loneliness measured in the second semester of the freshman year predicted SACQ variables in the senior year, again strongest with social adjustment. Shame was found to be associated negatively and rather strongly with all SACQ variables except institutional attachment. And guilt, as measured by several different instruments and in several different studies, relates negatively and frequently substantially with all SACQ variables. A social relations variable in a more general or even sociological sense as it relates to college adjustment has been studied in deaf students at a college for deaf students, in terms of whether they identify with the deaf community, the hearing community, or both. Students identifying with the deaf community had higher scores on SACQ variables, and those identifying with the hearing community, or both communities, had lower scores. One study introduced a social aspect into a variable that probably wouldn’t ordinarily be thought to have one, i.e., perfectionism. While perfectionistic expectations imposed on oneself by oneself (i.e., perfectionism as traditionally conceived) showed no relation with SACQ 282 variables, high standards imposed on others by the student had low negative correlations with social adjustment and institutional attachment, and felt obligation to conform to standards and expectations imposed on oneself by others (socially prescribed perfectionism) correlated negatively with all SACQ variables except academic adjustment, strongest with social adjustment. A variable possibly fairly typically, though not necessarily, associated with social relations, and not yet much focused upon in research using the SACQ, is the use of intoxicants and recreational drugs. This is surprising, given the apparently rather widespread use of such substances in the college population and expectations concerning the relation between such use and quality of adjustment. Findings thus far concerning such variables are fragmentary. *************** A seventh and final set of person characteristics that may be thought of as possible determinants of adjustment to college consists of group identity variables defined in terms of membership – however active or passive – in a category of persons assumed to have implications for an individual’s behavior. These categories tend to be more demographic or status-related than the person-characteristics considered earlier, and with few exceptions seem to be less fruitful in understanding adjustment to college. Gender is the group identity variable for which most information is available. The most common finding is no group differences between men and women on SACQ variables. When such differences are seen, they are consistent but of minor magnitude, with men having higher scores on the Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale and women higher scores on the other subscales. 283 Related to gender as a group identity variable, but much less investigated though apparently more promising for understanding adjustment to college, is sex-role orientation. In two studies, psychologically androgynous studemts had higher SACQ scores than low androgynous, masculine, or feminine students, or students undifferentisted as to sexual role. And there were no SACQ differences between androgynous male and female students. Racial/ethnic characteristics are group identity variables that have received a fair amount of attention in SACQ-using research, especially concerning the adaptation of minority students to an environment that in some ways may be “foreign” to them. But the most common finding (nine studies) in comparison of adjustment of African-American and white students at predominantly white American colleges is no difference in SACQ scores. In two studies, black students had better personal-emotional adjustment than white students. In four studies, white students had higher scores on SACQ variables except for academic adjustment. At a recently integrated South African university white students also had higher scores on some SACQ variables than black students, the latter sample excluding mixed-race students. The poorer adjustment of black students at predominantly white institutions, when it occurs, seems to be seen more in social adjustment and institutional attachment. Thus, findings from these studies in which students are identified simply by race have not been particularly productive or uniform. Important to note, black freshmen at a predominantly black university had better academic, social, and overall adjustment than either black or white freshmen at a predominantly white university, and stronger institutional attachment than black students at the predominantly white institution. This finding expands the inquiry considerably and suggests that any greater adjustment difficulty of black students than white students at predominantly white colleges could 284 be attributable at least in part to the nature of the institution. As yet unaddressed is the question of adjustment of white students to predominantly black colleges. In another potentially very important line of inquiry, some researchers have hypothesized that black student adjustment in college might be less a function of racial status than of particular characteristics in terms of which students may differ from each other, such as attitude toward their racial status, or adherence to black culture versus acculturation, or amount of prior interracial experience. Except for interracial experience, about which a fair amount of information is already available, findings from use of other individual difference variables among black students are relatively meager and beg inquiry. It may be worthwhile for investigators to assume that minority students will vary in the degree to which the college environment is actually “foreign” to them or otherwise presenting challenge. Very little in the way of difference in adjustment to college is found between Spanishorigin students and other students, including white students, or among different kinds of Spanishorigin students, or among Spanish-origin students varying in identification with their minority culture. Taken all together, racial/ethnic characteristics per se do not seem to have been as helpful in understanding student adjustment to college as might have been hoped. Presumably where differences among racial/ethnic groups in adjustment to college are found, they would be attributable in some degree to cultural- or subcultural-induced characteristics of students in interaction with the characteristics of the host institutional environment, with congruence between the two sets of characteristics ordinarily enhancing the adjustment. Because there very likely are sizeable differences within racial/ethnic groups regarding acquisition of, or 285 favorableness of attitude toward, cultural- or subcultural-induced personal characteristics, research that focuses on such characteristics rather than simply on racial/ethnic categories might be more fruitful, as might research that focuses more analytically on the nature of host institutional environments. The same reasoning as expressed above regarding race/ethnicity should apply as well to anothre group identity variable, i.e., foreign students. That is, the culture-congruence hypothesis would hold that the greater the similarity between the cultural characteristics of a foreign student’s country of origin and those of the host institution/country, the better will be the adjustment to college. In a study of the adjustment of foreign students to an Australian university, the more dissimilar were the culture-induced value characteristics of migrating ethnic Chinese students from those of the host institution and its indigenous students, the poorer was their personalemotional adjustment. A few studies have been done on foreign student adjustment at American colleges. In one, students from western European countries, that have cultural characteristics somewhat similar to those of the United States, show better social, personal-emotional, and overall adjustment than students from east Asian countries where cultural characteristics can be condiderably different. And there is other evidence from SACQ-using research that students from Asian nations experience more difficulty adjusting to American colleges than do American students or students from other parts of the world. Possibly demonstrating that the effects of cultural dissimilarity may be mitigated, foreign students at an American university who had lived in the United States prior to matriculation had 286 better social adjustment and institutional attachment than foreign students who had not lived in the U. S. Religion and religion-related person characteristics may also be seen, sometimes with some stretch of definition, as group identity variables. Several religion variables show no relation to tbe SACQ: church attendance; whether Catholic or non-Catholic; rated importance of religion in one’s family; degree of student’s current interest in religion; religious fundamentalism. A one-item rating of self-religiousness in one study was unrelated to overall college adjustment, but, in another study where a test was employed to measure religiousness, positive correlation with all SACQ variables was obtained.. Students who were members of formal religious organizations (churches, temples) reported better academic adjustment than non-affiliated students. Weak positive relation was obtained with overall adjustment for a one-item measure of the extent to which students describe themselves as still holding religious beliefs taught to them while growing up. And modest negative relation was found between entertaining doubts about religion and all SACQ variables except social adjustment; the more the doubt, the poorer the adjustment. Two studies focusing on fundamental personal values, which can possibly be regarded as religion-related variables, produced correlations with SACQ variables somewhat more regularly than most of the more narrowly defined religion variables.. Only one study has examined political orientation – yet another group identity variable -as a factor affecting college adjustment. Hunsberger et al. (1996) found no relation between right-wing authoritarianism and overall adjustment, the only SACQ variable reported. Socio-economic status and related variables fall in the group identity category and have 287 been looked at in several studies, always incidentally to larger research purposes and with somewhat inconsistent results. For socio-economic status itself, more studies found no relation with SACQ variables than did; two reported positive correlations and two negative. Similarly mixed findings were obtained for parental educational level, an aspect of socioeconomic status. Some studies found positive relation with SACQ variables but an approximately equal number report no relation, and much the same kind of mixed findings were obtained in comparisons of students whose parents had not attended college with students whose parents had attended. Findings for parental/family income or students’ personal financial circumstances, still other variables related to socio-economic status, were somewhat more consistent. In six studies there were positive correlations with SACQ indices, and in two no relation. There is not much information available concerning connection between student work status and adjustment to college as measured by the SACQ. For two investigators number of hours per week spent in paid employment was unrelated to SACQ variables and for a third it was negatively correlated with academic adjustment in non-traditional age female community college students. Yet another investigator found that students with no on- or off-campus employment had better social adjustment and insitutional attachment than students who did.have such employment. A number of investigators have reported findings for college year level (or number of years enrolled) in relation to SACQ variables, some using cross-sectional and some longitudinal methodology. In all longitudinal studies, college year level (or number of years enrolled) and age are confounded, and there is probably a large tendency toward that confounding in cross- 288 sectional studies as well. The more usual finding in the cross-sectional studies is for sophomores, juniors, and seniors to have higher scores on SACQ variables than freshmen, and one investigator reported a small positive correlation between college year level and all SACQ variables except social adjustment (i.e., the more advanced classes having the higher scores). But in three crosssectional studies there were no SACQ differences among college year levels. In the longitudinal studies, the slightly more common finding is for SACQ scores to increase as the student advances through the college years, but in some studies that effect is not seen. In one cross-sectional study, transfer juniors and seniors showed poorer social adjustment than a sample of freshman students, as well as poorer than sophomores and juniors who had entered the same institutution as freshmen. When age is examined in relation to adjustment in samples including students from all college year levels, positive correlations have occurred with all SACQ variables except, oddly, social adjustment. Studies using samples including only freshmen and sophomores found no relation for age. Studies using only freshmen report very mixed results, some positive association, some negative, and one none. In community college students, even in samples including only freshmen, probably more heterogeneous in age than four-year college students, positive relation between age and SACQ variables seems to be more regularly seen. Three studies found no SACQ differences between students varying in kinds of academic major. In one study comparing adjustment of American student-athletes and non-athlete 289 students, there were no differences on any SACQ variable. *************** The second major variety of determinant variables studied for their association with adjustment to college is environmental characteristics. A number of these, the reader will recall, are environmental or life events as perceived and reported by the student. Only a few are defined in terms independent of student report, perception, or experience. At perhaps the broadest level of definition of environmental variables are indices from a number of different instruments designed to evaluate the occurrence and consequences of significant events and stressors in general in the life of the student, which have been found in a number of studies by a number of investigators to be correlated in expected ways with adjustment to college, sometimes fairly strongly and occasionally even robustly. These variables have included the number of such events experienced within a given period as reported by the student, usually for the student’s life in general but in a some instances for life in college in particular, and also the events’ degree of positive and negative impact or degree of stressfulness as rated by the student. Perceived degree of stressfulness seems to be more closely related to adjustment in college than simple number of events experienced, life events/stressors experienced in college more so than those experienced in life in general, and those having negative impact more than those interpreted as positive, with personal-emotional adjustment often being the area most affected. And events to be consequential don’t have to be of major proportions, but can also be so-called “daily hassles.” Effects of more specific life stressors on adjustment to college have also been studied. Incidence of reported abuse (physical, psychological, sexual, combined) in the first fifteen years 290 of life, as measured by one instrument, was found to correlate negatively and moderately with the several SACQ subscales. Measures of psychological and physical maltreatment separately, by parents in particular, during the same time period each correlated negatively and modestly to moderately with all SACQ subscales, and physical abuse by other significant figures (siblings, relatives, friends, neighbors) showed weak effects on academic adjustment and institutional attachment. In one study, freshman women who reported experiencing sexual abuse during childhood and/or adolescence had lower scores on all SACQ indices except academic adjustment than freshman women reporting no such experiences; but no SACQ differences were found at the other college year levels between women reporting experience of abuse and those not. In another study using female students, there was a modest negative correlation between reporting nonconsenting experience in adolescence and social adjustment and institutional attachment. Perceived racial/ethnic discrimination as another form of life stressor was found in two studies, one with Mexican-American and the other with Latino students, to be associated with lower SACQ scores. Disruption of important relationships is yet another kind of life stressor that has been examined for consequences in adjustment to college. In one study, reported interpersonal loss in the first fifteen years of life correlated negatively with social and personal-emotional adjustment, and, in another, self-assessed success in having dealt with past interpersonal loss was positively associated with SACQ variables. In a study of students who had experienced the break-up of a romantic relationship within the previous six months, paradoxically (?) the longer the relationship had lasted the better the personal-emotional adjustment, and, less paradoxically, the more the student wanted resumption of the relationship the worse the personal-emotional 291 adjustment. Distance of one’s college from home as measured in miles shows no regular relation in a number of studies to adjustment to college. However, when students were asked to rate on a five point scale the distance as being “too far” to “just right,” with the latter description having the higher value, significant positive association occurs with all SACQ variables. *************** The nature of a student's family, and of experience within the family, as determinants of the student’s adjustment to college as measured by the SACQ, have been studied in many different aspects and by a large number of investigators. Variables employed represent properties, attributes, or characteristics of the family as a functioning organizational unit in both adaptive and maladaptive aspects, including measures of both general and specific aspects of a family’s “health” or functioning effectiveness. In addition to families’ functioning effectiveness in general, particular characteristics that have been studied are: the family’s cohesiveness, adaptability, and encouragement of development of autonomy/independence and intimacy in its members; its provision of social support; and, in a more general sense that encompasses the foregoing characteristics, the parenting styles involved. Some studies have focused more particularly on the maladaptive aspects of family functioning, including parent-child role-reversal, parental overinvolvement or overprotectiveness, fear or discouragement of separation of members, parental rejection, conflict between the parents, familial conflictfulness in general including the family’s means of coping with discord, and parental abuse of alcohol. Also investigated were relationship patterns within the family; roles played by students within the family; various aspects of the family’s social 292 climate, and effects of grandparenting. Regarding family health or functioning effectiveness in general as perceived by the student, six different studies employing four different tests show positive correlations with SACQ variables, usually with all five adjustment indices. The several particular aspects of perceived family effectiveness are also quite fruitful in producing association with SACQ variables. Six studies using three different measures of a family’s fostering of the development of autonomy/independence in its members, including one focusing on fathers’ encouragement of daughters’ independence, all regularly report positive correlations with the several SACQ variables, again usually with all of those variables. The particular family characteristic possibly most frequestly and thoroughly investigated in relation to the SACQ is familial cohesion and cognate variables. Many investigators using several different means of measuring family cohesiveness have produced considerable evidence that the more cohesive the student’s family, as perceived by the student, the better the adjustment to college. A dozen studies using eight different measures of family cohesion and related variables regularly find positive association with SACQ variables, most usually all of those adjustment variables. In three other studies employing a measure of a pathological form of family cohesion, fear of separation-individuation, there were negative correlations with SACQ variables. Family adaptability -- or capacity to be flexible in structure, roles, and rules in the face of changing circumstances – was used a little less frequently (eight studies, four different measures) with much less consistent results. With one measure of family adaptability in two studies there were positive correlations with all SACQ variavbles, but in the other studies expected direction correlations were infrequent or modest. 293 Social support provided by family, as assessed by the student and measured by several instruments, was found in a number of studies to be positively but only intermittently related with SACQ variables. As an example of the inconsistency of such findings, one standard measure of social support from familiy that was used in several studies correlated positively with all SACQ variables in two and with no SACQ variables in two others. But, interestingly, in two studies the more that students’ social networks consisted of family and other relatives, the poorer was the adjustment to college. Social support as received from parents in particular, not family in general as discussed above, including related variables such as provision of emotional support and “caringness,” was found in a dozen or more studies using several different instruments to relate positively and fairly regularly with SACQ variables, in some studies all SACQ variables. A number of investigators have examined variables that might be regarded as excessive, pathological forms of parental social support, e.g., parent-child overinvolvement, intrusiveness, and overprotectiveness. Such studies fairly regularly produce negative correlations with SACQ variables, some with all SACQ variables. Two variables that may be regarded as yet other forms of perversion of, or the obverse of, parental social support -- i.e., parent-child role reversal and parental rejection -- have been found to be negatively associated with SACQ variables,. In two studies the latter variable was negatively correlated with all SACQ variables, in some instances to a moderately strong degree. Amount of conflict within a student’s family as assessed by the student has been examined in a half dozen or so studies employing five different means of measurement and is regularly associated in expected direction with SACQ variables, in half of the studies with all SACQ variables. Still concerning family conflict, but focusing less on the family in general and 294 more on the parents in particular, several studies find much less consistent relation with SACQ indices, though in expected negative direction when it occurs and most frequently with personalemotional adjustment Parental divorce or separation would of course be an indicator of parental discord, and of many studies simply comparing college adjustment of students of divorced and non-divorced parents, only a minority (three out of fourteen) have found differences, students from intact families showing better adjust where differences do occur. One study indicates that the older the student at the time of the parental divorce the better the social adjustment, but two other studies finds no relation between that variable and SACQ indices. Student/parent postdivorce relations as they affect student adjustment have been examined, also relatively unfruitfully. In single studies, there were no differential effects between students whose mothers remained single postdivorce and those remarrying, or between students in maternal or joint parental custody. Another study found no relation between amount of time spent with either the custodial or noncustodial parent and college adjustment for women students, or with the custodial parent for men, but for men the more the contact with the noncustodial parent the better the social adjustment and institutional attachment. In terms of quality rather than quantity of time spent with custodial and noncustodial parents, for women there was no relation with any SACQ variable for time with the latter parent but as regards the custodial parent the better the quality the better the social and overall adjustment and institutional attachment. For men, the better the quality of time spent with both custodial and noncustodial parents, the better the social adjustment and institutional attachment. In another study, for male students from mother-remarried families, the quality of the mother-son relationship was 295 positively related with academic adjustment and institutional attachment. One investigator was interested in families’ means of coping with discord, i. e., problemsolving attitudes and skills, as determinants of student adjustment in college. “Reframing,” or redefining stressful events to make them more manageable, was the better predictor and related positively to all SACQ variables. “Passive appraisal,” or accepting problematic issues and thus minimizing reactivity; “acquiring social support” (from relatives, friends, and neighbors); and “acquiring help from community resources” also correlated positively with selected SACQ variables, but “seeking spiritual support” did not. A total score correlated with all SACQ variables, strongest with social and overall adjustment and institutional attachment. Parenting styles characteristic of students’ families have been shown in several studies to produce consistent findings regarding the students’ adjustment to college. Parental authoritativeness (providing clear and firm direction with warmth, reasonableness, and flexibility) on the part of father and mother separately as well as combined is associated quite regularly and positively with SACQ variables, possibly a little more strongly for mother than father authoritativeness and weakest among the SACQ subscales with institutional attachment. In the one study that analyzed data for male and female students separately, there is evidence that both mother and father authoritativeness predicts institutional attachment better for male than for female students, and that father authoritativeness also predicts other aspects of college adjustment better for males than females. Parental authoritarianism (highly directive, expecting unquestioning obedience, lacking in warmth, punitive) is associated fairly regularly but negatively with SACQ variables -- except for institutional attachment, where there is no correlation – and strongest with personal-emotional 296 adjustment. There seems to be no difference in correlation with SACQ variables between mother and father authoritarianism, and the degree of association for authoritarianism with college adjustment seems to be somewhat weaker than it was for authoritativeness, described in the foregoing paragraph. The parental style of permissiveness as originally defined and measured was unrelated to SACQ variables, but very interesting findings appear when two different kinds of permissiveness are identified and separately measured. Permissive-indulgence (relative absence of discipline, but in a context of parental warmth and caringness) remains essentially unrelated to college adjustment, but permissive-neglectfulness (relative absence of discipline in a context of parental uncaringness and detachment) correlated negatively with SACQ variables, in strength more like the findings for the authoritative than the authoritarian parenting style. With students assigned to groups on the basis of parenting styles experienced, those having authoritative parenting had higher scores on all SACQ variables than those having authoritarian or permissive-neglectful parenting, and also better academic adjustment than those with permissive-indulgent parenting. And those having permissive-indulgent parenting showed better personal-emotional adjustment than those with authoritarian parenting, and better social and overall adjustment than students who had permissive-neglectful parenting. Several studies have been unsuccessful in finding influence of relationship patterns within the family on students’ adjustment to college, focusing particularly on the formation of cross-generational alliances. Somehat more successful have been studies concerning roles played by students within the family, where the focus is on the behavioral characteristics of the student rather than on 297 characteristics of the family as a functioning unit. In two studies where the student in the Family Hero role is measured by two different tests but in both identified in socially desirable behavioral terms (organized, dependable, conscientious, mature) that role is associated with better college adjustment in all areas, especially academic. But in another study, employing yet a third means of measurement where the Family Hero role is defined in dysfunctional terms (neurotically driven, conscience-ridden, compensating for feelings of inadequacy) the association with SACQ variables is negative, suggesting need for refinement in the definition and measurement of the role. The Lost Child or Sick Child role is defined similarly in all three studies (emotionally vulnerable, withdrawn, socially isolated, low self-esteem), though still assessed by different tests, and has been found to be negatively related with all SACQ variables. The Scapegoat role (antisocial, acting-out, defiant) also correlates negatively with SACQ variables, but the Mascot role (comical, energetic, outgoing) shows no relation except for one modest positive correlation with social adjustment. Some investigators have examined familial/parental physical and mental health characteristics as determinants of adjustment to college. In three studies, students who regarded their parents or families as having problems with alcohol or other drugs had lower SACQ scores than students who did not so evaluate their parents/families. Students who perceived their mother as having psychological problems in general had lower SACQ scores than students who did not so assess their mothers, but this difference was not found in the case of fathers. Several aspects of family social climate have been examined. Family expressiveness (the extent to which family members are encouraged to act openly and/or to express feelings) was 298 positively associated with all SACQ variables in three studies. Degree of family interest in socio-political-cultural activities correlated (positively) with all SACQ variables in one study, but not with any in a second study. Two variables – extent to which family members are assertive and self-sufficient, and degree of familial emphasis on ethical and religious values – correlated positively but weakly in one study but not another with several SACQ indices. And in two studies family achievement orientation/competitiveness did not relate to any SACQ variable. There are some fairly sparse results of studies employing family demographic variables. One investigator found that students from families where English was the primary language spoken had better social adjustment than students from families where English was not the primary language, but another investigator obtained no such difference in overall adjustment for a sample of Chicano/Latino students. In a South African university where English was the medium of instruction, students for whom English was their first language had better social adjustment than students for whom English was not their first language. Students’ rated importance of ethnicity in their families was not related to any SACQ indices in one study, and in two other studies there was no relation between students’ immigrantgenerational level (first, second, third) and overall college adjustment. In one study earlier-born children had better overall adjustment to college than later-born, highest for only children, and in another study there was no relation between students’ number of siblings and any SACQ variable. Finally, regarding family demographic variables, students from urban/suburban homes had higher scores on all SACQ variables than students from rural homes, in a first semester testing but not second semester. 299 The role of grandparenting in students’ adjustment to college was investigated in one study. The more successfully that role was carried out, as evaluated by the grandparent, the better was the students’ adjustment. *************** Another category of environmental variables studied in relation to adjustment to college as measured by the SACQ is social support from other sources than the family. Friends as such a source have received considerable attention. One instrument measuring perceived social support from friends in a number of studies has consistently yielded positive correlations, sometimes fairly strong, with all SACQ subscales, especially social adjustment. Thus, the greater the sense that a student has of receiving support from friends, the better the adjustmen to college in all areas tapped by the SACQ. Other studies have focused on the prevalence and quality of friendship sources. Thus, it has been found that the more that a student’s social network is comprised of friends, and the more that a network is made up of mutual friends, the better the adjustment to college, particularly in the social area. One study, however, reported that the more friends a student had, the poorer was the academic adjustment. In another study, support from friends in college (any college, not just the one attended by the student) correlated strongly with all SACQ variables in second generation college freshmen (i.e., students with college-educated parents) but only with social adjustment and institutional attachment in first generation freshmen (students with parents who were not college-educated). Regarding support from friends not in college, there were modest positive correlations with academic and overall adjustment for second generation freshmen but none with any SACQ 300 variables for first generation freshmen. Social support from college friends was found by another investigator to be positively related to social adjustment and institutional attachment for white students but not to any SACQ variables for students of color. In that same study, obtaining support from pre-college friends away from campus was negatively associated withinstitutional attachment for stuents of color, and negativly associated with social adjustment and institutional attachment for EuropeanAmerican students. Other particular sources of social support among peers that have been less studied in relation to the SACQ are fellow students (who are not necessarily friends), roommates, coursemates, and peers otherwise unspecified. In two studies, the more that a student’s social network was composed of other students (not necessarily friends), the higher the scores on the several SACQ variables except for academuic adjustment. In another study, where the term “peers” was defined as including fellow students, both frequency of support from that source and satisfaction with frequency of support correlated with all SACQ variables, though weakly with academic adjustment, to about the same degree. Employing variables that connote receipt of social support from fellow students, one study found in a sample of sophomores that reporting having received help in adjusting as freshmen from other freshmen was correlated with poorer academic adjustment; having received help as freshmen from peer advisors was associated with poorer social adjustment; but having received help from upper class students correlated positively with social adjustment and institutional attachment. If such “help” can indeed be construed as a kind of social support, in some instances at least its consequences for college adjustment may not be favorable. 301 One investigator who was interested in social support from coursemates – i.e., fellow students taking same academic courses – examined the relation between five different kinds of such support and two of the SACQ variables only. All five kinds of social support correlated positively with social adjustment and three of them with overall adjustment. But in another study social support from fellow students in freshman/advisory seminars (i. e., not academic courses) did not relate to any SACQ variable. Finally, with regard to fellow students, in one study the extent to which social networks were comprised of roommates was unrelated to SACQ variables. The last particular source of social support for which SACQ findings have been reported is faculty and other authority figures. The investigator referred to above as identifying five aspects of social support found weak correlations with the two SACQ variables employed (social and overall adjustment) only for informational support from faculty. The investigator referred to above as interested in social support resulting from participation in freshman advisory/intervention seminars found no relation between such support received from instructors of those seminars and any of the SACQ indices. In a study using a single-item measure of amount of social support received from faculty, students reporting greater amounts of such support had higher scores on overall college adjustment. In yet another study, both frequency of social support from “authority figures” (including professors), and satisfaction with such frequency, correlated with the several SACQ variables. Spiritual support (from God, presumably the ultimate authority figure?) did not seem to be helpful in social or personal-emotional adjustment for a student sample as a whole, but it was associated with better personal-emotional adjustment in a subsample of students who recently had experienced high levels of stress. 302 Several studies have focused on the frequency or quality of students’ relationships with faculty which, though not necessarily couched in terms of social support, may be assumed to reflect its occurrence. In one study, the more frequent the students’ contact with faculty the better the social and overall adjustment. In another, students identified as having a secure relationship with faculty show greater institutional attachment than those having insecure relationship. Student-athletes’ ratings of the quality of their relationship with faculty, and their ratings of relationship with coaches (a kind of faculty?), correlate positively with SACQ variables. A few studies concerning social support combine certain of the support sources that are considered separately in the foregoing paragraphs. Both frequency of support, and satisfaction with frequency of support, from family and close friends merged correlated with SACQ variables – sometimes fairly strongly – for male students but not at all for female students. And it appears that for males satisfaction with frequency may be a better predictor of college adjustment than simple frequency itself. The same investigator also combined all separately identified sources – family, close friends, peers, authority figures (including professors) – and found that now satisfaction with frequency of social support correlated with all SACQ variables for the sample as a whole and for the male and female students separately. Perceived frequency of support also correlated, though not to the same extent or with the same regularity as the satisfaction variable. Except for academic adjustment, where correlations were of similar magnitude for both men and women, the latter had lower correlations with SACQ variables for both the perceived frequency of support and satisfaction with frequency variables. Two additional studies used merged sources variables: one, members of the campus 303 community and family combined, correlating with all SACQ subscales; the other, with a sample of student-athletes, satisfaction with support from the academic and athletic staffs and friends combined was associated positively with all SACQ variables. A number of studies employing a number of different measures have focused on support from the social environment in general, unspecified as to type of source. With one instrument used in three studies (Social Support Quest.) measures of number of persons providing support, satisfaction with the support provided, plus a total score (though not a measure of perceived availability of support) showed correlation with SACQ variables, in one of the studies moderately strongly with all SACQ indices, strongest for social adjustment. With another instrument (Social Support Inventory) employed in three studies, measures of satisfaction with social support received (one, interestingly, the discrepancy between amount reported to be needed and the amount actually received), correlated with the several SACQ indices, sometimes strongly, in one study strongest with personal-emotional adjustment and in another strongest with social adjustment and institutional attachment. Other variables from that same instrument used successfully in one of the studies were, separately (described above as used in combination as a discrepancy score), the amount of need for support felt by the student and amount actually received. A third scale (Social Provisions Scale), this one used in four studies, measures perceived availability of social support. Three of the studies employed just a total score that was found to correlate with all SACQ variables, some moderately strongly, especially social adjustment. The social support instrument administered prematriculation in one of the studies successfully predicted the SACQ administered the following March; and, in another study in a later year at the 304 same institution with a different sample, but the social support and adjustment instruments administered at the same time in March, there were correlations with all SACQ subscales, now much stronger for social and personal-emotional adjustment and institutional attachment than for the temporally removed administrations in the earlier study. A study done with older female community college students employed in addition to a total score from the social support instrument six subscales assessing different kinds of social support, but only two SACQ subscales. The total score predicted both SACQ variables as did some of the subscales.. A fourth measure (Interpersonal Support Evaluation List) that has been used with the SACQ in three studies assesses perceived availability of four kinds of social support plus a total score, thus yielding five scores. In one study 22 of the 25 correlations between the five social support scores and the five SACQ indices were significant, positive, and strong to robust, strongest with social and overall adjustment and institutional attachment. A second study also employed all five social support variables but only the Academic and Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscales from the SACQ, yielding nine out of 10 significant correlations. The third study used the total social support score only and all but one (institutional attachment) of the SACQ indices, all correlations being significant. A fifth measure (Young Adult SS Inv), designed to assess social support received in the first year of college, correlated positively with all SACQ variables. One of the studies cited above as using a standard measure of social support, which was typically verbal in character as is the SACQ, reported construction of an instrument that relied more on graphical/motor than verbal means of response in order to reduce common method variance. The new instrument yielded correlations in the expected direction with the same three 305 SACQ variables that had correlated most highly with the traditional means of measuring social support, strongest with the Social Adjustment subscale. Another nonstandard means of measuring support from the social environment in general was developed in one study by means of factor analysis of data from several instruments, and was found to correlate with all SACQ variables. Some half dozen or more studies concerned with the consequences for college adjustment of support from the social environment in general used several different instruments that focused more particularly on the measurement of social networks. Thus, where research already discussed in this section concentrated on amount and degree of satisfaction with social support received, the kinds of support provided, and the variety of support sources, the studies to be considered now are concerned more with the characteristics of the person networks providing support. (make sure that the gist of the previous sentence is included in the chapter covering soc’l supp from the env in gen’l – it may be new to this summary)Concerned generally with the composition and function of social networks, these variables include: size of networks; amount of interpersonal activity in the networks; similarity of network members’ attitudes and values; degree of intimacy in network relationships; amount of anger or conflict in the network relationships; the kinds of social support provided by the networks; and degree of satisfaction with the network and the support provided. Network size has not been a fruitful variable, yielding only a small negative correlation with academic adjustment in one study and none with any SACQ variable in two other studies, including for two subnetworks identified respectively as conflicted and unconflicted in one of the latter two studies. Frequency of interpersonal activity in the general social network correlated 306 positively with SACQ variables (social and overall adjustment adjustment and institutional attachment) in one study. Degree of intimacy in network relationships and similarity of network members’ attitudes and values also correlated positively with the same SACQ variables in one study but not in another study, while occurrence of anger/conflist in the network was associated negatively with some SACQ variables in one study but not another. Measures of satisfaction with support from social networks correlated positively with SACQ variables – sometimes all SACQ variables – in several studies, though failed to do so in one. Felt need for social support, the obverse of satisfaction, was associated negatively with SACQ variables. In one study, however, employing 19 variables representing various aspects of social networks, with five different samples of students, the number of significant correlations obtained barely exceeded chance. Results in two studies regarding kinds of support provided by social networks were rather mixed, except for provision of fun/relaxation, which correlated positively with all SACQ variables in one study and with social adjustment and institutional attachment in the other. Provision of information/advice, emotional support, and task assistance (three separate variables) showed no association with any SACQ variables in one of the two studies. In the other study there was positive association with social adjustment and institutional attachment for information/advice, but a mixture of positive and negative correlations with SACQ variables (negative with personal-emotional adjustment) for the other two support variables. Interesting to note, one investigator administered a measure of social networks and the SACQ in the freshman year and again in the same students’ senior year, finding on the latter occasion the same approximate pattern of relation between the support and adjustment variables 307 as earlier, though generally lesser in magnitude. Additionally, social network variables assessed in the freshman year were significantly related in expected direction to some variables from the SACQ administered three years later in the senior year. *************** Interracial exposure or experience is yet another category of environmental variables that have been studied in relation to college adjustment. Three studies have found that African-American freshmen with greater amount of precollege exposure to or experience with white persons in the high school context, especially in relation to high school friends, adjusted better to predominantly white colleges than did AfricanAmerican freshmen with lesser such pre-college exposure or experience. In two of those studies the better adjustment was seen in all areas tapped by the SACQ. There was also evidence of benefit from postmatriculation interracial experience. Thus, while SACQ scores of white students tend to decrease from the first semester of the freshman year to the second semester, scores of African-American students at predominantly white colleges show increases. Also, there was differential rate of improvement in adjustment over the course of the academic year among African-American students varying in amount of prior interracial experience, the gain being greatest for those with the least such earlier experience. Importantly, two studies showed that prior interracial experience in African-American students attending primarily black universities was negatively related to adjustment. In one of those studies African-American graduates of predominantly black high schools had better SACQ-measured overall adjustment to college than students of predominantly white or integrated high schools. 308 These findings from studies of African-American students at predominantly white and predominantly black universities, as well as findings from studies of white students at predomantly white institutions, were interpreted as pointing to the importance of the relation between person and environmental variables in the determination of adjustment. That is, whether prior interracial experience in African-American students is a help or a hindrance in adjusting to college depends on whether the college is predominantly white or predominantly black. Absent thus far in this line of investigation is research concerning the adjustment of white students at primarily black American colleges. At a primarily white, recently integrated university in the Republic of South Africa there were no SACQ differences among either white or black students varying in prior exposure to ethnocultural diversity. These data would not be directly comparable to data from American institutions because the black sample excluded so-called “coloureds,” or mixed-race students, who may be more the norm in the African-American college student population. Also relevant to the relation between African-American students’ interracial experience and adjustment to predominantly white colleges, though possibly more relevant to post- than prematriculation experience, is a study that was concerned with the racial composition of campus activities in which the students were involved. Social adjustment, the only SACQ variable employed, was highest in students involved in activities where sponsorship/participation was mixed black and white, followed in decreasing order by predominantly white and then predominantly black sponsorship/participation, and lowest in students with “little or no” involvement in any campus activities. In one study with Latino students, no relation was found between interracial/interethnic 309 experience and any of the SACQ variables. *************** A final category of environmental variables studied in relation to the SACQ consists of institutional characteristics of colleges and universities. In an earlier chapter there was mention of the role that institutional characteristics may play in interaction with students’ degree of decidedness concerning academic major in determining college adjustment. More particularly, certain findings concerning the relation between major decidedness and adjustment were interpreted as possibly reflecting institutional pressures to declare an academic major. Thus, in a liberal arts college, the relation between major decidedness and adjustment was less clearly seen in the first semester of the freshman year than the second, when institutional pressures to declare a major increased. In an engineering college, where decidedness regarding major may be expected to be a more salient factor for freshmen than it is in a liberal arts college, there was already clear relation between decidedness and adjustment in the first semester, and the effect involved more aspects of adjustment than was true for second semester liberal arts students. In another study, SACQ scores between freshmen varying in decidedness concerning major were more apparent in a college requiring declaration in the freshman year than one requiring it in the sophomore year. As another aspect of the relation between an institutional characteristic and students’decidedness regarding academic major, students in process of changing their major show less adjustment difficulty if the institution in which they are enrolled offers the new major than if it does not. Because the foregoing findings derive from a few single studies, and involve a fair 310 amount of speculation, further investigation of interactive relation between institutional factors and students’ decidedness regarding academic major in determing adjustment to college would be desirable. Another area of research concerning institutional characteristics has focused on the consequences of initial exposure to the college or university environment for the adjustment of matriculating students. In this research an adaptation of the SACQ has been employed to assess students’ prematriculation expectations regarding their capacity for dealing with the impending transition into college, against which subsequent actual adjustment as measured by the SACQ itself may be compared. Comparison of pre- and postmatriculation scores has been used to assess the extent to which prematriculation expectations are fulfilled, or, contrarily, the degree of disillusionment with one’s adaptive capacities. Studies employing this methodology at several institutions have shown rather consistent and sometimes substantial decline from prematriculation to first semester testing in all areas tapped by the SACQ except for personal-emotional adjustment, and in one study continued decline in the second semester on some SACQ variables. But wide individual differences among students in this effect have been observed, some showing varying amounts of disillusionment, some none at all, and others the opposite, implying another important interactive relation between person and environment characteristics. Two very interesting sets of research questions arise from these findings. First is whether there are differences among institutions in the occurrence of the generally found “disillusionment” of entering students, what are the institutional characteristics factors responsible, and what might be done to alleviate the effect. Second, what are the factors – 311 especially person characteristics – that play determining roles in the wide individual differences among students at a given institution in the occurrence of the disillusionment. College social or sports organizations are aspects of the institutional environment that might be expected to have consequences for student adjustment. Fraternity/sorority membership was found in one study to be negatively related to academic and personal-emotional adjustment, but positively to social adjustment. In another study fraternity members had higher scores than nonfraternity members on social and overall adjustment and institutional attachment. Members of intercollegiate athletic teams in one study had higher scores on all SACQ variables than nonmembers, while in another study team members had higher scores on academic adjustment only. Campus living facilities are aspects of institutional environment that have received some attention in SACQ-using research. In a first semester testing but not second semester, the smaller the number of students per dormitory staff member, the higher the scores on four of the five SACQ indices. In a second semester testing but not first, freshmen with freshman roommates had better social adjustment and institutional attachment than freshmen with upper class roommates. But in another study no adjustment differences were found between freshmen living in dormitory sections designated for freshmen versus freshmen in mixed-class sections. Two studies found no differential adjustment consequences between coeducational and unisex dormitories, and in one of the studies there were no differences between large versus small, highrise versus low-rise, or sylvan setting versus urban dormtories. In addition to characteristics of living arrangements defined “physically,” students’ perception or evaluation of, or reaction to, living arrangements have been examined. In one 312 study that asked students to evaluate four aspects of their dormitories that may readily been seen as social support relevant: availability of personal or social support; group cohesiveness; amount of conflict or contentiousness; and orderedness/regulation. Perceived availability of support correlated positively and moderately strongly with all SACQ variables for male students and somewhat more moderately with all SACQ indices except academic adjustment for female students. Amount of conflict seen as characteristic of dormitory living was negatively and moderately stringly correlated with all SACQ variables for females, but only with social adjustment for males. Perceived group cohesiveness in the dormitory was associated positively and relatively modestly with all SACQ variables for women students and with selected SACQ variables for men, but orderedness/regulation showed only one correlation, a positive one with academic adjustment for male students. Several studies have compared on-campus residence versus off-campus living for effects on student adjustment, most showing better social adjustment and institutional attachment for the former, only two indicating no differences. Adjustment consequences of service provision by various college staff persons have been investigated using the SACQ. No effects were found in one study for contacts with faculty advisors, career counselors, or financial counselors, but there were positive correlations between contacts with resident advisors and social adjustment and institutional attachment, and contacts with academic counselors and academic adjustment and institutional attachment. In another study with student athletes, positive association was obtained between student-rated quality of relationship with academic advisors and academic, social, and overall adjustment. No adjustment differences were found among students enrolled in five different types of 313 college within a university (arts and sciences, engineering, etc.). In another study Latino students in a private college had better social adjustment and institutional attachment than those at a public college, but there was no relation between institutional selectivity as measured by scholastic aptitude scores of entering freshmen and any SACQ variable. Other institution-related findings reported in that study using Latino students were: the larger the total undergraduate enrollment the better the social adjustment and institutional attachment; but, the more the faculty and administrators were perceived as student-centered, the better the academic adjustment.. In one study, the greater the cultural differences between a student’s college environment and hometown (ethnic composition, values, etc.), the poorer was the social and overall adjustment and institutional attachment. Related to cultural factors in defining institutional characteristics are additional findings from the study mentioned above using Latinoo students: the larger the Hispanic enrollment the better the academic adjustment; the greater the perception of racial/ethnic tension in the college environment, the poorer the adjustment in all areas tapped by the SACQ; and the more discrimination experienced by the student in the college environment, the poorer the institutional attachment. *************** The reader may recall that the chapter concerning use of the SACQ in facilitation of college adjustment presented and discussed relevant studies in series, one by one as units, because a presentation attempting some sort of integrated analysis seemed precluded by the variety of ways in which the studies differed and the extent of variation within each of those different ways. Rather than making a brief summary of those separate studies here, which would be needlessly repetitive, what will be offered now is a description of the kinds of differences that 314 are seen among SACQ-using intervention studies. For a sense of each study as a unit the reader is referred to Chapter 16. The essential ways in which SACQ-using intervention studies differ, and the order in which they will be considered, are: the tiiming of the intervention; the nature of the target population; the nature of the intervention; the means of assessment of consequences of the intervention; and the findings. The variation in timing of intervention has been quite broad. In two studies it occurred as early as the second half of the senior year of high school, with the intention of proactively influencing the subsequent transition from high school to college. A larger number of studies conducted interventions in the summer, varying from several weeks prior to matriculation to immediately prior. The majority of studies made their interventions postmatriculation, some immediaterly or very soon after matriculation and some at various times – even continuously -throughout the first semester or even the entire academic year. The population targeted in the two studies described above as involving interventions in the last year of high school obviously was college-bound high school seniors. Otherwise the students focused upon typically were matriculating freshmen, except one study also included transfer students and another used older, non-traditional age students from mixed college year levels. Most studies had samples presumably representative of the general student population, but two focused on ASACQ- or SACQ-indicated poorly adjusting students, another on student athletes, and others on educationally, aptitudinally, economically, or socially disadvantaged students. It is somewhat surprising that more attention has not been focused upon transfer students who, like freshmen, are experiencing an important life transition. The nature of the interventions employed varies in two ways, i.e., the timespan involved 315 and the kind of manipulation. The timespan of the interventions had huge variation, ranging from one to two hours (for an individual interview, or for each of two or three such interviews), to a few days, several weeks, a few months, several months, or an entire academic year. One study staggered its occasions for intervention, having one per week in the first four weeks of the program, then bi-weekly until November, followed by once in January and another in March. The kinds of manipulations employed were multifarious. The one to two hour individual interviews mentioned above provided feedback of information concerning students’ previously collected ASACQ or SACQ data, with discussion of the relation between those data and the adjustment as experienced by the student, plus, where appropriate, focus on problems that the student was experiencing and what might be done about them. Several studies had small group counseling sessions, usually weekly, varying in number from four to ten weeks. One such instance was one of the studies cited above as involving high school seniors, which used group counseling sessions for identification and discussion of challenges inherent in the high school-to-college transition and ways of dealing with them. In two studies the group sessions were described as “social support based,” involving “semistructured exercises”; staff presentations; and discussion of particular themes, like formation and maintenance of campus social ties, relations with folks at home, residential and academic issues, balancing academic work and social life, and peer pressures and personal values as implicated in drug, alcohol, and sex issues. Another small group intervention focused on the development of coping skills, like goal-setting, relaxation training, and social networking. Two studies employing small groups identified their interventions as kinds of therapy rather than simply counseling, i.e., therapeutic journal-writing covering adjustment-related themes, and 316 reminiscence therapy.focusing on learning/educational issues. More prevalent in these SACQ-using investigations were interventions identified as orientation programs for new students. Some of these were fairly traditional in nature, one or a few days long, with the primary intention of introducing the student to the new physical, social, and academic environment, including living arrangements, academic offerings, campus organizations, etc. Others were more extensive and ambitious, lasting numerous weeks or even a full semester or academic year, sometimes based on standardized or “packaged” programs, and sometimes carrying course credit. . Some programs were based in residence halls, providing experiences focused on academic and social skill development, or development of peer support, or provision of peer advising. A few programs were quite elaborate in the sense of involving considerable portions of time characterized by living and acting together or engaging in some unusual or personally significant activity. Thus, one intervention was a four week period in the summer consisting of on-campus intensive academic and social experiences; another was a ten-day “total immersion” during the summer in off-campus outdoor activities (canoeing, cycling, sailing); or a concentrated pursuit of an academic topic or public service project of special interest to the student. In one particularly comprehensive experience, groups of 25 to 30 students were each organized around a particular academic theme; took three of their courses together, one of which was a small class made up solely of fellow program participants; had weekly meeetings for discussion of their experiences; and had opportunities for outside-of-class interaction with faculty members. 317 The various studies considered in this section employed a variety of means for assessing consequences of interventions. However, the only such methods of direct relevance to this monograph, and the main ones to be discussed here, are those employing the SACQ and, in two instances, its related instrument, the Anticipated Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire, or ASACQ. In the two instances of ASACQ use, that instrument provided pre-test or base measures against which subsequent SACQ scores could be compared for relative net changes in the treatment and control groups, which is information concerning the consequences of interventions. That is, beneficial consequences of interventions could be inferred from either greater increase or lesser decrease of scores in the treatment group than in the control group. With the prematriculation-administered ASACQ presumed to measure students’ selfconfidence regarding their ability to deal with the impending transition into college, and the postmatriculation-administered SACQ yielding a measure of actual adjustment, the relation between scores from the two instruments are interpreted as indicating the degree to which expectations are fulfilled, or, conversely, dashed (i.e., desgree of disillusionment). This comparison itself might then be regarded as an index of adjustment, in which one could expect consequences of interventions to be reflected, e.g., lesser disillusionment in a treatment group than in a control group. A few studies employ the SACQ itself as a pre-test or base measure, and then again subsequently as a post-test measure. In these instances, as in the case described above of joint ASACQ and SACQ use, the expectation is that consequences of interventions would be reflected in differential pre- to post-test changes for the treatment and control groups. 318 The reader may be aware that the SACQ test manual urges users not to employ only the instrument’s full scale score, because to do so can wash out and thus obscure important differences among the different areas of college adjustment tapped by the SACQ’s subscales. Also, by implication, optimum employment of the instrument would require use of all four subscales. But, despite these considerations, some of the intervention studies have reported findings only for the full scale score, and others only for selected subscales. In those instances it must be considered possible that the SACQ’s opportunity for revealing consequences of interventions has been impaired. There are other less than fortunate design aspects of the studies that have used the SACQ for assessing consequences of intervention. A few did not use control or comparison groups, or control groups that were matched on relevant criteria with the treatment groups, or comparison groups that were not really comparable. Most did not use random assignment of students in the creation of matched treatment and control groups. Most did not take into consideration the serious problems associated with self-selection of students who volunteered to participate in the treatment conditions, sometimes populationg control groups with students who had opted not to participate in intervention programs. And, finally, some studies make assertions of effect without use of inferential statistics. Though difficult to accomplish, it is of critical importance for reducing effects of selfselection that control groups consist of students who had volunteered for participation in the study and were assigned to an innocuous condition or, where possible, an intervention deferred to a later time. Second, highly desirable would be matching of control and treatment groups on as many variables relevant to college adjustment as possible, including the measure of adjustment 319 itself used pre-test, or, possibly, in SACQ-using studies, the ASACQ employed as a pre-test measure. Findings from SACQ-using intervention studies to be summarized now will only include those supported by inferential statistics, excluding some observations and conclusions made without such statistics that were, nevertheless, included in Chapter 16. Findings from studies not employing appropriate control or comparison groups will also be excluded, because there is no way of knowing whether any such outcomes might not have happened in comparable students not exposed to the intervention. Thus, several of the following paragraphs are concerned exclusively with investigations that tested significance of differences between treatment and control groups. Approximately one third of the SACQ-using intervention studies obtained no significant differences on SACQ variables between treatment and control groups, i.e., no effects of interventions. These findings came from studies employing a variety of interventions, including a month-long prematriculation academic/social experience; more or less traditional two to four day freshman orientation programs; a half semester orientation class; a year-long dormitorybased peer-support program; and two group therapeutic experiences each covering approximately a month’s time. Almost half of the investigations did report significant differences between treatment and control groups, though in one instance dramatically contrary to expectation. The reader may recall from earlier portions of this monograph that particular person and environment variables not infrequently are related to several or all SACQ indices at a time, indicating rather strong and pervasive effects, sometimes even on repeated SACQ testing occasions for given samples. This 320 was not so true with intervention studies. Only one study reported treatment/control group differences in as many as four of the five SACQ variables, and only one other study found significant effects for three of those variables; and in some studies effect(s) would be found in an SACQ testing at one time of the academic year but not another. In the rest of the studies the effects occurred in one or two of the SACQ variables, spread across those variables in roughly equal incidence, though maybe slightly more frequent for institutional attachment and overall adjustment. The kinds of interventions associated with beneficial effects run the gamut from pre- and postmatriculation orientation programs lasting several weeks to a full semester (with SACQ consequences that seem fairly skimpy for the amount of time and effort expended), through series of group counseling/discussion/orientation sessions and dormitory-based peer support programs (that seem as though they may be more fruitful), to intervention by individual interview with focus on SACQ-based identification or diagnosis of adjustment difficulties and opportunity for considering ways of dealing with them (the apparently most fruitful). The individual interview technique seems to hold promise not only for improvement of adjustment as indicated by increase in SACQ scores but also by reduction of withdrawal from college. In addition to the foregoing findings regarding differences between treatment and control groups as reflected in SACQ scores, there are also some findings concerning relation between students’ evaluations of, or degree of participation in, intervention programs and SACQ scores. Thus, students’ ratings of satisfaction with a several week summer program upon its completion in August positively predicted all indices from the SACQ adminsitered the subsequent March; the number of meetings that students attended in a fifteen session orientation program correlated 321 positively with all SACQ variables except personal-emotional adjustment, strongest with social adjustment and institutional attachment; and the number of pages that students reported producing in a therapeutic journal-writing program was positively associated with improvement in overall college adjustment. Now, this summary of SACQ-using intervention studies completed, a few words regarding possible future directions of such investigations may be offered. The individual interview technique discussed above, though promising, is relatively restricted in its scope of possible remedial activities. One wonders whether outcomes might not be improved by interventions that are tailored to particular kinds of problems experienced by students – possibly as indicated by test data – and that are less circumscribed in time and purpose than an individual interview, e.g., a social skills program for students experiencing problems in social adjustment. In any event, the more promising results to date seem to indicate the possibility of more pronounced effects from interventions that involve direct, personal interaction with the student, as through individual interview or small group events. One wonders, too, whether such effects might be enhanced if the intervention were directed at students identified as at-risk on the basis of person characteristics known to be related to effectiveness of adjustment to college -- e.g., self-esteem, self-confidence regarding adaptive capacity -- and capable of being influenced for the better by appropriate efforts. Particularly desirable would be interventions that could preclude the occurrence of the disillusionment with one's adaptive capacity that has been found to have serious consequences for many students. Finally, it is to be hoped that progress in research concerning determinants of adjustment to college will continue and will contribute to the development of new and better forms of 322 intervention. 323 APPENDIXES A. Pearson Product-Moment Correlations Between SACQ Scores and Grade Point Average B. Point-Biserial Correlations Between SACQ Scores and Attrition C. Pearson Product-Moment Correlations Between SACQ Scores and Measures of Person Characteristics D. Pearson Product-Moment Correlations Between SACQ Scores and Measures of Mental and Physical Health and Adjustment E. Pearson Product-Moment Correlations Between SACQ Scores and Measures of Environment-Related Experience 324 Appendix A Correlations between SACQ Scores and Grade Point Averagea,b Academic Adjustment Social Adjustment PersonalEmot’l Adjustment Attachment Full Scale 132n .40** .10 .35** .09 .35** First semester final exams gpa 358 .14** .01 .10 .00 .09 Second semester final exams gpa 340 .10 -.05 .08 -.03 .05 Ensuing Sept. “repeat” exams gpa 255 .02 -.11 -.01 -.14* -.05 (Beyers, 2001) 969 -- -- -- -- .24** Clark University Semester 1 Semester 2 (Baker, 1993b) 187 158 .25** .41** -.03 -.09 -.08 .13 -.01 .04 .11 .21** Sept. SACQ admin. 171n .32** -.14 .07 -.15 .06 Nov. SACQ admin. 81n .35** -.15 .11 -.17 .07 .05 -.11 .05 -.18 -.05 Sample Source Arizona State University (Coatsworth, 2001) n Catholic University of Leuven (Beyers & Goosens, 2002a) Colgate University Feb. SACQ admin. 37 n (Conti, 2000a,c) DePaul University (Humfleet & Ribordy, 1990) 73 .48** -- -- -- .33** 213c .40** -- -- .13* -- Men 229 .25** .11 .25** .19** -- Women 213 .23** -.08 .18* -.04 -- Total sample 442 .25** .02 .20** .14** -- -- -- -- .35** George Washington University (Harris, 1991) Historically Black private college, otherwise unidentified (Washington, 1996) Hollins College (Gilkey, 1988; Gilkey et al., 1989) 105d,,j -- (Gold et al., 1990) 29i .48** .18 .09 .21 .46** Lebanon Valley College (Grella, 1989) 45d .47** .06 .16 .14 .26 Institution unidentified 325 PersonalEmot’l Adjustment Academic Adjustment Social Adjustment .21** -- -- -- -- .27** -- -- -- -- 108 .44** .21* .09 .34** -- 65 .25* .03 -.04 .22 108 .55** .21* .13 .35** -- 65 .41** .12 .04 .27* .31* 229j .40** .07 .27** .10 -- 98e 116f all 72d,I 95-98 .13 .40** .32** .39** .39** -.09 .13 .05 .19 .08 -.08 .20* .13 .18 .20* -.11 .14 .05 .31* .19* ---.35** .29** 80 .48** .21 .28* .24* .39** 493 .33** NS .16** NS .19** 1011 .37** .29** .23** .33** -- 144 .34** .12 .20* -- -- (Williams, 1996) 180 .33** -- -- -- -- Univ. of Maryland, Baltimore (Maton, 1989b) 75 .31** .01 .07 .04 .18 Sample Source n Attachment Full Scale Michigan State University Math/science courses 205d All courses (Lent, 1997) Purdue University Semester 1 Student athletes Non-athletes .18 Semester 2 Student athletes Non-athletes (Foster, 1997) Queen’s University (Ontario) (Marcotte, 1995) Rutgers University (Young, 1994) (Evans-Hughes, 1992) (Pfeil, 2000) San Diego State Univ. & Grossmont College (Wang & Smith, 1993) State Univ. of NY, New Paltz (Loveland, 1994) Suffolk Community College (Napoli & Wortman, 1998) University of Arizona (Natera, 1998) U. of Calif. at Los Angeles 326 Sample Source n Academic Adjustment Social Adjustment PersonalEmot’l Adjustment Attachment Full Scale Univ. of Missouri-Columbia (Dewein, 1994) 272d .42** -.05 -.17** .13* -- Semester I 142 .14 -.17* -- -- -- .41** .07 -- -- -- 433n .28** .16** .13* .18** .21** 85g 30h .20* .54** -.01 .20 .05 .13 .07 .36* .10 .37* .46** -.23** .04 .01 -- 49 95 35k .28* .34**i .37* ---.07 ---.03 --.05 ---- 446-451 .51** ns .23** ns -- 96 .29** -.14 .20* -- -- .31** -.01 .20** -.02 -- .53** .24* .18 .28** -- .38** .07 .14** .07 Semester II (Bettencourt et al., 1999) University of Missouri-Rolla (Montgomery, Haemmerlie, & Watkins, 2000; Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) University of Pennsylvania (Keenan, 1992) Univ. of Wisconsin, Madison (Brower, 1990b) 623 Virginia Commonwealth Univ. (Chartrand et al., 1990b) (Davis, 1988) Wilfrid Laurier University (Birnie-Lefcovitch, 1997) Yale University (Terrell, 1989) York Univ. (Canada) 212j (Sugar, 1999) (Yaffe, 1997; see also Wintre & 92 361 m Yaffe, 2000) a See also Tables 16 and 17, pp. 45 and 46, of the SACQ manual (Baker & Siryk, 1989). All freshman students unless otherwise indicated. c All seniors. d From mixed college year levels. e Minority students. f White students. g All academically at-risk because of relatively low aptitude and performance measures. h Those in the above n of 85 who participated in a several week special pre-freshman program. i In psychology courses only. j All females. k All student-athletes. b .24** 327 l African-American students. All males. n Self-reported grade point average. m *p<.05; **p<.01. 328 Appendix B Correlations Between SACQ Scores and Attrition (after 1 year unless otherwise indicated)a Sample Source Catholic Univ. Of Leuven As of January of freshman year As of June of freshman year As of Sept. of sophomore year (Beyers & Goossens, 2002a) Clark University 1990-91 Semester 1 Semester 2 (Baker, 1993) n 368 Academic Adjustment -.07 -.11* -.14** Social Adjustment -.12* -.20** -.12** PersonalEmotional Adjustment -.09 -.16** Attachment Full Scale -.14** -.18** -.14** -.13* -.20** -.18** -.16** 190 161 -.13* -.04 -.12* -.12 -.16* -.11 -.20** -.26** -.18** -.14* 311 -.02 -.13* -.03 -.29** -.14* 1011b -.36** -.34** -.24** -.33** -- Florida State University (Brunelle-Joiner, 1999) Suffolk Community College (Napoli & Wortman, 1998) University of Hartford (Santonicola, 1989) 85 -.12 -.34** -.22* -.43** -.32** University of Oregon (Gerdes, 1987) 112b -.15 -.16 -.03 -.36** -.22** University of Wisconsin, Madison (Brower, 1990a)c 512 -.15** -.18** -.10 -.28** -.21** a See also Table 21 on page 49 of the SACQ manual (Baker & Siryk, 1989). After one semester. c After two years. b *p<.05; **p<.01. 329 Appendix C Correlations between the SACQ and Measures of Person Characteristicsa Test/Test Variable n Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment Full Scale Academic Locus of Control Scale (Mooney, 1989) (Mooney et al., 1991) (Weinstock, 1995) (Bartels, 1995) 118r .64**q .44**q .45**q .47**q .62**q 88b .63**q .51**q .51**q .55**q .67**q 69 .50**q .12q .52**q .29*q 506 .63**q .18**q .39**q .31**q .52**q 114 .53** .11 .38** .10 .45** 358 .65** .40** .42** .48** -- 205w .77** -- -- -- -- 13621 .05 .16* .15 .04 -- 93b .23* .22* .06 .12 56 .17 .30* .44** .14 .39** 205w .09 -- -- -- -- .09 -- -- -- -- -- Academic Motivation Scale (McGowan, 1988) (Fuller, 2000) Academic Self-Concept Scale (Lent et al., 1997) Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican-Americans-II (Shibazaki, 1999) Achieving Tendency Scale (Wang & Smith, 1993) .20 ACT (Brooks & DuBois, 1995) Mathematics Composite score (Lent et al., 1997) (Dewein, 1994) 272w .21** -.06 .19** .07 -- 87 -- -- -- -- .25** 202 .22** .27** .07 .25** .25** 244w -- -- -- -- .25** -- -- -- -- .37** Adolescent Coping Orientation for Problem Experiences Problem-focused coping (Smith, 1994) (Just, 1998) Adult Attachment Scale Close Depend 330 Test/Test Variable n Anxiety Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment -- -- -- -- -.31** .17** .21** .27** .11* .23** -.17** -.25** -.23** -.17** -.25** -.20** -.18** -.35** -.12* -.26** Full Scale (Bartels, 1990) Adult Attachment Types Questionnaire (modified) Secure 292 Avoidant Anxious/Ambivalent (Shilkret, 2000) African American Acculturation Scale Cultural traditionality Black women students Black male students (Dewitt-Parker, 1999) 81 .10 -- -.30** -- -.20* 54 -.13 -- -.16 -- -.04 Alienation Scale (Liter, 1987) (McGowan, 1988) 100 -.50** -.37** -.32** -.24* -.50** 114 -.37** -.50** -.49** -.45** -.56** 76 .30** .46** .22* .34** .42** -.25* -.37** -.22* -.29** -.37** -.30** -.40** -.42** -.29** -.46** .45** .56** .65** .46** .63** -.29* -.36** -.42** -.30** -.41** -.42** -.47** -.57** -.35** -.54** .36** -.26** .44** .43** .34** .47** -.32** -.32** -.25** -.35** -.34** -.58** -.23** -.50** Attachment Style Questionnaire September testing Secure Avoidant Anxious/Ambivalent November testing Secure Avoidant 73 Anxious/Ambivalent (Klynn, 1997) Secure Avoidant 292 Anxious/Ambivalent (Shilkret, 2000) -.43** 331 n 82 .13 .43** .40** .35** .41** 36y .42** .45** .58** .54** .58** 256 .03 .07 -.16** .02 -.02 140 .14* .06 .05 .04 .12 .09 -.03 -.02 -.04 .02 .17* .18* .15* .16* .22** life events in general .04 -.04 -.06 -.07 -.02 bad life events -.11 -.24** -.12 -.24** -.21** good life events .13 .11 .01 .08 .12 .09 .05 -.10 .02 .03 .00 -.06 -.20** -.07 -.09 .18* .20** .11 .14 .20** 145 .27** .14* .36** .10 .31** 144 .01 -.06 -.18* .04 -.07 145 .07 .42** .23** .24** .30** 105s,w .50** .39** .45** .38** .51** Test/Test Variable Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Academic Adj Attachment Full Scale Attitudes and Beliefs Scale-II (Friedland, 1990) Attitudes Towards Disabled Persons Scale-Form O (Friedland, 1990) Attributional Complexity Scale (M. D. Smith, 1994) Attributional Style Questionnaire Internal attributions life events in general bad life events good life events Stable attributions Global attributions life events in general bad life events good life events (Rines, 1998) Autonomy Scale Self-Awareness Sensitivity to Others Capacity to Manage New Situations (Vivona, 2000b) Behavioral Attributes of Psychosocial Competence Scale Active coping 332 Test/Test Variable (Zea et al, 1995; Zea, 1997) (Zea et al., 1996) PersonalEmotional Adj Academic Adj Social Adj 56t,w .44** .25* .37** .33** .43** 66u,w .59** .39** .51** .38** .55** 71v,w .46** .47** .46** .35** .55** 298w,2 .50** .37** .45** .37** .51** 346 .51** .38** .46** .39** .52** 105s .56** .40** .45** .43** .55** 56t .45** .21 .27* .27* .37** 66u .52** .32** .42** .31* .46** 71v .46** .38** .35** .22 .44** 298 .51** .33** .39** .32** .47** 346 .52** .33** .40** .35** .48** .47** .28* .60** .30* .51** .22** .38** .34** .29** .29** .21** .26** .31** .30** .30** NA NA NA NA NA .36** .41** .51** .33** .30** .11* .25** .24** .28** .34** 90 -.54** -- -- -- 319 -.36** -.35** -.31** -.27** -.31** 156w -- .33** .26** -- -- -- -.42** -.29** -- -- n Full Scale Attachment Behavioral Attributes of Psychosocial Competence ScaleCondensed Active coping (Zea, 1997) (Zea et al., 1996) Belief Systems Analysis Scale (Hatter & Ottens, 1998) 64t,w “Big Five” Inventory Extraversion 372 Agreeableness Conscientiousness Emotional Stability Openness (Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) Career Decision Scale (Chartrand et al., 1990b) -- Cheek and Buss Shyness Scale (Montgomery et al., 2000; data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) Close Relationships Questionnaire Secure attachment style Fearful style 333 Test/Test Variable n Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Full Scale Attachment Preoccupied style -- -.23** -.26** -- -- Dismissing style -- .09 -.09 -- -- -- .31** -- -- -- Fearful -- -.21”” -- -- -- Preoccupied -- -.09 -- -- -- Dismissing -- -.07 -- -- -- (Lapsley & Edgerton, 1999a) Secure 304 (Lapsley et al., 2001) Secure 97-100 .20* .31** .12 .23* .25* Fearful -.27** -.14 -.21* -.13 -.24* Preoccupied -.32** -.23* -.32** -.26* -.32** Dusmissing .10 .03 .04 -.05 .04 Positivity of self-image .43** .34** .33** .25* .40** Positivity of image of others .01 .07 -.01 .07 .04 .09 .39** -- -- -- .18* .32** -- -- -- (Pfeil, 2000) Collective Self-Esteem Scale Semester I, 1st half 142 Semester II, 2nd half (Bettencourt et al., 1999) College Self-Efficacy Instrument Overall score (Bartels, 1995) 506 .47** .52** .35** .39** .56** (Fuller, 2000) 325 .57** .56** .32** .51** -- Course (academic) self-efficacy 144w .77** .39** .54** -- -- .36** .62** .43** -- -- .58** .39** .36** .23** -- .43** .59** .49** .40** -- .60** .46** .46** -- .31** .53** .50** -- .53** .55** .50** .64** .32** .50** .49** -- .60** .63** .60** Social self-efficacy (Natera, 1998) Course (academic) self-efficacy 13621 Social self-efficacy (Shibazaki, 1999) Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory (Crouse, 1990) (Mooney, 1989) (Mooney et al., 1991) (Frazier & Cook, 1993) 70 118r 88b 85 - - 334 Test/Test Variable (Clauss, 1995) (Rodriguez-Perez, 1991) n Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment Full Scale 110 66w,14 57w,15 123w,2 -.42** .40** .40** .46** .31** .49** .42** .66** .45** .54** .49** -.27* .50** .38** -.46** .69** .56** 229b Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations Task-oriented .53** .39** .45** .40** -- Emotion-oriented -.41** -.36** -.54** -.37** -- Avoidance-oriented -.17* .05 -.09 .03 -- -- -- .24**7 -- -- -- 8 -- -- 7 -- -- 8 (Marcotte, 1995) Task-oriented 109w -Emotion-oriented -- -- .23** -.48** -- -- -.28** -- -- Avoidance-oriented -- -- -.107 -- -- (Gallant, 1994) -- -- .068 -- -- .17 .21* .22* .22* -- .34** .32** .32** .32** -- -- -- -- -- .38** 241 .41** .44** .54** .41** .55** 252 -.34** -.18** -.34** -.45** -.31** -- -.23** -.21** -.19** -- .36** .07 .26** .26** .01 -.08 -.05 -.06 -.23** -.16* -.15* -.22** -.33** -.09 -.29** -.28** Coping With Barriers Scale Coping with career-related barrier Coping with education-related barriers Full scale score 127 (Hutz, 2002a) Edwards Social Desirability Scale (Rice et al., 1990; Lapsley, 1989) Experiences in Close Relationships Questionnaire Anxiety scale Avoidance scale (Kalsner-Silver, 2000) Extended Objective Measure of Ego-Identity Status-2 Achieved Foreclosed Moratorium Diffusion (Just, 1998) Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale 202 .15* -.06 -.16* -.21** 335 Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment Full Scale Test/Test Variable n (Wang & Smith, 1993) 93b -.18 -.09 -.21* -.05 -.21* 30c 66b .38* .44** .53** .45** .42* .33** .51** .47** .55** .52** Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale (Silverthorn, 1993) Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale (modified) 31x .65** .24 .40* .23 32 x .09 .37* .52** .28* .40* 34 x .45** .33* .36* .40** .42** 88 198 272w .30** .56** .54**6 .13 -.32**6 .28** .45** .49**6 .21* -.36**6 ---- 133w .11 .25** .35** .23** .28** Commitment .38** .34** .44** .33** .49** Control .34** .23** .28** .24** .37** Composite score .34** .33** .43** .32** .46** .25** .15* .01 .15* .17* Active-cognitive coping .11 .15* -.04 .17* .13 Avoidance coping -.42** -.30** -.37** -.38** -.44** 139 .45** .32** .34** -- .46** 435 .46** .37** .53** .29** .44** Ambition .41** .49** .44** .36** .44** Sociability .11* .22** .20** .17** .17** Likeability .25** .23** .23** .22** .22** Prudence .22** .14** .15** .17** .19** Intellectance .13** .10* .10* .05 .08 School Success .22** .15** .21** .12* .18** As a student As an athlete .38* As a person (Davis, 1988) Goal Instability Scale (goal directedness) (Robbins & Schwitzer, 1988) (Robbins et al., 1993) (Dewein, 1994) Hardiness Test Challenge (Posselt, 1992) Health and Daily Living Form Active-behavioral coping 140 (Rines, 1998) Composite score (Feenstra et al., 2001) Hogan Personality Inventory Adjustment 336 Test/Test Variable Academic Adj n Stress Tolerance Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment Full Scale .51** .46** .59** .35** .51** .10 -.27* -.32* .05 -.13 Interpersonal Distance .00 -.41** -.33* -.21 -.35** Grief .29* -.34* -.20 .00 -.02 Total score .11 -.40** -.35** -.06 -.21 .14 .12 .07 -.02 .14 Vertical individualism -.13 -.05 -.15* -.10 -.11 Horizontal collectivism .28** .45** .23** .37** .42** Vertical collectivism .12 .17* .09 .13 .17* Individualism -- -- -- -- .01 Collectivism -- -- -- -- .34** .30** .03 .06 .01 .16 Normative orientation subscale .19 .17 .14 .22* .22* Diffuse orientation subscale -.40** -.11 -.12 -.17 -.28** 106z .15 .32** .18 .22* .22* 1 .19 .41 .10 .48* .35 57 -.28** -.25* -.22* -.15 -.29** 105s,w 56t,w -.31** -.08 -.27** -.26* -.26** -.15 -.29** -.24* -.34** -.23* (Montgomery, Haemmerlie, & Watkins, 2000; data updated by Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) Homecomer Culture Shock Scale Cultural Distance 45w (Huff, 1998) Horizontal & Vertical Individualism & Collectivism Scale Horizontal individualism 170w,20 (Choi, 1999, 2000) Identity Style Inventory-2 Information orientation subscale 105 (Hollmann, 1995; Hollmann & Metzler, 1994) Individualism-Collectivism Scale General collectivism index (Kusaka, 1995) 18 Internal-External Locus of Control Scale (Shilkret & Taylor, 1992) 337 Test/Test Variable (Zea et al., 1995; Zea, 1997) (Evans-Hughes, 1992) Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj n Academic Adj 66u,w 71v,w 298w,2 72t,w -.52** -.06 -.27** -.19 -.25* -.08 -.20** -.19 -.42** -.16 -.27** -.27* -.27* .00 -.21** -.14 -.43** -.10 -.29** -.26* 96b -.22* -.29** -.42** -.19* -.34** -.07 -.21* -.29** -.09 -.24* Attachment Full Scale Interpersonal Dependency Inv. Emot’l reliance on other persons 65 c 161 Lack of social self-confidence 96 2 -.14* -.27** -.35** -.14* -.29** b -.18* -.22* -.25** -.22* -.28** c -.26* -.18 -.34** -.15 -.31** 65 161 2 -.17* -.23** -.27** -.18* -.28** b -.11 -.16 -.13 -.13 -.19* c -.05 .07 -.05 .04 -.01 -.05 -.09 -.08 -.04 -.10 96b -.23* -.31** -.37** -.24** -.37** 65c -.19 -.19 -.36** -.11 -.30** -.16* -.28** -.33** -.17* -.32** -.66* -.45 -.55* -.46 -.64* .27 -.21 -.17 -.04 .04 Self-hate guilt -.52* -.50 -.88** -.36 -.76** Omnipotent responsibility guilt -.43 -.80** -.74** -.68* -.72** Interpersonal guilt -.53* -.61* -.65* -.55* -.67* -.24** -.17* -.42** -.11 -.29** Separation guilt -.09 -.25** -,22** -.19* -.23** Self-hate guilt -.44** -.49** -.64** -.38** -.58** Omnipotent responsibility guilt -.16 -.16 -.43** -.10 -.25** Interpersonal guilt -.33** -.38** -.59** -.28** -.48** -.20** -.07 -.34** .02 -.22** Separation guilt -.10 -.19** -.18* -.19** -.21** Self-hate guilt -.41** -.33** -.56** -.21** -.52** Omnipotent responsibility guilt -.24** -.18* -.49** -.10 -.35** Assertion of autonomy 96 65 161 Total score (Polewchak, 1999) 161 2 2 Interpersonal Guilt Quest.-45 Survivor guilt 11b Separation guilt (Shilkret & Nigrosh, 1995) Survivor guilt 140b (Shilkret & Vecchiotti, 1995) Survivor guilt 156b 338 PersonalEmotional Adj Academic Adj Social Adj -.22** -.17* -.41** -.10 -.31** -.35** -.20* -.36** -.16 -.33** Separation guilt .10 -.02 -.02 .10 .05 Self-hate guilt -.47** -.24* -.56** -.24* -.47** Omnipotent responsibility guilt -.20* -.14 -.35** -.08 -.23* Total score -.22* -.16 -.32** .07 -.24* -.27** -.19** -.40** -.13* -.32** Separation guilt -.02 -.15** -.10* -.06 -.10* Self-hate guilt -.41** -.27** -.58** -.19** -.47** Omnipotent responsibility guilt -.17** -.08 -.35** -.05 -.21** Interpersonal guilt -.21** -.19** -.36** -.11* -.27** -.30** -.23* -.25** -.01 -.32** Separation guilt -.11 -.20* -.33** -.12 -.21* Self-hate guilt -.45** -.31** -.36** -.25** -.50** Omnipoptent responsibility guilt -.33** -.21* -.11 -.14 -.31** Interpersonal guilt -.30** -.28** -.29** -.12 -.35** 73b 66c 73b 66c .15 .43** .09 .16 .34** .31** .28* .17 .24* .42** .11* .29* ----- ----- 159b -- -- .13* -- -- 150 -.17* .20* .24** .21* -.16* .17* .24** .20* -.42** .26** .31** .35** NS .18* .18* .17* -.26** .22** .29** .27** Test/Test Variable n Total score Attachment Full Scale (Shilkret & Edwards, 1997) Survivor guilt 90b,w (Bailey & Shilkret, 2000b) Survivor guilt 292 (Shilkret, 2000) Survivor guilt 109 (Morray & Shilkret, 2001) Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment Parents Attachment to mother Attachment to father (Schultheiss & Blustein, 1994) Attachment to mother (Levin, 1996) Alienation Communication Trust Overall 339 Test/Test Variable Peers Alienation Communication Trust Overall Combined Parent/Peer Overall Attachment (Harste, 1996) Parents Alienation Communication Trust n PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment Full Scale -.21* .16* .24** .22** -.43** .30** .40** .44** -.47** NS .18* .24** -.36** .30** .38** .39** -.46** .22** .34** .38** 150 .25** .35** .36** .36** .37** 8111 -.22* .20* .29** -.23* .16 .09 -.19* -.01 .12 ---- ---- 8112 -.22* .27** .25* -.36** .28** .32** -.52** .24* .37** ---- ---- 8113 -.17 .28** .21* -.11 .19* .07 -.21* .11 .18 ---- ---- 118 .27** .33** .06 .29** .25** .16* .08 .30** .26** .36** 81 54 .62** .54** --- .11 .12 --- .16 .31* 81 54 .67** .72** --- .11 .08 --- .07 .32** Alienation Communication Trust (Rice, 1990/1991 Parents Attachment to mother Black female students Black male students Attachment to father Black female students Black male students (Dewitt-Parker, 1999) Social Adj 150 Alienation Communication Trust Parents Attachment to mother Attachment to father (Silver, 1995) Academic Adj 340 Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Test/Test Variable n Academic Adj Parents (combined) Overall score Peers Overall score (Just, 1998) 202 .39** .32** .38** .33** .46** .21** .43** .28** .35** .39** Parents (combined) Overall score (Pappas, 2000) 66 .23* .37** .18 .32** .34** 151 .13 .25** .20** .24** .25** Isolation -.30** -.42** -.40** -.48** -.48** Intimacy minus Isolation .27** .42** .38** .45** .46** 229b .63** .51** .51** .56** -- 407 .12* .12* .12* .09* .14** 224 .24** .13 .24** .16* .25** (Hunsberger, 2000) 226 .29** .39** .42** .34** .43** (Montgomery, Haemmerlie, & 299 .36** .36** .33** .35** .34** (Pratt, 2001) 96 .28** .45** .39** .40** -- Life Orientation Test-Revised (Johnson, 2001) 90 .28** .32** .34** .35** .40** 506 .20** .29** .05 .26** .25** Materialistic orientation .07 .18** .00 .18** .12** Intellectual and cultural interests .17** .25** .04 .19** .21** Valuing of achievement .30** .28** .09 .28** .31** Attachment Full Scale Inventory of Psychosocial Development Intimacy (Vivona, 2000b) Jackson Social Desirability Scale (Marcotte, 1995) Late Adolescents’ Relationships with Parents Scale Enabling independence (Yaffe, 1997) Abbreviated total score (Hunsberger et al., 1996) Life Orientation Test Ray, 2001) Life Values Inventory Conventionally defined success (Bartels, 1995) 341 n Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment 8122 .35** .25* .16 .19 -- .27* .08 .04 .07 -- 241 30c 66b 229b 137u,w 96 185 72 37 .24** .41* NS .28** --.04 .26** .31** .23 .24** .29 .26* .23** --.03 .27** .33** .12 .22* .43* .38** .26** -.20 .26** .26* .28 .23** .44* NS .24** --.25** .32** .13 142w -- -- -- -- .22** 205w .25** -- -- -- -- .11 -- -- -- -- -.47** -.38** -.32** -.39** -.53** -.42** -.25* -.32** -.46** -.46** .13* .19** .10 .18** -- Helplessness -.42** -.25** -.31** -.31** -- Luck -.20** -.08 -.24** -.07 -- 343 -.36** -.22** -.31** -.25** -- 103 .12 .05 -.16 -.14 -.21* -.30** .02 -.10 -.22* -.06 -.20* -.21* Test/Test Variable Full Scale Majority-Minority Relations Survey Ethnic social customs adherence Ethnic language use adherence (Amin, 2000) Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (Rice et al., 1990; Lapsley, 1989) (Silverthorn, 1993; Silverthorn & Gekoski, 1995) (Marcotte, 1995) (Rodriguez, 1994) (Terrell, 1989) Sept. (both tests admin’d) Nov. (SACQ admin’d) Feb. (SACQ admin’d) (Conti, 2000a,c) .28** .49** .26* -.31** -.32** .38** .23 Mastery Learning Scale (Lopez, 1997) Mathematics Self-Efficacy Scale Mathematics course Mathematics problem (Lent et al., 1997) Mother & Father Questionnaire Regarding Mother Regarding Father (Shilkret & Taylor, 1992) 57b 59b Multidimensional AcademicSpecific Locus of Control Scale Internality Total score 349 (Fuller, 2000) Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale Self-oriented Other-oriented .05 -.04 -.27** 342 Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment .00 -.29* -.13 -.20* -.13 -.19** -.16* -.25** -.16* -- .10 .09 .16* .13* -- -.02 -.02 .06 .06 -- .35** .34** .33** .24* -- .10 .11 .06 .14* -- Ethnic identity achievement .08 .09 .03 .12 -- Ethnic behaviors/practices .08 .06 .01 .09 -- Test/Test Variable n Socially-prescribed Full scale (Merryman & Zelezny, 1993) Multigenerational Interconnectedness Scale Psychological connectedness Functional connectedness Financial connectedness (Kalsner-Silver, 2000) 252 Full Scale Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure Ethnic identity achievement 8122 (Amin, 2000) Positive ethnic attitudes 252 (Kalsner-Silver, 2000) My Vocational Situation Vocational Identity Scale (Lopez, 1989) (Maton & Weisman, 1989; Maton, 1989b) 114c 185b .52** .61** --- --- --- --- 75d 75e .42** -- .31** -- .20* -- .30** -- .38** .27* (Fuller, 2000) 352 .46** .22** .31** .24** -- Narcissistic Injury Scale (Zamostny et al., 1993) 228 -.47** -.46** -.58** -.41** -- 255b -.61** -.44** -.68** -.42** -- 102 c -.30** -.47** -.67** -.30** -- 255 b .17** .46** .25** .34** -- 102c .16 .58** .35** .38** -- 255b -.01 .04 -.10 -.02 -- 102c .26** .23** .09 .26** -- 255 b .20** .26** .29** .24** -- 102 c .24** .43** .41** .34** -- 255 b .58** .28** .31** .32** -- 102 c .59** .25** .31** .33** -- NEO Five-Factor Inventory Neuroticism Extraversion Openness to experience Agreeableness Conscientiousness (Wintre & Sugar, 2000; see also Sugar, 1997) 343 Test/Test Variable Agreeableness n 1011 Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment Full Scale .20** .23** .16** .25** -- .34** .29** .26** .29** -- -- .30** .30** -- -- -- .40** .28** -- -- NAd 103e 219e 229b 206 -.64** -.49** --.40** -.35** ----.27** -.30** ----.35** -.30** ----.30** -.29** ---.41** --.29** 201b .17* .24** .18* .01 .15* .07 .24** .22** .17* .14* .24** .16* .12 .22** .14 116f .25** .08 .22* .25** -.06 .21* .26** .23* .22* .18 -.02 .24* ---- ---- .23* .30** .23* .25** .19* .15 ---- ---- 45w .27* .29* .09 .16 .26* .17 .36** .36** .37** .31* .33* .17 .37** .42** .29* 320w .33** .27** .28** .26** .39** .34** .33** .31** .42** .36** Conscientiousness (Napoli & Wortman, 1998) New Personal Fable Scale Invulnerability 156w Omnipotence (Lapsley & Edgerton, 1999b) Nowicki-Strickland InternalExternal Control Scale for Adults (Cooley & Carden, 1992) (Jampol, 1988/1989) (Marcotte, 1995) (Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) Parental Attachment Questionnaire Affective Quality of Attachment Parental support of autonomy Parents provide emot’l support (Kenny, 1992) Affective quality of attachment Parental support of autonomy Parents provide emot’l support (Kenny, 1994) Affective quality of attachment Parental support of autonomy Parents provide emot’l support (Clauss, 1995) Affective quality of attachment Parental support of autonomy Parents provide emot’l support (Huff, 1998) Affective Quality of Attachment Parental Support of Autonomy Parents Provide Emot’l Support 110 344 Test/Test Variable n (Hutto, 1998) Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment Full Scale .26** .33** .26** .32** .31** 152 .33** .28** .17* .19** .20** .17* .33** .27** .22** .20** .16* .13 .34** .30** .22** 252 .27** .24** .21** .17** .12* .22** .30** .33** .28** .16** .10 .21** ---- 93-100 .22* .14 .08 .18* .24* .16 .24* .23* .27** .24* .08 .22* .24* .09 .22* .19* .29** .19* .16 .24* (Lapsley & Edgerton, 1999a) 156w -- -.53** -.48** -- -- (Lapsley et al., 2001) 304 -- -.27** -- -- -- 272w -.28** -.29** -.31** -.25** -- 152 -.15* -.20** -.53** -.12 -.32** 127 -.09 -.22* -.24** -.08 -- -.35** -.32** -.40** -.29** -- -- -- -- -- -.42** 255b,16 .19** .25** .23** .13* -- 102 c,16 .40** .44** .35** .44** -- 255 b,16 .23** .22** .19** .13* -- 102 c,16 .18 .20* .26** .26** -- 255 b,16 .25** .22** .23** .15* -- 102 c,16 .15 .32** .24* .32** -- Affective Quality of Attachment Parental Support of Autonomy Parents Provide Emot’l Support (Vivona, 2000b) Affective Quality of Attachment Parental Support of Autonomy Parents Provide Emot’l Support (Kalsner-Silver, 2000) Affective Quality of Attachment Parental Support of Autonomy Parents Provide Emot’l Support Full scale (Pfeil, 2000) Pathological SeparationIndividuation Inventory Peer Group Dependence Scale (Dewein, 1994) Penn State Worry Quest. (Vivona, 2000b) Perception of Barriers Scale Career-related barriers Education-related barriers Full scale score (Hutz, 2002a,b) Perception of Parental Reciprocity Scale Parents unspecified Mother Father (Sugar, 1999) 345 n 255b,17 .28** .23** .23** .17** -- 102 c,17 .32** .50** .42** .45** -- 255 b,17 .23** .17** .18** .13* -- 102 c,17 .26** .34** .42** .28** -- (Wintre & Sugar, 2000; see also 255 b,17 .30** .27** .27** .21** -- Sugar, 1997) 102c,17 .12 .36** .25** .33** -- 384 .28** .32** .31** .24** .37** 41 .29 .44** .44** .30 .46** 105b,w .19 -.09 -.03 .03 .00 -- .19 .30** .30** .19 -.09 -- .18 .43** .34** .34** .05 -- .21* .29** .24* .20* -.04 -- .28** .36** .32** .30** -.02 .34** -.23* -.43** -.18 -.32** -.29** -.53** -.05 -.20 -.23* -.44** .37** .53** .41** .48** .49** Positive: Yourself .44** .43** .40** .38** .48** Focused .59** .40** .44** .39** .55** Flexible: Thoughts .15** .16** .14* .12* .16** Flexible: Social .22** .45** .25** .32** .34** Organized .42** .14* .17** .17** .31** Proactive .14* .19** .09 .14* .14* Test/Test Variable Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Academic Adj Attachment Full Scale Parents unspecified Mother Father Total score (Yaffe, 1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) Personal Attributes Questionnaire Androgyny (Bobier, 1989) Personal Authority in the Family System Questionnaire-Version C Intimacy Individuation Personal authority Intimidation subscale Triangulation subscale Full scale (Gilkey, 1988; Gilkey et al., 1989) Personal Feelings Questionnaire Guilt Shame (Shilkret & Taylor, 1992) 59b Personal Resilience Quest’aire Positive: World (Brunelle-Joiner, 1999) Personal Values Scales-Revised 311 346 Test/Test Variable Intellectualism n 1059 Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment Full Scale .01 .12 -.07 -.04 .06 10 .60** .45* .36 .49* .61** 2 .14 .21* .02 .10 .19* .11 .39** .25* .30** .30** Commitment .54** .42** .32** .37** .53** Control .44** .55** .21* .40** .49** Composite score .42** .55** .32** .43** .53** 30c 66b .39* .59** .47** .44** .47** .46** .43** .42** .53** .61** 63 .51** .67** .35** .52** .63** -.34** -.63** -.26* 25 (Hertel, 1996) 130 Personal Views Survey Challenge 63 (Mathis & Lecci, 1999) Personality Research Form Desirability subscale (Silverthorn, 1993; Silverthorn & Gekoski, 1995) Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Positive affect Negative affect -.22* -.46** (Mathis & Lecci, 1999) Prestatie Motivatie Test (Bartels, 1995) 506 .59** .27** .23** .34** .49** 218 ----- ----- ----- ----- .61** -.51** -.57** .60** 229b -.53** -.42** -.45** -.42** -- (Dewein, 1994) 272w -.28** -.08 -.29** -.18** -- Psychological Separation Invent. Conflictual independence In relation to father In relation to mother (Bobier, 1989) 41 .29 .22 .41** .41** .12 .30 .39* .36* .35* .36* ----- ----- ----- ----- Primary Appraisal Emotions Scale Challenge Threat Harm Benefit (Jampol, 1988/1989) Problem Solving Inventory (Marcotte, 1995) Pseudoautonomy Scale In relation to father In relation to mother In relation to father In relation to mother 114c 185b .39** .31** .27** .22** 347 Test/Test Variable (Lopez, 1989) n PersonalEmotional Adj Academic Adj Social Adj .15 .14 .31** .26** -.07 -.02 .09 .09 .25** .17* .29** .24** ----- ----- Attachment Full Scale In relation to father In relation to mother In relation to father In relation to mother (Lapsley et al., 1989) (Lapsley, 1989) (Humfleet, 1987) 130 241 44 .24** -- .16* -- .33** -- .10 -- .28** .33* In relation to father 105 .27** .16 .40** .19* .34** .27** .15 .36** .12 .31** 123g In relation to mother (Hollmann, 1995) In relation to father In relation to mother (Kenny, 1992) 201b .24** .12 .06 .08 .34** .16* .20** .09 .25** .11 In relation to father In relation to mother In relation to father In relation to mother (Sherman, 1992) 179h .15* .18** .14* .16* .11 .17* .12 .18* .24** .23** .26** .24** .06 .13* .07 .15* ----- In relation to father In relation to mother In relation to father In relation to mother (Silverthorn, 1993) 30c -.08 .19 .13 .22 .25 -.01 .14 .13 .37* .11 .30* .30* .17 .01 .22 .18 .17 .12 .26* .28* In relation to father In relation to mother (Wang & Smith, 1993) 93b .22* .25* .38** .22* .41** .39** .41** .19 .44** .34** In relation to father In relation to mother (Schultheiss & Blustein, 1994) (Haemmerlie et al., 1994) (Reeker, 1994) 73b 66c 73b 66c 109 126 .01 .35** .13 .54** --- .06 .00 .17 .31** --- .13 .49** .25* .52** .27** .26** -----.23** ----.35** .25** (Marcotte, 1995) 229b .35** .26** .41** .28** -- 110 -- .16 .34** -- -- -- .24* .42** -- -- .28** .15* .32** .16* .31** .34** .22** .45** .14* .40** In relation to father 168i,j 66b In relation to mother (Clauss, 1995) In relation to father In relation to mother (Shilkret & Edwards, 1997) 155b 348 Test/Test Variable (Beyers & Goossens, 1998; see n Academic Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Social Adj Attachment Full Scale 1,014 .33** .11** .36** .16** .32** 159b -- -- .18* -- -- -- -- -- -- .29** -- -- -- -- .20* also Beyers & Goossens, 2002b, and Beyers, 2001) (Levin, 1996) In relation to father In relation to mother u,w (Rodriguez, 1994) 137 (Weinstock, 1995) 69 .42** .20 .47** .25* -- 8111 .23* -.07 .20* -- -- 8112 .19* .41** .44** -- -- 8113 .11 .05 .23* -- -- .13** .13** .32** .13** .23** .18** .13** .26** .16** .24** .09 .16** .22** .13* -- .09 .14* .15* .11 -- 99b .40** .34** .42** .41** .48** 92 c .30** .24** .43** .23* .38** 191 2 .34** .29** .43** .31** .43** 99 b .34** .33** .27** .40** .41** 92c .19* .08 .38** .04 .21* 1912 .26** .21** .32** .22** .32** 99 b .41** .37** .38** .44** .49** 92 c .28** .18* .45** .15 .33** 191 2 .33** .28** .42** .30** .41** .20** .12 .31** .15* -- (Rice, 1990/1991) In relation to father 506 In relation to mother (Bartels, 1995) In relation to father 272w In relation to mother (Dewein, 1994) In relation to father In relation to mother Both parents combined (Kline, 1992) In relation to father w In relation to mother (Ropar,1997) In relation to father 156 w In relation to mother w W W (n=176 ) (n=149 ) (n=188 ) (n=174 ) .33** .28** .36** .32** w w w -- w (n=176 ) (n=149 ) (n=188 ) (n=174 ) -- .32** .42** -- -- -- .32** .25** -- -- .31** .17* .18* .15 .29** .33** .03 .22** .03 .24** (Lapsley & Edgerton, 1999a) In relation to father In relation to mother (Silver, 1995) 118 349 Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment .16* .12 .17* .16* .21** .33** .27** .26** .27** .39** NA NA .18* NA .18* NA NA .21* NA .19* .23** .25** .27** .35** .35** 109 .34** .11 .27** .03 .31** 41 .30 .19 -.20 -.15 .12 -.12 In relation to father In relation to mother In relation to father In relation to mother Lopez, 1989) 114c .00 .10 .01 .11 ----- ----- ----- ----- In relation to father In relation to mother In relation to father In relation to mother (Lapsley et al., 1989) (Lapsley, 1989) (Humfleet, 1987) 130 Test/Test Variable In relation to father n 170w,20 In relation to mother Full Scale (Choi, 1999, 2000) In relation to father 114 In relation to mother Both parents combined (Haemmerlie, Montgomery, & Consolvo, 1993) In relation to mother (Morray & Shilkret, 2001) Emotional independence In relation to father In relation to mother (Bobier, 1989) 185b -.11 -.14 .10 -.01 -.14 -.04 -.08 .22** -.01 .00 -.06 .12 .17* .31** .11 .39** ----- ----- 241 44 .06 -- .03 -- .19** -- .03 -- .11 -.04 In relation to father In relation to mother (Kenny, 1992) 201b .04 .03 .03 .18** .06 .22** .01 .14* .00 .17* In relation to father In relation to mother In relation to father In relation to mother (Sherman, 1992) 179h .07 .03 -.01 .05 .01 -.01 -.08 -.03 .17* .07 .04 .07 .02 .01 -.04 -.04 In relation to father In relation to mother In relation to father In relation to mother (Silverthorn, 1993) 30c -.09 -.28 .31* .20 -.35 -.17 .28* .28* -.18 -.11 .26* .23 -.38* -.27 .26* .07 -.27 -.27 .33* .24 .12 -.18 .09 -.19* -.02 .05 -.07 .18 -.06 .05 In relation to father In relation to mother 123g 168i,j 66b 105 ----- 350 Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj -.01 .06 .11 .04 -- .16* .17* .22** .15* .23** .16* .17* .10 .20** .20** .24** .21** .17** .17** -- -- -- -- -.08 -- -- -- -- .13 .01 -.14 .22** -.17* -.06 -.07 .01 .01 -.03 -.04 8111 -.12 -.10 .15 -- -- 81 12 -.16 .05 .10 -- -- 81 13 -.18 -.09 .04 -- -- 272 w -.14* -.09 .01 -.09 -- .05 .04 .15* .10 -- -- .12 .06 -- -- -- .13 .18* -- -- ns ns ns ns -- .19* ns ns ns -- 109 .02 .25** .12 .19* .15 404 .31** .33** .26** .30** .38** Identity .34** .43** .42** .36** .49** Work orientation .37** .25** .22** .25** .35** Autonomy (composite score) .41** .41** .37** .36** .49** Test/Test Variable Academic Adj n Attachment Full Scale (Hollmann, 1995) (Marcotte, 1995) In relation to father 229b 155 b In relation to mother (Shilkret & Edwards, 1997) (Beyers & Goossens, 1998; see 1,014 -.02 also Beyers & Goossens, 2002b, and Beyers, 2001) In relation to father 137u,w In relation to mother (Rodriguez, 1994) In relation to father 118 In relation to mother (Silver, 1995) (Rice, 1990/1991) In relation to father In relation to mother (Dewein, 1994) In relation to father 156w In relation to mother (Lapsley & Edgerton, 1999a) In relation to father 114 In relation to mother (Haemmerlie, Montgomery, & Consolvo, 1993) In relation to mother (Morray & Shilkret, 2001) Psychosocial Maturity Scale Self-reliance (Yaffe, 1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) 351 PersonalEmotional Adj n Academic Adj Social Adj 100 .04 .15 .28** .23* .21* NSl NSm NSn .22* NS .23* NS NS .46* .28* .32** .28* NS NS NS .37** .35** .27* NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS .30** .36** .25* NS NS .67** .21* .28** .31** ------------- ------------- 209 -.26** -.06 -.22** -.24** -.24** 261 -.25** ns -.14** ns -- Activity-sleep -.15** ns -.24** ns -- Approach-Withdrawal .12* .45** ns .34** -- Flexibility-Rigidity .19** .36** .35** .35** -- ns .44** .20** .41** -- Rhythmicity-sleep .16** ns ns ns -- Rhythmicity-eating .20** .21** .28** .17** -- Rhythmicity-daily habits .16** ns .11* ns -- Distractibility .38** .14** .22** .11* -- Persistence .28** .19** .12* .16** -- 213o 83p 137 286 -.25** --.26**18 -.42** -.71** -.40** --.40** -.45** -.26** --.39** -.58** -.29** --.32** ----.44** 96 368 -.24** -.27** -.77** -.66** -.44** -.42** -.74** -.56** --.55** Test/Test Variable Attachment Full Scale Purpose-in-Life Test (Liter, 1987) Relationship Questionnaire Parental attachment To mother 33c 98b To father 33c 98b (Rice & Whaley, 1994) Religious Doubts Scale (Hunsberger et al., 1996) Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey Activity-general Mood (McAndrew-Miller, 1989) Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (Harris, 1991) (Sutton, 1996) (Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) (Pratt, 2001) (Beyers & Goossens, 2002a) Revised Ways of Coping Checklist 352 PersonalEmotional Adj n Academic Adj Social Adj 118 .30** .17* .28** .20* .30** Seeks support .20* -.01 .08 -.03 .16* Blames self -.28** .05 -.20* -.02 -.22** Wishful thinking -.11 -.14 -.02 -.10 -.11 Avoidance -.18* -.13 -.25** -.18* -.24** Blames others -.21* -.13 -.18* -.22** -.30** Counts blessings .26** .12 .32** .19* .34** Religiosity .06 .01 .05 .03 .04 105b,w 69 130 40716 404d -.35** .46** .32** .46** -.45** .48** .32** .44** -.48** .55** .38** .52** -.43** .39** .28** .40** .55** -.64** .42** .59** 273e 273d 137 142 31** .42** .41**18 .28** .48** .35** .43** -.27** .42** .49** .62** ---- .30** .37** ---- .46** .60** ---- 101 224e 224d 1011 432 .33** .32** .46** .48** .43** .50** .33** .43** .46** .44** .55** .51** .66** .38** .48** .33** .28** .38** .42** .41** .54** .46** .62** -.53** 66 446-451 .29** .52** .29** .54** .38** .56** .29** .52** .40** -- 90 .40** -- -- -- -- 75 95 100 96 .23* .25* .06 .36** -.21* .02 --.13 .26** -.23** .11 -.24** .08 .06 .20* -.00 --- .17 --.07 --- Test/Test Variable Problem-focused Attachment Full Scale (Silver, 1995) Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Gilkey, 1988; Gilkey et al., 1989) (Weinstock, 1995) (Hertel, 1996) (Yaffe, 1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) (M. D. Smith, 1994) (Sutton, 1996) Semester I, 1st half Semester II, 2nd half (Bettencourt et al., 1999) (Hickman et al., 2000) (Hunsberger et al., 1996; Hunsberger, 2000) (Napoli & Wortman, 1998) (Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) (Pappas, 2000) (Birnie-Lefcovitch, 1997) Salience Inventory (Chartrand et al., 1990b, 1992) Scholastic Aptitude Test Verbal & Quantitative (Maton, 1989b) (Chartrand et al., 1990b, 1992) (Liter, 1987) Verbal Quantitative 353 Test/Test Variable (Terrell, 1989) (Williams, 1996) Verbal Sept. SACQ admin. Nov. SACQ admin. Feb. SACQ admin. Quantitative Sept. SACQ admin. Nov. SACQ admin. Feb. SACQ admin. Verbal & Quantitative Sept. SACQ admin. Nov. SACQ admin. (Conti, 2000a,c) Verbal & Quantitative (Just, 1998) n Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment 180 -- -- -- -- .15* 174 82 38 .06 .13 -.37* -.15 -.11 -.20 .11 .26* -.06 -.16* -.11 -.23 -.03 .05 -.28 174 82 38 .00 .11 -.12 -.17* -.10 -.29 .05 .22* -.12 -.18* -.09 -.27 -.09 .04 -.24 174 82 .03 .12 -.17* -.11 .08 .25* --- --- 202 .16* .03 .26** .04 .16* 137 -.26**18 -- -- -- -- 205w .37** -- -- -- -- 334s,w -- .55** .32** -- -- 166 t,w -- .53** .31** -- -- 189 c,w -- .53** .27** -- -- 310 b,w -- .55** .34** -- -- -.17* -.24** -.43** -.27** -- Full Scale Self-Consciousness Scales19 (Sutton, 1996) Self-Efficacy for Broad Academic Milestones Scale (Lent et al., 1997) Self-Efficacy Scale Social self-efficacy (Rice et al., 1997) Self-Handicapping Scale (Montgomery & Zoellner, 100 1994) Self-Other Differentiation Scale (Morray & Shilkret, 2001) 109 .28** .31** .25** .24** .38** 320 .45** .49** .52** .47** .60** Creativity .25** .18** .30** .12* .28** Intellectual Ability .48** .21** .51** .26** .48** Scholastic Competence .68** .25** .50** .33** .59** Self-Perception Profile for College Students Global Self-Worth 354 Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Attachment Athletic Competence .10 .13* .08 .11 .14* Job Competence .43** .37** .35** .39** .48** Appearance .21** .25** .33** .22** .32** Romantic Relationships .26** .35** .23** .24** .35** Social Acceptance .25** .59** .33** .48** .48** Close Friendships .23** .55** .26** .45** .44** Parent Relationships .28** .24** .33** .27** .35** Morality .25** .22** .36** .17** .32** Finding Humor in One’s Life .22** .35** .28** .31** .35** Test/Test Variable n Full Scale (Hutto, 2001) Sense of Coherence Questionnaire (Posselt, 1992) 133w .45** .38** .57** .44** .60** Separation Anxiety Test Individuation score (Rice et al., 1990; Lapsley, 1989) 240 .19** .14* .23** .12* .22** 240 -.25** -.14* .12* .12* -.21** -.25** -.06 .17** .06 -.21** -.34** -.10 .11* -.07 -.15* -.25** -.09 .13* .10 -.24** -.34** -.13* .16** .06 -.24** 93b -.25* -.03 -.17 .26* -.43** .01 -.22* .08 -.37** .07 200b -.27** -.16* .23** NS -.39** -.35** .22** NS .18** -.40** NS .36** -.18** -.44** -.42** .37** .22** .32** -.58** -.34** NS -.17* -.49** -.35** .16* -.19** .24** -.36** NS .26** NS -.40** -.36** .26** .17* .23** ---------- Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence Separation anxiety subscale Engulfment anxiety subscale Self-centeredness subscale Nurturance seeking subscale Rejection expectancy subscale (Rice et al., 1990; Lapsley, 1989) Separation anxiety subscale Healthy separation subscale (Wang & Smith, 1993) Separation anxiety subscale Engulfment anxiety subscale Self-centeredness subscale Nurturance seeking subscale Rejection expectancy subscale Dependency denial subscale Peer enmeshment subscale Teacher enmeshment subscale Healthy separation subscale (Cooler, 1995) Social Avoidance and Distress Scale 355 Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Test/Test Variable n Academic Adj (Kim et al., 1992) NA -.33** -.61** -.40** -- -.54** Attachment Full Scale Social Competence Scale Interpersonal assertiveness prematriculation 451-453 .22** .33** .17** .26** .32** postmatriculation 451-453 .23** .41** .23** .31** .38** prematriculation 441-443 .10* .22** .07 .12** .17** postmatricukation 435-437 .11* .32** .09* .22** .24** prematriculation 449-451 .15** .25** .13** .19** .23** postmatriculation 448-450 .19** .44** .22** .36** .38** prematriculation 440-442 .20** .33** .14** .23** .29** postmatriculation 433-435 .20** .48** .21** .36** .40** -- .22* -- -- -- Dating skills Social skills with same-sex others Full scale score (Birnie-Lefcovitch, 1997) Social Propensity Scale (Sullivan, 1991) NA Student Developmental Task & Lifestyle Inventory Establishing and clarifying purpose task 73b 66c -.11 .26* .08 .22 -.16 .04 --- --- Developing mature interpersonal relationships task 73b 66c .26* .28* .30* .15 .36** .39** --- --- Academic autonomy (Schultheiss & Blustein, 1994) 73b 66c .71** .59** .26* .16 .48** .42** --- --- Superiority Scale Realistic self-appraisal (Robbins & Schwitzer, 1988) (Dewein, 1994) 88 272w .18* -.016 -.23* -.12*6 .16 -.046 -.16 -.14*6 --- 1625 -- -- -- -- .45** 108 .35** .25** NA .26** Tennessee Self-Concept Scale Total self-concept score (Caplan, 1996/1997) Self-satisfaction student-athletes NA 356 Academic Adj Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj 65 .37** .23 .26* .21 .32* 108 .21* .11 NA .21* NA 65 .34** .34** .53** .37** .47** 108 .36** .35** NA .32** NA 65 .57** .25* .33** .33** .49** 108 .49** .39** NA .44** NA 65 .48** .31* .45** .38** .51** 108 .30** .23* NA .30** NA .18 .07 .17 .14 .14 .32** .44** NA .40** NA 65 .23 .48** .27* .41** .39** 108 .13 .21* NA .22* NA 65 .45** .41** .45** .42** .55** 108 .30** .33** NA .34** NA 65 .41** .34** .51** .48** .51** 108 .47** .33** NA .38** NA 65 .43** .39** .36** .43** .48** 11b -.50 -.63* -.30 -.65* -.53* Guilt .08 -.53* -.04 -.61* -.14 Detachment -.23 -.11 -.04 .19 -.20 Externalization -.23 -.39 -.54* -.09 -.45 Alpha pride .56* -.12 .42 -.34 .49 Beta pride .18 -.06 .38 -.22 .33 Test/Test Variable non-athletes n Attachment Full Scale Physical self student-athletes non-athletes Moral-ethical self student-athletes non-athletes Personal self student-athletes non-athletes Family self student-athletes non-athletes 65 Social self student-athletes non-athletes 108 Behavior student-athletes non-athletes Identity student-athletes non-athletes Total P Score student-athletes non-athletes (Foster, 1997) Test of Self-Conscious Affect (TOSCA) Shame (Shilkret & Nigrosh, 1995) Texas Social Behavior Inventory 357 Social Adj PersonalEmotional Adj Test/Test Variable n Academic Adj (Kenny, 1995) 199 .26** .41** .28** .35** -- late September 159 .31** .12 .18** -- -- late November 86 .16 -.08 .01 -- -- late September 159 .02 -.09 -.19** -- -- late November 86 .01 -.06 -.17 -- -- -.38** -.30** -.53** -.31** -.49** -.28** -.34** -.51** -.30** -.46** Attachment Full Scale Work Preference Inventory College Student Version Intrinsic motivation Extrinsic motivation (Conti, 2000a) Worry Domains Questionnaire Relationships Self-Confidence 152 (Vivona, 2000b) a See also Table 22, pp. 50-51, of the SACQ manual (Baker & Siryk, 1989). All females. c All males. d Personality measure administered postmatriculation. e Personality measure administered prematricualtion. f African-Americans, Asians and Latinos. g Juniors and Seniors. h 6th week of semester 1. i 10th week of semester 1. j Same subjects as in 6th week testing. k Revision of the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment. l Betas for 3rd week of spring semester. m Betas for 9th week of spring semester. n Betas for 15th week of spring semester. o For data collected in senior year. p For Loneliness Scale administered freshman year vs. SACQ administered senior year. q Reverse of customary locus of control scoring, high scores indicate greater internality. r Includes the n of 88 cited next. S White students. t African-American students. u Latino students. v Asian-American students. w Mixed college year levels. x All student-athletes. y All physically disabled. z East Asian international students. 1 West European international students. 2 Total sample. b 358 3 Sample includes traditional and nontraditional age undergraduates and graduate students. Mixed foreign first-year graduate students. 5 Early entrant freshmen, who skipped the junior and/or senior years of high school, age 14-17. 6 Correlation valences changed because of reversal of customary rating scale. 7 SACQ administered 3rd week of October. 8 SACQ administered 3rd week of November. 9 Second-generation college attenders. 10 First-generation college attenders. 11 Independent variable and SACQ both administered in freshman year. 12 Independent variable and SACQ both administered in junior year. 13 Independent variable administered in freshman year and SACQ in junior year. 14 Puerto Rican students born and raised in Puerto Rico attending a Puerto Rican college. 15 Puerto Rican students born and raised in mainland U.S.A. attending two New Jersey colleges. 16 Independent variable administered early in first semester, SACQ around middle of second semester. 17 Independent variable and SACQ both administered middle of second semester. 18 Used truncated, 10-item version of the Academic Adjustment subscale. 19 Shortened form. 20 Korean-American students. 21 Mexican-American students. 22 Arab-American students. 4 *p<.05; **p<.01. 359 Appendix D Correlations between SACQ Scores and Measures of Mental and Physical Health and Adjustmenta Test/Test Variable n Academic Adjustment Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale Adjustment Questionnaire (Beyers & Goossens, 2002a) 368 .51** .83** (Oliver et al., 1998; Reed, 1994) 248h -- -- (Vivona, 2000b) 152 -.30** NAb 103c 220 103 NA 93d 248h 202d 109e 311g 407n 407o .67** .85** .86** -.67** -- -- -.17* -.52** -.17* -.39** -.64** -.48** --.30** -.38** -.51** --.53** -.44** -.49** -.35** -.53** ----.37** -.53** -.37** --.55** -.38** -.48** -.39** -.46** ----.63** -.68** -.60** -.64** -.70** -.33** -.56** -.51** -.73** ----.26** --.32** --.50** -.38** -.44** -.38** -.45** ---.72** -.51** -.63** -.62** --.70** -.46** -.60** -.52** -.71** 152 130 -.43** -.41** -.39** -.33** -.63** -.57** -.40** -.31** -.59** -.51** 228 -.39** -.29** -.58** -.32** -- 75b 75c -.44** -.16 -.50** -.31** -.60** -.28** -.50** -.24* -.61** -.29* 11d -.48 .07 -.59* -.41 -.36 -.42 -.37 -.39 -.20 -.08 -.64* -.46 -.51 -.31 -.49 -.12 -.28 -.69** -.59* -.65* -.71** -.26 -,19 .01 -.52* -.44 -.46 .08 -.53* -.21 -.43 -.67* -.47 -.58* -.48 Beck Anxiety Inventory Beck Depression Inventory (Cooley & Carden, 1992) (Jampol, 1988/1989) (Merryman & Zelezny, 1993) (Kim et al., 1992) (Wang & Smith, 1993) (Oliver et al., 1998; Reed, 1994) (Dodgen-Magee, 1992) (Yaffe, 1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) (Vivona, 2000b) (Montgomery & Haemmerlie, 2001) Bell Global Psychopathology Scale Global psychopathology severity (Zamostny et al., 1993) Brief Symptom Inventory Depression subscale (Maton & Weisman, 1989; Maton, 1989b) Depression subscale Somatization subscale Obsessive-compulsive subscale Interpersonal sensitivity subscale Anxiety subscale Hostility subscale Phobic anxiety subscale 360 Test/Test Variable Academic Adjustment n Paranoid ideation subscale Psychoticism subscale Positive symptom distress subscale Positive symptom total Global Severity Index (Shilkret & Nigrosh, 1995) Positive symptom distress subscale Positive symptom tatal Global Severity Index (Leong, 1999) Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale -.21 -.62* -.34 -.55* -.50 -.48 -.28 -.35 -.49 -.45 -.58* -.55* -.72** -.80** -.82** -.34 -.18 -.06 -.32 -.22 -.53* -.59* -.58* -.75** -.72** 161 -.38** -.22** -.34** -.37** -.37** -.47** -.43** -.38** -.55** -.20** -.19** -.25** -.42** -.35** -.47** 273c -.23** -.33** -.54** -.27** -.43** b -.42** -.53** -.78** -.48** -.70** -.48** -- -- -- -- Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (M. D. Smith, 1994) 273 l (Sutton, 1996) 137 (Hunsberger et al., 1996; 222c -.28** -.35** -.60** -.30** -.48** Hunsberger, 2000) 222 b -.44** -.55** -.81** -.52** -.73** (Shibazaki, 1999) 136p -.40** -.54** -.75** -.46** -- 96 -.35** -.48** -.76** -.42** -- 368 -.34** -.59** -.73** -.55** -.68** 241 .72** .58** .41** .32** .57** .33** .42** .39** .69** .53** .56** .48** .60** .50** .43** .38** .15* .33** .32** .29** .38** .31** .48** .54** .37** .40** .18** .27** .34** .31** .55** .39** .57** .54** .44** .26** .42** .36** .34** .41** -.09** -.22** -.03 -.20** -.15** (Pratt, 2001) (Beyers & Goossens, 2002a) College Inventory of Academic Adjustment Global score Curricular adjustment Maturity of goals and level of aspiration Personal efficiency Study skills Mental health Personal relations (Lapsley, 1989; Rice et al., 1990) College Life Task Assessment Instrument Forming an identity (sense of integrity of the self, self-esteem) Developing autonomy (closeness of continued involvement with home, family, and high school friends) 623 361 Test/Test Variable Academic Adjustment Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment .07 .31** .18** .10** -.11** .29** .05 .06 -.02 -.01 -.02 .11** .25** -.10** -.15** .25** .08* .08* .00 -.03 .16** .19** .18** .01 -.09** 157 -- -- -- -- -.46** 248h -- -- -.26** -- -- 136 -.13 -.26** -.49** -.20* -- 100 -.06 .07 -.17* .02 -.03 229d -.14* -.09 -.14* -.15* -- 228 -.36** -.34** -.45** -.33** -.48** 201d -.09 -.27** -.18* -.50** -.28** -.12 -.27** -.20** -.57** -.32** -.28** -.36** -.28** -.61** -.32** -.11 -.29** -.15* -.60** -.38** -.18* -.36** -.25** -.71** -.40** 248h --- --- -.42** -.58** --- --- 105 -- -- -- -- -.29** 116f -.30** -.34** -.44** -.19 -- 124d -.08 -.08 -.37** -.07 -- n Establishing friendships Academic achievement Maintaining one's physical self Determining future goals Managing time (Brower, 1990b) Full Scale College Maladjustment Scale (Mt) (Schriver, 1996) Core Alcohol and Drug Survey Negative consequences of alcohol use (Oliver et al., 1998) Costello-Comrey Anxiety Scale (Shibazaki, 1999) Death Anxiety Scale (Liter, 1987) Dissociative Experiences Scale (Marcotte, 1995) Dysfunctional Attitudes ScaleForm A (Walker, 1996) Eating Disorder Inventory Drive for thinness Bulimia Body dissatisfaction Ineffectiveness Maturity fears (Kenny, 1992) Eating problems (composite score) Eating problems-related traits (composite score) (Oliver et al., 1998; Reed, 1994) Health Checklist (Gilkey et al., 1989) Hopkins Symptom Checklist Total score (Kenny, 1994) Somatic subscale 362 Test/Test Variable Academic Adjustment n Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale Obsessive-compulsive subscale -.27** -.07 -.41** -.11 -- Depression subscale -.32** -.35** -.65** -.41** -- Interpersonal sensitivity subscale -.24** -.40** -.48** -.40** -- Anxiety subscale -.19* -.12 -.53** -.10 -- Total score -.29** -.26** -.59** -.28** -- 334i,h -- -.37** -.75** -- -- 166j,h -- -.27** -.72** -- -- 189e,h -- -.36** -.69** -- -- 310d,h -- -.31** -.75** -- -- 136p -.36** -.39** -.75** -.33** -- 84 ---------- ---------.46** ---------.59** ---------.39** -.31** -.55** -.28** -.34** -.40** -.48** -.51** -.16 -- 198 -.64** -.47** -.68** -.45** -- 273c -.22** -.27** -.51** -.23** -.39** 273 b -.53** -.49** -.80** -.47** -.73** 406 n -.38** -.39** -.55** -.33** -.54** 407 o -.57** -.42** -.75** -.35** -.70** (Hunsberger et al., 1996; 224c -.26** -.28** -.55** -.25** -.42** Hunsberger, 2000) 224b -.55** -.50** -.81** -.50** -.75** 96 -.50** -.40** -.56** -.38** -- 220 -- -- -- -- -.69** (Kenny, 1995) Kandel Depression Scale (Rice et al., 1997) Mental Health Inventory-5 (Shibazaki, 1999) Mini-Mult Hypochondriasis Depression Hysteria Psychopathic deviate Paranoia Psychasthenia Schizophrenia Hypomania Total pathology score (Humfleet & Ribordy, 1990) Patient’s (ADHD) Behavior Checklist Panori (1997) Perceived Stress Scale (M. D. Smith, 1994) (Yaffe, 1997; see also Wintre & Yaffe, 2000) (Pratt, 2001) State-Trait Anxiety Inventory State anxiety (Jampol, 1988/1989) 363 Test/Test Variable (Wang & Smith, 1993) (Kline, 1992) n Academic Adjustment Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment 93d 99d 92e 191g -.40** -.32** -.31** -.32** -.28** -.38** -.40** -.39** -.66** -.63** -.55** -.59** -.28** -.36** -.25** -.30** -.55** -.51** -.48** -.50** 114e 185d 93d -.57** -.47** -.40** ---.30** ---.64** ---.29** ---.56** 137l -.43** -- -- -- -- 248h -- -- -.54** -- -- -- -- -.47** -- -- NA -.33** -.50** -.67** -- -.55** 320 -.29** -.43** -.34** -.44** -.32** -.27** -.13* -.31** -.32** -.27** -.36** -.42** -.44** -.37** -.25** -.33** -.45** -.32** -.54** -.61** -.54** -.67** -.59** -.50** -.33** -.51** -.55** -.25** -.39** -.37** -.43** -.36** -.25** -.32** -.44** -.35** -.43** -.56** -.51** -.61** -.51** -.39** -.33** -.52** -.47** Full Scale Trait anxiety (Lopez, 1989) (Wang & Smith, 1993) Strain Questionnairem Physical symptoms (Sutton, 1996) Stress Audit Emotional stress Physical symptoms (Oliver et al., 1998; Reed, 1994) Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90-R) Global severity index (Kim et al., 1992) Somatization Obsessive-Compulsive Interpersonal Sensitivity Depression Anxiety Hostility Phobic Anxiety Paranoid Ideation Psychoticism (Hutto, 2001) a See also Table 23 on p. 53 of the SACQ manual (Baker & Siryk, 1989). Independent variable measure administered postmatriculation. c Independent variable measure administered prematriculation. d All females. e All males. f African-Americans, Asians, and Latinos (Independent variable measure administered first semester, SACQ second semester). g Male and female samples combined. h Mixed college year levels. i White students. j African-American students. k Mixed foreign first year graduate students. b 364 l Used truncated, 10-item version of the Academic Adjustment subscale. Shortened form. N Independent variable administered in first week of first semester. o Independent variable administered middle of second semester. p Mexican-American students. M *p<.05; **p<.01. 365 Appendix E Correlations between SACQ Scores and Measures of Environment-Related Experiencea Academi c Adjustm ent Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale -.08 -.07 -.16 -.02 -.13 -.22 -.29* -.37** -.06 -.35** -.05 .01 .04 .05 .01 Unconflicted network size -.01 .06 .05 .11 .05 Conflicted network size -.10 -.14 -.04 -.16 -.13 Support satisfaction -.08 .23* .07 .03 .08 Support need -.23* -.22* -.35** -.07 -.33** 105e,i .21* .32** .30** .30** .34** 56 f,i .26* .14 .40** .11 .29* 66 g,i .28* .23* .19 .29** .29** 71 h,i .18 .16 .20* .13 .21* 298 i,k .20** .26** .22** .28** Test/Test Variable n Adolescent Perceived Events Scale Negative major life events 56 Daily hassles (Brooks & DuBois, 1995) Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule Network size 56 (Brooks & DuBois, 1995) Support satisfaction (Zea et al., 1995; Zea, 1997) .24** Emotional support satisfaction 77g,i NS NS NS .25* Instrumental support satisfaction .24* NS NS NS .18 Family/kin support satisfaction NS -- -- -- .22* Friend/other support satisfaction -- -- -- -- .22* Overall social support satisfaction -- -- -- -- .28** (Jarama Alvan et al., 1996) -- -.23** -.60** -.28** -.50** -- -- -- -.52** Brief Coll. Student Hassles Scale (M. D. Smith, 1994) 273 (Hunsberger et al., 1996) 222 .44** -- Brief Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (adapted to identify 366 Test/Test Variable n Academi c Adjustm ent Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale alcohol abuse in student’s family) (Buelow, 1990) 275 -- -- -- -- -.20** 229c -.07 -.01 -.14* -.05 -- As a child with an adult -.04 .08 .01 .05 -- As a nonconsenting adolescent -.12 -.13* -.08 -.14* -- (n=172i) (n=140i) (n=182i) (n=166i) Hero role .37** .14* .21** .16* -- Scapegoat role -.28** -.07 -.23** -.20** -- Lost Child role -.04 -.11 -.19** -.19** -- Mascot role .00 .13 -.03 .04 -- -- -- -.51**q -- -- -- r -- -- q -- -- r -- Childhood Sex’l Experiences Ques. As a child with a child (Marcotte, 1995) Children’s Roles Inventory (Ropar, 1997) Coll. Studt. Life Events Sched. 3rd week of October 109i -rd 3 week of November -- (Gallant, 1994) Early Resources Checklist Interpersonal relationships Achievement Play (Zamostny et al., 1993) Early Trauma Checklist Loss (interpersonal) Chaos (familial/personal/ economic disruptions) Parental dysfunction Abuse (Zamostny et al., 1993) -- -.27** -.35** -- -- -.42** -- .09 .13* .13* .25** .17** .11 .07 .05 .12 .13* .11 .08 -.06 -.02 -.14** -.05 -.18** -.14** -.12 .01 --- -.12 .19** -.13* -.29** -.29** -.24** -.09 -.20** --- .32** .37** .27** .41** .31** .34** .21** .38** .22** .19** .22** .26** -228 228 --- FACES-II Family cohesion 228 .30** Family adaptability (Walker, 1996) FACES-III Family cohesion .29** 241 367 Academi c Adjustm ent Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale .22** .11* .11* .02 .05 .09 .16* .06 .15 .05 --- --- .07 .21* .08 .24** .18* Reframing .24** .32** .32** .34** .37** Seeking Spiritual Support .03 .12 .08 .13 .09 Acquiring Help from Community .01 .19* .01 .19* .13 Passive Appraisal .30** .05 .17* .11 .21* Total score .17* .28** .17* .31** .29** 241 .24** .17** -.16** .12* .23** .05 .09 22** .28** .26** -.11* .10 .21** .26** .15* .26** .26** .15* -.30** .16** .14* .10 .11 .20** .23** .20** -.12* .10 .18** .24** .13* .23** .32** .23** -.22** .14* .24** .17** .14* .31** 162p ---- ---- ---- ---- .21** -.35** .09 140 .30** .26** -28** .15 -.10 .15 .28** .02 .22** .22** -.17* .06 -.04 .06 .29** .05 .27** .22** -.34** .09 -.16 -.02 .25** .23** -.22** .10 -.05 .03 .32** .28** -.31** .13 -.10 .09 Test/Test Variable Family adaptability (Rice et al., 1990; Lapsley, 1989) Family cohesion Family adaptability (Lapsley & Edgerton, 1999b) n 156i --- Family Crisis Oriented Personal Evaluation Scale (F-COPES) Acquiring Social Support 140 Resources (Hopkins, 1998; see also Feenstra et al, 2001) Family Environment Scale Family cohesion Expressiveness Conflict Independence Intellectual-cultural Activity-recreation Moral-religious Organization (Rice et al., 1990; Lapsley, 1989) Family cohesion Conflict Expressiveness (Caplan, 1996/1997) Family cohesion Expressiveness Conflict Independence Achievement Intellectual-cultural Activity-recreation 368 Test/Test Variable n Moral-religious Organization Control (Hopkins, 1998; see also Feenstra et al., 2001) Academi c Adjustm ent Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale .25** -.13 .15 -.16 .24** -.09 .13 -.23** .26** .09 .15 -.12 .33** .01 .21* -.20* -- -- -- -- .29** .41** .33** .40** .47** .30** .28** .31** .34** .36** .38** .35** .30** .30** .25** .29** .28** .33** .32** .27** .38** .38** .37** .37** .32** .35** .29** .33** .37** .33** .27** .36** .36** .37** .34** .35** .30** .32** .35** .34** .37** .37** .39** .43** .40** .25** .17 .29** .30** .11 -.01 .14 .10 .19** .06 .22** .21** Family Functioning Scales Total score 275 (Buelow, 1990) Family of Origin Scale (fostering autonomy and intimacy) (Hollmann & Metzler, 1994) Autonomy Clarity of Expression Personal Responsibility Respect Other Family Members Openness to Other Members Accept Separation and Loss Intimacy Expressive of Feelings Warm Home Atmosphere Handle Conflicts Promote Sensitivity Trust in Human Nature (Hutto, 1998) 105 .34** 320i .29** .32** .27** .25** .19** .26** .27** .28** .30** .29** Family Ritual Questionnaire Family ritualization (Kline, 1992) 99c 92 b 191 k .26** .07 369 Test/Test Variable n Academi c Adjustm ent Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale .19** Family Structure Survey Parent-child role reversal Marital conflict Fear of separation Parent-child overinvolvement (Grella, 1989) 45 -.07 -.29 -.23 .18 -.08 -.31* -.19 -.06 -.19 -.26 -.32* -.15 -.12 -.36* -.28 -.15 -.13 -.37* -.30* -.04 Marital conflict (Lopez, 1989) 114b 185c -.14 -.08 --- --- --- --- Parent-child role reversald Marital conflict Fear of separation Parent-child overinvolvement (Kenny, 1995) 124 -.03 -.08 -.17* -.06 -.03 -.07 -.10 -.03 -.05 -.14* -.12 -.04 -.02 -.05 -.07 -.04 ----- 93c .34** .17 .23* .29** .27** .21* .21* .28** .36** .28** 198 .28** .26** .25** .08 .28** ------ .37** .28** .28** .17** .36** ------ ------ Total (Kim et al., 1992) NA .24** .40** .26** -- .39** Self-esteem support Appraisal support Belonging support Tangible support Total (Kambach, 1994) 53 .28* .30* .13 .22 .30* .41** .45** .64** .51** .67** .38** .18 .29* .29* .35** .44** .38** .42** .48** .56** .46** .41** .44** .46** .58** 77g,i -- -- -- -- -.40** Father-Daughter Relationship Inventory Affectional interaction subscale Time spent actively involved (Wang & Smith, 1993) Interpersonal Support Evaluation List Self-esteem support Appraisal support Belonging support Tangible support Total (Robbins et al., 1993) Life Experiences Survey Overall stress (Jarama Alvan et al., 1996) 370 Test/Test Variable n Academi c Adjustm ent Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale -.09** -.21** -.08* -- -.32** -.31** -.30** -- Negative impact rating life events in general 1011v life events in college (Napoli & Wortman, 1998) Life Stress (Maton & Weisman, 1989) .11** .34** -.27* 75 -- -- -- -- .24* .23* .26* .36** .03 .06 -.03 .07 .02 .25* .22* .31** .10 .29** .18* .09 .14* .09 .15* .13 .08 .15 .12 .16 .38** .45** .33** .46** .20** .28** .21** .29** .33** .14 .29** .30** .47** .35** .30** .34** .36** .25** .29** .32** .15 .22* .20* .26** .34** .36** .28* .32** .24** .28** .24** .28** .20* .07 .18* .12 .39** .09 .48** .38** .29** .08 .33** .25** .19* .23* .14 .18* .23* .14 .36** .25* .21** .19** .25** .21** .31** .10 .28** .23* McMaster Family Assessment Device General Functioning score 66 .34** (Pappas, 2000) Multidimensional Support Scale From family & close friends frequency 96c 65 b 161 satisfaction with frequency k 96 c 65 b 161 k From peers frequency 96c 65b 161k satisfaction with frequency 96 c 65 b 161 k 96c 65 k 96 c 65 b 161 Total frequency score .16 .04 .13* .17* .12 b 161 satisfaction with frequency .21** .10 From authority figures frequency .33** k 96c .11 .26* .16* .18* .14 371 Test/Test Variable n 65b 161 Total satisfaction with freq. score (Polewchak, 1999) k Academi c Adjustm ent Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale .15* .49** .27* .42** .40** .16 .36** .20** .36** .32** 96 c .13 .21* .29** .22* .28** 65 b .17* .41** .41** .45** .45** 161 k .23* .31** .34** .33** .36** .25* .22** Parental Authority Questionnaire Mother authoritativeness Father authoritativeness Mother authoritarianism Father authoritarianism Mother permissiveness Father permissiveness (Wintre & Sugar, 2000; see also 255c .16* .19** .19** .06 -- 102 b .16 .24** .21* .21* -- 255 c .12* .19** .13* .08 -- 102 b .11 .38** .27** .28** -- 255 c -.14* -.09 -.23** .01 -- 102 b -.04 -.17* -.10 -.06 -- 255 c -.12* -.08 -.15** .04 -- 102 b -.10 -.21* -.24** -.10 -- 255c .02 .08 .03 -.06 -- 102b .00 -.11 -.04 -.05 -- 255 c .02 .09 .05 -.06 -- 102 b .02 .03 .03 .05 -- .21** .18** .09* .20** Sugar, 1997) Mother authoritativeness Father authoritativeness Mother authoritarianism Father authoritarianism 398 379 .14** .23** .17** .14** .20** 398 .12* -.12* -.15** .00 -.14** 378 -.11* -.13** -.18** -.02 -.16** .04 .01 -.03 .03 .08 .06 -.01 .06 .26* .21* .29** .35** .07 -.05 -.02 -.03 Mother permissiveness 398 Father permissiveness 377 (Yaffe, 1997; see also Wintre & .13** .04 .06 Yaffe, 2000) Most influential parent Authoritativeness Authoritarianism 66 372 Test/Test Variable n Academi c Adjustm ent .35** Permissiveness Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale -.11 -.03 -.08 -.12 -.10 (Pappas, 2000) -.14 Parental Authority Questionnaire (modified) Mother authoritativeness 141c .31** Father authoritativeness .19* Mother permissive-neglectfulness .27** Father permissive-neglectfulness Mother authoritarianism .24** Father authoritarianism .27** .35** .23** .35** .20* .26** .16 .24* -.29** -.25** -.26** -.32** -.17* -.22** -.15 -.23** -.16 -.32** -.11 -.22** -.17* -.29** -.14 -.21** Mother permissive-indulgence -.18* .01 .15 -.03 .03 Father permissive-indulgence -.12 .01 .10 .00 .01 (Shilkret & Vecchiotti, 1995) .00 .15* .17* .09 .18* .03 .08 .00 .06 -.04 Mother authoritativeness 156c Father authoritativeness Mother permissive-neglectfulness .12 -.10 -.21** -.05 -.16* Father permissive-neglectfulness .05 -.11 -.14* -.04 -.14* Mother authoritarianism -.11 -.09 -.15* -.04 -.13* Father authoritarianism -.11 -.01 -.11 .01 -.04 Mother permissive-indulgence -.12 .11 .04 .08 .07 Father permissive-indulgence -.02 -.04 .00 .00 -.06 (Shilkret & Edwards, 1997) .01 .18** .26** .16** .28** Father authoritativeness .10* .30** .08 .26** Mother permissive-neglectfulness -.17** -.26** -.19** -.29** -.12* -.25** -.12* -.25** -.08 -.17** .08 -.16** -.03 -.23** -.03 -.17** .03 .03 .00 .05 -.02 .09 -.06 .06 -.10 Mother authoritativeness Father permissive-neglectfulness 292 .27** Mother authoritarianism Father authoritarianism Mother permissive-indulgence Father permissive-indulgence (Shilkret, 2000) .29** .32** .29** 373 Test/Test Variable n Academi c Adjustm ent Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale .15** .20** .07 .11* Parental Bonding Instrument Mother caringness 142i -- -- -- .29** -- Father caringness -- -- -- .09 -- Mother protectiveness -- -- -- -.11 -- Father protectiveness -- -- -- -.03 -- -- .28** .17** -- -- -- .22** .14** -- -- -- .30** .21** -- -- -- .40** .29** -- -- -- .28** .13* -- -- -- .33** .26** -- -- -- .29** .20** -- -- -- .27** .18** -- -- -- -.16** -.19** -- -- -- -.14** -.25** -- -- -- -.25** -.14* -- -- -- -.18* -.29** -- -- -- -.12* -.18** -- -- -- -.10 -.26** -- -- -- -.22** -.15** -- -- -- -.17** -.22** -- -- -- .07 -.03 -- -- Father caringness -- .12 .19* -- -- Mother protectiveness -- -.08 .10 -- -- Father protectiveness -- -.18* -.30** -- -- (Lopez, 1997) Mother caringness 334e Father caringness Mother caringness 166 f 189 b Father caringness Mother caringness Father caringness Mother caringness 310c Father caringness Mother protectiveness 334 e 166 f 189 b 310 c Father protectiveness Mother protectiveness Father protectiveness Mother protectiveness Father protectiveness Mother protectiveness Father protectiveness (Rice et al., 1997) Mother caringness 156i 374 Test/Test Variable n Academi c Adjustm ent Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale (Lapsley & Edgerton, 1999b) Mother caringness 90 .29** Father caringness .27** .29** .26** .33** .00 .13 -.03 .06 Mother protectiveness .06 -.10 -.19* .02 -.13 Father protectiveness -.12 .13 -.10 .13 .01 (Bailey & Shilkret, 2000b) -.06 .32** .38** .30** -- -.24** -.35** -.25** -- Parents combined caringness 261 Parents combined protectiveness .30** (McAndrew-Miller, 1989) .22** Parental Physical Maltreatment Scale (Marcotte, 1995) 229c .17** -.17** -.16** -.26** -- 229c .21** -.12* -.25** -.15* -- --- .26** .23** --- --- Parental Psychological Maltreatment Scale (Marcotte, 1995) Parent-Child Relations Questionnaire II The love-reject factor score (Mendelson, 1987/1988) Perceived Parental Attitude Scale Encouragement of independence by father (Wang & Smith, 1993) 114b 118c --- 93c .20 .28** .24** .28** .30** 68 506 202 451-453 449-451 -.18** .35** .18** .31** .24* .19** .28** .14** .21** .16 .16** .24** .16** .23** -.18** .27** .09* .18** -.23** .37** .18** .30** Perceived Social Support from Family (Maton, 1989a) (Bartels, 1995) (Just, 1998) prematriculation postmatriculation (Birnie-Lefcovitch, 1997) 375 Test/Test Variable n Academi c Adjustm ent Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale Perceived Social Support from Friends (Maton & Weisman, 1989; Maton, 1989b) (Frazier & Cook, 1993) (Bartels, 1995) Friends in college Friends not in college (Hertel, 1996) (Just, 1998) (McAndrew-Miller, 1989) (Shibazaki, 1999) 75 85 506 105s 25t 130k 105s 25t 130k 202 261 136w .37** -.24** .38** .08 .33** .18* .02 .14 .55** -.41** .31** .19* .21** .39** -.31** .48** -.35** .68** .56** .66** .15 -.25 .04 .36** .51** .26** .31** .22 .30** .07 -.16 .02 .19** .15** .27** .56** .51** .56** .00 -.26 -.07 .20** .40** .09 .56** .29 .51** .19* -.22 .09 .28** NA -- .15** ns .20* Physical Maltreatment by Others Scale (Marcotte, 1995) 229c -.14* -.09 -.09 -.14* -- 244i -- -- -- -- -.32** 136w -.01 -.10 -.24** -.08 -- .57** .54** .61** .67** Psychological Distress Inventory Stress level (Bartels, 1990) Racism and Life Experiences Scale-Brief Version (Shibazaki, 1999) Residence Hall Climate Inventory Personal support subscale Conflict subscale Group cohesiveness subscale Order subscale 44b .41** 77 c .40** .20* .45** .30** 44 b .07 -.33* -.10 -.24 -.13 77 c -.03 -.45** -.35** -.53** -.48** 44 b .35* .07 .35* .27* 77 c .26* .19* .32** .30** 44 b -.18 .08 .19 .25 .31** .16 376 Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale -.13 -.02 -.07 -.04 .16 -.25 -.22 -.22 -- Scapegoat role -.09 -.24 -.17 -.13 -- Sick or Lost Child role .19 -.20 -.04 -.13 -- Mascot role -.24 .17 -.10 -.04 -- -.10 -.09 -.38** -.22* -- Scapegoat role -.21 -.29** -.29** -.14 -- Sick or Lost Child role .17 -.31** -.29** -.20 -- Mascot role -.02 .13 .00 .03 -- -.22* .03 -.32** .01 -- .29** -.17* -.35** -.10 -- -.21* -.40** -.05 -- .34** .17* -.13 .01 -- -- -- -.32** Test/Test Variable (Barthelemy & Fine, 1995) n 77c Academi c Adjustm ent .23* .36** .03 Role Relationship Inventory Alcohol/drug-abusing families Hero role 57 Disrupted, non-drug-abusing families Hero role 81 Normal families Hero role 137 Scapegoat role Sick or Lost Child role Mascot role Overall score, total sample 275 -.16 (Buelow, 1990) -- -- Self-Report Family InventoryVersion II Health/competence subscale 110 -- -.19* -.21* -- -- -- -.08 -.21* -- -- .31** -.33** -.38** -.31** -.41** -.19** -.31** -.20** -.30** .24** -.30** -.37** -.30** -.40** -.19** -.18** -.18** -.19** Cohesion subscale (Clauss, 1995) Health/Competence subscale Cohesion subscale Conflict subscale Leadership subscale 320i - 377 Test/Test Variable n Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment -.32** -.31** -.28** -.34** .09 .35** -.22* .27** -.02 .08 -.04 .31** .11 -.05 .08 .11 .11 .04 -.04 -.24* .16 -.10 -.02 -.06 .25* -.01 .01 -.09 -.04 .09 .17 .28** -.22* .26** -.04 .07 .00 .39** .11 -.15 .14 .17 .11 .14 .20 -.27** .26** -.06 .07 -.04 .38** .08 -.07 .07 .10 .16 -.13* -.05 -.14* -.12 -.05 -.16* -.06 -.04 -.09 -.03 -.05 -.08 -.08 -.04 .00 -.02 .07 .08 .14* .17* .16* .27** -.03 .43** .29** .28** .14* .29** .27** .32** -.08 .26** .37** .66** -.02 .05 .09 .11 .24** -.22** -.04 .01 .06 -.18** -.04 -.16* -.12 -.29** .06 .05 .22** .09 .11 .13 .11 .30** -.04 .29** .23** .15* .03 .14* .17* .16* -.04 .20** .27** .62** -.03 .04 .04 .06 .20** -.17* .18* .13 .12 -.03 .10 .04 .07 -.14* .17* .19** .45** .13* .25** .07 .19** .23** NS -- -- -- Academi c Adjustm ent .30** Emotional Expressiveness subscale Attachment Full Scale -.09 (Hutto, 1998) .22** Social Network Questionnaire Network size Percent students Percent relatives Percent friends Percent roommates Value and attitude similarity Intimacy of relationship Fun/relaxation Task assistance Anger/conflict Emotional support Information/advice Location of interaction (Serafica et al., 1990) Soc’l Network Quest. (adapted) Network size Density Number of friends Percent friends Percent students Percent family Intimacy of relationship Attitude similarity Fun/relaxation Task assistance Information/advice Emotional support Total support Anger/conflict Frequency of contact Current satisfaction with network Expected satisfaction with network Satisfaction with family relationship (Harris, 1988) 88 .08 .02 -.10 .07 .13 .16 .10 .38** .17 -.07 .16 .17 .12 166 Social Provisions Scale Guidance subscale 152c,j .29** 378 Test/Test Variable n Reliable alliance subscale Academi c Adjustm ent NS Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale -- .28** -- -- -- .37** -- -- -- NS -- -- -- .27** -- -- -- .20* -- -- -- .35** -- -- 224v .32** .24** .25** .32** 96 .68** .39** .63** -- .25** .22** .16** .26** -.43** -.37** -.48** -.40** .43** .33** .44** .42** Reassurance of worth subscale .22** Opportunity for nurturance NS subscale Attachment subscale NS Social integration subscale Total score .22** (Ortiz, 1995) .18* Total score (Hunsberger et al., 1996) (Pratt, 2001) .23** .22* Social Provisions Scale-Parent Version Total score (Yaffe, 1997; see also Wintre & 405 .18** Yaffe, 2000) Social Support Inventory Perceived fit score 121 -.22* Subjective satisfaction score .29** (Corbett, 1991) Perceived fit score 244i -- -- -- -.53** Subjective satisfaction score -- -- -- -- .49** Need strength score -- -- -- -- -.36** Perceived supply score -- -- -- -- .13* (Bartels, 1990) -- -.32** -.45** -.33** -- .33** .44** .35** -- Perceived Fit score Subjective Satisfaction score 358 348 .33** (Fuller, 2000) .37** 379 Test/Test Variable n Academi c Adjustm ent Social Adjustment PersonalEmotional Adjustment Attachment Full Scale Social Support Questionnaire 444l Number of support persons Satisfaction with support 246 m 444 l 246 m Total score 444l (Savino, 1987) 246m .05 .25** .06 .14** .14** .11* .37** .19** .25** .27** .30** .21** .18** .26** .31** .24** .22** .31** .30** .12** .17** .20** .40** .23** .28** .32** -- .16*q -- -- -- r -- -- q -- -- -- -- .17** .22** .10** Number of support persons u 109 i .16** Satisfaction with support u --- (Gallant, 1994) .20* .21* r -- .13 -- .32* .13 .33* .32* -- -.03 .15 .08 .22 .29** .11** .32** -- -Satisfaction with support 45 i Perceived availability (Huff, 1998) .21 .22 Student Opinion Survey Overall satisfaction with one’s 1101 .30** college (Napoli & Wortman, 1998) Student-Oriented Life Events Survey 229c .42** -.35** -.48** -.34** -- 144 .29** -.14 -.37** -- -- -.28** -.49** -- -- .34** .24** .34** -- (Marcotte, 1995) Student Stress Scale Stress score Degree of stressfulness .40** (Natera, 1998) Young Adult Social Support Inventory (Marcotte, 1995) 229c .30** 380 a See also Table 24, pp. 54-55, of the SACQ manual (Baker & Siryk, 1989). All males. c All females. d Independent variable administered in fall semester, SACQ in spring semester. e White students. f African-American students. g Latino students. h Asian-American students. i Mixed college year levels. j Age 25-45. k Total sample. l First semester testing. m Second semester testing. n Sample includes traditional- and nontraditional-age undergraduate and graduate students. o Mixed foreign first year graduate students. p Early entrant freshmen, who skipped the junior and/or senior years of high school, age 14-17. q SACQ administered 3rd week of October. r SACQ administered 3rd week of November. s Second generation college attenders. t First generation college attenders. u SSQ administered 3rd week of October. v Independent variable administered prematriculation. W Mexican-American students. b *p<.05; **p<.01. 381 REFERENCES Abe, J., Talbot, D. M.. & Geelhoed, R. J. (1998). Effects of a peer program on international student adjustment. Journal of College Student Development, 39, 539-547. Acunzo, M. E. (1989). The relation between childhood/adolescent sexual abuse and women's college adjustment. Unpublished master's thesis, Clark University, Worcester, MA. Adan, A. M., & Felner, R. D. (1987). Unpublished raw data. Adan, A. M., & Felner, R. D. (1995). Ecological congruence and adaptation of minority youth during the transition to college. Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 256-269. Addison, N. E. (1996). Spirituality and religious affiliation among college students: Effects on psychological well-being and college adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of California at Los Angeles, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 3807A. Ainsworth, M. D. & Bell, S. (1970). Attachment, separation and exploration: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Development, 41, 49-67. Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Albert, M. M. (1988). Physical and psychological separation of late adolescents from their parents as it relates to adjustment to college. (Doctoral dissertation, Barry University, 1988). Dissertation Abstracts International, 49, 2808A. Allen, J. L. (1985). Psychological separation of late adolescents from their parents as it relates to adjustment to college. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Colorado, 1985). Dissertation Abstracts International, 46, 2964A. Allen, J. L. (1986, April). Psychological separation of late adolescents from their parents as it 382 relates to adjustment to college. Paper presented at the meeting of the American College Personnel Association, New Orleans, LA. Allen, S. F. (1989). Psychological separation of older adolescents and young adults from their parents: An investigation of gender differences in divorced and intact families. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oklahoma, 1989). Dissertation Abstracts International, 51, 0109A. Allen, S. F., & Stoltenberg, C. D. (1990). Psychological separation of older adolescents from their parents: An investigation of gender differences in divorced and intact families. Unpublished manuscript, University of Oklahoma, Norman. Allen, S. F., Stoltenberg, C. D., & Rosko, C. K. (1990). Perceived psychological separation of older adolescents and young adults from their parents: A comparison of divorced versus intact families. Journal of Counseling and Development, 69, 57-61. Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. E. (1992). Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. The International Journal for the Study of Religion, 2, 113-133. Amabile, T. M., Hill, K. G., Hennessey, B. A., & Tighe, E. M. (1994). The Work Preference Inventory: Assessing intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 950-967. American College Testing Program, Research and Development Division. (1973). Assessing students on the way to college: Technical report for the ACT assessment program. Iowa City, IA: ACT Publications. American College Testing Program (1994). The ACT Evaluation/Service for Educational Institutions and Agencies: User’s Guide, 6th ed., Iowa City:ACT. 383 Amin, A. H. (2000). Cultural adaptation and psychosocial adjustment among Arab-American college students. (Doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University, 2000). Dissertation Abstracts International, 61, 6183B. Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unraveling the mystery of health: How people manage stress and stay well. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Appenzeller, E. A. (1998). Transition to college: An assessment of the adjustment process for atrisk college students. (Doctoral dissertation, Claremont Graduate University and San Diego State University, 1998). Dissertation Abstracts International, 59, 0099A. Aries, E., Olver, R. R., & Batgos, J. (1985). Self-other differentiation and the mother-child relationship: The effects of sex and birth order. Unpublished manuscript, Amherst College, Amherst, MA. Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment: Individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 427-453. Bachman, J. G., & O'Malley, P. M. (1977). Self-esteem in young men: A longitudinal analysis of the impact of educational and occupational attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 365-380. Baik, J. (1997). Individuation, college adjustment and ego identity: Construction of a measure of individuation and psychometric analysis. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1997). Dissertation Abstracts International, 58, 5157B. Bailey, J. S., & Shilkret, R. B. (2000a). Effects of divorce, custody arrangements, and guilt on college adjustment. Paper presented at annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological 384 Association, Baltimore, MD. Bailey, J. S., & Shilkret, R. B. (2000b). Unpublished raw data. Baker, R. W. (1990). An analysis of the relation between a measure of psychological separation from parents and a measure of student adjustment to college. Unpublished manuscript, Clark University, Worcester, MA. Baker, R. W. (1993). Unpublished raw data. Baker, R. W., McNeil, O. V., & Siryk, B. (1985). Expectation and reality in freshman adjustment to college. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32, 94-103. Baker, R. W., & Schultz, K. L. (1992a). Measuring expectations about college adjustment. NACADA Journal, 12, 23-32. Baker, R. W., & Schultz, K. L. (1992b). Experiential counterparts of test-indicated selfdisillusionment during freshman adjustment to college. NACADA Journal, 12, 13-22. Baker, R. W., & Schultz, K. L. (1993). Interventions using scales measuring expected and actual adjustment to college. NACADA Journal, 13, 9-17. Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1980). Alienation and freshman transition into college. Journal of College Student Personnel, 21, 437-442. Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1983). Social propensity and college adjustment. Journal of College Student Personnel, 24, 331- 336. Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1984a). Measuring academic motivation of matriculating college freshmen. Journal of College Student Personnel, 25, 459-464. Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1984b). Measuring adjustment to college. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 179-189. 385 Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1986). Exploratory intervention with a scale measuring adjustment to college. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33, 31-38. Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1989). Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Baker, R. W., & Smith, M. A. (1991). A research-based precaution for academic advisors concerning use of psychological tests. Unpublished manuscript, Clark University, Worcester, MA. Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York: The Guilford Press. Barrera, M., Jr. (1981). Social support in the adjustment of pregnant adolescents: Assessessment issues. In B. H. Gottlieb (Ed.), Social networks and social support, (pp. 69-96). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Bartels, K. M. (1990). Attachment style, social support, and stress: Relations to adjustment to college. Master’s thesis, University of Missouri-Columbia. Bartels, K. M. (1995). Psychosocial predictors of adjustment to the first year of college: A comparison of first-generation and second-generation students. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri-Columbia, 1995). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 5974B. Barthelemy, K. J., & Fine, M. A. (1995). The relations between residence hall climate and adjustment in college students. College Student Journal, 29, 465-475. Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a four category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226-244. 386 Beavers, W. R., & Hampson, R. B. (1990). Successful families: Assessment and intervention. New York: W. W. Norton & Company. Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 893-897. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571. Bekker, M. H. J. (1993). The development of an autonomy scale based on recent insights into gender identity. European Journal of Personality, 7, 177-194. Belvedere, M. C. (2000). Social aspects of coping: Social support and adjustment among firstyear and transfer students. (Doctoral dissertation, Loyola University - Chicago, 2000). Dissertation Abstracts International, , . Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155-162. Bennion, L. D., & Adams, G. R. (1986). A revision of the Extended Version of the Objective Measure of Ego-Identity Status: An identity instrument for use with late adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 1, 183-198. Benson, R. T. (1999). Untitled research progress report. Unpublished manuscript, Edgewood College, Madison, Wisconsin. Berglas, S., & Jones, E. E. (1978). Drug choice as a self-handicapping strategy in response to 387 noncontingent success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 405-417. Bernstein, E. M., & Putnam, F. W. (1986). Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 727-735. Berwick, D. M., Murphy, J. M., Goldman, P. A., Ware, J. E., Barsky, A. J., & Weinstein, M. C. (1991). Performance of a five-item mental health screening test. Medical Care, 29, 169176. Berzonsky, M. D. (1992). Identity style and coping strategies. Journal of Personality, 60, 771788. Bettencourt, B. A., Charlton, K., Eubanks, J., Kernahan, C., & Fuller, B. (1999). Development of collective self-esteem among students: Predicting adjustment to college. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 213-222. Betz, N. E., & Hackett, G. (1983). The relationship of mathematics self-efficacy expectations to the selection of science-based college majors. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 23, 329345. Beyers, W. (2001). The detachment debate: The separation-adjustment link in adolescence. Doctoral dissertation, Catholic University of Leuven, Belgium. Beyers, W., & Goossens, L. (1998). Students’ adaptation to university and psychological separation from parents: A study on Belgian university students. Research report, Catholic University of Leuven, Belgium. Beyers, W., & Goossens, L. (2002a). Concurrent and predictive validity of the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (SACQ) in a sample of European freshmen students. Educational and Psychological Measurement, __, 388 Beyers, W., & Goossens, L. (2002b). Psychological separation and adjustment to university: Moderating effects of gender, age, and perceived parenting style. Journal of Adolescent Research, __, Beyers, W., & Goossens, L. (2002c). Unpublished raw data. Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1984). Coping, stress, and social resources among adults with unipolar depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 877-891. Birnie-Lefcovitch, S. J. (1997). Adaptation during the transition from high school to university: An examination of selected person, environment and transition perception variables. (Doctoral dissertation, Wilfrid Laurier University, 1997). Dissertation Abstracts International, 58, 3435A. Birnie-Lefcovitch, S. J. (2001). Unpublished raw data. Blankstein, K. R., Flett, G. L., & Koledin, S. (1991). The Brief College Student Hassles Scale: Development, validation, and relation with pessimism. Journal of College Student Development, 32, 258-264. Blatt, S. J., Wein, S. J., Chevron, E., & Quinlan, D. M. (1979). Parental representations and depression in normal young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 388-397. Bloom, B. L. (1985). A factor analysis of self-report measures of family functioning. Family Process, 24, 225-239. Bobier, D. M. (1989). Adjustment to university, psychological separation and sex role orientation. Unpublished B.A. honours thesis, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada. Borow, H. (1949). Manual for the College Inventory of Academic Adjustment. Palo Alto, CA: 389 Stanford University Press. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Attachment. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Separation. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1977). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 201-210. Bragg, T. A. (1994a). A study of the relationship between adjustment to college and freshman retention. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oklahoma, 1994). Dissertation Abstracts International, 56, 0104A. Bragg, T. A. (1994b). A study of the relationship between adjustment to college and freshman retention. Paper presented at the 1994 MIDAIR Conference, St. Louis, MO. Braithwaite, V. A., & Scott, W. A. (1991). Values. (Chap.12). In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Vol. 1. Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich. Bray, J. H., Williamson, D. S., & Malone, P. E. (1984). Personal authority in the family system: Development of a questionnaire to measure personal authority in intergenerational family processes. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 10, 167-178. Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.). Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46-76). New York: Guilford Press. Brewer, B. W., Van Radte, J. L., & Linder, D. E. (1993). Athletic identity: Hercules’ muscle or Achille’s heel? International Journal of Sport Psychology, 24, 235-254. Briere, J., & Runtz, M. (1988). Multivariate correlates of childhood psychological and physical 390 maltreatment among university women. Child Abuse and Neglect, 12, 331-341. Brooks, J. H., II, & DuBois, D. L. (1995). Individual and environmental predictors of adjustment during the first year of college. Journal of College Student Development, 36, 347-360. Brower, A. M. (1990a). Unpublished raw data. Brower, A. M. (1990b). Measuring student performances and performance appraisals with the College Life Task Assessment Instrument. Unpublished manuscript, University of Wisconsin, Madison. Brown, D. A. (1996). Assessment of anticipated and actual college adjustment in freshmanoriented students. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland at College Park, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, 58, 0102A. Brown, D. A. (1997). The effects of three orientation programs on college freshmen. Paper in preparation. Brown, D. A. (1998). Does an outdoor orientation program really work? College and University, 73(4), 17-23. Brown, S. D., Brady, T., Lent, R. W., Wolfert, J., & Hall, S. (1987). Perceived social support among college students: Three studies of the psychometric characteristics and counseling uses of the social support inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 34, 337-354. Brunelle-Joiner, K. M. (1999). Effects of an extended orientation program on personal resiliency and adjustment to college as it relates to academic performance and retention. (Doctoral dissertation, Florida State University, 1999). Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 354A. Brunig, C. A. (1983). Father-daughter relationship correlates of the separation-individuation 391 process. (Doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology, San Diego, 1983). Dissertation Abstracts International, 43, 3352B. Buchanan, S. R. (1991). A longitudinal study of the effects of concurrently taking a modified university 101 course during the high school senior year on students’ anticipated adjustment to college and actual adjustment to college as compared to the adjustment to college of students who concurrently took a traditional lecture course or who took no course during the high school senior year. (Doctoral dissertation, University of South Carolina, 1991). Dissertation Abstracts International, 52, 2386A. Buchanan, S. R. (1993). University 101 for high school students. Journal of the Freshman Year Experience, 5, 49-68. Buelow, G. (1990). An analysis of the influence of family role, family functioning and chemical dependence on the college adjustment of students from chemically dependent and disrupted homes. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, 1990). Dissertation Abstracts International, 52, 1709B. Buelow, G. (1995). Comparing students from substance abusing and dysfunctional families: Implications for counseling. Journal of Counseling and Development, 73, 327-330 Buri, J. R. (1991). Parental Authority Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57, 110-119. Burr, M. (1992). A study of the determinants of student integration and retention at Doña Ana Branch Community College. (Doctoral dissertation, New Mexico State University, 1992). Dissertation Abstracts International, 53, 1772A. Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, 392 NJ: Erlbaum. Butler, W. M., & Ginsburg, G. (1989, November). Family functioning and college student adjustment. Poster presented at the Annual Vermont Psychological Association Conference, Burlington, VT. Camp, C. C., & Chartrand, J. M. (1992). A comparison and evaluation of interest congruence indices. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 41, 162-182. Campbell, J. M., Amerikaner, M., Swank, P., & Vincent, K. (1989). The relationship between the Hardiness Test and the Personal Orientation Inventory. Journal of Research in Personality, 23, 373-380. Caplan, S. M. (1997). Family and self-concept factors contributing to the adjustment and achievement of early entrants. (Doctoral dissertation, University of North Texas, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, 58, 2527A. Carlson, D. L. (1986). Identity status: Its relationship to psychological adjustment and academic achievement. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Wisconsin, River Falls. Caro, J. E. (1986). An empirical investigation of attachment in adulthood and its relationship to adjustment in college. (Doctoral dissertation, Syracuse University, 1985). Dissertation Abstracts International, 47, 2670B. Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 267-283. Chartrand, J. M., Camp, C. C., & McFadden, K. L. (1990a). Predicting academic adjustment and career indecision: A comparison of self-efficacy, interest congruence, and commitment. Poster presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Boston, MA. 393 Chartrand, J. M., Camp, C. C., & McFadden, K. L. (1990b). Unpublished raw data. Chartrand, J. M., Camp, C. C., & McFadden, K. L. (1992). Predicting academic adjustment and career indicision: A comparison of self-efficacy, interest congruence, and commitment. Journal of College Student Development, 33, 293-300. Cheek, J. M. (1983). The Revised Cheek and Buss Shyness Scale. Unpublished manuscript, Wellesley College. Cheek, J. M., & Melchior, L. A. (1990). Shyness, self-esteem, and self-consciousness. In H. Leitenberg (Ed.), Handbook of social and evaluation anxiety (pp. 47-82). New York: Plenum. Chizhik, E. W. (1999). The relationship between prematriculation college knowledge and disillusionment: Was college what students expected? NACADA Journal, 19, 12-21. Choi, K-H. (1999). Unpublished raw data. Choi, K-H. (2000). Psychological separation-individuation and college adjustment among Korean-American students: The role of collectivism-individualism. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Loyola University of Chicago, Chicago. Christenson, R. M., & Wilson, W. P. (1985). Assessing pathology in the separationindividuation process by an inventory: A preliminary report. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 173, 561-565. Clauss, K. (1995). The relationships of family style, family competence, parental attachment, self-esteem, and separation-individuation with social and emotional adjustment to college of first-year students. (Doctoral dissertation, Seton Hall University, 1995). Dissertation Abstracts International, 56, 3885A. 394 Cline, D., & Chosy, J. (1972). A prospective study of life changes and subsequent health changes. Archives of General Psychiatry, 27, 51-53. Coatsworth, D. J. (2001). Emotional, instrumental and financial support by divorced parents of their college aged children. Undergraduate honors thesis, Arizona State University, Tempe. Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385-396. Cohen, S., Mermelstein, R., Kamarck, T., & Hoberman, H. M. (1985). Measuring the functional components of social support. In I. G. Sarason & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Social support: Theory, research and application (pp. 73-94). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Martraus Nighriff. Cohen, S., Sherrod, D. R., & Clark, M. S. (1986). Social skills and the stress-protective role of social support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 963-973. Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 644-663. Compas, B. E., Davis, G. E., Forsythe, C. J., & Wagner, B. M. (1987). Assessment of major and daily stressful events during adolescence: The Adolescent Perceived Events Scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 534-541. Constantinople, A. (1969). An Eriksonian measure of personality development in college students. Developmental Psychology, 1, 357-372. Conti, R. (2000a). College goals: Do self-determined and carefully considered goals predict intrinsic motivation, academic performance, and adjustment during the first semester? 395 Social Psychology of Education, 4, 189-211. Conti, R. (2000b). Why pursue a college education? The influence of early reflection and goal orientation on adjustment during the first semester. In C. Chiu, F. Salili, & Y. Hong (Eds.), Multiple competencies and self-regulated learning: The psychological foundation of life-long learning. Plenum Press, pp. (need date & p. #’s; NA as of 11/16/00 – au says will notify) Conti, R. (2000c). Unpublished raw data. Cooler, J. R. (1995). The relationship among family roles, psychological separationindividuation, and adjustment in first-year college students. (Doctoral dissertation, Teachers College, Columbia University, 1995). Dissertation Abstracts International, 56, 4049B. Cooley, E. L., & Carden, A. I. (1992, March). Depression, locus of control, and homesickness: Predictors of college adjustment? Poster presented at the meeting of the Southeastern Psychological Association, Knoxville, TN. Cooper, S. A. (1991). Overseas students, their values and adjustment to studying in Australia. Honours thesis for bachelor’s degree, University College of Southern Queensland, Toowoomba, Queensland, Australia. Cooper, S. E., & Robinson, D. A. G. (1988a). Psychometric properties of the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire with engineering and science students. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 21, 124-129. Cooper, S. E., & Robinson, D. A. G. (1988b, August). Assessing successful adaptation to college. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, 396 Atlanta, GA. Coopersmith, S. (1981). Self-Esteem Inventories manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Corbett, C. A. (1991). Dual minority status and college adjustment: An examination of social and academic adjustment in Black deaf college students. (Doctoral dissertation, Pennsylvania State University, 1991). Dissertation Abstracts International, 53, 2055B. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). The NEO-PI-R/NEO-FFI Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Costello, C. G., & Comrey, A. L. (1967). Scales for measuring depression and anxiety. Journal of Psychology, 66, 303-313. Crastnopol, M. G. (1980). Separation-individuation in a woman's identity vis-a-vis mother. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Cincinnati, 1980). Dissertation Abstracts International, 41, 345B. Crombag, H. F. M. (1968). Study motivation and study attitude: Membership of various organizations and its effect on study motivation and study attitude in freshmen students. Groningen, The Netherlands: Walters. Crouse, D. W. (1990). A comparison of men in nontraditional and traditional majors. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Kansas, Lawrence. Crumbaugh, J., & Mahalick, L. (1964). An experimental study in existentialism: The psychometric approach to Frankl’s concept of noogenic neurosis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 20, 200-207. Cuellar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican- 397 Americans-II: A revision of the original ARSMA scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17, 275-304. Cuellar, I., Harris, L. C., & Jasso, R. (1980). An acculturation scale for Mexican-American normal and clinical populations. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 2, 199-217. Cutrona, C. E. (1989). Ratings of social support by adolescents and adult informants: Degree of correspondence and prediction of depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 723-732. Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. W. (1987). The provisions of social relationships and adaptation to stress. In W. H. Jones & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships, Vol. 1. JAI Press, Inc., 37-67. Danielson, H. (1995). First year student adaptation to college and the impact of residence hall structure. Unpublished master’s research project, Drake University, Des Moines, Iowa. Davis, E. R. (1988). The effects of the life development intervention model on freshman and transfer student-athletes. (Doctoral dissertation, Virginia Commonwealth University, 1988). Dissertation Abstracts International, 50, 3149B. Derogatis, L. R. (1984). SCL-90-R: Administration, scoring, and procedure manual-II. Towson, MD: Clinical Psychometric Research. Derogatis, L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The Brief Symptom Inventory: An introductory report. Psychological Medicine, 13, 595-605. Dewein, K. M. (1994). The relationships among divorce, narcissistic vulnerability, psychological separation, and adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of MissouriColumbia, 1994). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 1487B. 398 Dewitt-Parker, J. (2000). Predicting the successful adjustment of black freshmen to a predominately white university: The contribution of parental attachment and cultural traditionality. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Albany, the State University of New York, 1999). Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 2812A. Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffen, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71-75. Diener, E., Sandvik, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1985). Age and sex effects for emotional intensity. Developmental Psychology, 21, 542-546. DiGiuseppe, R., Leaf, R., Exner, T., & Robin, M. W. (1988). The development of a measure of rational/irrational thinking. Paper presented at the World Congress of Behavior Therapy, Edinborough, Scotland. Dodgen-Magee, D. J. (1992). The relationship between social, interpersonal, academic, and emotional adjustment and levels of depression in college freshmen. (Doctoral dissertation, Biola University, 1992). Dissertation Abstracts International, 53, 2043B. Dowd, E. T., Yesenosky, J. M., Wallbrown, F., & Sanders, D. (1993). Psychological reactance and its relationship to normal personality variables. Paper read at meetings of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada. Edgerton, J. (1997). Separation-individuation in young adulthood: Attachment patterns, family cohesion, and mental health considerations. (Master’s thesis, University of Alberta, 1997). Master’s Abstracts International, 36, 0024. (But see Lapsley & Edgerton, 1999a&b for amended findings) Elacqua, T. C. (1992a). Factors affecting the adjustment among first year students at a women's . 399 college. Unpublished senior honors project, Russell Sage College, Troy, NY. Elacqua, T. C. (1992b). Factors affecting the adjustment among first year students at a women's college. Paper presented at the Sixth National Conference for Undergraduate Research, Minneapolis, MN. Ellison, C. W. (1983). Spiritual well-being: Conceptualization and measurement. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 11, 330-340. Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. A. (1990). Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations (CISS): Manual. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems. Epstein, N. B., Baldwin, L. M., & Bishop, D. S. (1983). The McMaster Family Assessment Device. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 9, 171-180. Erickson, C. J. (1996). Impact of grandparent-student relationship on student adaptation to college environment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Northern Colorado, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 3402. .Evans-Hughes, G. (1992). The influence of racial identity and locus of control on the adjustment and academic achievement of African-American college students. (Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers University, 1992). Dissertation Abstracts International, 54, 1239A Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1968). Manual for the Eysenck Personality Inventory. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service. Fackelman, P., & Shilkret, R. B. (1994). Putting object relations to the test: An examination of the object relational world of college women using five scales. Unpublished manuscript, Mount Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA. Farber, S. S., & Felner, R. D. (1980). Stress and coping in college students. Unpublished 400 manuscript. Feeney, J. A., Noller, P., & Hanrahan, M. (1994). Assessing adult attachment. In M. B. Sperling & W. H. Berman (Eds.). Attachment in adults: Clinical and developmental perspectives. New York: The Guilford Press, Chapter 5, 128-152. Feenstra, J. S., Banyard, V. L., Rines, E. N., & Hopkins, K. R. (2001). First-year students’ adaptation to college: The role of family variables and individual coping. Journal of College Student Development, 42, 106-113. Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 522-527. Fible, B., & Hale, W. D. (1978). The Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale -- A new measure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 924-931. Fiese, B. H., & Kline, C. A. (1992). The psychometric properties of the Family Ritual Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, Syracuse University. Fitts, W. H. (1965). The Tennessee Self-Concept Scale manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Flanagan, C., Schulenberg, J., & Fuligni, A. (1993). Residential setting and parent-adolescent relationships during the college years. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 22, 171-189. Flescher, M. (1986a, August). Social networks and student adjustment following parental divorce and remarriage. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC. Flescher, M. (l986b). Unpublished raw data. Flescher, M., Clingempeel, W. G., & Stein, P. N. (1986). The adjustment to college of students 401 from divorce-engendered mother-headed and stepfather families: A multimethod study. Unpublished manuscript, Temple University, Philadelphia. Fletcher, G. J. O., Danilovics, P., Fernandez, G., Peterson, D., & Reeder, G. D. (1986). Attributional complexity: An individual differences measure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 875-884. Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must be a process: Study of emotion and coping during three stages of a college examination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 150-170. Foster, T. R. (1997). A comparative study of the study skills, self-concept, academic achievement and adjustment to college of freshman intercollegiate athletes and nonathletes. (Doctoral dissertation, Purdue University, 1997). Dissertation Abstracts International, 58, 4565A. Fox, D. P. (2000). Interactive effects of gender, ethnicity, and first year experience program participation on college student adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Northern Colorado, 2000). Dissertation Abstracts International, 61, 3007A. Fray, J. S. (????). An exploratory study of the culture shock experience of missionary children homecomers. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, 1988). Dissertation Abstracts International, , . Frazier, P. A., & Cook, S. W. (1993). Correlates of distress following heterosexual relationship dissolution. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 55-67. Freeman, S. K. (1987a). Psychological separation and adjustment to college. Unpublished manuscript, State University of New York, Stony Brook. 402 Freeman, S. K. (1987b). Unpublished raw data. Friedland, C. D. (1990). Relationship between adjustment to physical disability and belief systems in persons with physical disability. (Doctoral dissertation, Hofstra University, 1990). Dissertation Abstracts International, 51, 6150B. Fuller, B. E., & Heppner, M. J. (1995). The college transition inventory: Measuring psychosocial factors in the transition process. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, New York, NY. Fuller, B. E. (2000). Unpublished raw data. Gallant, L. M. (1994). The relationship of gender, stressful life events, social support, and coping style to adaptation to university. (Master’s thesis, Acadia University, 1994). Master’s Abstracts International, 33, 0647. Garbarino, C., & Strange, C. (1993). College adjustment and family environments of students reporting parental alcohol problems. Journal of College Student Development, 34, 261266. Garner, D. M. (1991). Eating Disorder Inventory - 2. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Garner, H. D. (1986). Effects of marital and cross generational alliances on the adjustment and separation of late adolescents. (Doctoral dissertation, Texas Tech University, 1986). Dissertation Abstracts International, 47, 2162B. Garrett, S. E. (1994). The relationship between self-esteem and adjustment, persistence, and involvement among first-year female college students. Unpublished master’s thesis, Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, Ohio. 403 Gavazzi, S. M., Sabatelli, R. M., & Reese-Weber, M. (1999). Measurement of financial, functional, and psychological connections in families: Conceptual development and empirical use of the Multigenerational Interconnectedness Scale. Psychological Reports, 84, 1361-1371. Gerdes, H. (l986). Freshman adjustment to college: Expectations, satisfaction, and persistence. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Oregon, Eugene. Gerdes, H. (1987). Unpublished raw data. Gilkey, J. K. (1988). From their daughter’s eyes: Parent/daughter relationships and college adjustment in young women. (Doctoral dissertation, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, 1988). Dissertation Abstracts International, 50, 0267A. Gilkey, J. K., Protinsky, H. O., & Lichtman, M. (1989). Parent/daughter relationships and college adjustment in young women. Unpublished manuscript, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg. Gilleylen, C. E. (1993). A comparative study of the science-related attitudes and the factors associated with persisting in science of African-American college students in science majors and African-American college students in non-science majors. (Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University of Pennsylvania, 1993). Dissertation Abstracts International, 54, 4390A. Gold, J., Burrell, S., Haynes, C., & Nardecchia, D. (1990). Black undergraduate adaptation to college as a predictor of academic success. Paper presented at annual meeting of the American Association for Counseling and Development, Reno, NV. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 338 730). 404 Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4, 26-42. Gough, H. G. (1987). California Psychological Inventory: Administrator's guide. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Graham, C., Baker, R. W., & Wapner, S. (1984). Prior interracial experience and Black student transition into predominantly white colleges. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1146-1154. Grasley, C. (1992). Measuring the impact of stressful life events on undergraduate students: The Student-Oriented Life Events Survey. Unpublished master’s thesis, Queen’s University, Kingston, Ontario. Greenberger, E., Josselson, R., Knerr, C., & Knerr, B. (1975). The measurement and structure of psychosocial maturity. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 4, 127-143. Grella, R. S. (1989). Family structure and student adaptation to college: A correlational study. Unpublished undergraduate research report, Lebanon Valley College, Annville, PA. Griffen, D. W., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Models of the self and other: Fundamental dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 430-445. Grochowski, J. R., & McCubbin, H. I. (1987). YA-SSI: Young adult social support inventory. In H. I. McCubbin, & A. I. Thompson (Eds.), Family Assessment Inventories for Research and Practice (Chapter 18, pp. 272-279). Madison: University of WisconsinMadison. Grotevant, H. D., & Adams, G. R. (1984). Development of an objective measure to assess ego 405 identity in adolescence: Validation and replication. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 13, 419-438. Grych, J. H., Seid, M., & Finchman, F. D. (1992). Assessing marital conflict from the child’s perspective: The Children’s Perception of Interparental Conflict Scale. Child Development, 63, 558-572. Haemmerlie, F. M., & Merz, C. J. (1991). Concurrent validity between the California Psychological Inventory-Revised and the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 664-668. Haemmerlie, F. M., & Montgomery, R. L. (1994). Psychological reactance and its relationship to college adjustment. Paper presented at meetings of the Southwestern Psychological Association, Tulsa, Oklahoma. Haemmerlie, F. M., Montgomery, R. L., & Consolvo, C. A. (1993). Psychological separation, adjustment, and alcohol use by college students. Paper presented at the INPR Conference on Personal Relationships, Milwaukee, Wisconsin. Haemmerlie, F. M., Montgomery, R. L., & Saling, C. (1994). Age of first use and present use of alcohol by undergraduates. Psychological Reports, 75, 1268-1270. Haemmerlie, F. M., Robinson, D. A. G., & Carmen, R. C. (1991). "Type A" personality traits and adjustment to college. Journal of College Student Development, 32, 81-82. Haemmerlie, F. M., Steen, S. C., & Benedicto, J. A. (1994). Undergraduates' conflictual independence, adjustment, and alcohol use: The importance of the mother-student relationship. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50, 644-650. Halloran, N. H. (2000). Freshman learning communities and their effectiveness on college 406 student adjustment and adaptation. (Doctoral dissertation, Georgia State University, 2000). Dissertation Abstracts International, 61, 2195A. Hansburg, H. G. (1980). Adolescent separation anxiety. Vol. 2. New York: Krieger. Harder, D. H., & Zalma, A. (1990). Two promising shame and guilt scales: A construct validity comparison. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 729-745. Harrell, S. P. (1997). The Racism and Life Experiences Scale-Revised. Unpublished instrument. Harris, B. C. (1988). How the structure and function of social networks relate to loneliness and college adaptation. Unpublished master's thesis, The George Washington University, Washington, DC. Harris, B. C. (1991). The college student experience: How social support relates to adaptation and attrition. (Doctoral dissertation, The George Washington University, 1991). Dissertation Abstracts International, 52, 4520B. Harste, J. M. (1996). Adolescent attachment and adjustment to college. Unpublished undergraduate honors project, Hamline University, St. Paul, MN. Hatter, D. Y., & Ottens, A. J. (1998). Afrocentric world view and black students’ adjustment to a predominantly white university: Does world view matter? College Student Journal, 32. 472-480. Hays, R. B., & Oxley, D. (1986). Social network development and functioning during a life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 305-313. Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511-524. Hazzard-Patterson, T. (1999). Parental loss and first year college adjustment. Unpublished 407 master’s thesis, Radford University, Radford, Virginia. Helman, C. K. (1999). Adjustment to college: The contribution of a living-learning program for science and engineering students. (Doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1999). Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 3615A. Helmreich, R., & Stapp, J. (1974). Short forms of the Texas Social Behavior Inventory (TSBI), an objective measure of self-esteem. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4(5A), 473475. Henderson, S., Duncan-Jones, P., Byrne, D. G., & Scott, R. (1980). Measuring social relationships: The Interview Schedule for Social Interaction. Psychological Medicine, 10, 723-734. Heppner, P. P., & Petersen, C. H. (1982). The development and implications of a personal problem-solving inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 29, 66-75. Hermans, H. J. M. (1970). A questionnaire measure of achievement motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 34, 353-363. Hertel, J. B. (1996). First-generation and second-generation college students: Similarities, differences, and differential factors in their adjustment to college. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri-Columbia, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, 58, 2070A. Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456-470. Hickman, G. P., Bartholomae, S., & McKenry, P. C. (2000). Influence of parenting styles on the 408 adjustment and academic achievement of traditional college freshmen. Journal of College Student Development, 41, 41-54. Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Gough, H. G., Barrett, J., Korchin, S. J., & Chodoff, P. (1977). A measure of interpersonal dependency. Journal of Personality Assessment, 41, 610-618. Hoffman, J. A. (1984). Psychological separation of late adolescents from their parents. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 170-178. Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (1995). Hogan Personality Inventory Manual (2nd ed.), Tulsa, OK: Hogan Assessment Systems. Hogan, M. P. (l986). Unpublished raw data. Hogan, M. P. (1988). The relationship of past interpersonal loss to college adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland, 1987). Dissertation Abstracts International, 48, 3112B. Holahan, C. J., & Moos, R. H. (1987). Personal and contextual determinants of coping strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 946-955. Holland, J. L., Gottfredson, D. C., & Power, P. G. (1980). Some diagnostic scales for research in decision making and personality: Identity, information, and barriers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1191-1200. Hollmann, N. C. (1995). Unpublished raw data. Hollmann, N. C., & Metzler, A. E. (1994). Adjustment to college: The self in the system. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Los Angeles. Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The Social Readjustment Rating Scale. Journal of 409 Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213-218. Hopkins, K. R. (1998). Leaving home: Family functioning in relation to adjustment to college. Undergraduate honors research report, University of New Hampshire, Durham. Hovestadt, A. J., Anderson, W. T., Piercy, F. P., Cochran, S. W., & Fine, M. (1985). A familyof-origin scale. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 11, 287-297. Howard, J. A., Morey, K. M., & Briancesco, L. A. (April, 2001). Roots and wings: The role of attachment theory in adjustment to college. Paper presented at meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis. Huff, J. L. (1998). Parental attachment, perceived social support, reverse culture shock and college adjustment of missionary children. Master’s thesis, California State University at Fullerton. Hui, C. H. (1988). Measurement of individualism-collectivism. Journal of Research in Personality, 22, 17-36. Humfleet, G. L. (1987). Leaving home: Adjustment to college as related to separationindividuation. Doctoral dissertation, DePaul University, Chicago. Humfleet, G. L., & Ribordy, S. C. (1990). The reliability andvalidity of the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire as a measure of adjustment to college. Poster presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Boston, MA. Hunsberger, B. (2000). Unpublished raw data. Hunsberger, B., Pancer, S. M., Pratt, M., & Alisat, S. (1996). The transition to university: Is religion related to adjustment? Research in the Social Scientific Study of Religion, 7, 181-199. 410 Hurlbut, S. C., & Sher, K. J. (1992). Assessing alcohol problems in college students. Journal of American College Health, 41, 49-58. Hurtado, S., Carter, D. F., & Spuler, A. (1996). Latino student transition to college: Assessing difficulties and factors in successful college adjustment. Research in Higher Education, 37, 135-157. Hutto, C. A. (1998). The relationship of selected family variables to college student adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of South Carolina, 1998). Dissertation Abstracts International, 59, 2455B. Hutto, C. A. (2001). Unpublished raw data. Hutz, A. (2002a). Relationships among perceptions and coping with barriers, ethnic identity and ethnocultural person-environment fit to college student adjustment. (Docroral dissertation, Northern Arizona University, 2002). Dissertation Abstracts International, __, (get DAI info) Hutz, A. (2002b). Unpublished raw data. Hutz, A., Fabian, T., & Martin, W. E., Jr. (2000). College adjustment of first-year undergraduate students from various ethnocultural backgrounds. Poster session at the annual meeting of the Rocky Mountain Psychological Association, Tucson, Arizona. Ibrahim, F. A., & Kahn, H. (1987). Assessment of world views. Psychological Reports, 60, 163176. Insel, P. M., & Roth, W. T. (1985). Core Concepts in Health. Palo Alto, CA: Mayfield. Injejikian, A. M. (1995). Psychosocial adjustment of the Northridge earthquake survivors. (Doctoral dissertation, University of California at Los Angeles, 1995). Dissertation 411 Abstracts International, 56, 1142A. Jackson, A. L. (1998). Factors related to freshman adjustment to college. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Southern Mississippi, 1998). Dissertation Abstracts International, 59, 2882A. Jackson, L. M., Pancer, S. M., Pratt, M. W., & Hunsberger, B. E. (2000). Great expectations: The relation between expectancies and adjustment during the transition to university. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 2100-2125. Jagels, C. T., & Burger, J. M. (1993). Adjustment to college as a function of extraversionintroversion. Paper presented at the meeting of the Western Psychological Association, Phoenix. Jampol, R. C. (1989). Coping with stress: The role of locus of control, appraisals, and coping responses in adjusting to college. (Doctoral dissertation, Emory University, 1988). Dissertation Abstracts International, 49, 4545B. Jarama Alvan, S. L., Belgrave, F. Z., & Zea, M. C. (1996). Stress, social support, and college adjustment among Latino students. Cultural Diversity and Mental Health, 2, 193-203. Jenkins, C. D., Zyzanski, S. J., & Rosenman, R. H. (1979). Jenkins Activity Survey manual (Form C). New York: The Psychological Corporation. Johansson, C. B. (1986). Career Assessment Inventory. Minneapolis: National Computer Systems, Inc. John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The “Big Five” Inventory-Versions 4a and 54 (Tech. Report). Berkeley, CA: Institute of Personality Assessment and Research. Johnson, A. R. (2001). Optimism, college adjustment, and campus resources. Unpublished 412 master’s thesis, University of Kansas, Lawrence. Johnson, J. H. (1982). Life events as stressors in childhood and adolescence. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in Clinical Child Psychology, pp. 219-253. New York: Plenum Press. Jones, C. R. (1996). Client expectations, locus of control, and counseling effectiveness. (Doctoral dissertation, New Mexico State University, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 1031A. Jones, E. E., & Rhodewalt, F. (1982). Self-Handicapping Scale. Available from the authors at the Department of Psychology, Princeton University, or the Department of Psychology, University of Utah. Just, H. D. (1998). Freshman adjustment and retention: Combining traditional risk factors with psychosocial variables. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas-Austin, 1998). Dissertation Abstracts International, 59, 5138B. Kaase, K. J. (1994). Testing the limits of the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire. Paper presented at the 34th Annual Forum of the Association for Institutional Research, New Orleans. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 373 619). Kaczmarek, P. G., Matlock, C. G., & Franco, J. N. (1990). Assessment of college adjustment in three freshman groups. Psychological Reports, 66, 1195-1202. Kaczmarek, P. G., Matlock, C. G., Mesta, R., Ames, M. H., & Ross, M. (1994). An assessment of international college student adjustment. International Journal for the Advancement of Counseling, 17, 241-247. Kalsner-Silver, L. (2000). Adjustment in a multicultural undergraduate population: Attachment 413 quality, ethnic identity, and separation-individuation. (Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers University and University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, 2000). Dissertation Abstracts International, 61, 1290A. Kalsner-Silver, L. (2002). Unpublished raw data. Kambach, M. (1994). A study of social support and college adjustment including with a variation of a traditional measure of social support. Unpublished master's thesis, Clark University, Worcester, MA. Kandel, D. B., & Davies, M. (1982). Epidemiology of depressive mood in adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 1205-1212. Kane, M. A., Lennon, K., & Petrosky, G. (1989). Transitions into academia: A comparison of traditional freshmen, non-traditional undergraduates and graduate students. Unpublished manuscript, Boston University, Boston, MA and Boston College, Chestnut Hill, MA. Keenan, J. T. (1992). Unpublished raw data. Kenny, M. E. (1990). College seniors' perceptions of parental attachments: The value and stability of family ties. Journal of College Student Development, 31, 39-46. Kenny, M. E. (1992). Unpublished raw data. Kenny, M. E. (1993). The high school-to-college transition. In F. G. Lopez & D. L. Blustein (Chairs), Contemporary extensions of attachment theory: Implications for theory and practice. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada. Kenny, M. E. (1994). Unpublished raw data. Kenny, M. E. (1995). Unpublished raw data. 414 Kenny, M. E., & Donaldson, G. A. (1992). The relationship of parental attachment and psychological separation to the adjustment of first-year college women. Journal of College Student Development, 33, 431-438. Kenny, M. E., & Perez, V. (1996). Attachment and psychological well-being among racially and ethnically diverse first year college students. Journal of College Student Development, 37, 527-535. Kenny, M. E., & Stryker, S. (1994). Social network characteristics of White, Afro-American, Asian and Latino/a college students and college adjustment: A longitudinal study. Poster presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Los Angeles, CA. Kenny, M. E., & Stryker, S. (1995). Unpublished raw data. Kenny, M. E., & Stryker, S. (1996). Social network characteristics and college adjustment among racially and ethnically diverse first-year students. Journal of College Student Development, 37, 649-658. Kim, D. D. (1996). A study of the relationship between psychological androgyny and college adjustment among Korean-American college students. (Doctoral dissertation, Andrews University, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 2857A. Kim, E. J., Lee, J. Y., & Oh, K. J. (1992). The effects of self-perception, perceived social support and coping behavior on adjustment of university students: Six-month follow up study. Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Korean Psychological Association, 525-534. Kincannon, J. C. (1968). Prediction of the standard MMPI scale scores from 71 items: The MiniMult. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 32, 319-325. 415 Kintner, D. E. (1998). Locus of control and college adjustment related to the freshman orientation seminar at Trevecca Nazarene University. (Doctoral dissertation, Tennessee State University, 1998). Dissertation Abstracts International, 59, 3349A. Kleinmuntz, B. (1960). Identification of maladjusted college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 7, 209-211. Kleinmuntz, B. (1961). The college maladjustment scale (Mt): Norms and predictive ability. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 21, 1029-1033. Kline, C. A. (1992). Family rituals, psychological separation and college adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, Syracuse University, 1992). Dissertation Abstracts International, 54, 1101B. Klynn, B. (1997). Influences of attachment style on adjustment of first-year students. Unpublished undergraduate honors thesis, Colgate University, Hamilton, New York. Kobasa, S. C. (1985). Personal Views Survey. Chicago: Hardiness Institute. Kobasa, S. C., Maddi, S. R., & Kahn, S. (1982). Hardiness and health: A prospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 168-177. Koutralakos, J. (1971). Perceived paternal attitudes and demographic variables as related to maladjustment. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 32, 371-382. Krotseng, M. V. (1991, May). Predicting persistence from the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire: Early warning or siren song? Paper delivered at the Association for Institutional Research Annual Forum, San Francisco, CA. Krotseng, M. V. (1992). Predicting persistence from the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire: Early warning or siren song? Research in Higher Education, 33, 99-111. Kulley, J. C. (1994). Attachment style categorization and the relationships among attachment 416 styles and psychosocial adjustment during college. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Austin, 1994). Dissertation Abstracts International, 55, 4623B. Kurdek, L. A., & Fine, M. A. (1994). Family acceptance and family control as predictors of adjustment problems in young adolescents: Linear, curvilinear, or interactive effects? Child Development, 65, 1137-1146. Kusaka, T. (1995). The influence of individualism-collectivism orientations on east Asian international students’ college adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of the Pacific, 1995). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 0115A. Lamothe, D., Currie, F., Alisat, S., Sullivan, T., Pratt, M. W., Pancer, S. M., & Hunsberger, B. E. (1995). Impact of a social support intervention on the transition to university. Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health, 14, 167-180. Landrine, H., & Klonoff, E. A. (1994). The African American Acculturation Scale: Development, reliability, and validity. Journal of Black Psychology, 20, 104-127. Lapan, R., & Patton, M. J. (1986). Self-psychology and the adolescent process: Measures of pseudoautonomy and peer-group dependence. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33, 136-142. Lapsley, D. K. (1989). Unpublished raw data. Lapsley, D. K., Aalsma, M. C., & Varshney, N. M. (2001). A factor analytic and psychometric examination of pathology of separation-individuation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57, 915-932. Lapsley, D. K., & Edgerton, J. (1999a). Normal and pathological separation-individuation in early adulthood: Implications for attachment style and college adjustment. Manuscript in 417 preparation, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana. Lapsley, D. K., & Edgerton, J. (1999b). Unpublished raw data. Lapsley, D. K., Rice, K. G., & FitzGerald, D. P. (1990). Adolescent attachment, identity, and adjustment to college: Implications for the continuity of adaptation hypothesis. Journal of Counseling and Development, 68, 561-565. Lapsley, D. K., Rice, K. G., & Shadid, G. E. (1989). Psychological separation and adjustment to college. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 286-294. Leatherman-Sommers, S. (1999). Attachment and adjustment to college among students with physical disabilities. (Doctoral dissertation, University of South Carolina, 1999). Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 3570B.(delete if x cite fndgs) Lefebvre, R. C., & Sandford, S. L. (1985). A multi-modal questionnaire for stress. Journal of Human Stress, 11, 69-75. Lent, R. W. (1997). Unpublished raw data. Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Gore, P. A. (1997). Discriminant and predictive validity of academic self-concept, academic self-efficacy, and mathematics-specific self-efficacy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44, 307-315. Leonard, J. B. (1990). Adjusting to college: An ecological perspective of undecided first-year students. Unpublished orals paper, the University of Vermont, Burlington. Leong, F. T. L. (1999). Unpublished raw data. Leong, F. T. L., Bonz, M. H., & Zachar, P. (1997). Coping styles as predictors of college adjustment among freshmen. Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 10 (No. 2), 211-220. Levenson, H. (1973). Multidimensional locus of control in psychiatric patients. Journal of 418 Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 41, 397-404. Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1978). Toward the assessment of social competence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 453-462. Levin, M. A. (1996). Mother-daughter relationships in divorced versus intact families: Conflictual independence, attachment and college adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, State University of New York at Albany, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 2691A. Levine, J. B., Green, C. J., & Millon, T. (1986). The Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 123-137. Levine, J. B., & Saintonge, S. (1993). Psychometric properties of the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence within a clinical population. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49, 492-507. Lipsmeyer, M. E. (1984). The measurement of religiosity and its relationship to mental health/impairment. (Doctoral dissertation, St. Louis University). Dissertation Abstracts International, 45, 1918B. Liter, S. D. (1987). The existential factors of death anxiety, purpose in life, and alienation and their relationship to adjustment during the freshman year of college. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Denver, 1987). Dissertation Abstracts International, 48, 1153A. Lopez, F. G. (1989). Current family dynamics, trait anxiety, and academic adjustment: Test of a family-based model of vocational identity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 35, 76-87. Lopez, F. G. (1991). Patterns of family conflict and their relation to college student adjustment. 419 Journal of Counseling and Development, 69, 257-260. Lopez, F. G. (1997). Student-professor relationship styles, childhood attachment bonds and current academic orientations. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14, 271-282. Lopez, F. G., Campbell, V. L., & Watkins, C. E., Jr. (1986). Depression, psychological separation, and college adjustment: An investigation of sex differences. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33, 52-56. Lopez, F. G., Campbell, V. L., & Watkins, C. E., Jr. (1988a). The relation of parental divorce to college student development. Journal of Divorce, 12, 83-98. Lopez, F. G., Campbell, V. L., & Watkins, C. E., Jr. (1988b). Family structure, psychological separation, and college adjustment: A canonical analysis and cross-validation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 35, 402-409. Lopez, F. G., Campbell, V. L., & Watkins, C. E., Jr. (1989). Effects of marital conflict and family coalition patterns on college student adjustment. Journal of College Student Development, 30, 46-52. Loveland, D. (1992). Analysis of the relationship between students' perceptions of the environment, drinking behavior, and their adjustment to college. Unpublished master's thesis, State University of New York, New Paltz. Loveland, D. (1994). Unpublished raw data. Low, C. A. (1994). An examination of the relationship between religion and adjustment to college. Unpublished master's thesis, St. Louis University, St. Louis, MO. Low, C. A., & Handal, P. J. (1995). The relationship between religion and adjustment to college. Journal of College Student Development, 36, 406-412. 420 Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1992). A collective self-esteem scale: Self-evaluation of one’s social identity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 302-318. Lustman, P. J., Sowa, C. J., & O'Hara, D. J. (1984). Factors influencing college student health: Development of the Psychological Distress Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 28-35. Luzzo, D. A., & McWhirter, E. H. (2001). Sex and ethnic differences in the perception of educational and career-related barriers and levels of coping efficacy. Journal of Counseling and Development, 79, 61-67. Mallinckrodt, B. (1988). Student retention, social support, and drop-out intentions: Comparison of black and white students. Journal of College Student Development, 29, 60-64. Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego identity status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 551-558. Marcia, J. E. (1980). Identity in adolescence. In J. Adelson (Ed.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 159-187). New York: Wiley, Marcotte, G. M. R. (1995). A model investigating adjustment to university in first year students. (Doctoral dissertation, Queen’s University at Kingston, 1995). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 2858A. . Marcotte, G. M. R. (1996). Unpublished raw data. Marcy, T. P. (1996, October). The nature of the relationship between adaptation to college, student characteristics, and freshman retention at a small church-affiliated college. Paper presented at the Mid-America Association for Institutional Research Conference, Excelsior Springs, MO. 421 Martin, B., & Burks, N. (1985). Family and nonfamily components of social support as buffers of stress for college women. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15, 448-465. Martin, N. K. (1988). The effects of freshman orientation and locus of control on adjustment to college. (Doctoral dissertation, Texas Tech University, 1988). Dissertation Abstracts International, 49, 3271A. Martin, N. K., & Dixon, P. N. (1989). The effects of freshman orientation and locus of control on adjustment to college. Journal of College Student Development, 30, 362-367. Martin, N. K., & Dixon, P. N. (1994). The effects of freshman orientation and locus of control on adjustment to college: A follow-up study. Social Behavior and Personality, 22, 201208. Martin, W. E., Jr., Swartz-Kulstad, J. L., Hutz, A., & Fabian, T. (2000). Psychosocial correlates to college adjustment of first-year undergraduate students. Poster session, annual meeting of the Rocky Mountain Psychological Association, Tucson, Arizona. Martin, W. E., Jr., Swartz-Kulstad, J. L., & Madson, M. (1999). Psychosocial factors that predict the college adjustment of first-year undergraduate students: Implications for college counselors. Journal of College Counseling, 2, 121-133. Mathis, M., & Lecci, L. (1999). Hardiness and college adjustment: Identifying students in need of services. Journal of College Student Development, 40, 305-309. Maton, K. I. (1989a). The stress-buffering role of spiritual support: Cross sectional and prospective investigations. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 28, 310-323. Maton, K. I. (1989b). Unpublished raw data. Maton, K. I., & Weisman, A. G. (1989). College adjustment: Psychosocial predictors, and race 422 and gender moderators. Unpublished manuscript, University of Maryland, Baltimore County. Matthews, C. A. (1992). The impact of parental divorce on undergraduates: Its effects on adjustment to college. (Doctoral dissertation, Baylor University, 1992). Dissertation Abstracts International, 54, 464A. McAndrew-Miller, C. A. (1989). The relationships among temperament, attachment and initial adjustment to college. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland-College Park, 1989). Dissertation Abstracts International, 51, 3160B. McCartney, M. S. (1992). A survey comparing adjustment to college for in-state athletes and out-of-state athletes. Unpublished master's thesis, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill. McCubbin, H. I., Larsen, A., & Olson, D. (1985). F-Copes: Family Crisis Oriented Personal Evaluation Scales. In D. Olson, H. I. McCubbin, H. Barnes, A. Larsen, M. Muxen, & M. Wilson (Eds.), Family inventories (revised edition). St. Paul, MN: Family Social Science, University of Minnesota. McGillin, V. A. (1986, April). Identification and intervention with high risk students. Paper presented at the meeting of the American College Personnel Association, New Orleans, LA. McGowan, W. R. (1987). Unpublished raw data. McGowan, W. R. (1988). Freshman adjustment at the University of Mississippi: A comparison of the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire and the Student Developmental Task Inventory-Revised. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Mississippi, 1988). Dissertation 423 Abstracts International, 49, 1375A. Mehrabian, A., & Bank, L. (1978). A questionnaire measure of individual differences in achieving tendency. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 38, 475-478. Melendez, M. C. (2001). Contributions of gender, ethnic status, athletic participation, and athletic identity to college adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 2001). (get DAI info; DAI date may be 2002?) Mendelowitz, D. E. (1991). The effect of a group counseling and problem-solving model for college-bound high school seniors on perceived adjustment to college. (Doctoral dissertation, St. John's University, 1990). Dissertation Abstracts International, 52, 2028A. Mendelson, C. N. (1988). Adjustment to college: The role of perceptions of childhood interactions with parents, autonomy, and social support. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Maryland, 1987). Dissertation Abstracts International, 48, 3406B. Merryman, C. J., & Zelezny, L. C. (1993, April). College adjustment and its multidimensional association with depression and perfectionism. Poster presented at the meeting of the Western and Rocky Mountain Psychological Associations, Phoenix, AZ. Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 487-495. Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Tolmacz, R. (1990). Attachment styles and fear of personal death: A case study of affect regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 273-280. 424 Miller, L. H., Smith, A. D., & Mehler, B. L. (1987). The Stress Audit manual. Brookline, MA: Biobehavioral Associates, 1415 Beacon Street, 02146. Mitchell, J. V., Jr. (1984). Personality correlates of life values. Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 1-14. Montgomery, D. E., Fine, M. A., & James-Myers, L. (1990). The development and validation of an instrument to assess an optimal Afrocentric world view. Journal of Black Psychology, 17, 37-54. Montgomery, R. L., & Haemmerlie, F. M. (1993). Undergraduate adjustment to college, drinking behavior, and fraternity membership. Psychological Reports, 73, 801-802. Montgomery, R. L., & Haemmerlie, F. M. (2001). Unpublished raw data. Montgomery, R. L., Haemmerlie, F. M., & Amsden, C. (2001). Self-rated physical attractiveness and accompanying personal benefits. Paper presented at meetings of the Southwestern Psychological Association, Houston. Montgomery, R. L., Haemmerlie, F. M., Pyatt, W., & Laycock, M. (2000). Shyness and personal deficits. Paper presented at meetings of the Southwestern Psychological Association, Dallas, Texas. Montgomery, R. L., Haemmerlie, F. M., & Ray, D. (2001). The many benefits of optimism. Paper presented at meetings of the Southwestern Psychological Association, Houston. Montgomery, R. L., Haemmerlie, F. M., & Watkins, D. (2000). Personality and success in college. Paper presented at meetings of the Southwestern Psychological Association, Dallas, Texas. Montgomery, R. L., & Howdeshell, K. E. (1993). Concurrent validity of the Student Adaptation 425 to College Questionnaire. Paper presented at meetings of the Southwestern Psychological Association, Corpus Christi, Texas. Montgomery, R. L., & Zoellner, S. (1994). Self-handicapping and its relationship to college adjustment. Paper presented at meetings of the Southwestern Psychological Association, Tulsa, Oklahoma. Mooney, S. P. (1989). Locus of control, self-esteem, gender and geographical distance as predictors of college adjustment. Unpublished master's thesis, Loyola College, Baltimore, MD. Mooney, S. P., Sherman, M. F., & LoPresto, C. T. (1991). Academic locus of control, selfesteem, and perceived distance from home as predictors of college adjustment. Journal of Counseling and Development, 69, 445-448. Moos, R. H., Cronkite, R. C., Billings, A. G., & Finney, J. W. (1983). Health and Daily Living Form manual. (Available from Social Ecology Laboratory, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305). Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1981). Family Environment Scale manual. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. Morray, M. G., & Shilkret, R. B. (2001). The effects of intrusiveness, separation, and guilt on college adjustment. Unpublished manuscript, Mt. Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA. Morray, M. G., & Shilkret, R. B. (2002). “Mother, please! I’d rather do it myself!” Maternal intrusiveness, daughters’ guilt, and separation as related to college adjustment. Poster presented at meetings of the Society for Research on Adolescence, New Orleans. Mosley, D. L. (1990). The academic, social and emotional adjustment of Black students at 426 Howard University. Unpublished master's thesis, Howard University, Washington, D.C. Napoli, A. R., & Wortman, P. M. (1998). Psychosocial factors related to retention and early departure of two-year community college students. Research in Higher Education, 39, 419-455. Natera, L. (1998). Self-efficacy, stress, and adjustment in Latino college students. (Master’s thesis, University of Arizona, 1998). Master’s Abstracts International, 37, 0368. Neemann, J., & Harter, S. (1986). Manual for the Self-Perception Profile for College Students. Denver: University of Denver. Nigrosh, E. E. (1994). The role of conscious and unconscious guilt in adaptation to college. (Doctoral dissertation, Smith College School for Social Work, 1993). Dissertation Abstracts International, 55, 2144A. Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (1974). A locus of control scale for adults. Journal of Personality Assessment, 38, 136-137. O’Connor, L., Berry, J., Weiss, J., Bush, M., & Sampson, H. (1997). Interpersonal guilt: The development of a new measure. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53, 73-89. . Ogden, E. P., & Trice, A. D. (l986). Unpublished raw data. Oliver, J. M., Reed, C. K. S., & Smith, B. W. (1998). Patterns of psychological problems in university undergraduates: Factor structure of symptoms of anxiety and depression, physical symptoms, alcohol use, and eating problems. Social Behavior and Personality, 26, 211-232. Ollendick, T. H., Lease, C. A., & Cooper, C. (1993). Separation anxiety in young adults: A preliminary examination. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 7, 293-305. 427 Olson, D. H., Portner, J., & Lavee, Y. (1985). FACES-III. St. Paul: University of Minnesota. Olson, D. H., Sprenkle, D. H., & Russell, C. S. (1979). Circumplex model of marital and family systems: I. Cohesion and adaptability dimensions, family types, and clinical applications. Family Process, 18, 3-28. Olver, R. R., Aries, E., & Batgos, J. (1989). Self-other differentiation and the mother-child relationship: The effects of sex and birth order. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 150, 311321. Organizational Development Resources. (1996). The personal resilience profile handbook. Atlanta:ODR, Inc. Oritt, E. J., Paul, S. C., & Behrman, J. A. (1985). The Perceived Support Network Inventory. American Journal of Community Psychology, 13, 565-582. Ortiz, J. (1995). Parental responsibility, work, and social support: The academic adjustment and well-being of nontraditional women students. (Doctoral dissertation, Arizona State University, 1995). Dissertation Abstracts International, 56, 4721A. Osipow, S. H. (1987). Career Decision Scale. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Osipow, S. H., Carney, C. G., Winer, J. L., Yanico, B., & Koschier, M. (1976). The Career Decision Scale (3rd rev. ed.). Columbus, OH: Marathon Consulting Press. Ousley, L. B. (1986). Differences among bulimic subgroups, binge eaters, and normal eaters in a female college population. (Doctoral dissertation, University of California at Santa Barbara, 1986). Dissertation Abstracts International, 47, 2178B. Pace, C. R. (1987). College Student Experiences Questionnaire (CSEQ): Test manual and 428 norms. Los Angeles: UCLA Center for the Study of Evaluation. Palenzuela, D. L. (1988). Refining the theory and measurement of expectancy of internal versus external control of reinforcement. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 607-629. Pancer, S. M., Hunsberger, B., Pratt, M. W., & Alisat, S. (2000). Cognitive complexity of expectations and adjustment to university in the first year. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15, 38-57. Pancer, S. M., Pratt, M. W., Hunsberger, B. E., & Alisat, S. (1995). Great expectations: Parentchild discussion and the impact of pre-university expectations on adolescent transition to university. Paper presented at meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis. Panori, S. A. (1997). The effect of attention-deficit hyperactivity symptoms on well-being in college students: Implications for academic achievement and retention. Master’s thesis, California State University, San Bernardino. Pappas, E. (2000). The impact of family relationships on self-esteem and adjustment to college. Unpublished undergraduate thesis (honors? senior?), Connecticut College, New London, CT. Parham, T. A., & Helms, J. E. (1981). The influences of Black students' racial identity attitudes on preference for counselor's race. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 28, 250-257. Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A parental bonding instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52, 1- 10. Pascarella, E. T., and Terenzini, P. T. (!980). Predicting freshman persistence and voluntary drop-out decisions from a theoretical model. Journal of Higher Education, 51, 60-75. 429 Patterson, J. M., & McCubbin, H. I. (1987a). A-COPE: Adolescent coping orientation for problem experiences. In H. I. McCubbin & A. I. Thompson (Eds.), Family assessment inventories for research and practice (pp. 225-243). Madison, WI: The University of Wisconsin Press. Patterson, J. M., & McCubbin, H. I. (1987b). Adolescent coping style and behaviors: Conceptualization and measurement. Journal of Adolescence, 10, 163-186. Paulhus, D., & Christie, R. (1981). Spheres of control: An interactionist approach to assessment of perceived control. In H. M. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the locus of control construct: Assessment methods, Vol. 1, pp. 161-188. New York: Academic press. Paulhus, D., Molin, J., & Schuchts, R. (1979). Control profiles of football players, tennis players, and nonathletes. Journal of Social Psychology, 108, 199-205. Paulshock, S. B. (1994). An investigation of the predictors and effectiveness of selfadministered writing workbooks as a therapeutic treatment. (Doctoral dissertation, Boston College, 1994). Dissertation Abstracts International, 56, 1706B. Pearlin, L. I., & Schooler, C. (l978). The structure of coping. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, l9, 2-2l. Perosa, L. M., & Perosa, S. L. (1990a). The revision and validation of the Structural Family Interaction Scale. Paper presented at the 98th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Boston, MA. Perosa, L. M., & Perosa, S. L. (1990b). Convergent and discriminant validity for family selfreport measures. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 50, 855-868. Peterson, C., Semmel, A., von Baeyer, C., Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Seligman, M. E. 430 P. (1982). The Attributional Style Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6, 287-300. Pfeil, L. R. (2000). Parental attachment, attachment style and college adjustment in AfricanAmerican students. (Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers University, 2000). Dissertation Abstracts International, __, . Pfeil, L. R. (2002). Unpublished raw data. Phinney, J. S. (1992). The multigroup ethnic identity measure: A new scale for use with diverse groups. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7, 156-176. Plaud, J. J., Baker, R. W., & Groccia, J. E. (l986). Unpublished raw data. Plaud, J. J., Baker, R. W., & Groccia, J. E. (1990). Freshman decidedness regarding academic major and anticipated and actual adjustment to an engineering college. NACADA Journal, 10, 20-26. Pokorny, A. D., Miller, B. A., & Kaplan, H. B. (1972). The Brief MAST: A shortened version of the Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test. American Journal of Psychiatry, 129, 342-345. Polewchak, J. L. (1998). Interpersonal dependency and social support: Implications for emotional adjustment among entering college students. Master’s thesis, University of Dayton. Polewchak, J. L. (1999). Unpublished raw data. Posselt, E. P. (1992). Sense of coherence and hardiness: Effects on adjustment to college. (Doctoral dissertation, Boston University, 1992). Dissertation Abstracts International, 53, 0760A. Potter, A. E., & Williams, D. E. (1991). Development of a measure examining children’s roles 431 in alcoholic families. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 52, 70-77. Pratt, M. W. (2001). Unpublished raw data. Pratt, M. W., Hunsberger, B., Pancer, S. M., Alisat, S., Bowers, C., Mackey, K., Ostaniewicz, A., Rog, E., Terzian, B., & Thomas, N. (2000). Facilitating the transition to university: Evaluation of a social support discussion intervention program. Journal of College Student Development, 41, 427-441. Presley, C. A., Meilman, P. W., & Lyerla, R. (1993, 1995). Alcohol and drugs on American college campuses: Use, consequences, and perceptions of the campus environment. Vols. I & II: 1988-1990 and 1990-1992. Carbondale, IL: The Core Institute. Procidano, M. E., & Heller, K. (1983). Measures of perceived social support from friends and from family: Three validation studies. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 1-24. Protinsky, H. O., & Gilkey, J. K. (1996). An empirical investigation of the construct of personality authority in late adolescent women and their level of college adjustment. Adolescence, 31, 291-295. Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401. Ratta, M. D. (1994). Participation in college athletics and its effects on adjustment to college. B.A. thesis, Department of Psychology, Washington College, Chestertown, MD. Reed, C. K. S. (1994). Disordered behaviors among college students. (Doctoral dissertation, St. Louis University, 1994). Dissertation Abstracts International, 56, 2886B. Reeker, J. A. (1994). The relationship of family structure and physical and psychological 432 separation from parents on college student adjustment. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH. Reynolds, W. M. (1988). Measurement of academic self-concept in college students. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 223-240. Rice, K. G. (1991). Late adolescent attachment, separation-individuation, and adjustment to college: A time-sequential study. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Notre Dame, 1990). Dissertation Abstracts International, 52, 2474A. Rice, K. G. (1992). Separation-individuation and adjustment to college: A longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39, 203-213. Rice, K. G., Cole, D. A., & Lapsley, D. K. (1990). Separation-individuation, family cohesion, and adjustment to college: Measurement validation and test of a theoretical model. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37, 195-202. Rice, K. G., Cunningham, T. J., & Young, M. B. (1997). Attachment to parents, social competence, and emotional well-being: A comparison of Black and White late adolescents. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44, 89-101. Rice, K. G., FitzGerald, D. P., Whaley, T. J., & Gibbs, C. L. (1995). Cross-sectional and longitudinal examination of attachment, separation-individuation, and college student adjustment. Journal of Counseling and Development, 73, 463-474. Rice, K. G., & Whaley, T. J. (1994). A short-term longitudinal study of within-semester stability and change in attachment and college student adjustment. Journal of College Student Development, 35, 324-330. Ridinger, L. L. (1998). Acculturation antecedents and outcomes associated with international 433 and domestic student-athlete adjustment to college. (Doctoral dissertation, The Ohio State University, 1998). Dissertation Abstracts International, 59, 1488A. Rines, E. N. (1998). Moving from high school to college: The role of individual coping mechanisms and locus of control in students’ adjustment. Undergraduate honors research report, University of New Hampshire, Durham. Robbins, S. B., Lese, K. P., & Herrick, S. M. (1993). Interactions between goal instability and social support on college freshman adjustment. Journal of Counseling and Development, 71, 343-348. Robbins, S. B., & Patton, M. J. (1985). Self-psychology and career development: Construction of the Superiority and Goal Instability Scales. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32, 221-231. Robbins, S. B., & Schwitzer, A. M. (1988). Validity of the Superiority and Goal Instability Scales as predictors of women's adjustment to college life. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 21, 117-123. Roberts, R. E., Phinney, J. S., Masse, L. C., Chen, Y. R., Roberts, C. R., & Romero, A. (1999). The structure of ethnic identity in young adolescents from diverse ethnocultural groups. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 301-322. Roberts, Thomas W. (1995). Differentiation from the family of origin and adjustment to college in young adults. Journal of the Freshman Year Experience, 7, 53-66. Rocha-Singh, I. A. (1992). Doctoral students' perceptions of stress and social support: Implications for the retention of targeted students of color. (Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Barbara, 1990). Dissertation Abstracts International, 52, 434 3200A. Rodriguez, E. R. (1994). The role of psychological separation, ethnic identity, and worldview in college adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, Arizona State University, 1994). Dissertation Abstracts International, 55, 4105B. Rodriguez-Perez, L. (1991). Acculturation, self-esteem, and adjustment to college: Island and mainland Puerto Ricans. (Doctoral dissertation, Seton Hall University, 1991). Dissertation Abstracts International, 53, 0087A. Roid, G. H., & Fitts, W. H. (1988). Tennessee Self-Concept Scale, Revised Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Ropar, J. M. (1997). The relationship between family interaction, separation, childhood roles, and college student adjustment and drinking. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Akron, 1997). Dissertation Abstracts International, 58, 3025A. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Rosko, C. K. (1990). Separation-individuation and adjustment to college among bulimic and nonbulimic college women. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oklahoma, 1990). Dissertation Abstracts International, 51, 5039B. Rosko, C. K., Stoltenberg, C. D., & Allen, S. F. (1990). Perceptions of psychological separation and family environment in college bulimics. Unpublished manuscript, University of Oklahoma, Norman. Ross, J. M. (1995). College adjustment, familial characteristics, and beliefs about alcohol in adult children of alcoholics, other adults from dysfunctional families, and adults from 435 nondysfunctional families. (Master’s thesis, Lamar University-Beaumont, 1995). Master’s Abstracts International, 34, 1699. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80, 1-28. Russell, D. W., & Cutrona, C. E. (1984). The provisions of social relationships and adaptation to stress. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada. Russell, D. W., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 472-480. Salone, L. (1995). Relationships of Black racial identity attitudes and institutional fit with social adjustment to a small liberal arts college. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, 1995). Dissertation Abstracts International, 56, 0873A. Sandberg, D. A., & Lynn, S. J. (1992). Dissociative experiences, psychopathology and adjustment, and child and adolescent maltreatment in female college students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 717-723. Sandler, I. N., & Lakey, B. (1982). Locus of control as a stress moderator: The role of control perceptions and social support. American Journal of Community Psychology, 10, 65-80. Santonicola, A. (1989). Unpublished raw data. Saracoglu, B. N. (1987). Adjustment of learning disabled students to university as related to selfesteem and self-efficacy beliefs. Unpublished master's thesis, York University, North York, Ontario, Canada. 436 Saracoglu, B. N., Minden, H., & Wilchesky, M. (1989). The adjustment of students with learning disabilities to university and its relationship to self-esteem and self-efficacy. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 590-592. Sarason, I. G., Johnson, J. H., & Siegel, J. M. (1978). Assessing the impact of life changes: Development of the Life Experiences Survey. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 932-946. Sarason, I. G., Levine, H. M., Basham, R. B., & Sarason, B. R. (l983). Assessing social support: The Social Support Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, l27l39. Savino, F. (1987). Social support, psychological coping resources, interpersonal orientation, and living environment as predictors of adjustment in first-year college undergraduates. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1987). Dissertation Abstracts International, 48, 1802B. Savino, F., Reuter-Krohn, K., & Costar, D. (1986a, April). Freshman adjustment at UWMadison residence halls: A preliminary analysis. Paper presented at the meeting of the American College Personnel Association, New Orleans, LA. Savino, F., Reuter-Krohn, K., & Costar, D. (l986b). Unpublished raw data. Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Children’s reports of parental behavior: An inventory. Child Development, 36, 413-424. Schatzman, B. I. (1994). Effect of reminiscence therapy on nontraditional, undergraduate, college students’ self-esteem, self-confidence, and college adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri-Columbia, 1994). Dissertation Abstracts 437 International, 56, 0476A. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4, 219-247. Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063-1078. Schludermann, E. H., & Schludermann, S. M. (1988). Children’s Report on Parent Behavior for Older Children and Adolescents (CRPBI-108, CRPBI-30) (Tech. Rep.). Winnipeg, MB, Canada: University of Manitoba, Department of Psychology. Schmiedeck, R. (1974). The personal sphere model: A new projective tool. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 38, 113-128. Schriver, K. J. (1996). Predicting first-year college adjustment from behavioral indicators and the MMPI-2 College Maladjustment Scale. (Doctoral dissertation, Forest Institute of Professional Psychology, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, , . Schultheiss, D. E. P., & Blustein, D. L. (1994). Role of adolescent-parent relationships in college student development and adjustment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41, 248-255. Schwab, J. J., Bell, R. A., Warheit, G. J., & Schwab, R. S. (1979). Social order and mental health. New York: Brunner/Magel. Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 25, pp. 1-65). New York: Academic Press. 438 Schwitzer, A. M., McGovern, T. V., & Robbins, S. B. (1991). Adjustment outcomes of a freshman seminar: A utilization-focused approach. Journal of College Student Development, 32, 484-489. Schwitzer, A. M., & Robbins, S. B. (1986, April). Personality constructs as predictors of adjustment to college. Paper presented at the meeting of the American College Personnel Association, New Orleans, LA. Schwitzer, A. M., Robbins, S. B., & McGovern, T. V. (1993). Influences of goal instability and social support on college adjustment. Journal of College Student Development, 34, 2125. Scott, L. C. (1991). An investigation of social involvement, social adjustment, and academic achievement of Black undergraduates attending a four year predominantly White public institution of higher education in northeastern Massachusetts. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, 1991). Dissertation Abstracts International, 52, 441A. Sears, J., Brewer, T., & Szarlan, J. (2002). Unpublished raw data. Sennett, J. (2000). A preliminary investigation into the adjustment to university of first-year students at the University of Cape Town, with particular emphasis on the relative adjustment of black students. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa. Serafica, F. C., Allen, D., Brown, S., & Stewart, D. (1990, May). Prior interracial experience, social networks, and adaptation of Black students at predominantly White institutions. Poster presented at the meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago, IL. 439 Sherer, M., Maddox, J. E., Mercandante, B., Prentice-Dunn, S., Jacobs, B., & Rogers, R. W. (1982). The Self-Efficacy Scale: Construction and validation. Psychological Reports, 51, 663-671. Sherman, M. F. (1992). Unpublished raw data. Shibazaki, K. (1999). Ethnic identity, acculturation, perceived discrimination, and college adjustment in Mexican-American students. (Doctoral dissertation, Texas Tech University, 1999). Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 1872B. Shilkret, R. B. (2000). Relations between attachment style, parenting style, guilt, and college adjustment. Unpublished raw data. Shilkret, R. B., & Edwards, H. L. (1997). Effects of parenting style on separation and college adjustment. Unpublished manuscript, Mt. Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA. Shilkret, R. B., & Nigrosh, E. E. (1995). The assessment of students’ “Plans for College.” Unpublished raw data. Shilkret, R. B., & Nigrosh, E. E. (1997). Assessing students’ “Plans for College.” Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44, 222-231. Shilkret, R. B., & Taylor, E. L. (1992). College adjustment from a control mastery perspective: The development of a new questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, Mt. Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA. Shilkret, R. B., & Vecchiotti, S. (1995). Parenting styles and college adjustment. Unpublished paper, Mt. Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA. Shilkret, R. B., & Vecchiotti, S. (1997). Parenting styles, guilt, and college adjustment. Poster presented at meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD), 440 Washington, D. C., April. Siegelman, M., & Roe, A. (1979). Manual: The Parent-Child Relations Questionnaire II. Unpublished manuscript. Silver, A. R. (1995). College adjustment: Relationships to attachment security, separationindividuation, and style of coping. (Doctoral dissertation, Seton Hall University, 1995). Dissertation Abstracts International, 56, 2177A. Silverthorn, N. A. (1993). Predicting adjustment to university: Stress from parental pressure, independence from parents, and self-efficacy. Unpublished B.A. honours thesis, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario, Canada. Silverthorn, N. A., & Gekoski, W. L. (1995). Social desirability effects on measures of adjustment to university, independence from parents, and self-efficacy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 244-251. Simpson, J. A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles on romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 971-980. Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan J. S. (1992). Support seeking and support giving within couples in an anxiety-provoking situation: The role of attachment styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 434-446. Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, P. S., & Gelfand, M. J. (1995). Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement. Cross-Cultural Research, 29, 240-275. Slyter, S. L. (1991). Narcissistic Injury Scale (Rev. version). Unpublished instrument. Smith, A. W. (1994). Separation-individuation and coping: Contributions to freshman college 441 adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, 1994). Dissertation Abstracts International, 56, 0831A. Smith, M. A., & Baker, R. W. (1987). Freshman decidedness regarding academic major and adjustment to college. Psychological Reports, 61, 847-853. Smith, M. D. (1994). The College Attributional Complexity Scale: Development, validation, and relationship to university adjustment. (Master’s thesis, Wilfrid Laurier University, 1994). Master’s Abstracts International, 33, 1596. Sodowsky, G. R., Lai, E. W. M., & Plake, B. S. (1991). Moderating effects of sociocultural variables on acculturation attitudes of Hispanics and Asian Americans. Journal of Counseling and Development, 70, 194-204. Sodowsky, G. R., & Plake, B. S. (1991). Psychometric properties of the American International Relations Scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51, 207-216. Solberg, V. S., O’Brien, K., Villareal, P., Kennel, R., & Davis, B. (1993). Self-efficacy and Hispanic college students: Validation of the college self-efficacy instrument. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 15, 80-95. Solberg, V. S., Valdez, J., & Villareal, P. (1994). Social support, stress, and Hispanic college adjustment: Test of a diathesis-stress model. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 16, 230-239. Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R., & Stapp, J. (1975). Ratings of self and peers on sex role attributes and their relation to self-esteem and conceptions of masculinity and femininity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 29-39. Sperling, M. B., Berman, W. H., & Fagen, G. (1991). Assessment of adult attachment: An 442 integrative taxonomy from attachment and psychoanalytic theories. Paper presented at midwinter meeting of the Society for Personality Assessment (March), New Orleans, LA. Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI Form Y). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Steinberg, L., & Silverberg, S. B. (1986). The vicissitudes of autonomy in early adolescence. Child Development, 57, 841-851. Stinson, M. H., & Hendrick, S. S. (1992). Reported childhood sexual abuse in university counseling center clients. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39, 370-374. Stoltenberg, C. D., Garner, H. D., & Kell, K. (1986, April). Effects of marital and crossgenerational alliances on the adjustment and separation of late adolescents. Paper presented at the meeting of the American College Personnel Association, New Orleans, LA. Stone, A. A., & Neale, J. M. (1982). Development of a methodology for assessing daily experiences. In A. Baum & J. E. Singer (Eds.), Advances in environmental psychology: Volume 4. Environment and Health (pp. 49-183). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Straus, M. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The conflict tactics (CT) scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75-87. Strom, R. D., & Strom, S. K. (1989). Grandparent development. Washington, DC: American Association of Retired Persons Andrus Foundation. Sugar, L. A. (1997). An examination of parent-adolescent/young adult relationships, personality, and the university transition. Master’s thesis, York University, Canada. 443 Sugar, L. A. (1999). Unpublished raw data. Sullivan, C. F. (1991). Supportive communication: An investigation of the effects of the Freshman Interest Group Program on perceptions of support and college adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Washington, 1991). Dissertation Abstracts International, 52, 1944A. Super, D. E., & Nevill, D. D. (1985). The Salience Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. Sutton, L. (1996). Gender differences in sibling/parent relationships and adjustment. (Doctoral dissertation, Boston University, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 2183B. Swap, W. C., & Rubin, J. Z. (l983). Measurement of interpersonal orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 208-2l9. Tallis, F., Eysenck, M., & Mathews, A. (1992). A questionnaire for the measurement of nonpathological worry. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 161-168. Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P. E., & Gramzow, R. (1989). The Test of Self-Conscious Affect. Fairfax, VA: George Mason University. Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (in press). Exploring personality through test construction: Development of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. In S. R. Briggs & J. M. Cheek (Eds.), Personality measures: Development and evaluation (Vol. 1). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Templer, D. I. (1970). The construction and validation of a death anxiety scale. Journal of General Psychology, 82, 165-177. Terrell, M. D. (1989). The relationship of personal attributes, coping efforts, and social support 444 to adaptation to college. (Doctoral dissertation, Yale University, 1989). Dissertation Abstracts International, 51, 0462A. Tipton, R. M., & Worthington, E. L., Jr. (1984). The measurement of generalized self-efficacy: A study of construct validity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 545-548. Tolsdorf, C. C. (l976). Social networks, support, and coping: An exploratory study. Family Process, l5, 407-4l7. Tomlinson-Clarke, S. (1998). Dimensions of adjustment among college women. Journal of College Student Development, 39, 364-372. Tomlinson-Clarke, S., & Clarke, D. (1994). Predicting social adjustment and academic achievement for college women with and without precollege leadership. Journal of College Student Development, 35, 120-124. Trice, A. D. (l985). An academic locus of control scale for college students. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 6l, l043-l046. Trickett, E. J., & Moos, R. H. (1973). Social environment of junior high and high school classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 93-102. Tyler, F. B. (1978). Individual psychosocial competence: A personality configuration. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 38, 309-323. Veit, C. T., & Ware, J. E., Jr. (1983). The structure of psychological distress and well-being in general populations. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 730-742. Verdiano, D. L., Peterson, G. W., & Hicks, M. W. (1990). Toward an empirical confirmation of the Wegscheider role theory. Psychological Reports, 66, 723-730. Vitaliano, P. P. (1993). Revised Ways of Coping Checklist (RWCCL) and a dimensions of stress 445 scale (DSS) manual. Unpublished manuscript, University of Washington at Seattle. Vivona, J. M. (2000a). Parental attachment styles of late adolescents: Qualities of attachment relationships and consequences for adjustment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47, 316-329. Vivona, J. M. (2000b). Unpublished raw data. Walker, W. F., Jr. (1996). Family functioning and dysfunctional attitudes as predictors of adjustment to college in a first semester college population. (Doctoral dissertation, East Texas State University, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 2883A. Wang, E. Y. (1993). Women's adjustment to college and their perception of father's role. (Doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology, San Diego, 1993). Dissertation Abstracts International, 54, 4903B. Wang, E. Y., & Smith, T. L. (1993). Unpublished raw data. Washington, C. M. (1996). A study of early academic performance, attrition, and retention as related to selected cognitive, noncognitive, and adjustment variables for AfricanAmerican college students attending a private, open admission, historically Black institution. (Doctoral dissertation, University of South Carolina, Columbia). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 4671A. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063-1070. Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social- evaluative anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 448-457. 446 Wegscheider-Cruse, S. (1989). Another chance: Hope and health for the alcoholic family. Palo Alto, CA: Science and Behavior Books, Inc. Weinberg, N., & Sterritt, M. (1986). Disability and identity: A study of identity patterns in adolescents with hearing impairments. Rehabilitation Psychology, 31, 95-102. Weinstock, M. C. (1995). Adjustment to college: The influences of personality and family variables. Unpublished master’s level research project, State University of New York at Buffalo. Weiss, J., Sampson, H., & the Mt. Zion Psychotherapy Research Group (1986). The psychoanlaytic process. New York: Guilford Press. Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978). Development and validation of the dysfunctional attitudes scale: A preliminary investigation. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Toronto, Canada. West, M., & Sheldon, A. E. R. (1988). Classification of pathological attachment patterns in adults. Journal of Personality Disorders, 2, 153-159. Westen, D., Lohr, N., Silk, K., & Goodrich, S. (1990). Social cognition and object relations scale (SCORS): Manual for coding TAT data. University of Michigan, Department of Psychology, Ann Arbor. Wick, S., & Shilkret, R. B. (1986a). College adjustment and parental influence. Unpublished manuscript, Mount Holyoke College, South Hadley, MA. Wick, S., & Shilkret, R. B. (l986b). Unpublished raw data. Wildman, E. R. (1998). Helping first year students adjust to college: An evaluation of the Livingston College Advisor Seminar. (Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers the State University 447 of New Jersey, G. S. A. P. P., 1998). Dissertation Abstracts International, 59, 2857A. Williams, E. M. (1996). The effect of prior post-secondary institutional knowledge on college student disillusionment. (Doctoral dissertation, University of California at Los Angeles, 1996). Dissertation Abstracts International, 57, 0554A. Windle, M., & Lerner, R. M. (1986). Reassessing the dimensions of temperamental individuality across the life span: The Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS-R). Journal of Adolescent Research, 1, 213-230. Winefield, H. R., Winefield, A. H., & Tiggermann, M. (1992). Social support and psychological well-being in young adults: The Multi-Dimensional Support Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58, 198-210. Winston, R. B., Jr., Miller, T. K., & Prince, J. S. (1987). Student Developmental Task and Lifestyle Inventory. Athens, GA: Student Development Associates. Wintre, M. G., & Sugar, L. A. (2000). Relationships with parents, personality, and the university transition. Journal of College Student Development, 41, 202-214. Wintre, M. G., & Yaffe, M. (2000). First-year students’ adjustment to university life as a function of relationships with parents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15, 9-37. Wintre, M. G., Yaffe, M., & Crowley, J. (1995). Perception of Parental Reciprocity Scale (POPRS): Development and validation with adolescents and young adults. Social Development, 4, 129-148. Woo, T. O., & Bilynsky, J. (1994). Involvement in extracurricular activities and adjustment to college. Research/Technical Reports. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 378 474). 448 Yaffe, M. (1997). First-year university students’ adjustment to university life as a function of relationships with parents. (Doctoral dissertation, York University, Canada, 1997). Dissertation Abstracts International, 58, 5675B. Young, J. W. (1994). Predictors of minority students' academic achievement. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA. Yuker, H. E., Block, J. R., & Campbell, W. J. (1960). A scale to measure attitudes toward disabled persons. Albertson, NY: Human Resources Center. Zamostny, K. P., Slyter, S. L., & Rios, P. (1993). Narcissistic injury and its relationship to trauma, early resources, and adjustment to college. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40, 501-510. Zea, M. C. (1997). Unpublished raw data. Zea, M. C., Jarama, S. L., & Bianchi, F. T. (1995). Social support and psychosocial competence: Explaining the adaptation to college of ethnically diverse students. American Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 509-531. Zea, M. C., Reisen, C. A., & Tyler, F. B. (1996). Reliability, ethnic comparability, and validity evidence for a condensed measure of proactive coping: The BAPC-C. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 56, 330-343. Zimet, G. D., Dahlen, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K. (1988). The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 30-41. Zion, C. L. (1990). Facilitating adjustment to college: An evaluation of a peer-advising program in university housing. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Wisconsin, Madison. 449 aaa Abe, J., 88, 89, 150, 213 Abrahamson, L. Y., 47 Abramson, L. Y., 23, 249 Acunzo, M. E., 104, 213 Adams, G. R., 49, 231 Adan, A. M., 8, 20, 23, 30, 80, 82, 83, 101, 134, 135, 213 Addison, N. E., 22, 81, 95, 213 Ainsworth, M. D., 71, 213 Albert, M. M., 6, 8, 10, 51, 55, 81, 84, 92, 94, 95, 109, 119, 140, 213 Alisat, S., 44, 80, 102, 149, 239, 247 Allen, D., 80, 126, 127, 134, 140, 209, 210, 256 Allen, J. L., 5, 51, 55, 119, 122, 213 Allen, S. F., 9, 81, 119, 120, 214, 253 American College Testing Program, Research and Development Division, 214 Amerikaner, M., 34, 221 Ames, M. H., 94, 236 Anderson, W. T., 107, 234 Antonovsky, A., 34, 214 Appenzeller, E. A., 147, 214 Aries, E., 109, 214 Armsden, G. C., 61, 68, 214 Armstead, C. D., 22, 26, 103, 128, 184, 186, 191, 211, 215 Aydin, F., 19, 72, 73, 89, 90, 96, 105, 109, 111, 129, 177, 194, 199, 201, 204, 206, 207, 215 Bachman, J. G., 37, 215 Baik, J., 60, 61, 215 Bailey, J. S., 77, 119, 181, 182, 215 Baker, R. W., 1, 2, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 11, 12, 13, 14, 42, 50, 51, 52, 53, 55, 56, 66, 68, 80, 134, 135, 136, 137, 138, 142, 143, 153, 154, 155, 173, 174, 176, 194, 199, 211, 215, 216, 231, 249, 258 Bank, L., 48, 244 Barkley, R. A., 29, 217 Barrera, M., Jr., 127, 217 Barrett, J., 39, 75, 233 Bartels, K. M., 41, 48, 55, 72, 80, 90, 95, 102, 110, 123, 130, 177, 179, 183, 186, 188, 208, 210, 211, 217 Barthelemy, K. J., 81, 139, 208, 209, 217 Bartholomew, K., 72, 217 Basham, R. B., 128, 255 Batgos, J., 109, 214 Baum, A., 259 Beavers, W. R., 106, 108, 217 Beck, A. T., 20, 21, 22, 24, 29, 217, 218, 263 Behrman, J. A., 128, 247 Belgrave, F. Z., 102, 128, 201, 204, 236 Bell, R. A., 19, 256 Bem, S. L., 81, 218 Benedicto, J. A., 55, 188, 232 Benson, R. T., 149, 218 Berman, W. H., 142, 259 Bernstein, E. M., 27, 218 Berry, J., 77, 246 Berzonsky, M. D., 49, 218 Betz, N. E., 40, 218 450 Beyers, W., 5, 55, 57, 92, 188, 190, 218 Bianchi, F. T., 33, 45, 80, 82, 84, 85, 128, 178, 183, 201, 265 Billings, A. G., 26, 32, 34, 67, 218, 246 Bilynsky, J., 7, 80, 264 Blankstein, K. R., 101, 218 Blatt, S. J., 59, 219 Blehar, M. C., 71, 213 Block, J. R., 41, 264 Bloom, B. L., 107, 121, 219 Blustein, D. L., 50, 54, 55, 63, 69, 182, 188, 193, 237, 255 Bobier, D. M., 55, 81, 186, 187, 189, 219 Bonz, M. H., 31, 80, 240 Borow, H., 10, 219 Brady, T., 129, 220 Bragg, T. A., 10, 219 Braithwaite, V. A., 54, 219 Bray, J. H., 58, 219 Briere, J., 103, 219 Briggs, S. R., 260 Brooks, J. H., II, 19, 40, 66, 80, 90, 91, 94, 101, 127, 144, 177, 201, 220 Brower, A. M., 5, 9, 10, 35, 174, 176, 197, 220 Brown, D. A., 153, 220 Brown, G., 24, 217 Brown, L. B., 109, 111, 248 Brown, S., 80, 126, 127, 134, 140, 209, 210, 256 Brown, S. D., 40, 54, 80, 92, 129, 177, 184, 192, 220, 240 Brunig, C. A., 112, 220 Buchanan, S. R., 146, 220, 221 Buelow, G., 107, 117, 118, 119, 121, 122, 201, 202, 203, 209, 221 Burger, J. M., 66, 235 Buri, J. R., 112, 114, 221 Burks, N., 110, 124, 127, 243 Burr, M., 7, 9, 81, 82, 85, 94, 122, 221 Burrell, S., 6, 173, 230 Bush, M., 77, 246 Buss, A. H., 26, 33, 67, 74, 221, 227 Butler, W. M., 106, 221 Byrne, D. G., 133, 233 Camp, C. C., 5, 6, 40, 50, 51, 53, 91, 174, 179, 191, 192, 221, 222 Campbell, J. M., 34, 221 Campbell, V. L., 22, 55, 80, 81, 108, 116, 119, 242 Campbell, W. J., 41, 264 Caplan, S. M., 37, 106, 193, 203, 222 Carden, A. I., 8, 21, 46, 76, 183, 196, 223 Carlson, D. L., 5, 6, 8, 23, 49, 222 Carmen, R. C., 30, 232 Carney, C. G., 50, 247 Caro, J. E., 39, 55, 59, 75, 76, 109, 110, 111, 123, 133, 222 Carter, D. F., 7, 80, 84, 90, 91, 110, 125, 136, 140, 141, 144, 235 Carver, C. S., 31, 222 Chartrand, J. M., 5, 6, 40, 50, 51, 53, 91, 174, 179, 191, 192, 221, 222 Cheek, J. M., 260 Chevron, E., 59, 219 Chodoff, P., 39, 75, 233 451 Chosy, J., 28, 223 Christenson, R. M., 59, 222 Christie, R., 33, 67, 248 Clark, L. A., 22, 26, 262 Clarke, D., 7, 261 Clauss, K., 37, 55, 60, 106, 108, 179, 185, 188, 209, 223 Cline, D., 28, 223 Clingempeel, W. G., 80, 120, 228 Cochran, S. W., 107, 234 Cohen, S., 26, 44, 102, 131, 132, 223 Cole, D. A., 10, 25, 55, 56, 58, 69, 70, 105, 106, 179, 180, 183, 192, 197, 202, 203, 251 Collins, N. L., 70, 72, 223 Compas, B. E., 101, 223 Cook, S. W., 37, 81, 104, 123, 179, 208, 228 Cooler, J. R., 10, 25, 39, 57, 69, 70, 116, 117, 118, 119, 192, 193, 223 Cooley, E. L., 8, 21, 46, 76, 183, 196, 223 Cooper, C., 24, 25, 246 Cooper, S. A., 87, 224 Cooper, S. E., 137, 224 Coopersmith, S., 36, 37, 224 Corbett, C. A., 14, 83, 84, 96, 97, 129, 210, 211, 224 Costa, P. T., Jr., 27, 48, 66, 67, 99, 224 Costar, D., 36, 51, 74, 80, 128, 138, 139, 143, 148, 255 Crastnopol, M. G., 59, 224 Cronkite, R. C., 32, 246 Crouse, D. W., 36, 82, 93, 179, 224 Crowley, J., 64, 264 Crumbaugh, J., 50, 225 Cuellar, I., 85, 225 Cunningham, T. J., 8, 21, 39, 81, 83, 109, 111, 192, 198, 206, 207, 251 Currie, F., 80, 149, 239 Cutrona, C. E., 76, 110, 130, 225, 253, 254 Dahlen, N. W., 129, 265 Danielson, H., 80, 139, 225 Danilovics, P., 99, 228 Davies, M., 21, 237 Davis, B., 38, 41, 47, 258 Davis, E. R., 5, 37, 40, 41, 49, 150, 174, 180, 225 Davis, G. E., 101, 223 Derogatis, L. R., 18, 19, 20, 21, 24, 25, 37, 225 Dewein, K. M., 5, 45, 49, 55, 68, 75, 91, 94, 119, 120, 174, 177, 180, 185, 187, 188, 190, 193, 225 Diener, E., 22, 78, 226 DiGiuseppe, R., 45, 226 Dixon, P. N., 46, 148, 243 Dodgen-Magee, D. J., 20, 196, 226 Donaldson, G. A., 55, 237 DuBois, D. L., 25, 40, 66, 80, 90, 91, 94, 101, 127, 144, 177, 201, 220 Duke, M. P., 46, 246 Duncan-Jones, P., 133, 233 Edgerton, J., 38, 55, 59, 72, 73, 106, 109, 111, 179, 185, 189, 190, 202, 203, 206, 207, 226, 240 Edwards, H. L., 55, 77, 112, 113, 114, 115, 181, 188, 190, 206, 257 Elacqua, T. C., 7, 82, 140, 142, 147, 226 Ellison, C. W., 22, 226 Emery, G., 29, 217 452 Emmons, R. A., 22, 226 Endler, N. S., 31, 227 Epstein, N., 24, 217 Erbaugh, J., 20, 21, 22, 218 Erickson, C. J., 65, 115, 227 Evans-Hughes, G., 5, 7, 46, 80, 84, 174, 183, 227 Exner, T., 45, 226 Eysenck, H. J., 66, 227 Eysenck, S. B. G., 66, 227 Fackelman, P., 74, 75, 227 Fagen, G., 142, 259 Farber, S. S., 101, 227 Farley, G. K., 129, 265 Felner, R. D., 8, 20, 23, 30, 80, 82, 83, 101, 134, 135, 213, 227 Fenigstein, A., 74, 227 Fernandez, G., 99, 228 Fible, B., 36, 227 Fiese, B. H., 107, 227 Fine, M., 107, 234 Fine, M. A., 81, 139, 208, 209, 217, 239 Finney, J. W., 32, 246 Fitts, W. H., 30, 36, 37, 38, 39, 228, 253 FitzGerald, D. P., 62, 63, 68, 92, 240, 251 Flanagan, C., 60, 228 Flescher, M., 8, 18, 20, 24, 80, 101, 120, 133, 228 Fletcher, G. J. O., 99, 228 Flett, G. L., 78, 101, 218, 233 Florian, V., 73, 245 Folkman, S., 44, 228 Forsythe, C. J., 101, 223 Foster, T. R., 5, 30, 36, 38, 39, 141, 173, 193, 194, 228 Franco, J. N., 80, 84, 236 Frazier, P. A., 37, 81, 104, 123, 179, 208, 228 Freeman, S. K., 8, 55, 229 Friedland, C. D., 30, 41, 45, 80, 140, 177, 178, 229 Friend, R., 76, 262 Fuligni, A., 60, 228 Fuller, B. E., 37, 47, 129, 229 Gallant, L. M., 14, 31, 80, 102, 129, 179, 202, 211, 229 Garbarino, C., 81, 121, 229 Garner, D. M., 27, 36, 229 Garner, H. D., 55, 105, 116, 229, 259 Garrett, S. E., 36, 229 Geelhoed, R. J., 88, 89, 150, 213 Gekoski, W. L., 80, 183, 186, 258 Gerdes, H., 5, 9, 10, 12, 80, 137, 176, 230 Gibbs, C. L., 63, 92, 251 Gilkey, J. K., 5, 9, 28, 37, 57, 93, 94, 112, 173, 186, 191, 198, 230, 250 Gilleylen, C. E., 93, 230 Ginsburg, G., 106, 221 Gold, J., 6, 173, 230 Goldberg, L. R., 25, 40, 67, 230 Goodrich, S., 74, 263 Goossens, L., 5, 55, 57, 92, 188, 190, 218 453 Gore, P. A., 40, 54, 80, 92, 177, 184, 192, 240 Gottfredson, D. C., 50, 234 Gottlieb, B. H., 217 Gough, H. G., 30, 39, 75, 231, 233 Graham, C., 13, 134, 135, 231 Gramzow, R., 78, 260 Grasley, C., 101, 231 Green, C. J., 25, 39, 57, 59, 69, 241 Greenberg, M. T., 61, 68, 214 Greenberger, E., 58, 231 Grella, R. S., 6, 81, 108, 173, 203, 204, 231 Griffen, S., 22, 226 Groccia, J. E., 6, 50, 51, 52, 134, 137, 249 Grochowski, J. R., 129, 231 Grotevant, H. D., 49, 231 Hackett, G., 40, 218 Haemmerlie, F. M., 7, 8, 30, 55, 79, 93, 94, 141, 188, 231, 232, 245 Hale, W. D., 36, 227 Hall, S., 129, 220 Hampson, R. B., 106, 108, 217 Handal, P. J., 81, 95, 140, 242 Hansburg, H. G., 58, 232 Harder, D. H., 77, 78, 232 Harris, B. C., 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 76, 80, 92, 126, 127, 173, 191, 210, 232 Harris, L. C., 85, 225 Harste, J. M., 61, 68, 69, 182, 232 Harter, S., 25, 40, 67, 246 Haynes, C., 6, 173, 230 Hays, R. B., 110, 124, 126, 127, 232 Hazan, C., 70, 72, 73, 232 Heller, K., 67, 110, 123, 250 Helmreich, R., 39, 81, 233, 259 Helms, J. E., 84, 248 Henderson, S., 133, 233 Hendrick, S. S., 104, 259 Heppner, M. J., 37, 47, 129, 229 Heppner, P. P., 25, 40, 67, 233 Hermans, H. J. M., 38, 47, 48, 233 Herrick, S. M., 49, 80, 82, 131, 180, 204, 252 Hertel, J. B., 35, 54, 90, 96, 123, 186, 208, 233 Hewitt, P. L., 78, 233 Hicks, M. W., 116, 262 Hirschfeld, R. M. A., 39, 75, 233 Hoberman, H. M., 131, 132, 223 Hoffman, J. A., 55, 59, 233 Hogan, M. P., 6, 8, 21, 81, 101, 104, 110, 123, 144, 234 Holahan, C. J., 32, 234 Holland, J. L., 50, 234 Hollmann, N. C., 49, 50, 55, 107, 180, 187, 190, 203, 234 Holmes, T. H., 102, 234 Hopkins, K. R., 106, 203, 234 Horowitz, L. M., 72, 217 Hovestadt, A. J., 107, 234 Hui, C. H., 74, 234 454 Humfleet, G. L., 6, 7, 8, 19, 22, 55, 112, 173, 187, 189, 198, 234, 235 Hunsberger, B. E., 42, 43, 44, 80, 92, 102, 149, 235, 239, 247 Hurlbut, S. C., 79, 235 Hurtado, S., 7, 80, 84, 90, 91, 110, 125, 136, 140, 141, 144, 235 Ibrahim, F. A., 100, 235 Injejikian, A. M., 105, 235 Jackson, L. M., 42, 43, 92, 235 Jacobs, B., 37, 39, 257 Jagels, C. T., 66, 235 Jampol, R. C., 8, 22, 24, 44, 46, 48, 80, 183, 186, 187, 196, 199, 235 Jarama Alvan, S. L., 102, 128, 201, 204, 236 Jarama, S. L., 33, 45, 80, 82, 84, 85, 127, 178, 183, 201, 265 Jasso, R., 85, 225 Jenkins, C. D., 30, 236 Johansson, C. B., 51, 236 Johnson, J. H., 101, 103, 236, 254 Jones, C. R., 47, 236 Josselson, R., 58, 231 Kaase, K. J., 9, 236 Kaczmarek, P. G., 80, 84, 94, 236 Kahn, H., 100, 235 Kahn, S., 34, 35, 238 Kamarck, T., 26, 44, 102, 131, 132, 223 Kambach, M., 131, 204, 236 Kandel, D. B., 21, 237 Kane, M. A., 93, 237 Kaplan, H. B., 121, 249 Kazdin, A. E., 236 Keenan, J. T., 6, 7, 10, 11, 146, 174, 237 Kell, K., 55, 105, 116, 259 Kennel, R., 38, 41, 47, 258 Kenny, M. E., 8, 9, 18, 20, 24, 27, 36, 39, 55, 59, 60, 81, 82, 85, 86, 108, 110, 123, 124, 127, 185, 187, 189, 194, 198, 203, 204, 237, 238 Kim, D. D., 81, 82, 238 Kim, E. J., 19, 21, 76, 131, 193, 196, 199, 204, 238 Kincannon, J. C., 19, 22, 238 Kintner, D. E., 46, 137, 149, 238 Kleinmuntz, B., 11, 238 Klerman, G. L., 39, 75, 233 Kline, C. A., 7, 23, 55, 80, 90, 95, 107, 122, 123, 144, 188, 189, 199, 203, 227, 238 Knerr, B., 58, 231 Knerr, C., 58, 231 Kobasa, S. C., 34, 35, 238 Koledin, S., 101, 218 Korchin, S. J., 39, 75, 233 Koschier, M., 50, 247 Koutralakos, J., 112, 239 Krotseng, M. V., 9, 239 Kulley, J. C., 70, 71, 72, 81, 239 Kurdek, L. A., 139, 239 Kusaka, T., 74, 88, 93, 180, 239 Lahey, B. B., 236 Lakey, B., 102, 254 Lamothe, D., 80, 149, 239 455 Lapan, R., 68, 76, 239 Lapsley, D. K., 10, 25, 38, 39, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59, 62, 68, 69, 70, 72, 73, 80, 92, 105, 106, 109, 111, 179, 180, 183, 185, 187, 189, 190, 192, 197, 202, 203, 206, 207, 226, 240, 251 Larsen, R. J., 22, 78, 226 Lavee, Y., 105, 247 Lazarus, R. S., 44, 45, 228 Leaf, R., 45, 226 Lease, C. A., 24, 25, 246 Lee, J. Y., 19, 21, 76, 131, 193, 196, 199, 204, 238 Lefebvre, R. C., 9, 28, 240 Lennon, K., 93, 237 Lent, R. W., 6, 40, 51, 54, 80, 92, 129, 173, 177, 184, 192, 220, 240 Leonard, J. B., 6, 51, 52, 134, 240 Leong, F. T. L., 31, 32, 80, 240 Lese, K. P., 49, 80, 82, 131, 180, 204, 252 Levenson, H., 46, 240 Levin, M. A., 55, 63, 116, 119, 120, 182, 188, 241 Levine, H. M., 128, 255 Levine, J. B., 25, 39, 57, 59, 69, 241 Lichtman, M., 5, 9, 28, 37, 57, 93, 94, 173, 186, 191, 198, 230 Lipsmeyer, M. E., 95, 241 Liter, S. D., 25, 50, 68, 80, 91, 177, 190, 191, 192, 197, 241 Lohr, N., 74, 263 Lopez, F. G., 6, 8, 22, 24, 50, 53, 55, 56, 80, 81, 94, 108, 109, 111, 116, 119, 142, 184, 187, 189, 199, 203, 204, 206, 207, 237, 241, 242 LoPresto, C. T., 36, 48, 144, 177, 179, 245 Loveland, D., 6, 79, 92, 94, 140, 174, 242 Low, C. A., 81, 95, 140, 242 Lustman, P. J., 21, 101, 102, 242 Lyerla, R., 79, 250 Lynn, S. J., 8, 27, 254 Maddi, S. R., 34, 35, 238 Maddox, J. E., 37, 39, 257 Mahalick, L., 50, 225 Malone, P. E., 58, 219 Marcotte, G. M. R., 6, 27, 31, 41, 46, 55, 101, 104, 129, 174, 179, 183, 187, 188, 190, 197, 202, 207, 208, 211, 243 Marcy, T. P., 6, 42, 51, 52, 80, 92, 137, 243 Martin, B., 110, 124, 127, 243 Martin, N. K., 10, 46, 51, 148, 243 Matlock, C. G., 80, 84, 94, 236 Maton, K. I., 6, 8, 21, 50, 80, 82, 83, 90, 91, 95, 102, 110, 123, 133, 174, 184, 191, 196, 204, 208, 243, 244 Matthews, C. A., 119, 244 McCartney, M. S., 144, 244 McCrae, R. R., 27, 48, 66, 67, 99, 224 McCubbin, H. I., 32, 129, 231, 248 McFadden, K. L., 5, 6, 40, 50, 51, 53, 91, 174, 179, 191, 192, 222 McGillin, V. A., 6, 244 McGovern, T. V., 49, 132, 148, 256 McGowan, W. R., 6, 51, 53, 66, 68, 81, 82, 177, 244 McNeil, O. V., 9, 11, 12, 42, 136, 137, 138, 216 Mehler, B. L., 9, 26, 28, 245 Mehrabian, A., 48, 244 Meilman, P. W., 79, 250 456 Melisaratos, N., 18, 21, 24, 25, 225 Mendelowitz, D. E., 81, 137, 145, 244 Mendelson, C. N., 111, 128, 208, 245 Mendelson, M., 20, 21, 22, 218 Mercandante, B., 37, 39, 257 Mermelstein, R., 26, 44, 102, 131, 132, 223 Merryman, C. J., 20, 78, 79, 184, 196, 245 Merz, C. J., 30, 231 Mesta, R., 94, 236 Metalsky, G. I., 23, 47, 249 Metzler, A. E., 49, 107, 180, 203, 234 Mikulincer, M., 73, 245 Miller, B. A., 121, 249 Miller, L. H., 9, 26, 28, 245 Miller, T. K., 50, 54, 69, 264 Millon, T., 25, 39, 57, 59, 69, 241 Minden, H., 28, 37, 254 Mitchell, J. V., Jr., 48, 95, 245 Mock, J., 20, 21, 22, 218 Molin, J., 33, 67, 248 Montgomery, R. L., 7, 8, 79, 93, 94, 141, 232, 245 Mooney, S. P., 36, 48, 80, 144, 177, 179, 245 Moos, B. S., 106, 246 Moos, R. H., 26, 32, 34, 67, 106, 132, 218, 234, 246, 261 Mosley, D. L., 135, 246 Mt. Zion Psychotherapy Research Group, 77, 263 Nardecchia, D., 6, 173, 230 Neale, J. M., 103, 259 Neeman, J., 25, 40, 67, 246 Nevill, D. D., 53, 260 Nigrosh, E. E., 8, 14, 18, 21, 24, 25, 77, 78, 181, 194, 196, 246, 257 Nowicki, S., 46, 246 O'Brien, K., 38, 41, 47, 258 O'Connor, L., 77, 246 Ogden, E. P., 6, 48, 246 Oh, K. J., 19, 21, 76, 131, 193, 196, 199, 204, 238 O'Hara, D. J., 21, 101, 102, 242 Oliver, J. M., 8, 9, 22, 24, 26, 27, 28, 79, 196, 197, 198, 199, 246 Ollendick, T. H., 24, 25, 246 Olson, D. H., 105, 247 Olver, R. R., 109, 214 O'Malley, P. M., 37, 215 Oritt, E. J., 128, 247 Ortiz, J., 95, 96, 130, 210, 247 Osipow, S. H., 38, 47, 50, 247 Ousley, L. B., 27, 247 Oxley, D., 110, 124, 126, 127, 232 Pace, C. R., 11, 247 Palenzuela, D. L., 38, 47, 247 Pancer, S. M., 42, 43, 44, 80, 92, 102, 149, 235, 239, 247 Panori, S. A., 29, 90, 93, 94, 198, 199, 248 Parham, T. A., 84, 248 Parker, G., 109, 111, 248 Parker, J. D. A., 31, 227 457 Patterson, J. M., 32, 248 Patton, M. J., 45, 49, 68, 76, 132, 239, 252 Paul, S. C., 128, 247 Paulhus, D., 33, 67, 248 Paulshock, S. B., 80, 155, 248 Pearlin, L. I., 36, 249 Peplau, L. A., 76, 254 Perosa, L. M., 106, 108, 110, 116, 249 Perosa, S. L., 106, 108, 110, 116, 249 Petersen, C. H., 25, 40, 67, 233 Peterson, C., 23, 47, 249 Peterson, D., 99, 228 Peterson, G. W., 116, 262 Petrosky, G., 93, 237 Phinney, J. S., 85, 249 Piercy, F. P., 107, 234 Plake, B. S., 90, 105, 258 Plaud, J. J., 6, 50, 51, 52, 134, 137, 249 Plomin, R., 26, 33, 67, 221 Pokorny, A. D., 121, 249 Polewchak, J. L., 39, 75, 81, 124, 125, 130, 181, 205, 249 Portner, J., 105, 247 Posselt, E. P., 34, 35, 81, 94, 102, 180, 192, 250 Potter, A. E., 117, 250 Power, P. G., 50, 234 Pratt, M. W., 42, 43, 44, 80, 92, 102, 149, 235, 239, 247 Prentice-Dunn, S., 37, 39, 257 Presley, C. A., 79, 250 Prince, J. S., 50, 54, 69, 264 Procidano, M. E., 67, 110, 123, 250 Protinsky, H. O., 5, 9, 28, 37, 57, 93, 94, 112, 173, 186, 191, 198, 230, 250 Putnam, F. W., 27, 218 Quinlan, D. M., 59, 219 Radloff, L. S., 20, 250 Rahe, R. H., 102, 234 Ratta, M. D., 141, 250 Read, S. J., 70, 72, 223 Reed, C. K. S., 8, 9, 22, 24, 26, 27, 28, 79, 80, 196, 197, 198, 199, 246, 250 Reeder, G. D., 99, 228 Reeker, J. A., 7, 55, 80, 82, 85, 93, 94, 110, 119, 143, 188, 250 Reisen, C. A., 33, 178, 179, 265 Reuter-Krohn, K., 36, 51, 74, 80, 128, 138, 139, 143, 148, 255 Reynolds, W. M., 54, 251 Ribordy, S. C., 6, 8, 19, 22, 173, 198, 235 Rice, K. G., 8, 10, 14, 21, 25, 39, 55, 56, 58, 62, 63, 64, 68, 69, 70, 80, 81, 83, 92, 105, 106, 109, 111, 179, 180, 182, 183, 187, 188, 189, 190, 191, 192, 197, 198, 202, 203, 206, 207, 240, 251 Ridinger, L. L., 7, 40, 41, 81, 82, 89, 92, 93, 98, 99, 131, 140, 142, 143, 144, 251 Rines, E. N., 23, 32, 47, 178, 180, 252 Rios, P., 19, 37, 103, 132, 184, 196, 202, 264 Robbins, S. B., 45, 49, 55, 80, 82, 119, 131, 132, 148, 180, 193, 204, 252, 256 Robin, M. W., 45, 226 Robinson, D. A. G., 30, 137, 224, 232 Robinson, J. P., 219 Rocha-Singh, I. A., 252 458 Rodriguez, E. R., 55, 80, 84, 85, 90, 94, 100, 122, 123, 183, 184, 188, 190, 252 Rodriguez-Perez, L., 36, 88, 179, 252 Roe, A., 111, 257 Rogers, R. W., 37, 39, 257 Roid, G. H., 36, 37, 38, 39, 253 Ropar, J. M., 55, 79, 80, 106, 108, 110, 116, 117, 118, 119, 189, 202, 253 Rosenberg, M., 26, 35, 36, 37, 67, 253 Rosenman, R. H., 30, 236 Rosko, C. K., 9, 27, 119, 214, 253 Ross, J. M., 81, 122, 253 Ross, M., 94, 236 Rotter, J. B., 45, 46, 253 Rubin, J. Z., 74, 260 Runtz, M., 103, 219 Rush, A. J., 29, 217 Russell, C. S., 105, 247 Russell, D. W., 76, 130, 225, 253, 254 Saintonge, S., 57, 241 Saling, C., 79, 232 Salone, L., 80, 83, 93, 141, 254 Sampson, H., 77, 246, 263 Sandberg, D. A., 8, 27, 254 Sandford, S. L., 9, 28, 240 Sandler, I. N., 102, 254 Sandvik, E., 78, 226 Santonicola, A., 6, 7, 9, 176, 254 Saracoglu, B. N., 28, 37, 38, 80, 254 Sarason, B. R., 128, 223, 255 Sarason, I. G., 101, 103, 128, 223, 254, 255 Savino, F., 7, 36, 51, 74, 80, 128, 138, 139, 143, 148, 211, 255 Schatzman, B. I., 155, 255 Scheier, M. F., 31, 74, 222, 227 Schmiedeck, R., 75, 255 Schooler, C., 36, 249 Schriver, K. J., 7, 11, 80, 92, 144, 149, 255 Schuchts, R., 33, 67, 248 Schulenberg, J., 60, 228 Schultheiss, D. E. P., 50, 54, 55, 63, 69, 182, 188, 193, 255 Schultz, K. L., 11, 12, 42, 137, 154, 155, 216 Schwab, J. J., 19, 256 Schwab, R. S., 19, 256 Schwartz, S. H., 256 Schwitzer, A. M., 45, 49, 55, 119, 132, 148, 180, 193, 252, 256 Scott, L. C., 11, 80, 93, 136, 141, 256 Scott, R., 133, 233 Scott, W. A., 54, 219 Seligman, M. E. P., 23, 47, 249 Semmel, A., 23, 47, 249 Serafica, F. C., 80, 126, 127, 134, 140, 209, 210, 256 Shadid, G. E., 55, 80, 92, 187, 189, 240 Shaver, P., 70, 72, 73, 232 Shaver, P. R., 219 Shaw, B. F., 29, 217 Sher, K. J., 79, 235 459 Sherer, M., 37, 39, 257 Sherman, M. F., 14, 36, 48, 55, 144, 177, 179, 187, 189, 245, 257 Shilkret, R. B., 6, 7, 8, 14, 18, 21, 24, 25, 46, 55, 59, 65, 74, 75, 77, 78, 109, 110, 112, 113, 114, 115, 119, 181, 182, 183, 184, 186, 188, 190, 194, 196, 206, 215, 227, 257, 263 Siegel, J. M., 101, 103, 254 Siegelman, M., 111, 257 Silk, K., 74, 263 Silver, A. R., 32, 55, 63, 182, 183, 189, 190, 191, 257 Silverthorn, N. A., 36, 55, 80, 180, 183, 186, 187, 188, 189, 190, 258 Singer, J. E., 259 Siryk, B., 1, 2, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 11, 12, 42, 53, 66, 68, 80, 136, 137, 138, 153, 154, 155, 174, 176, 194, 199, 211, 216 Slyter, S. L., 19, 37, 103, 132, 184, 196, 202, 258, 264 Smith, A. D., 9, 26, 28, 245 Smith, A. W., 32, 56, 59, 177, 258 Smith, B. W., 8, 9, 22, 24, 26, 27, 28, 79, 196, 197, 198, 199, 246 Smith, M. A., 6, 51, 134, 142, 143, 216, 258 Smith, M. D., 8, 20, 21, 26, 35, 99, 101, 178, 191, 196, 197, 199, 201, 258 Smith, T. L., 6, 8, 20, 23, 25, 48, 55, 59, 69, 70, 90, 95, 112, 174, 177, 180, 188, 192, 196, 199, 204, 208, 262 Sodowsky, G. R., 90, 105, 258 Solberg, V. S., 8, 38, 41, 47, 85, 130, 258, 259 Sowa, C. J., 21, 101, 102, 242 Spence, J. T., 81, 259 Sperling, M. B., 142, 259 Spielberger, C. D., 23, 24, 259 Sprenkle, D. H., 105, 247 Spuler, A., 7, 80, 84, 90, 91, 110, 125, 136, 140, 141, 144, 235 Stapp, J., 39, 81, 233, 259 Steen, S. C., 55, 188, 232 Steer, R. A., 24, 217 Stein, P. N., 80, 120, 228 Sterritt, M., 96, 263 Stewart, D., 80, 126, 127, 134, 140, 209, 210, 256 Stinson, M. H., 104, 259 Stoltenberg, C. D., 9, 55, 105, 116, 119, 214, 253, 259 Stone, A. A., 103, 259 Strange, C., 81, 121, 229 Straus, M., 110, 259 Strickland, B. R., 46 Strom, R. D., 65, 115, 260 Strom, S. K., 65, 115, 260 Stryker, S., 81, 82, 85, 86, 110, 123, 124, 237, 238 Sugar, L. A., 6, 8, 9, 27, 48, 49, 64, 65, 66, 67, 81, 99, 112, 113, 114, 174, 184, 185, 186, 205, 206, 260 Sullivan, C. F., 42, 66, 124, 151, 152, 193, 260 Sullivan, T., 80, 149, 239 Super, D. E., 53, 260 Sutton, L., 9, 22, 28, 37, 74, 76, 78, 191, 192, 196, 197, 199, 260 Swank, P., 34, 221 Swap, W. C., 74, 260 Talbot, D. M., 88, 89, 150, 213 Tangney, J. P., 78, 260 Taylor, E. L., 46, 65, 77, 183, 184, 186, 257 Tellegen, A., 22, 26, 27, 260, 262 Templer, D. I., 25, 261 460 Terrell, M. D., 6, 8, 26, 33, 34, 67, 80, 91, 174, 183, 184, 191, 192, 261 Thompson, A. I., 231, 248 Tiggermann, M., 124, 126, 130, 264 Tipton, R. M., 37, 40, 41, 261 Tolmacz, R., 73, 245 Tolsdorf, C. C., 133, 261 Tomlinson-Clarke, S., 6, 7, 82, 92, 261 Trice, A. D., 6, 48, 246, 261 Trickett, E. J., 132, 261 Tupling, H., 109, 111, 248 Tyler, F. B., 33, 178, 179, 261, 265 Valdez, J., 8, 85, 130, 259 Vecchiotti, S., 77, 112, 113, 114, 181, 206, 257 Veit, C. T., 18, 20, 24, 262 Verdiano, D. L., 116, 262 Villareal, P., 8, 38, 41, 47, 85, 130, 258, 259 Vincent, K., 34, 221 Vitaliano, P. P., 32, 262 von Baeyer, C., 23, 47, 249 Wagner, B. M., 101, 223 Wagner, P. E., 78, 260 Walker, W. F., Jr., 29, 105, 197, 202, 262 Wall, S., 71, 213 Waller, N. G., 22, 27, 260 Wang, E. Y., 6, 8, 20, 23, 25, 48, 55, 59, 69, 70, 90, 95, 112, 174, 177, 180, 188, 192, 196, 199, 204, 208, 262 Wapner, S., 13, 134, 135, 231 Ward, C. H., 20, 21, 22, 218 Ware, J. E., Jr., 18, 20, 24, 262 Warheit, G. J., 19, 256 Washington, C. M., 6, 81, 131, 173, 262 Waters, E., 71, 213 Watkins, C. E., Jr., 22, 55, 80, 81, 108, 116, 119, 242 Watson, D., 22, 26, 76, 262 Wegscheider-Cruse, S., 116, 117, 263 Wein, S. J., 59, 219 Weinberg, N., 96, 263 Weinstock, M. C., 35, 48, 55, 177, 188, 191, 263 Weintraub, J. K., 31, 222 Weisman, A. G., 6, 8, 21, 50, 80, 83, 102, 123, 184, 196, 204, 208, 244 Weiss, J., 77, 246, 263 Weissman, A. N., 29, 263 Westen, D., 74, 263 Whaley, T. J., 63, 64, 92, 190, 191, 251 Wick, S., 6, 7, 59, 109, 110, 111, 263 Wilchesky, M., 28, 37, 254 Williams, D. E., 117, 250 Williams, E. M., 6, 12, 42, 82, 85, 90, 91, 97, 98, 137, 174, 191, 192, 263 Williamson, D. S., 58, 219 Wilson, W. P., 59, 222 Winefield, A. H., 124, 126, 130, 264 Winefield, H. R., 124, 126, 130, 264 Winer, J. L., 50, 247 Winston, R. B., Jr., 50, 54, 69, 264 Wintre, M. G., 8, 27, 49, 64, 65, 66, 67, 99, 112, 113, 114, 184, 185, 186, 205, 206, 260, 264 461 Wolfert, J., 129, 220 Woo, T. O., 7, 80, 264 Worthington, E. L., Jr., 37, 40, 41, 261 Wrightsman, L. S., 219 Yaffe, M., 6, 8, 20, 26, 35, 36, 44, 58, 60, 64, 65, 81, 90, 110, 112, 113, 114, 119, 140, 174, 183, 185, 186, 190, 191, 196, 199, 205, 206, 210, 264 Yanico, B., 50, 247 Young, J. W., 6, 86, 174, 264 Young, M. B., 8, 21, 39, 81, 83, 109, 111, 192, 198, 206, 207, 251 Yuker, H. E., 41, 264 Zachar, P., 31, 80, 240 Zalma, A., 77, 78, 232 Zamostny, K. P., 19, 37, 103, 132, 184, 196, 202, 264 Zea, M. C., 33, 45, 80, 82, 84, 85, 102, 127, 128, 178, 179, 183, 201, 204, 236, 264, 265 Zelezny, L. C., 20, 78, 79, 184, 196, 245 Zimet, G. D., 129, 265 Zimet, S. G., 129, 265 Zion, C. L., 149, 265 Zyzanski, S. J., 30, 236 Abuse physical, 103, 122 psychological, 103 sexual, 103, 104, 122, 166, 168 Abuse subscale (from the Early Trauma Checklist), 202 Academic accomplishment. (see Academic performance) Academic achievement subscale (from the College Life Task Assessment Instrument), 197 Academic adjustment, 5, 6, 12, 17, 21, 23, 26, 29, 31, 32, 33, 35, 40, 41, 46, 47, 49, 51, 53, 54, 55, 57, 62, 63, 64, 66, 67, 69, 71, 75, 79, 83, 85, 89, 90, 91, 92, 95, 96, 97, 98, 99, 100, 103, 107, 109, 111, 113, 114, 115, 116, 1 17, 122, 125, 126, 128, 130, 132, 137, 139, 140, 141, 149, 152, 156, 241 anticipated vs actual, 42 aspects of, 4, 17, 156 behavioral correlates of, 5, 6, 7 Academic Adjustment subscale, 37 shortened version of, 195, 200 Academic Adjustment subscale (from the SACQ), 3, 6, 9, 10, 12, 13, 14, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 42, 44, 45, 46, 47, 48, 49, 50, 51, 52, 53, 54, 55, 56, 57, 58, 61, 62, 63, 65, 66, 68, 70, 71, 73, 74, 75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 84, 85, 86, 87, 89, 91, 92, 93, 94, 95, 96, 97, 98, 100, 101, 103, 104, 105, 106, 108, 109, 111, 112, 113, 114, 115, 118, 119, 120, 121, 123, 126, 127, 128, 129, 130, 131, 132, 133, 135, 136, 137, 139, 140, 141, 143, 144, 145, 146, 147, 149, 151, 152, 154, 155, 158, 173, 174, 176, 177, 180, 184, 186, 187, 188, 191, 196, 199, 201, 204, 210 item-clusters within, 4, 28, 29 Academic Advising Center, 142, 143 Academic advisors/counselors, 140 quality of relationship with, 140 Academic aptitude, 175 Academic autonomy, 54, 162 Academic autonomy subscale (from the Student Developmental Task and Lifestyle Inventory), 54, 193 Academic commitment. (see Academic motivation) Academic credits, number of. (see Course credits earned) Academic honor society membership, 6, 156 Academic honors annual, 12, 156 at graduation, 12, 156 departmental, 12 462 general, 12 Academic institution, type of. (see College, type of) Academic Locus of Control Scale, 47, 177 Academic major availability of, 52, 134, 162, 167 change of, 52, 134 decidedness, 6, 17, 51, 52, 133, 134, 143, 156, 162, 163, 167, 168 engineering, 137 non-science, 93 relative salience of, 134 required declaration of/pressure to declare, 52, 134, 162, 163, 167 satisfaction with, 6, 53, 156, 162 science, 93, 137 social science, 93 stability of decidedness, 52, 162 status, 52, 53, 142, 143, 162 traditionally female, 82, 93 traditionally male, 82, 93 Academic motivation, 10, 53, 54, 162 measure of, 53 motivation for being in college and doing college work, 4, 29 Academic performance, 5, 11, 29, 86, 152, 155, 156, 175 Academic planning, 10, 17, 48, 53, 161, 162 Academic purpose, seriousness of, 6, 156 Academic Self-Concept Scale (ASCS), 54 Academic self-efficacy, 40 Academic work, 7, 72 Academic year, time of, 12, 48, 66, 134, 135, 137, 145, 146, 147, 148, 154, 162, 167, 169 Academically advantaged students, 147 Academically at-risk students. (see At-risk students) Academically disadvantaged students. (see At-risk students) Acculturation subscale (from the American International Relations Scale), 90, 177 Achievement experiences (in school, sports, or creative pursuits), 132 Achievement need, 38, 48, 161 Achievement striving, 48 Achievement subscale (from the Early Resources Checklist), 202 Achievement subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 203 Achieving Tendency Scale, 48, 177 ACT. (see American College Testing Program) Active coping (from the Behavioral Attributes of Psychosocial Competence Scale), 33, 178 Active coping (from the Behavioral Attributes of Psychosocial Competence Scale-Condensed), 178 Active-behavioral coping (from the Health and Daily Living Form), 180 Active-cognitive coping (from the Health and Daily Living Form), 180 Activity (in meeting challenges), 33 Activity-recreation subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 203 Adaptability, family, 105, 106, 166 Adjusting students, types of, 158 Adjustive capacity disillusionment regarding, 12, 86, 98, 136, 138, 145, 157, 171 expectations concerning, 11, 12 prematriculation, 11, 12, 85, 136, 138 self-confidence regarding, 11, 42, 161, 170 Adjustment to college anticipated, 11, 42, 137 463 as a construct, 2, 3, 4, 5, 13, 15, 156, 157, 158, 160 as measured by the relation between the ASACQ and SACQ, 11, 145 as multifaceted, 5, 156 aspects of, 3, 4, 5, 10, 64, 134, 158, 161, 162 intraindividual variation in, 4, 15, 156 behavioral correlates of, 5, 13, 15, 138, 156, 158 capacity for. (see Adjustive capacity) changeability in, 13, 156 confidence level concerning, 11, 42, 161 definition of, 1, 2, 3, 5, 10, 17, 18, 156, 157, 158 from composition/structure of SACQ, 3, 5, 13 from research-identified correlates of SACQ, 3, 4, 11, 13, 14, 156, 157 determinants of, 1, 2, 10, 16, 17, 18, 35, 48, 75, 138, 145, 158, 159, 160, 161, 162, 163, 164, 165, 166, 167, 168, 171 difficulties in, 8, 154, 161, 168, 170 experience of, 3 experiential correlates of, 13, 15, 156, 158 extent of domain, 157 facilitation of, 1, 2, 18, 145, 153, 155, 168, 170 fulfillment of prematriculation expectations concerning, 11, 12 future investigations in, 2, 156, 163, 165 interindividual variation, 15, 156 intraindividual variation, 4, 15, 156 measurement of, 1, 2, 3, 13, 142 other measures than the SACQ, 10, 11, 159 operational meaning of, 5, 156 prediction of, 6, 64, 67, 86, 160, 161 prematriculation expectations, 11, 12, 42, 136, 138, 161 recall of, 12 received help from others in, 125 retrospective assessment of, 9 studies of, not using the SACQ, 1 types of, 158 understanding of, 1, 2, 4, 156, 159, 160, 161 Administrators, 141 Admission status regular, 147 special, 146 Admissions status regular, 147 Adolescence, 101, 104, 168 Adolescent Hassles Scale, 101 Adolescent Perceived Events Scale (APES), 101, 201 Adolescent Coping Orientation for Problem Experiences (A-COPE), 32, 177 Adult Attachment Scale, 70, 72, 177 Adult attachment styles, 70, 72, 73 Adult Attachment Types Questionnaire, 70, 72, 73 Adult EAS Temperament Survey, 26, 33, 67 Affect Intensity Measure, 78 Affectional interaction subscale (from the Father-Daughter Relationship Inventory), 204 Affective and behavioral commitment to the student role (from the Salience Inventory), 53 Affective quality score (from the Parental Attachment Questionnaire), 60, 185 Affective regulation subscale (from the Coping Response Indices Inventory), 34 Affects, 22, 75, 164 464 negative, 26, 27 Affiliation subscale (from the Classroom Environment Scale), 132 African-American students, 21, 33, 39, 46, 82, 83, 84, 85, 86, 93, 97, 98, 109, 110, 111, 128, 129, 135, 136, 147, 165, 167, 175, 195, 199, 211 interracial experience in, 134, 135, 167 social support in, 83, 124, 165 Age, 79, 94, 95, 96, 119, 120, 132, 164, 165 traditional/nontraditional age students, 93, 94 Agreeableness (from the NEO Five Factor Inventory), 184, 185 Agreeableness (from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory), 67 Alcohol abuse, 121, 122, 201 age of first use, 79 consumption of, 79 drinking problem of parents, 117, 121, 122, 166, 201, 209 kind used, 79 negative consequences of use, 79 present use of, 79 Alienation, 67, 164 Alienation Scale, 67, 68, 177 Alienation subscale (from the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment), 61, 62, 68, 182 Alpha pride subscale (from the Test of Self-Conscious Affect), 194 Ambiguous students (from the Late Adolescent Individuation Questionnaire), 60, 61 American College Testing Program (ACT), 91, 92, 177 composite score, 91 mathematics score, 92 American Indian students, 85 American International Relations Scale, 90, 177, 201 American-International Relations Scale, 105 Androgyny, 81, 82, 186 Anger/conflict score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209, 210 Anger/conflict score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Anticipated Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire, 145 Anticipated Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (ASACQ), 11, 12, 42, 85, 98, 136, 137, 145, 148 Anxiety, 23, 24, 152, 159 death, 25 engulfment, 57 phobic, 25 separation, 24, 25, 159, 160 social, 72, 76, 164 state, 8, 23, 24, 199 trait, 8, 23, 24, 199 Anxiety subscale (from the Adult Attachment Scale), 72, 177 Anxiety subscale (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 24, 196 Anxiety subscale (from the Hopkins Symptom Checklist), 24, 198 Anxiety subscale (from the Mental Health Inventory), 24 Appraisal of control, situation-specific, 48 Appraisal support subscale (from the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List), 204 Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule, 127, 128, 201 As a child with a child subscale (from the Childhood Sexual Experiences Questionnaire), 202 As a child with an adult subscale (from the Childhood Sexual Experiences Questionnaire), 202 As a nonconsenting adolescent subscale (from the Childhood Sexual Experiences Questionnaire), 202 ASACQ. (see Anticipated Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire) ASACQ/SACQ score comparison, 11, 12, 13, 42, 85, 98, 137, 145 465 Asian students, 85, 89, 94, 110, 124, 195, 199 Asian-American students, 33, 46, 85, 86, 98, 128, 147, 195, 212 Assertion of Autonomy subscale (from the Interpersonal Dependency Inventory), 75, 181 Assessing Daily Experiences Inventory, 103 Assets for adjustment, 30 Athletes, 141, 144, 168 Athletic aptitude, 41 Athletic performance self-assessment of, 99 Athletic self-efficacy, 41 Athletic teams, intercollegiate, 141, 168 At-risk students, 155, 169, 170 academically, 6, 147, 175 economically, 147 Attachment anxious-ambivalent, 71, 72, 73, 194 avoidant, 71, 72, 73, 194 dismissing, 72, 73, 179 fearful, 72, 73, 179 infant-parent, 71 insecure, 71 needs, preoccupation with gratification of, 72 preoccupied, 72, 73, 179 secure, 71, 72, 73, 179, 194 style, 73 styles, 70, 72, 179, 194 to father, 63, 64 to mother, 63, 64 to one's college, 9, 32, 39, 52, 69, 75, 79, 80, 82, 83, 88, 89, 94, 99, 107, 113, 114, 117, 118, 124, 125, 132, 140, 141, 142, 143, 144, 153, 157 to parents, 55, 59, 60, 61, 63, 64, 69, 163 to peers, 68, 69, 164 to significant others, 70 to the college experience, 3, 4 types of, 70 Attachment figure fusing of identity with, 71 preoccupation with, 71 Attachment preoccupied, 73 Attachment Style Inventory (ASI), 142 Attachment subscale (from the SACQ), 6, 9, 10, 12, 13, 14, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39, 41, 42, 44, 45, 46, 47, 48, 49, 50, 51, 52, 53, 54, 55, 57, 58, 62, 63, 64, 65, 67, 68, 69, 70, 71, 74, 75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 81, 83, 84, 85, 87, 88, 91, 92, 93, 94, 95, 97, 98, 101, 103, 104, 105, 106, 108, 109, 111, 112, 113, 114, 115, 118, 120, 121, 123, 124, 125, 126, 127, 128, 129, 131, 132, 133, 135, 137, 138, 139, 140, 141, 142, 143, 144, 146, 147, 148, 149, 151, 153, 154, 155, 173, 174, 176, 177, 180, 184, 186, 187, 188, 191, 196, 199, 201, 204, 210 Attachment subscale (from the Social Provisions Scale), 210 Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), 29 behavioral characteristics or symptoms associated with, 29 Attitude similarity score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Attitudes and Beliefs Scale-II, 45, 177 Attitudes Towards Disabled Persons Scale-Form O (ATDP), 41 Attitudes Towards Disabled Persons-Form O (ATDP-O), 178 Attitudinal independence subscale (from the Psychological Separation Inventory), 56 466 Attribution of causality global, 23 Attribution of causation, 47, 99 global, 23 stable, 23 Attributional Complexity Scale, 99, 178 Attributional Style Questionnaire, 23, 47, 178 Attrition. (see Withdrawal from college) Authoritarian parenting style (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 113, 114, 115 Authoritative parenting style (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 113, 114, 115 Autonomy, 58 Autonomy (from the Psychosocial Maturity Scale), 190 Autonomy from family and high school friends (from the College Life Task Assessment Instrument), 11 Availability of social integration subscale (from the Interview Schedule for Social Interaction), 133 Avoidance coping (from the Health and Daily Living Form), 180 Avoidance coping (from the Revised Ways of Coping Checklist), 191 Avoidance-oriented subscale (from the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations), 179 Beck Anxiety Inventory, 24, 196 Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), 20, 21, 22, 196 Beer drinking of, 79 getting drunk on, 79 Behavior (from the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale), 193, 194 Behavioral Attributes of Psychosocial Competence Scale (BAPC), 33, 178 Behavioral Attributes of Psychosocial Competence Scale-Condensed (BAPC-C), 33, 178 Belgian university, 57 Bell Global Psychopathology Scale, 19, 196 Belonging support subscale (from the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List), 204 Bem Sex-Role Inventory-Short Form, 81 Beta pride subscale (from the Test of Self-Conscious Affect), 194 Binge eaters, 28 Birth order, 122, 166 Black students. (see African-American students) Blames others (from the Revised Ways of Coping Checklist), 191 Blames self (from the Revised Ways of Coping Checklist), 191 Blatt Parental Representation Scale, 59 Body dissatisfaction subscale (from the Eating Disorder Inventory), 198 Brief College Student Hassles Scale, 201 Brief Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (Brief MAST), 121, 201 Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI), 18, 21, 24, 25, 196 Bulimia subscale (from the Eating Disorder Inventory), 198 Bulimics clinical, 28 subthreshold, 28 California Psychological Inventory-Revised (CPI), 30 Campus activities. (see Extracurricular activities in college) Care (caringness) subscale (from the Parental Bonding Instrument), 111 Career counselors, 140 Career Decision Scale, 38, 47, 50, 179 Career issues, 146 Career plans, certainty of, 6, 17, 50, 156, 162 Catholic students, 95 467 Causal explanation complexity of, 99 Causality, 16, 17, 47 Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), 20, 22, 196 Challenge (from the Hardiness Test), 34, 35, 180 Chaos subscale (from the Early Trauma Checklist), 202 Chicano students, 84, 85, 122, 123 Childhood, 77, 104, 168 Childhood Sexual Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ), 104, 202 Children's Role Inventory, 117 Children's Roles Inventory, 202 Church attendance, 95, 164 Clark University, 13, 173, 176 Class attendance, 7, 156 Class levels. (see College year level) Classroom Environment Scale, 132 Close Relationships Questionnaire, 72, 179 Close subscale (from the Adult Attachment Scale), 72, 177 Coaches relationship with, 142 Cognitive variable, 99 structural rather than contentual, 99 Cognitive-intellective, 22, 67 Cognitive-perceptual factors, 23 Cohesion subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 106 Cohesion subscale (from the Self-Report Family Inventory-Version II), 106, 209 Cohesiveness, family, 105, 106, 166 Collectivistic orientation, 74 College Attributional Complexity Scale, 99 College experience, 77, 165 commitment to, 3, 4, 9, 157 expenditure of effort in various aspects of, 11 satisfaction with, 10, 12, 157 College experiences hassles, 99 College Inventory of Academic Adjustment (CIAA), 10, 197 aspects of adjustment measured, 10 College Life Task Assessment Instrument (CLT), 10, 35, 197 College Maladjustment Scale (Mt), 11, 197 College of, 97 arts and sciences, 141 business administration, 141 engineering, 51, 52, 134, 141, 167 health professions, 141 liberal arts, 51, 97, 133, 163, 167 music, 141 technology/trade, 97 College Self-Efficacy Instrument, 179 College Self-Efficacy Instrument (CSEI), 41 College Self-Efficacy Instrument (CSEL), 38, 47 College staff persons, 138, 140, 153, 158, 168 College Student Experiences Questionnaire, 11 College Student Hassles Scale, 101 College Student Life Events Schedule, 102, 202 468 College year level, 92, 164, 165 all years, 14, 63, 92, 93, 94, 148, 174 mixed, 195, 200, 212 unspecified, 150 upper classes, 28, 63, 92, 104, 125, 138, 165, 168 Colleges, characteristics of, 97 commuter/residential, 140, 141 evening college/day college, 141 for deaf students, 96 knowledgeability concerning, 97, 98 predominantly Black, 167, 173 predominantly White, 83, 84, 134, 136, 165, 167 private/public, 97, 141 selectivity, 142 timing of required major declaration, 52, 134, 162, 167 two year/four year, 97 Combined Parent/Peer Overall Attachment Score (from the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment), 69, 182 Commitment academic. (see Academic motivation) to occupational and ideological goals, 49, 162 to other persons and activities, 34 to the college experience, 3, 4, 9, 157 to the student role, 53, 162 Commitment (from the Hardiness Test), 180 Communication subscale (from the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment), 61, 62, 68, 182, 183 Community college students, 85, 94, 95, 96, 130 Commuter college, 140 Commuter students, 140, 141 Compatibility in the self-world relation (alienation), 67, 164 Competitiveness (from the Jenkins Activity Survey, Form C), 30 Complexity, 44, 99 Complexity of representations of people (from the Social Cognition and Object Relations Scale), 75 Confidence (a variable derived by factor analysis), 38 Confidence regarding academic capabilities, 40 Confidence regarding capacity for adjusting to college. (see Adjustive capacity) Confidence regarding meeting challenges, 34 Conflict subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 203 Conflict subscale (from the Residence Hall Climate Inventory), 208 Conflict Tactics Scale, 110 Conflict with attachment figures subscale (from the Interview Schedule for Social Interaction), 133 Conflictual independence subscale (from the Psychological Separation Inventory), 55, 56, 59, 187 Connection to Puerto Rico, 88 Conscientiousness (from the NEO Five Factor Inventory), 184, 185 Conscientiousness (from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory), 48 Consensuality (from the California Psychological Inventory-Revised), 30 Control over events in one's life, 34 Control (from the California Psychological Inventory- Revised), 30 Control (from the Hardiness Test), 180 Control subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 203 Control-Mastery Theory, 77 Conventionally Defined Success (from the Life Values Inventory), 95, 183 Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory, 36, 37, 179 COPE Scales, 31, 32 469 Coping, 34 active, 31, 32, 178 active-behavioral, 32 active-cognitive, 32 avoidance-oriented, 31, 32 behavioral disengagement, 32 Blames Others, 33 Blames Self, 33 Count Blessings, 33 Emotion-focused, 32 emotion-oriented, 31 expectations regarding, 42 mental disengagement, 32 planning, 32 positive reinterpretation and growth, 32 problem-focused, 32 problem-solving, 31 Religiosity, 33 seeking social support for emotional reasons, 32 Seeks Support, 33 skills, 43, 150 task-oriented, 31 venting of emotiona, 31 Wishful Thinking, 33 Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations, 31, 179 Coping Response Indices Inventory, 26, 34, 67 Core Alcohol and Drug Survey, 79, 197 Counseling consequences of, 145 feedback interviews, 169 institutional agency, 143 small-group, 145, 169, 170 Counts blessings (from the Revised Ways of Coping Checklist), 191 Course credits earned, 12, 155, 156 Criterion groups, 160 Cross-generational coalitions, 116 Cultural characteristics, 88, 89, 165 Cultural distance/differences, 143 Culture-induced student characteristics, 86, 87 Current satisfaction with network score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Curricular adjustment subscale (from the College Inventory of Academic Adjustment), 197 Daily hassles, 101 Daily hassles (from the Adolescent Perceived Events Scale), 201 Daughters father's encouragement of independence in, 112 father's expression of affection toward, 112 father's role in relation to, 112 perception of time and attention from father, 112 Day college students, 141 Deaf identity, 96, 97 Deaf Identity Scale, 96 Deaf students, 83, 84, 130 attitude towards sign language, 97 470 attitude towards their deafness, 96, 97 Death Anxiety Scale, 25, 197 Decision-making, style of, 49, 161 Demographic variables, 80, 164 Density score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 DePaul University, 173 Depend subscale (from the Adult Attachment Scale), 72, 177 Dependency denial subscale (from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence), 70, 192 Dependency on other persons excessive, 75 Dependent/enmeshed students (from the Late Adolescent Individuation Questionnaire), 60 Depression, 8, 19, 20, 21, 23, 29, 99, 159, 198 Depression Scale (from the Psychological Distress Inventory), 21 Depression subscale (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 21, 196 Depression subscale (from the Hopkins Symptom Checklist), 20, 198 Detachment subscale (from the Test of Self-Conscious Affect), 194 Determining future goals subscale (from the College Life Task Assessment Instrument), 197 Developing autonomy subscale (from the College Life Task Assessment Instrument), 197 Developing mature interpersonal relationships task subscale (from the Student Developmental Task and Lifestyle Inventory), 69, 193 Developmentally-related variables, 77, 163 Differentiation, 44 Diffuse orientation (from the Identity Style Inventory-2), 49, 180 Disadvantaged students educationally, 86 financially, 86 Disillusionment regarding adjustive capacity. (see Adjustive capacity) Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES), 27, 197 Dissociative experiences/symptoms, 8, 27, 159, 160 Divorce, 116, 119, 120, 166 Dormitories, 140, 149 amount of conflict in, 139 assistants or advisors, 140 availability of personal or social support in, 139 characteristics of, 100, 140 physical, 139 climate, 139 differences in student adjustment among, 139 group cohesiveness in, 139 high rise or low rise, 138 Intervention efforts in, 148, 149, 169 orderedness or regulation of, 139, 140 peer support program in, 148 roommate, 138, 168 sections designated for freshmen, 139 sections designated for mixed classes, 139 size, 139 staff/student ratio, 138, 168 student perception of or reaction to, 139, 140 sylvan setting or downtown, 138 unisex or coed, 138, 139 versus off-campus living, 168 Dormitory assistantships, likelihood of being hired for, 8, 157 Drive for thinness subscale (from the Eating Disorder Inventory), 198 471 Dropping out. (see Withdrawal from college) Dual identity, in deaf students, 96, 97 Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale-Form A, 29, 197 Early Resources Checklist, 132, 202 Early Trauma Checklist, 103, 202 Earthquake, 105 East Asian international students, 195 Eating Disorder Inventory (EDI), 27, 36, 198 Eating disorders, 9, 27, 159, 160 psychological characteristics associated with, 9 Eating problems score (from the Eating Disorder Inventory), 27, 198 Eating problems-related traits (from the Eating Disorder Inventory), 198 Economic disruptions, 202 Educational goals, 10, 50, 51, 161 capacity to attain without direction from others, 54 obstacles to realization of, 77 Educational planning. (see Academic planning) Educational-working circumstances, 93 Edwards Social Desirability Scale, 179 Efficacy or success of academic effort, 4 Efficient time use, 10 Effort expenditure in college, 11 Ego identity status (from the Extended Version of the Objective Measure of Ego Identity Status), 49 Emotional distress, 71, 84, 104 difficulty in regulating, 71 Emotional independence subscale (from the Psychological Separation Inventory), 56, 189 Emotional inhibition, 71 Emotional reliance on other persons subscale (from the Interpersonal Dependency Inventory), 75, 181 Emotional reliance on others (from the Interpersonal Dependency Inventory), 164 Emotional stability, 25, 26 Emotional stress (from the Stress Audit), 26, 199 Emotional support satisfaction (from the Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule), 201 Emotional support score (from the Social network Questionnaire), 209, 210 Emotional support score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Emotion-oriented subscale (from the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations), 179 Employment, amount of time in, 7, 96, 157 Enabling Independence subscale (from the Late Adolescents' Relationships with Parents Scale), 60, 183 Encouragement of independence by father subscale (from the Perceived Parental Attitude Scale), 208 Engineering college. (see College of) Engulfment anxiety subscale (from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence), 57, 192 Enrollment, 141 discontinuance of. (see Withdrawal from college) full-time, 7, 157 having thoughts of discontinuing, 10, 157 part-time, 7, 157 persistence in, 9, 93, 157 Environment academic, 4, 29 as determinant of adjustment to college, 48, 65, 134, 136, 145, 163, 165, 167 as focus of one's attention, 74 as source of social support, 132 characteristics of, 16, 87, 88, 100, 159 classroom, 132 472 college, 7, 132, 136, 143, 146, 168 compatibility with, 67, 68, 164 experience related to, 201 family-related characteristics as, 55, 106 in interaction with person characteristics, 52, 88, 101, 133, 135, 161, 168 independent of student characteristics, 100, 166 institutional characteristics, 88, 134 manipulation of, 145 mastery of, 36, 71 person-environment factors, 138 physical-environment factors, 138 pressure from, 71, 134 social, 4, 123, 133, 142, 167 student's, 13 student's perception or interpretation of, 100, 165 Establishing and clarifying purpose task subscale (from the Student Developmental Task and Lifestyle Inventory), 50, 193 Establishing friendships subscale (from the College Life Task Assessmenr Instrument), 197 Ethnic status of the college, 167, 168 of the student, 45, 82, 83, 86, 147, 164, 165, 168 Ethnicity importance of in one's family, 123 European-American students. (see White students) Evening classes students, 141 Examinations, 64, 152 Existential well-being, 22 Existential well-being subscale (from the Spiritual Well-Being Scale), 22 Expectancies. (see Expectations) Expectancy styles, 43 complacent, 43, 44 fearful, 43 optimistic, 43 prepared, 43 Expectations about college life, 98 about the self, 160 complexity of, 44 disillusionment regarding, 86 fulfillment of, 11, 85 integratve complexity of, 44 negative academic, 42, 43 negative social, 42, 43 of positive outcomes, 75 perfectionistic, 78, 79, 164 positive academic, 42, 43 positive social, 42, 43 prematriculation, 12, 42, 44, 136, 138, 161 realistic, 43 regarding adaptation/coping efforts, 42, 43 regarding adaptive/coping efforts, 43 regarding impending transition into college, 11, 42, 43, 44, 136, 161 Expected satisfaction with network score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Experiential world of the student, 13 473 Expressiveness subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 203 Extended Version of the Objective Measure of Ego Identity Status, 49 External Control, 47 Externality, 47 Externality/externals (from the Internal-External Locus of Control Scale), 45, 46, 161 Externalization subscale (from the Test of Self-Conscious Affect), 194 Extracurricular activities in college Black-sponsored, 136 number of organizations joined, 7 participation in, 7, 136, 157 racial composition of, 136, 167 White-sponsored, 136 Extracurricular activities in high school leadership positions in, 157 Extraversion (from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory), 184 Extraversion/extroverts, 66, 67, 164 Eysenck Personality Inventory, 66 FACES-II Scale, 105, 202 FACES-III Scale, 105, 106, 202 Facilitation of adjustment. (see Adjustment to college, facilitation of; Intervention) Factor analysis, 3, 26, 33, 34, 35, 38, 47, 54, 67, 107, 129 Faculty. (see Professors) relationship with, 142 Faculty advisors, 140 False positives, 161 Family, 59, 77 adaptability, 105, 106, 107, 166 adaptive interactions in, 110 alcohol-abusing, 117, 118, 121, 122, 201, 209 amount of student contact with, 7, 112 as a functioning organizational unit, 116, 166 as environmental factors, 55, 106, 167 autonomy from, 11 balanced, 106 birth order of children in, 122, 166 characteristics of, 105, 107, 166 climate, 139 closeness of continued involvement with, 197 coalitions within, 56, 116 cohesiveness of, 105, 106, 107, 166 conflict, 106, 109, 110, 166 degree of organization of activities in, 106 difficulties, 110 disengaged, 106 disruption, 117, 121, 202, 209 drug-abusing, 117, 118, 121, 209 dysfunctional, 117, 118, 119 enmeshed, 106 enmeshment/disengagement, 107 experience within, 166 expressiveness, 106 fear of separation, 108, 166 fostering autonomy, 60, 107, 166 474 fostering intimacy, 107, 166 functioning, effectiveness of, 107 functioning, negative aspects of, 107 functioning, positive aspects of, 107 functioning, student's perception of, 107 generation level within, 123 habitual behaviors of, 107 health or functioning, 107, 121 importance of ethnicity in, 123 importance of religion in, 95 income, 90 intactness, 116, 119, 166 language spoken in, 100, 122, 166 mail contact with, 7 maladaptive, 108, 166 marital conflict in, 108 maternal intrusiveness, 109, 166 mother-custody, 121 normal, 117, 118, 121, 209 only child in, 122, 166 parental conflict in, 56, 100, 109, 110, 166 parent-child bond, 111 parent-child cohesion, 107 parent-child overinvolvement in, 108, 109, 166 parent-child role reversal in, 108, 166 parenting styles, 100, 112, 113, 114, 115 parent-student relationship, 56 participation in social and recreational activities, 106 pathology, 119 postdivorce structure, 120 primary relationship in, 116 related variables, 55 relationship patterns within, 116, 120 relationships, 110 ritualization, 107 roles, 116, 117, 118 separation from, 11, 157 single-parent, 121 social climate, 106 social support from, 110, 111, 112, 123, 124, 128, 130, 166, 167, 205 stepfather, 133 structure, 108 student-perceived difficulties in, 110 telephone contacts with, 7 visits with, 7 Family adaptability subscale (from FACES-II), 202 Family adaptability subscale (from FACES-III), 105, 106, 202 Family cohesion subscale (from FACES-II), 202 Family cohesion subscale (from FACES-III), 105, 106, 202 Family cohesion subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 203 Family Conflict subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 106 Family Environment Scale, 106, 203 Family Expressiveness subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 106 Family Functioning Scales, 107, 121, 203 475 Family health/competence subscale (from the Self-Report Family Inventory-Version II), 108 Family Hero role (from the Family Role Behavior Inventory), 116, 117, 119 Family Hero role (from the Role Relationship Inventory), 118, 119 Family intactness, 116 Family of Origin Scale (FOS), 107, 203 Family Ritual Questionnaire, 107, 203 Family ritualization (from the Family Ritual Questionnaire), 203 Family Role Behavior Inventory (FRBI), 116, 117 Family roles, typology of, 116, 117, 119 Family self (from the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale), 193, 194 Family Structure Survey (FSS), 108, 109, 203 Family/kin support satisfaction (from the Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule), 201 Father, 56, 63, 65, 113, 114, 115, 184, 185, 186, 187, 188, 189, 190 attachment to, 61, 63, 64 caringness, 111 encouragement of daughter independence, 112 expression of affection toward daughter, 112 intrusiveness, 109 protectiveness, 109 relationship with, 64, 65 role in relation to daughter, 112 time and attention devoted to daughter, 112 Father authoritarianism (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire), 205, 206 Father authoritarianism (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 206 Father authoritativeness (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire), 205, 206 Father authoritativeness (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 206 Father caringness (from the Parental Bonding Instrument), 111, 206, 207 Father permissive-indulgence (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 206 Father permissive-neglectfulness (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 206 Father permissiveness (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire), 205, 206 Father protectiveness (from the Parental Bonding Instrument), 109, 206, 207 Father-Daughter Relationship Inventory (FDRI), 112, 204 Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale, 76, 180 Fear of rejection, 73 Fear of separation subscale (from the Family Structure Survey), 108, 203, 204 Feeling states. (see Affects) Feelings about past relationships related to development of self, 37 Feminine sex-role orientation, 81 Financial aid counselors, 140 First generation college attenders, 35, 54, 90, 123, 195, 212 Flexibility (from the California Psychological Inventory-Revised), 30 Focus of interest inward, 164 on others, 74, 164 on self, 74, 164 outward, 164 Foreign students, 19, 73, 87, 94, 96, 100, 105, 109, 111, 129, 143, 150, 195, 200, 212 African, 89 Asian, 89, 100 athletes, 89 Australian, 89 Canadian, 89, 100 Central American, 89 Chinese, 87 476 European, 89 from east Asian countries, 74, 88 from Southeast Asia, 89 from the Far East, 89 from western European countries, 74, 88, 89, 100 graduate, 89, 90 Latin American, 89 Middle-Eastern, 89 Pacific Islander, 87 South American, 89 who lived in U. S. prior to matriculation, 88 Forming an identity subscale (from the College Life Task Assessmenr Instrument), 197 Fostering autonomy, 203 Fostering autonomy score (from the Parental Attachment Questionnaire), 60 Fostering intimacy, 203 Fraternity membership, 141, 168 Frequency of contact score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Freshman orientation. (see Orientation program) Freshman students, 13, 14, 42, 52, 53, 57, 62, 63, 66, 76, 82, 83, 90, 92, 93, 94, 95, 96, 104, 116, 121, 125, 127, 134, 138, 139, 141, 142, 146, 147, 148, 149, 150, 151, 153, 154, 155, 163, 168, 169, 174, 212 early entrant, 195 engineering, 52 traditional-age, 93 Freshman year, 7, 12, 14, 28, 42, 43, 52, 53, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66, 68, 76, 92, 127, 129, 132, 134, 137, 147, 148, 149, 155, 167, 169, 195 Friend/other support satisfaction (from the Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule), 201 Friends, 103 at home, 7 close, 7, 130, 157 college, 124 high school, 134 high school, autonomy from, 11 high school, closeness of continued involvement with, 197 in college (any college), 123, 208 not in college, 123, 208 number of, 7, 157 percentage of, in social networks, 126 pre-college, away from campus, 124 separation from, 11, 157 social support from, 83, 100, 123, 128, 130, 167, 208 socializing with, 7 Friendships establishment of, 11, 158 length of, 157 maintenance of over time, 7 Full scale score (ASACQ), 11, 42 Full scale score (ASACQ/SACQ), 86 Full scale score (SACQ), 3, 9, 10, 11, 13, 14, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 27, 28, 29, 30, 32, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37, 39, 40, 41, 42, 43, 44, 45, 46, 47, 48, 49, 50, 51, 52, 53, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59, 61, 64, 65, 67, 68, 71, 72, 74, 75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 84, 85, 87, 88, 89, 90, 91, 92, 93, 94, 95, 97, 98, 100, 101, 102, 104, 105, 106, 108, 109, 110, 111, 112, 113, 114, 115, 118, 119, 120, 121, 122, 123, 124, 126, 127, 128, 129, 131, 132, 133, 135, 136, 137, 139, 140, 141, 142, 143, 144, 146, 147, 148, 149, 151, 153, 154, 155, 173, 174, 176, 177, 180, 184, 186, 187, 188, 191, 196, 199, 201, 204, 210 Fun/relaxation score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209 477 Fun/relaxation score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Functional independence subscale (from the Psychological Separation Inventory), 56 Gender findings, 20, 21, 24, 27, 39, 49, 50, 54, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 69, 75, 78, 80, 81, 82, 83, 85, 94, 95, 96, 99, 104, 107, 109, 111, 112, 113, 114, 116, 119, 120, 121, 125, 126, 127, 128, 130, 133, 139, 140, 147, 163, 164, 165, 173, 175, 194, 199, 211 General collectivism index (from the Individualism-Collectivism Scale), 180 Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale, 36, 180 Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale, 37, 40, 41 Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale (modified), 180 Geographic variables, 143 George Washington University, 173 Global attributions (from the Attributional Style Questionnaire), 178 Global Self-Worth score (from the Self-Perception Profile for College Students), 25, 40, 67 Global Severity Index (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 18, 196 Global Severity Index (from the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised), 19, 199 Goal directedness, 34, 49, 132, 161, 180 Goal Instability Scale, 49, 132, 180 Goal orientation, 48, 53, 161 educational, 48, 51 vocational, 48, 51 Goal setting skills, 49 Goals, 197 ability to set, 150 career. (see Career plans, certainty of) educational. (see Educational goals) ideological, 49, 162 life. (see Life goals) motivation to achieve, 10 occupational, 49, 51, 162 persistence in attaining, 33 realistic, 33 success in attaining, 49 God, relationship with, 133 Goldberg Big-Five Factor Markers, 25, 40, 67 Grades in college, 5, 6, 12, 86, 122, 156, 173, 174 in high school, 6, 86, 146, 147, 152 minority students', 86 overprediction of, 86 Graduate students, 19, 22, 26, 28, 47, 73, 89, 90, 93, 94, 96, 103, 105, 109, 111, 129, 150, 195, 200, 212 Graduation from high school, 146 honors at, 12, 156 on time, 12, 157 rate, 12 Grandparent Strengths and Needs Inventory-Grandchild Form, 65 Grandparent Strengths and Needs Inventory-Grandparent Form, 115 Grandparents, 65, 115 relationship with, 65 relationship with student, 115, 116 Grossmont College, 174 Group cohesiveness subscale (from the Residence Hall Climate Inventory), 208, 209 Guidance subscale (from the Social Provisions Scale), 210 478 Guilt, 77, 78, 119, 164, 186 experienced in relation to fathers, 78 Guilt subscale (from the Test of Self-Conscious Affect), 194 Hard liquor, getting drunk on, 79 Hardiness, 34 Hardiness Test, 34, 35, 180 Health general, 28 self-assessed, 28 Health and Daily Living Form, 32, 180 Health Checklist, 28, 198 Health/competence subscale (from the Self-Report Family Inventory-Version II), 209 Healthy personality, the, 34 Healthy separation subscale (from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence), 70, 192, 193 Hearing identity, 96, 97 Help resources on campus, 8 Hero role (from the Children's Role Inventory), 117 Hero role (from the Children's Roles Inventory), 202 Hero role (from the Role Relationship Inventory), 209 High school, 146 friends, 11, 134 grades, 6, 86, 146, 147, 152 graduation from, 146 high academic achievers in, 91 integrated, 135 interracial experience in, 134 intervention efforts during, 145, 146, 169 involvement in life of, 157 Junior year, 195, 212 leadership positions, 157 predominantly Black. (see Predominantly Black high school) predominantly White. (see Predominantly White high school) Puerto Rican, 88 senior year, 195 seniors, 97, 98, 137, 145, 146, 169, 170 transition from, 8 Hispanic students, 84, 85, 86, 130, 141 Hollins College, 173 Holy Cross College, 13 Home closeness of continued involvement with, 197 commute from, 140 distance from, 143, 144 language spoken in, 122, 166 perceived distance from, 144 relocation away from, 4 separation from, 157 visits to, 7, 157 Hometown, 143 in-state, 144 out-of-state, 144 rural, 143 urban/suburban, 143 479 Hopkins Symptom Checklist, 18, 20, 24, 198 Hostility subscale (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 196 Hypochondriasis subscale (from the Mini-Mult), 198 Hypomania subscale (from the Mini-Mult), 198 Hysteria subscale (from the Mini-Mult), 198 Identification with one's minority culture, 85 Identity confusion, 119 separate, 71 Identity (from the Psychosocial Maturity Scale), 190 Identity (from the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale), 193, 194 Identity scale (from the Psychosocial Maturity Inventory), 58 Identity status Black, 84 Identity Style Inventory-2, 49, 180 Identity Vis-a-vis Mother Questionnaire (IVM-20), 59 Immersion-emersion score (from the Racial Identity Attitude Scale), 84 Impairment to feelings about self and past relationships (from the Narcissistic Injury Scale), 37 Inadequacy, feelings of, 119 Income, parental, 90 Indecision, educational/vocational, 50 Independence, 71 compulsive, 71 pathological, 68 Independence subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 203 Individual adequacy scales (from the Psychosocial Maturity Inventory), 58 Individual differences, 15, 17, 138, 156 Individualism-Collectivism Scale, 74, 180 Individualistic orientation, 74 Individuated students (from the Late Adolescent Individuation Questionnaire), 60, 61 Individuation score (from the Separation Anxiety Test), 58, 192 Individuation subscale (from the Identity Vis-a-vis Mother Questionnaire), 59 Individuation subscale (from the Personal Authority in the Family System Questionnaire-Version C), 58, 186 Ineffectiveness subscale (from the Eating Disorder Inventory), 198 Information orientation (from the Identity Style Inventory-2), 49, 180 Information/advice score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209, 210 Information/advice score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Initiative, 33 Inner-directed students, 161 Institutional attachment. (see Attachment, to one's college) Institutional characteristics, 52, 82, 83, 87, 88, 134, 140, 141, 162, 167, 168 Instrumental support satisfaction (from the Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule), 201 Integration, 44 Intellectual and Cultural Interests (from the Life Values Inventory), 95, 183 Intellectual-cultural subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 203 Intellectualism subscale (from the Personal Values Scale-Revised), 53 Intellectualism subscale (from the Personal Values Scales-Revised), 186 Interactive effects, 52, 83, 88, 101, 133, 134, 135, 138, 161, 167, 168 Interest congruence, 51 Internal attributions (from the Attributional Style Questionnaire), 178 Internal-External Locus of Control Scale, 45, 46 Internality, 34, 45, 47, 195 Internality/internals (from the Internal-External Locus of Control Scale), 45, 46, 161 480 Interpersonal competence, 8, 157 Interpersonal Dependency Inventory, 39, 75, 181 Interpersonal experiences, 7, 100 Interpersonal Guilt Questionnaire-45 (IGQ-45), 77, 181 Interpersonal guilt subscale (from the Interpersonal Guilt Questionnaire-45), 77, 181 Interpersonal orientation, 74, 164 Interpersonal relationships, 4, 71, 132 associated with positive affects and outcomes, 75 capacity for emotional investment in, 75 capacity for establishing, 68, 69, 164 capacity to be comfortable apart from, 70 capacity to engage in and enjoy, 70 detachment from and rejection of, 70 establishment of, 69 formation of, 7, 68, 74, 157 in general, individuation-separation issues in, 57 loss of, success in dealing with, 104 maintenance of, 7, 74, 157 understanding of causal factors in, 75 Interpersonal relationships subscale (from the Early Resources Checklist), 202 Interpersonal sensitivity subscale (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 196 Interpersonal sensitivity subscale (from the Hopkins Symptom Checklist), 36, 198 Interpersonal situations, 74, 164 Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (ISEL), 131, 204 variation of, 131 Interracial experience, 100, 136 beneficial effect of, 135 during college, 167 in Black students, 83, 134, 135, 167 in high school, 134 in Latino students, 136 pre-college, 134, 167 variation in amount of, 83, 135 Intervention, 145, 146, 150, 151, 152, 169 academic, 146, 147, 169 theme groups, 151 as manipulation of environmental characteristics, 145 by interview, 154, 155, 169, 170 by small group meetings, 145, 149, 150, 151, 169, 170 consequences of, 145, 146, 147, 148, 150, 151, 154, 155, 169, 170 duration of, 169, 170 effect of election to participate in, 152 facilitative, 145, 170 first quarter, 151 first semester, 150, 155 in dormitories, 147, 148, 149, 169 measure of effects of, 145, 146, 148, 149, 154, 155, 169, 170 peer support program, 148, 149, 150, 151 postmatriculation, 148, 153, 169 practical significance of, 145 prematriculation, 145, 146, 147, 148, 169 problems in design, 153, 169 remedial, 145, 153, 168, 170, 171 seminar, 150 481 social, 146, 147, 149, 150, 151, 169, 170 tailored to particular problems, 170 therapeutic, 155 timing of, 145, 154 timing of, 170 to preclude disillusionment with one's adjustive capacity, 171 voluntary, 148, 149, 150 with arranged faculty-student interaction, 151 with at-risk students, 86, 146, 147, 154, 155, 168, 169, 170 with college-bound high school seniors, 145, 169, 170 with transfer students, 169 Interview as a means of intervention, 154, 155 consequences of, 154, 155, 170 counseling, 169 data, 12, 77 discussion of adjustment problems in, 154, 170 feedback of test information in, 154, 170 individual, 170 SACQ findings as primary content of, 154, 169 schedule for social interaction, 133 structured, 77 Interview Schedule for Social Interaction, 133 Intimacy of relationship score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209 Intimacy of relationship score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Intimacy subscale (from the Personal Authority in the Family System Questionnaire-Version C), 58, 186 Intimidation subscale (from the Personal Authority in the Family System Questionnaire-Version C), 58, 186 Intoxicants, 79, 164 Introversion/introverts, 66, 164 Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA), 61, 62, 63, 64, 68, 69, 182, 195 Involvement in life of college. (see Extracurricular activities in college) Involvement in life of high school. (see Extracurricular activities in high school) Involvement with campus social system. (see Extracurricular activities in college) Involvement with other persons on campus. (see Interpersonal relationships) Invulnerability (from the New Personal Fable Scale), 185 Invulnerability, sense of, 38 Item-clusters, 4, 28, 29, 158 Jackson Social Desirability Scale, 183 Jenkins Activity Survey (JAS, Form C), 30 Job involvement (from the Jenkins Activity Survey, Form C), 31 Journal-writing program, as therapy, 155 Junior year, 12, 14, 43, 62, 63, 195 in high school, 212 Juniors, 57, 63, 92, 147, 148, 195 Kandel Depression Scale, 21, 198 Korean-American students, 81 Lack of social self-confidence subscale (from the Interpersonal Dependency Inventory), 181 Language of community at large, 166 of family, 88, 100, 122, 148, 166 Late Adolescent Individuation Questionnaire, 60 482 Late Adolescents' Relationships with Parents Scale, 183 Latin-American students, 85 Latino students, 33, 46, 84, 85, 86, 91, 98, 102, 110, 122, 123, 124, 128, 136, 141, 147, 195, 199, 212 Leadership, 157 Learning disability, 28, 29, 38, 159, 160 Lebanon Valley College, 173 Liberal arts students, 134, 163 Life decisions, 161 avoiding, 49 conforming to expectations of others in, 49 unstable, 49 Life events, 13, 101 academic, 103 as perceived by the student, 23, 165 aspects of, 101 bad/negative, 23, 47, 102, 178 desirable, 103 good/positive, 23, 47, 178 in general, 47, 101, 166, 178 major, 101, 201 measure of, 103 negative impact of, 101, 166 number of, 101, 166 positive impact of, 101, 166 social, 103 specific to the college experience, 102 undesirable, 103 Life Events Checklist, 101 Life Experiences Survey, 101, 102, 103, 204 Life goals, 10, 50 Life stress/stressors general, 101, 102, 104, 166 high levels of, 133 in the past, 101 low levels of, 133 measure of, 102, 204 negative impact of, 166 number of, 166 positive impact of, 166 specific forms of, 104, 166 Life styles, 95 Life value preferences, 95 Life Values Inventory, 48, 95, 183 Living arrangements, 93, 100, 138, 140 on or off-campus, 140, 141, 168 Location of interaction score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209, 210 Locus of control, 34, 45, 46, 47, 48, 160, 161, 183, 195 academic, 48 regarding specific areas of function, 47 Logical analysis subscale (from the Coping Response Indices Inventory), 34 Loneliness, 76, 164 Loss subscale (from the Early Trauma Checklist), 202 Loss, interpersonal, 104, 202 Lost Child Role (from the Children's Role Inventory), 117 483 Lost Child role (from the Children's Roles Inventory), 202 Lost Child role (from the Family Role Behavior Inventory), 117 Lost Child role (from the Role Relationship Inventory), 118 Love-reject factor score (from the Parent-Child Relations Questionnaire II), 111, 208 Main effects of conditions, 138 Maintaining one's physical self subscale (from the College Life Task Assessment Instrument), 197 Major. (see Academic major) Maladaptive attitudes and ways of thinking, 29 Managing time subscale (from the College Life Task Assessmenr Instrument), 197 Marital conflict. (see Parents, conflict between) Marital conflict subscale (from the Family Structure Survey), 108, 203, 204 Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale, 183 Married students, 94, 96 Mascot role (from the Children's Role Inventory), 117 Mascot role (from the Children's Roles Inventory), 202 Mascot role (from the Family Role Behavior Inventory), 117, 118 Mascot role (from the Role Relationship Inventory), 118, 209 Masculine sex-role orientation, 81 Mastery Learning Scale, 53, 184 Materialistic Orientation (from the Life Values Inventory), 95, 183 Maternal intrusiveness, 109, 166 Mathematics course score (from the Mathematics Self-Efficacy Scale), 184 Mathematics Course Self-Efficacy subscale (from the Mathematics Self-Efficacy Scale), 40 Mathematics courses, 173 Mathematics problem score (from the Mathematics Self-Efficacy Scale), 184 Mathematics Problem Self-Efficacy subscale (from the Mathematics Self-Efficacy Scale), 40 Mathematics score (from the American College Testing Program), 177 Mathematics Self-Efficacy Scale, 40, 184 Matriculating students, 136 Maturation, 59, 61 Maturity of goals and level of aspiration subscale(from the College Inventory of Academic Adjustment), 197 Maturity subscale (from the Eating Disorder Inventory), 198 Men. (see Gender findings) Mental health, 17, 18, 20, 21, 22, 24, 25, 26, 27, 28, 30, 157, 159, 160, 196, 199 general, 8, 18, 159 good, 33 specific aspects of, 8, 18 variables associated with or contributing to, 30, 31 Mental Health Inventory, 18 Anxiety subscale, 24 Depression subscale, 20 Mental health subscale (from the College Inventory of Academic Adjustment), 197 Method variance, 131 Michigan State University, 173 Mini-Mult, 198 clinical scales, 19 Depression scale, 22, 198 Total Pathology Score, 19 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), 19 Minority culture of origin, identification with, 85 Minority students, 60, 86, 124, 175 Money, management of, 7 Mood state, 22 484 Moral-ethical self (from the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale), 193 Moral-religious subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 203 Mother, 56, 63, 65, 109, 113, 114, 115, 184, 185, 186, 187, 188, 189, 190 attachment to, 61, 63, 64 caringness, 111 has custody of children, 119, 121 intrusiveness, 109 protectiveness, 109 relationship with, 64, 65 relationship with daughter, 116 relationship with son, 120 single/remarried, 120 Mother and Father Questionnaire, 65, 184 Mother authoritarianism (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire), 205, 206 Mother authoritarianism (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 206 Mother authoritativeness (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire), 205, 206 Mother authoritativeness (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 206 Mother caringness (from the Parental Bonding Instrument), 111, 206, 207 Mother permissive-indulgence (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 206 Mother permissive-neglectfulness (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 206 Mother permissiveness (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire), 205, 206 Mother protectiveness (from the Parental Bonding Instrument), 109, 206, 207 Motivation for being in college and doing college work. (see Academic motivation) Mt. Zion Psychotherapy Research Group, 77 Multidimensional Academic-Specific Locus of Control Scale (MASLOC), 38, 47 Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (MPS), 78, 184 Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire, 22, 27, 184 Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support, 129, 204 Multi-Dimensional Support Scale, 124, 125, 130 Multidimensional Support Scale, 205 Multigroup Measure of Ethnic Identity (MEIM), 85 Multivariate approach, 161, 165 Narcissistic injury, 37, 160 Narcissistic Injury Scale, 37, 184 Need for social approval (from the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale), 76, 164 Need Strength (from the Social Support Inventory), 130 Need strength score (from the Social Support Inventory), 210, 211 Negative affect (from the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule), 186 Negative consequences of alcohol use (from the Core Alcohol and Drug Survey), 197 Negative emotionality (from the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire), 26, 184 Neighbors, 103 NEO Five-Factor Inventory, 27, 48, 66, 67, 99 NEO-Five Factor Inventory, 184 Network size score (from the Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule), 201 conflicted, 201 unconflicted, 201 Network size score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209 Network size score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Neuroticism, 8 Neuroticism (from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory, 184 Neuroticism (from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory), 27 New Personal Fable Scale, 38, 185 Non-Catholic students, 95 485 Non-interpersonal activity preoccupation with, 71, 72 Non-traditional age students, 22, 26, 93, 95, 96, 128, 130, 155, 195, 212 Nontraditional-age students, 103, 212 Normative considerations, 165 Normative orientation (from the Identity Style Inventory-2), 50, 180 Nowicki-Strickland Internal-External Control Scale for Adults, 46, 47 Number of friends score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Number of support persons score (from the Social Support Questionnaire), 211 Number of years enrolled in college, 93 Nurturance seeking subscale (from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence), 57, 192 Object relations, 74, 75, 164 Obsessive-compulsive subscale (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 196 Obsessive-compulsive subscale (from the Hopkins Symptom Checklist), 198 Occupational competence, 157 Occupational field, 51 Occupational goals, 49, 162 mathematics/science-related, 51 Occupational preparation, 10, 157 Omnipotence (from the New Personal Fable Scale), 185 Omnipotence Guilt subscale (from the Interpersonal Guilt Questionnaire-45), 78 Omnipotence, sense of, 38 Omnipotent responsibility guilt subscale (from the Interpersonal Guilt Questionnaire-45), 181, 182 Only child status, 122, 166 Openness to Experience (from the NEO Five-Factor Inventory), 99, 184 Opportunity for nurturance subscale (from the Social Provisions Scale), 130, 210 Optimism, 23 Order subscale (from the Residence Hall Climate Inventory), 208, 209 Organization subscale (from the Family Environment Scale), 203 Orientation program, 153 consequences of in SACQ scores, 147, 148, 149, 150 freshman, 132, 148, 149, 150, 153 peer advising, 149 prematriculation, 48, 52, 147, 148, 153, 175 satisfaction with, 147 summer, 52, 137 University 101 course, 146 voluntary, 148, 149 Orientation to Life Questionnaire, 34 Overall adjustment, 114. (see Full scale score, SACQ) Overall satisfaction with social support score (from the Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule), 127, 201 Paranoia subscale (from the Mini-Mult), 198 Paranoid ideation subscale (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 196 Parental attachment, 55, 61, 63, 64, 68, 182, 183 aspects of, 59 perceived, 55, 163 relation to college adjustment, 60, 69 security of, 61, 63 to father, 63, 64, 182, 190, 191 to mother, 63, 64, 182, 183, 190 transformations in, 59 underemphasis on role of, 59 486 Parental Attachment Questionnaire (PAQ), 59, 60, 185 Parental Authority Questionnaire, 205 Parental Authority Questionnaire (modified version of), 114 Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of, 112, 206 Parental Bonding Instrument, 109, 111, 206 Parental Conflict Resolution (from the Structural Family Interaction Scale-Revised), 108 Parental contribution to the parent-child bond as assessed by the child (from the Parental Bonding Instrument), 111 Parental control, resentment of, 57 Parental difficulties, 110 Parental dysfunction subscale (from the Early Trauma Checklist), 202 Parental intrusiveness, 109 Parental Physical Maltreatment Scale, 103, 207 modification of, to assess abuse by significant nonparental figures, 103 Parental Psychological Maltreatment Scale, 103, 208 Parental responsibility, student's, 96 Parent-child cohesion, 107 Parent-child overinvolvement, 108, 109, 166 Parent-child overinvolvement subscale (from the Family Structure Survey), 108, 109, 203, 204 Parent-Child Relations Questionnaire II, 111, 208 Parent-child role reversal, 108, 166 Parent-child role reversal subscale (from the Family Structure Survey), 108, 203, 204 Parent-infant experience, 70 Parenting styles. (see Family, parenting styles) Parents, 59, 62, 185, 188, 189 alive/deceased, 119, 122 amount of student contact with, 112, 120 appropriate boundaries with, 58 attachment. (see Parental attachment) authoritarian, 113 authoritative, 113 cold and distant, 112 conflict between, 56, 100, 108, 109, 166 conflict resolution, 108 conflict with, 55, 56, 57 continued connectedness with, 60 continued connection with, 60, 61 contribution to parent-child bond, 111 custodial (postdivorce), 120 differentiation from, 59 divorced, 116, 119, 120, 121, 122, 166 drinking problems in, 121, 122, 166 drug problems in, 121 dysfunctional, 121, 122 educational level, 90 father. (see Father) function autonomously from, 58 income, 90 joint custody, 119 mother. (see Mother) noncustodial (postdivorce), 120 of high SACQ scorers, 112 of low SACQ scorers, 112 overprotectiveness, 109 perceived as rejecting, callous, indifferent, hostile, 57 487 physically abusive, 122 postdivorce student relationship with, 120 psychological separation from. (see Psychological separation from) Puerto Rican, 88 quality of time spent with, 120 reciprocity in current relationships with, 64, 65 remarriage, 120 separated, 119 sexually abusive, 122 social support from, 111, 210 student recollections of as loving, 111 student relationship with, 55, 56, 57, 59, 60, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65, 112, 116, 120, 121, 163 supportive of student's college career, 112 triangulated relationship with, 58 unconflicted feelings of closeness with and dependency on, 57 unduly influenced by, 58 viewed as fostering autonomy, 60, 107, 166, 185 viewed as providing emotional support, 60, 185 voluntary closeness with, 58 Participant solicitation, method of, 142, 143 for intervention, 143 for research, 143 Part-time students, 7, 157 Pathological Separation-Individuation Inventory, 59, 185 Patient's (ADHD) Behavior Checklist, 29, 198 Peer advising program, 149 Peer advisors/counselors, 125, 153 Peer attachments, 61, 64, 68, 69, 164, 182 Peer enmeshment subscale (from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence), 70, 192, 193 Peer Group Dependence Scale, 75, 185 Peer support program, 148 Perceived adequacy of attachment (from the Interview Schedule for Social Interaction), 133 Perceived attachment to parents. (see Parental attachment) Perceived availability of social resources (from the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List), 131 Perceived distance from home, 144 Perceived fit score (from the Social Support Inventory), 129, 210, 211 Perceived mastery of the environment, 36 Perceived maternal intrusiveness. (see Maternal intrusiveness) Perceived Parental Attitude Scale, 112, 208 Perceived prejudice subscale (from the American International Relations Scale), 201 Perceived Prejudice subscale (from the American-International Relations Scale), 105 Perceived relationship with parents. (see Parents, student relationship with) Perceived self-effectiveness/ineffectiveness, 36, 160 Perceived separation from parents. (see Psychological separation from parents) Perceived social support. (see Social support) Perceived social support from family. (see Social support, from family) Perceived Social Support from Family Scale, 208 Perceived social support from friends. (see Social support, from friends) Perceived Social Support from Friends Scale, 67, 208 Perceived Stress Scale, 26, 44, 102, 199 Perceived Supply (from the Social Support Inventory), 130 Perceived supply score (from the Social Support Inventory), 210, 211 Perceived support, 129 Perceived Support Network Inventory, 128 488 Percent family score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Percent friends score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209 Percent friends score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Percent relatives score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209 Percent roommates score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209 Percent students score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209 Percent students score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Perception of Parental Reciprocity Scale, 64, 65, 185 Perfectionism, expectations of a measure of, 78 imposed on oneself by oneself, 78 imposed on oneself by others, 79, 164 imposed on others, 78, 164 non-social, 78 other-oriented, 79, 184 self-oriented, 78, 184 socially-prescribed, 79, 184 Permissive parenting style (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 114 Permissive-indulgent parenting style (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 114, 115 Permissive-neglectful parenting style (from the Parental Authority Questionnaire, modified version of), 114, 115 Persistence toward a degree. (see Enrollment, persistence in) Person characteristics, 13, 15, 17, 18, 21, 22, 24, 25, 26, 27, 28, 29, 38, 41, 44, 45, 46, 48, 50, 51, 52, 53, 54, 55, 57, 59, 63, 66, 67, 74, 75, 76, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 84, 85, 86, 93, 94, 100, 136, 159, 160, 161, 168 as changeable, 13 as determinants of adjustment, 16, 17, 75, 100, 152, 159, 164, 170 as stable states, 13, 156 culture-induced, 87 defined in terms of social relations in general, 66, 67, 74, 75, 76, 78, 79, 163 having a social component, 78, 164 in interaction with environmental characteristics, 52, 88, 100, 133, 134, 135, 138, 161, 162, 167, 168 individual differences in, 17, 153, 156 means of measuring or codifying, 17, 38, 47, 129, 177 of developmental importance, 163 personality variables, 17, 34, 177, 195 as determinants of adjustment, 10, 17 psychoanalytically conceived, 74 psychopathology-related, 160 socially desirable, 119 Personal Attributes Questionnaire, 81, 186 Personal Authority in the Family System Questionnaire-Version C, 57 Personal Authority in the Family System Questionnaire-Version C (PAFSQ-C), 186 Personal authority subscale (from the Personal Authority in the Family System Questionnaire-Version C), 58, 186 Personal counselors, 140 Personal disruptions, 202 Personal efficiency subscale (from the College Inventory of Academic Adjustment), 197 Personal Feelings Questionnaire, 77, 78, 186 Personal relations subscale (from the College Inventory of Academic Adjustment), 197 Personal Religiosity Inventory, 95 Personal self (from the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale), 193 Personal Sphere Model, 75 Personal support subscale (from the Residence Hall Climate Inventory), 208 Personal values, 95 Personal Values Scale-Revised, 54 Personal Values Scales-Revised, 186 489 Personal Views Survey, 34 Personal-emotional adjustment, 4, 12, 17, 18, 19, 20, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 29, 31, 32, 33, 34, 35, 37, 46, 47, 54, 56, 57, 62, 63, 64, 67, 69, 70, 73, 75, 79, 80, 83, 84, 86, 87, 90, 91, 92, 96, 97, 99, 101, 102, 103, 107, 109, 110, 111, 113, 114, 115, 117, 121, 126, 128, 129, 130, 132, 133, 141, 152, 157 Personal-Emotional Adjustment subscale (from the SACQ), 3, 8, 9, 10, 13, 14, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 42, 44, 45, 46, 47, 48, 49, 50, 51, 52, 53, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 61, 62, 63, 65, 66, 68, 70, 71, 73, 74, 75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 84, 86, 87, 88, 89, 91 , 92, 93, 94, 95, 96, 97, 98, 101, 102, 103, 104, 105, 106, 108, 109, 110, 111, 112, 113, 114, 115, 116, 118, 119, 120, 121, 123, 126, 127, 128, 129, 130, 131, 132, 133, 135, 136, 137, 138, 139, 140, 143, 144, 147, 151, 152, 154, 155, 173, 174, 176, 177, 180, 184, 186, 187, 188, 191, 196, 199, 201, 204, 210 Personality Research Form, 186 Personality traits associated with eating problems score (from the Eating Disorder Inventory), 27 Personality variables. (see Person characteristics, personality variables Person-environment factors, 138 Persosnal-Emotional Adjustment subscale (from the SACQ), 14, 22 Pessimism, 23 Phobic anxiety subscale (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 25, 196 Physical health, 4, 8, 9, 17, 18, 28, 30, 157, 196, 199 maintenance of, 11 variables associated with or contributing to, 30 Physical Maltreatment by Others Scale, 208 Physical self (from the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale), 193 Physical symptoms (from the Strain Questionnaire), 9, 28, 199 Physical symptoms (from the Stress Audit), 199 Physical well-being, sense of, 4, 8 Physical-environment factors, 138 Physically disabled students, 30, 41, 195 types of, 30 Planning, 31, 32, 48, 53, 161 academic/educational, 10, 17, 48, 50, 53, 162 indecision, 50 of academic efforts, efficiency in, 10 use of time, 10 vocational, 6, 17, 48, 50, 156, 162 Play subscale (from the Early Resources Checklist), 202 Positive Affect scale (from the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule), 22 Positive afftect (from the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule), 186 Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, 22, 26, 186 Positive emotionality (from the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire), 184 Positive Emotionality Scale (from the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire), 22 Positive illusions, 38 Positive self-fulfillment (from the California Psychological Inventory-Revised), 30 Positive symptom distress subscale (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 196 Positive symptom distress subscale (from the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised), 199 Positive Symptom Total Score (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 196 Positive Symptom Total Score (from the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised), 199 Postdivorce family structure, 120 Postmatriculation test administration, 11, 20, 21, 22, 26, 36, 43, 46, 50, 68, 85, 98, 137, 138, 159, 160, 162, 195, 199 Practicing-mirroring subscale (from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence), 39 Predominantly Black college/university, 83, 135, 165, 167 Predominantly Black high school, 135 Predominantly Black sponsored activities, 136 Predominantly White college/university, 82, 83, 84, 134, 135, 136, 165, 167 490 Predominantly White high school, 135 Predominantly White sponsored activities, 136 Predominantly White student samples, 135 Preference for one's college over others, 10, 157 Prematriculation, 44 expectations, 11, 12, 42, 43, 44, 85, 136, 138, 161 interventions, 145, 146, 147, 148, 153, 169 measures typically employed prematriculation, 6, 11, 42, 86, 161 test administration, 20, 21, 26, 35, 36, 42, 46, 48, 50, 53, 66, 67, 85, 102, 136, 145, 159, 160, 162, 195, 199 Prestatie Motivatie Test (PMT), 38, 47, 48, 186 Primary Appraisal Emotions Scale, 45, 186 Primary appraisals in the coping process, 44, 160 benefit, 45, 186, 187 challenge, 45, 186 harm, 45, 186, 187 threat, 45, 186, 187 Principal components analysis, 25, 40, 66 Prior knowledge about college life, 98 Problem focused strategies, 31 Problem solving effectiveness, 40, 41 Problem Solving Inventory, 25, 40, 41, 67, 187 Problem-focused coping (from the Adolescent Coping Orientation for Problem Experiences), 32, 177 Problem-focused coping (from the Revised Ways of Coping Checklist), 191 Problem-solving subscale (from the Coping Response Indices Inventory), 34 Professors, 153 amount of contact with, 142, 168 attitude toward, 157 interacting with, 7, 151 perception of, 7, 141 relationship with, 142, 158 Protective subscale (from the Parental Bonding Instrument), 109 Providing emotional support score (from the Parental Attachment Questionnaire), 60 Pseudoautonomous students (from the Late Adolescent Individuation Questionnaire), 60, 61 Pseudoautonomy (from the Pseudoautonomy Scale), 68 Pseudoautonomy Scale, 68, 187 Psychasthenia subscale (from the Mini-Mult), 198 Psychoanalytically conceived variables, 74, 164 Psychological abuse. (see Abuse, psychological) Psychological attachment to parents. (see Parental attachment) Psychological Coping Resources Scale, 36 Psychological Distress Inventory, 21, 101, 102, 208 Psychological health. (see Mental health) Psychological separation from parents, 55, 58, 60, 61, 163 as a mark of maturation, 59 excessive focus on, 59 measures of, 55, 56, 57, 58 negative forms of, 58 positive forms of, 58, 59 self-assessed, 55 welcome lack of, 57 Psychological Separation Inventory (PSI), 55, 56, 57, 59, 187 psychometric problems with, 56 Psychological services, 8, 12, 157 Psychological stability, 10 491 Psychological well-being, sense of, 4, 8 Psychology courses, 175 Psychometric instruments, 17, 160, 163 Psychopathic deviate subscale (from the Mini-Mult), 198 Psychopathology, 19, 30, 160 underlying predispositions to, 29 Psychosocial competence, 33 Psychosocial Maturity Inventory, 58 Psychosocial Maturity Scale, 190 Psychoticism subscale (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 196 Puerto Rican Index, 88 Puerto Rican students, 86 attending a Puerto Rican college, 88, 195 born and raised in mainland U.S. attending U.S. colleges, 195 born and raised in mainland U. S., attending U. S. colleges, 88 Purdue University, 173 Purpose, sense of/purposefulness, 6, 50, 156, 161 Purpose-in-Life Test, 50, 190 Quantitative score (from the Scholastic Aptitude Test), 191, 192 Queen's University (Ontario), 174 Racial composition of campus activities. (see Extracurricular activities in college, racial composition of) Racial discrimination, 142 Racial Identity Attitude Scale (RIAS), 84 Racial identity, attitude towards, 84, 85, 165 Racial status, 84, 86 Racial/ethnic tension, 141 Rational thinking about oneself (from the Attitudes and Beliefs Scale-II), 45 Realistic self-appraisal. (see Self-appraisal) Reassurance of worth subscale (from the Social Provisions Scale), 130, 210 Recollections of the parents as loving in early interactions (from the Parent-Child Relations Questionnaire II), 111 Recreational/play activities, 132 Redundant variables, 161, 164 Regularly admitted students, 147 Rejection expectancy subscale (from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence), 57, 192 Relationship density, 133 Relationship patterns within family, 56, 116 primary relationship between student and a parent, 116 primary relationship between the parents, 116 Relationship Questionnaire, 64, 190 Relationships close, 72 disruption of, 71, 104, 166 formation and maintenance of, 7, 68, 69, 70, 71, 74, 149, 157 with age-mates, 70 with teachers, 70 inordinate need to establish, 71 interracial/interethnic, 136 number of close friends, 7, 157 of quality, 7 past, 37, 104, 149 reciprocally close and interdependent, 71 romantic, 7, 100, 104, 157 492 break-up of, 104 with college staff persons, 158 with parents. (see Parents, student relationship with) with professors, 142, 158, 168 Relatives, 103 Relatives, percentage of in social networks, 121, 126 Relaxation training, 150 Reliability, 1 Reliable alliance subscale (from the Social Provisions Scale), 210 Religion importance of in family, 95 Religiosity (from the Revised Ways of Coping Checklist), 191 Religious affiliation, 95, 164 Religious organization, member of, 95 Religiousness, 95 Relocation away from home and significant persons there, 4 Reminiscence therapy, 155 Representations of people, complexity of (from the Social Cognition and Object Relations Scale), 75 Residence Hall Climate Inventory (RHCI), 139, 140, 208 Residence halls. (see Dormitories) Residential college, 140 Resources, management of, 7 Retrospective assessment of adjustment, 9 Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale, 76, 191, 195 Revised Ways of Coping Checklist, 32, 33, 191 Ritualization (from the Family Ritual Questionnaire), 107 Role Relationship Inventory (RRI), 117, 118, 209 Romantic relationships. (see Relationships, romantic) Roommates freshman, 138, 168 percentage of in social networks, 126 upper class, 138, 168 Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, 26, 35, 36, 37, 67 Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale), 191 Rutgers University, 174 SACQ. (see Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire) Salience Inventory, 53, 191 San Diego State University, 174 SAT. (see Scholastic Aptitude Test) Satisfaction with academic environment, 4, 29 with academic major, 6, 53, 156, 162 with being in college in general, 4 with college experience, 10, 12, 157 with college in which enrolled, 4, 10, 157 with college work routine, 10 with courses, 10 with occupational preparation by college, 10, 157 with orientation program upon completion, 147 with social environment, 4 with social networks, 127 with social support, 128, 129, 211 Satisfaction with family relationship score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 493 Satisfaction with Life Scale, 22, 191 Scale to Assess World Views, 100 Scapegoat role (from the Children's Role Inventory), 117 Scapegoat role (from the Children's Roles Inventory), 202 Scapegoat role (from the Family Role Behavior Inventory), 117, 118 Scapegoat role (from the Role Relationship Inventory), 118, 209 Schizophrenia subscale (from the Mini-Mult), 198 Scholarly effort, 54 Scholastic achievement in high school, measures of, 146 Scholastic aptitude, 91, 92, 146, 152, 164 quantitative, 91 verbal, 91 Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT), 6, 86, 91, 92, 142, 146, 191 composite score, 91 quantitative score, 91 verbal score, 91 Schwartz Value Survey-Form A, 87 Science courses, 173 Score distribution extremes, 154 Second generation college attenders, 35, 54, 90, 123, 195, 212 Seeks support (from the Revised Ways of Coping Checklist), 191 Self, 161 as focus of one's attention, 74 as focus of one's interest, 74, 164 as primary determinant of adjustment to college, 48 development of, 37 expectancies about, 160 facets of, 161 feelings about, 37 in relation to one's world, 67, 164 in relation to others, 164 integrity of, 10, 35, 160 physical, 197 seeking information about, to change, 49 unitary, 161 Self-appraisal, 160, 161 as aspects or determinants of adjustment, 35 realistic, 45, 160 variables related to, 35, 38, 41, 44, 45, 47, 48, 160 Self-centeredness subscale (from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence), 39, 192 Self-concept, 30, 37, 38, 39 academic, 54 familial, 38 improvement, 150 moral-ethical, 38 physical, 38 social, 38 Self-confidence, 33, 35, 38, 160 regarding capacity for adjusting to college, 11, 42, 161, 170 social, 39 Self-consciousness, 74 Self-Consciousness Scales, 74, 192 Self-disillusionment. (see Adjustive capacity, disillusionment regarding) Self-effectiveness. (see Perceived self-effectiveness/ineffectiveness) 494 Self-efficaciousness, sense of, 45 Self-efficacy, 34, 35, 36, 37, 41, 44, 47, 160 "as a person", 37 academic, 40, 41 as student-athletes, 41 athletic, 41 in physically disabled students, 41 regarding capacity for adjusting to college, 42 regarding cognitive activity, 40, 41 regarding completion of psychology courses, 40 regarding impending transition, 42 regarding mathematics, 40 regarding particular areas of function or activities, 38, 40 social, 39, 41 variants of, 38 Self-Efficacy for Broad Academic Milestones Scale, 40, 192 Self-Efficacy Scale, 37, 39, 192 Self-esteem, 10, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39, 73, 160, 170, 197 general measures of, 35 Self-esteem support subscale (from the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List), 204 Self-hate guilt subscale (from the Interpersonal Guilt Questionnaire-45), 77, 181, 182 Self-integrity, sense of, 10, 35, 160, 197 Self-other relations, particular aspects of, 164 Self-Perception Profile for College Students, 25, 40, 67 Self-regard, variables related to, 35, 38, 41, 44, 45, 47, 48, 160, 161 Self-reliance (from the Psychosocial Maturity Scale), 190 Self-Reliance scale (from the Psychosocial Maturity Inventory), 58 Self-Report Family Inventory-Version II, 106, 108, 209 Self-responsibility, 58 Self-satisfaction (from the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale), 193 Self-world compatibility. (see Alienation) Semester first, 9, 12, 13, 14, 20, 21, 26, 31, 36, 46, 51, 53, 61, 64, 65, 66, 67, 76, 94, 134, 135, 136, 137, 138, 143, 148, 149, 150, 151, 152, 153, 154, 155, 169, 173, 176, 195, 199, 200, 212 second, 9, 12, 13, 14, 20, 26, 35, 36, 51, 53, 64, 65, 66, 67, 76, 134, 135, 137, 138, 143, 147, 150, 151, 152, 154, 163, 169, 173, 176, 195, 199, 200, 212 third, 9, 148 Senior year, 14, 42, 43, 44, 63, 92 in college, 6, 12, 76, 127, 195 in high school, 97, 138, 145, 146, 169, 212 Seniors, 57, 77, 92 in college, 6, 63, 76, 92, 93, 127, 147, 148, 174, 195 in high school, college bound, 137, 145, 169, 170 Sense of coherence, 34, 35 Sense of Coherence Questionnaire (SOCQ), 34, 192 Separation from family, 11, 108, 157, 166 from friends at home, 157 from high school friends, 11 from parents. (see Psychological separation from parents) Separation anxiety disorder, 24, 25, 160 a more benign form of, 25 Separation anxiety subscale (from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence), 25, 59, 192 Separation Anxiety Test, 58, 192 495 Separation guilt subscale (from the Interpersonal Guilt Questionnaire-45), 77, 78, 181, 182 Separation-individuation process, 38, 60 adult behavioral characteristics resulting from disturbances in, 59 resolution of, 70 Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence (SITA), 25, 39, 57, 58, 59, 69, 192 Sex-role orientation, 81, 82, 164 Sexual abuse. (see Abuse, sexual) Sexual experiences as a child with an adult subscale (from the Childhood Sexual Experiences Questionnaire), 104 Sexual experiences as a child with another child subscale (from the Childhood Sexual Experiences Questionnaire), 104 Sexual experiences as a nonconsenting adolescent subscale (from the Childhood Sexual Experiences Questionnaire), 104 Shame, 77, 78, 164, 186 experienced in relation to fathers, 78 experienced in relation to mothers, 78 Shame subscale (from the Test of Self-Conscious Affect), 194 Siblings, 103 number of, 122 Sick or Lost Child role (from the Role Relationship Inventory), 118, 209 Sign language, 97 Social activities and functioning in general, extent and success of, 4 Social adjustment, 7, 12, 23, 26, 27, 31, 32, 33, 39, 40, 41, 42, 49, 51, 54, 57, 58, 61, 62, 63, 64, 66, 67, 69, 70, 72, 73, 75, 76, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 84, 86, 88, 89, 90, 94, 96, 97, 99, 100, 103, 105, 107, 109, 111, 113, 114, 115, 116, 117, 118, 120, 122, 123, 124, 125, 127, 128, 132, 136, 140, 141, 143, 144, 148, 150, 153, 157, 170 aspects of, 4 Social Adjustment subscale (from the SACQ), 3, 4, 9, 10, 12, 13, 14, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 33, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39, 41, 42, 44, 45, 46, 47, 48, 49, 50, 51, 52, 55, 57, 58, 59, 60, 61, 62, 63, 65, 66, 67, 68, 69, 70, 71, 73, 74, 75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 81, 83, 84, 85, 87, 88, 89, 90, 91, 92, 93, 94, 95, 97, 98, 101, 103, 104, 105, 106, 108, 109, 110, 111, 112, 113, 114, 118, 122, 123, 124, 125, 126, 127, 128, 129, 130, 131, 132, 133, 135, 136, 137, 138, 139, 140, 141, 142, 143, 144, 145, 146, 147, 148, 149, 150, 151, 153, 154, 155, 173, 174, 176, 177, 180, 184, 186, 187, 188, 191, 196, 199, 201, 204, 210 Social anxiety (from the Social Avoidance and Distress Scale), 76, 164 Social approval, need for, 76, 164 Social Avoidance and Distress Scale, 76, 193 Social Cognition and Object Relations Scale (SCORS), 74 Social competence, 164 Social confidence, 39 Social confidence, lack of (from the Interpersonal Dependency Inventory), 39 Social desirability, 119 Social environment general characteristics of, 123, 125, 126, 128, 131, 132, 133, 167 particular aspects of, 133, 134, 135, 136, 137, 138, 139, 141, 142, 143, 167 satisfaction with, 4 Social integration subscale (from the Social Provisions Scale), 130, 210 Social interests, 66 Social involvement, 38 detachment and isolation from, 71 Social Network Questionnaire, 126, 209 adaptation of, 126, 127 Social Network Questionnaire (adapted), 210 Social networks, 126, 127, 128, 133 aspects of, 127 composition of, 126, 167 conflicted/unconflicted, 127 496 establishing, 150 fun function of, 126 function of, 126, 167 gender findings, 127 percentage of friends in, 126 percentage of relatives in, 121, 126 percentage of students in, 126 roommate percentage in, 126 satisfaction with, 127 size of, 126, 127 Social propensity, 66, 67, 68, 164 Social Propensity Scale, 193 Social Provisions Scale, 130, 210 Social Provisions Scale-Parent Version, 110, 210 Social Readjustment Rating Scale, 102 Social relations, 66, 67, 74, 75, 76, 78, 79, 99, 163 capacity for forming, 68, 69 with instructors, 10 with peers, 10 Social resources, 67 Social resources, availability of, 131 Social self (from the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale), 193, 194 Social self-efficacy, 39, 41, 192 Social skills, 66 training program, 170 Social subscale (of the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale), 39 Social support, 71, 127, 129, 133, 139, 149 amount of need for, 130 amount received, 130 and ethnicity, 168 as buffer, 132 emotional, 124, 128 felt need for, 127, 129 frequency of, 125, 126, 130, 205 from a strong person, 131 from authority figures, 124, 125, 130, 205 from campus environment in general, 132 from college friends, 124, 128 from coursemates, 124 from faculty, 124 from family, 110, 111, 112, 123, 124, 128, 130, 166, 167 from family and close friends combined, 125, 126, 205 from family, friends, and significant others combined, 129 from family/kin, 128 from friends, 83, 100, 123, 128, 167, 208 from parents, 111, 210 from peers, 124, 130, 148, 149, 205 from pre-college friends, 124 in Asians, 110, 124 in Blacks, 83, 110, 124, 165 in earlier years of life, 167 in general, 128, 129, 167 in Hispanic students, 130 in Latinos, 110, 124 497 in White students, 83, 110, 124, 165 informational, 124 instrumental, 124, 128 level of, 83, 132, 165, 168 motivational, 124 networks, 127, 128 number of persons obtained from, 129 perceived, 131, 132, 166 satisfaction with, 127, 128, 129, 131, 201, 211 satisfaction with frequency of, 125, 126, 130, 205 social companionship, 124 sources of, 128, 131, 132 Social Support Inventory, 129, 130, 210 Social Support Questionnaire, 128, 129, 211 Social-living circumstances, 93 Socially desirable responses, 143 Socially-defined feeling states, 77, 164 Socio-economic status, 90, 164 Somatic subscale (from the Hopkins Symptom Checklist), 198 Somatization subscale (from the Brief Symptom Inventory), 18, 196 Sophomore year, 7, 12, 14, 42, 43, 53, 63, 84, 91, 92, 125, 134, 141, 154, 163, 167 Sophomores, 77, 92, 93, 94, 147, 148 Sorority membership, 141, 168 Special admission status. (see Admission status) Spheres of Control Battery, 33, 67 Spiritual support, 133 Spiritual Well-Being Scale (SWBS), 22 Stable attributions (from the Attributional Style Questionnaire), 178 State University of New York, New Paltz, 174 State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), 23, 24, 199 Status variables, 80, 164 Stepfather families, 133 Strain Questionnaire, 9, 28, 199 Stress, 8, 31, 32, 34, 44, 64, 95, 99, 101, 102, 104, 105, 133, 157, 166, 204 experience of, 26 failure to confront source of, 31 management of one's perception of or emotional response to, 31 means of coping with, 31 Stress Audit, 9, 26, 28, 199 Stress Level Scale (from the Psychological Distress Inventory), 102, 208 Structural Family Interaction Scale-Revised, 106, 108, 110, 116 Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (SACQ) adaptation of, as measure of situation-specific appraisal of control, 48 adaptation of, to assess adjustment retrospectively, 9 as a means of constructing treatment and control groups, 154, 169 as a means of identifying at-risk students, 153, 154, 155, 168, 169, 170 as a means of identifying students varying in adjustment, 153 as a means of measuring effects of intervention, 145, 146, 147, 150, 151, 152, 154, 155, 169, 170 as a source of dependent variables, 16 as a source of interview topics, 154, 169, 170 composition and structure of, 3, 4, 13 conditions of administration, 142, 143, 163 consistency of, 13 developers of, 1, 80 498 earlier version of, 53, 66, 67 facets of adjustment area addressed by a subscale, 4 first semester/second semester correlations, 13, 14 item-clusters, 4, 28, 29, 158 items, 3, 4, 85, 130, 158 limitations or flaws of, 1 manual, 1, 2, 174, 176, 194, 199, 211 unpublished addenda to tables, 2 patterns of subscale scores, 158 practical usefulness of, 153 primary subscales, 3, 4 reliability of, 1 research-derived correlates of, 3, 4, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 156, 157, 158 score distribution extremes, 154 second semester findings. (see Semester) shorter version of, 53, 85, 130 sophomore year/senior year correlations, 14 subscales, 3 behavioral and experiential indices associated differentially with, 5, 158 intercorrelations among, 3, 4 intraindividual variation in scores, 4, 15, 156 item-clusters within. (see Item-clusters) primary. (see Primary subscales) validity of, 1 Student athletes foreign, 89 Student Developmental Task and Lifestyle Inventory, 50, 54, 69, 193 Student, characteristics of. (see Person characteristics) Student-athletes, 7, 37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 49, 89, 92, 98, 99, 100, 131, 140, 142, 143, 150, 151, 173, 175, 180, 193, 194, 195 domestic, 89 in-state, 144 out-of-state, 144 Student-Oriented Life Events Survey, 101, 211 Student-parent relationship. (see Parents, student relationship with) Students of color, 110, 124 Students, percentage of in social networks, 126 Study habits, 6, 17, 157 Study skills, 10, 17 Study skills subscale (from the College Inventory of Academic Adjustment), 197 Subjective satisfaction score (from the Social Support Inventory), 210, 211 Success/failure (capacity to enjoy/endure), 33 Superiority Scale, 45, 193 Support need (from the Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule), 201 Support satisfaction (from the Arizona Social Support Interview Schedule), 201 Surgency-intellect, 66, 67 Survivor guilt subscale (from the Interpersonal Guilt Questionnaire-45), 77, 181 Symptom Checklist-90-Revised, 19, 199 Tangible support subscale (from the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List), 204 Task assistance score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209 Task assistance score (from the Social Network Questionnaire, adapted), 210 Task-oriented subscale (from the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations), 179 Teacher enmeshment subscale (from the Separation-Individuation Test of Adolescence), 70, 192, 193 499 Teacher support subscale (from the Classroom Environment Scale), 132 Tennessee Self-Concept Scale, 30, 36, 37, 38, 39, 193 Test conditions, effects of. (see Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire, conditions of administration) Test manual (for the SACQ). (see Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire, manual) Test of Self-Conscious Affect (TOSCA), 78, 194 Texas Social Behavior Inventory, 39, 194 Time spent actively involved subscale (from the Father-Daughter Relationship Inventory), 204 Time, management of, 7 Total P Score (from the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale), 193, 194 Total Pathology Score (from the Mini-Mult), 19, 198 Total Severity of Psychopathology Score (from the Bell Global Psychopathology Scale), 19 Traditional-age students. (see Age, traditional/nontraditional age students) Trait, 8, 23 general, 160 specific, 160 Type A, 30 Transfer students, 148, 150, 169 Transition from high school to college, 8 facilitation of, 170 Translation of motivation to academic effort, 4, 29 Trauma, in the first 15 years of life, 103, 132 Triangulation subscale (from the Personal Authority in the Family System Questionnaire-Version C), 58, 186 Trust subscale (from the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment), 61, 62, 68, 182, 183 Type A traits, 30 Understanding of causal factors in interpersonal relationships (from the Social Cognition and Object Relations Scale), 75 Undifferentiated sex-role orientation, 81 Universities, SACQ differences among, 95 east or west coast, 82 private or public, 82 University 101 course, 146 University of California at Los Angeles, 174 University of Hartford, 176 University of Maryland, Baltimore, 174 University of Missouri-Columbia, 174 University of Oregon, 176 University of Pennsylvania, 174 University of Wisconsin, Madison, 174, 176 Upper class years. (see College year level) Validity, 1, 54 Value and attitude similarity score (from the Social Network Questionnaire), 209 Value characteristics of students, 87 culture-induced, 87 Values American, accepting of, 90 personal, 95 Valuing of Achievement (from the Life Values Inventory), 48, 95, 183 Verbal score (from the Scholastic Aptitude Test), 191, 192 Virginia Commonwealth University, 174 Visits home, frequency of, 7, 157 Vocational Identity Scale of My Vocational Situation, 50, 184 Vocational plans. (see Planning, vocational) 500 Weekend classes students, 141 Weight Management, Eating, and Exercise Habits Questionnaire, 27 Well-being, sense of, 22, 23, 34 existential, 22 personal, 22 West European international students, 195 White students, 21, 33, 39, 45, 82, 83, 84, 85, 86, 97, 98, 109, 111, 124, 128, 130, 135, 136, 147, 165, 175, 195, 200, 211 in high school, 134 social support in, 83, 110, 124, 165 Wishful thinking (fromthe Revised Ways of Coping Checklist), 191 Withdrawal from college, 9, 12, 154, 155, 157, 170, 176 Women. (see Gender findings) Work orientation (from the Psychosocial Maturity Scale), 190 Work Orientation scale (from the Psychosocial Maturity Inventory), 58 World view, 100 Yale University, 174 York University (Canada), 174 Young Adult Alcohol Problems Screening Test, 79 Young Adult Social Support Inventory (YA-SSI), 129, 211 501