Digital Technology and Academic Integrity Running head: DIGITAL TECHNOLOGY AND ACADEMIC INTEGRITY Digital Technology and Academic Integrity Thomas Calabrese, Jason Stephens, Michael Young University of Connecticut 1 Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 2 Abstract Given the popular media’s attention to an apparent rise in digital cheating, it is important to educators that those claims be measured, tested, and qualified. An on-line study of 116 college students was conducted. The study focuses on determining whether undergraduates report engaging in forms of digital cheating more frequently than traditional (non-technical) forms of cheating. Additionally, the study investigates the relations between students’ engagement in digital cheating, their technical knowledge, and their involvement in Internet based socializations (i.e., on-line gaming, digital communications, etc.) A greater proportion of students reported engaging in conventional collaboration compared to its digital counterpart (47% versus 35%) with the notable exception of plagiarism (38% versus 51%). The study revealed students who engage in conventional cheating are also likely engaging in some form of digital cheating. Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 3 Digital Technology and Academic Integrity The growth in classroom and communication technology has created an increased capacity for access to information (i.e., digital phones, Internet search engines, digital cameras, calculators, etc.) As these technologies continue to evolve and become commonplace in the classroom and at home, they become more familiar to students. These technologies have many educational advantages, especially in a constructivist classroom setting. They also may make the possibility of academic misconduct easier. Increasingly, digital technologies are cited as being more commonplace in cheating scenarios. While the problem of academic cheating has long since been characterized as “epidemic” (Haines, Diekoff, LaBeff, & Clark, 1986, p. 775), recent reports in the popular press (e.g., Thomas, 2001, McCarroll, 2001) have suggested that digital cheating is on the rise, particularly Internet plagiarism. While anecdotal accounts abound and it appears likely the use of technology may facilitate or amplify the cheating problem, little empirical research has been conducted to substantiate the growing concern (for an exception see McCabe, 2005). In his study McCabe surveyed almost 50,000 undergraduates on more than 60 campuses since the fall of 2002 regarding their cheating behaviors (CAI Research, 2005). Data from that study suggest that 70% of students admit to some form of cheating. 25% admit to serious test cheating in the past year and 50% admitted to cheating on written assignments. In a related longitudinal study conducted by the Center for Academic Integrity data from nine medium to large universities suggests that disallowed collaboration between students rose from 11% in 1963 to 49% in 1993 (CAI Research, 2002). Clearly cheating in a general sense is prevalent on the university campus. Both of Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 4 these surveys pay only tacit attention to the realm of digital cheating, as it is a more recent phenomenon. The present University of Connecticut research seeks to help fill the gap in the empirical work concerning digital cheating and to seek ways of addressing this behavior. The primary purpose of the present pilot study is to determine what college students report about their own digital cheating behaviors, characterize the level of technical competency of the student sample, and determine relationships between characteristics of the sample. It is our beliefs that: 1.) students who would cheat using traditional means would use digital technologies to cheat if they had the technical competency to do so; 2.) most students believe that digital technologies can be used to cheat; and 3.) affordances (from an eco-psych perspective) created by technologies may foster cheating behaviors in-situ for those students that might not otherwise cheat. Method Participants in the pilot study were undergraduates from two universities in the northeastern United States. The sample of 116 students was: 59% male; 35% seniors, 33% juniors, 20% sophomores, and 12% freshmen. The final sample will include approximately 1,000 undergraduates from several universities in the southeastern and northeastern United States. The questionnaire was administered online. All students in pilot study completed the questionnaire in May 2005 and were asked to confine their responses to the 2004-2005 academic year. Data for the final sample will be collected in December 2005. Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 5 The measures for this pilot survey were obtained from a questionnaire fashioned similarly to noted studies on academic misconduct in the traditional sense with added emphasis (and particular questions) regarding digital forms of cheating. The questionnaire was administered online and included original items as well as measures adapted from McCabe’s (2001) Survey of Academic Integrity, Beck and Ajzen (1991) moral responsibility scale, and Diekoff’s et al. (1996) neutralization scale. Cheating behavior. The measure of cheating included original items as well as items adapted from McCabe (2001). Students were asked to respond to a set of 12 academic behaviors by indicating on a five-point Likert-type scale how often they engaged in each behavior during the current academic year (0= Never, 1= Once, 2= Twice, 3= Three times, 4= Four or more times). The twelve items included six traditional (non-digitally technological) forms of cheating and six analogous forms of digital cheating. For example, students were asked to indicate how often they “copied a few sentences from a book, magazine, or journal (not electronic or Web-based) without footnoting them in a paper you submitted” as well as how often they “copied a few sentences from the Internet without footnoting them”. Judgment of responsibility. The measure of moral responsibility was adapted from Beck and Ajzen (1991) and assessed the extent to which students judge cheating (or refraining from doing so) as a personal moral responsibility. Specifically, students used a five-point Likert-type scale (1= Strongly Disagree to 5= Strongly Agree) to indicate to what extent they agreed with statement such as, “It would be morally wrong for me to cheat on a test or exam” and “Cheating on tests or exams goes against my moral principles” (4 items; = .71). Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 6 Neutralization of responsibility. The measure of neutralization was adapted from Diekoff et al. (1996) and assessed the extent to students minimized personal responsibility for engaging in academic cheating by displacing responsibility to social or situational circumstances. Students used a five-point Likert-type scale (1= Strongly disagree to 5= Strongly agree) to rate the extent to which they “should not be blamed for cheating if…,” for example, “everyone else in the room seems to be cheating,” a “friend asked him to help him/her cheat,” or “the instructor left the room” (items = 10; = .93). Acceptability of digital cheating. Students’ perception of the acceptability of digital cheating was assessed with an original measure. Using a five-point Likert-type scale (1= Strongly disagree to 5= Strongly agree), students indicated the what extent they agreed with statement such as, “Computers and other digital technologies have made cheating on homework, papers and exams more acceptable.” and “I don’t feel as bad or guilty when I use digital technologies (such as computers, cell phones, and PDAs) to cheat on homework, papers, and exams.” (6 items; = .83). Technological ability. Students’ technological ability was assessed with an original scale. Using a five-point Likert-type scale (1= Never used to 5= Expert) to rate their ability to use various digital technologies, such as PDA, or perform various tasks, such as “install computer software” or “hack a computer or network” (7 items; = .81). Demographic information. Participants provided information about their gender and year in school. Finally, the questionnaire included one open ended question: “some students believe that cheating is justified in certain situations and some students believe that it is always wrong. What do you think?” Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 7 Analysis Guidelines Because students’ self-reported engagement in conventional and digital forms were positively skewed (2.67 and 2.99, respectively), we dichotomized both of these variables. For conventional cheating, students who reported that they never cheated (30%) were coded with a “0” while those who reported engaging at least once in one of the six behaviors (70%) were coded with a “1”. Similarly, students who reported that they did not engage in any of the six digital forms of cheating (34%) were coded with a “0” while those who reported doing so (66%) were coded with a “1”. Nonparametric analytic techniques are deemed the most appropriate for data that is not normally distributed. We conducted both parametric and nonparametric analyses. The results were highly similar and in this report we report the results from the parametric analyses because they are more easily interpreted. Results The results of our investigation helped us both better understand the characteristics of the sample group, as well as, compare some of the key measures regarding how people cheat. The sample was drawn from two universities in the northeastern United States and subjects were chosen using a convenience sample. The following results can be used to gauge the level of technical competency of the respondents. 69.8% of students surveyed identified themselves as either expert or advanced with respect to their use of technology. 13.8% felt as though they would be able to successfully hack into a computer at the advanced or expert level. 78.2% felt they would be able to adequately install computer software. 25% of respondents play massively multi-player on-line role playing games (MMORPGS). 29.3% see themselves Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 8 as video gamers. 96.6% of students think of themselves as fair and honest. 88% believe that plagiarism is cheating. 74.8% believe that computers and other digital technologies have made cheating easier. The students ranked the forms of technology that they felt could be used to cheat. The rankings are shown in Table 1 and indicate that without exception students felt strongly that all forms of technology listed could in someway be used to cheat. With respect to our question concerning the relative frequency of conventional and digital cheating, results indicated significant differences on three of the six paired behaviors. As seen in Table 2, a significantly greater proportion of students reported engaging in conventional not permitted collaboration compared to its digital counterpart (47% versus 35%, respectively). Similarly, more students reported using handwritten “cheat sheets” to cheat on a test as opposed to using notes stored on a digital device, such as a phone, PDA or calculator (19% versus 7%, respectively). Conversely, while only 38% of students reported conventional plagiarism (i.e., copying a few sentences from written material without referencing the source), 51% reported engaging in digital plagiarism (i.e., cutting and pasting a few sentences from the Internet without attribution). These results suggest that plagiarism seems to be most impacted by student access to digital technology than any other form of cheating. However, it is important to note that there was not a significant difference in students’ overall self-reported rates of conventional and digital cheating. This suggest that students who engage in some of form of conventional cheating are also likely engaging in some form of digital cheating as well. Although not discussed here, the nonparametric analyses presented in Table 3 offers support for this conjecture. Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 9 As seen in Table 4, correlational analyses indicated a strong positive relation between student engagement in conventional and digital cheating (r =.70). Gender and grade level were not significant to either type of cheating. As hypothesized, students’ technological ability was not significantly related to conventional cheating but it was significantly related to digital cheating. Finally, gender was negatively correlated with technological ability and perceiving digital cheating as more acceptable; women reported feeling less technologically able and were less likely to rate digital cheating as more acceptable. Analysis of one specific group of students (those that play MMORPGS) there was found to be a negative correlation between those students and cheating behaviors Discussion Generally, the results of the study do not agree with the consensus of the popular media that access to technology by students has inflated the sheer numbers of incidents of cheating as compared to non-technological forms of cheating. As seen by the data presented, (with the notable exception of plagiarism) most students who cheat still engage in traditional (non-technical) forms of cheating. Given that a large number of respondents reported having an acceptable grasp of technology, it is not likely that this result reflects any aversion to the use of technology in general. In fact, while most students agree (see Table 1) that almost any kind of technology could be used to cheat most do not report doing so. The data do support the notion that students who would cheat anyway are incorporating digital techniques into their “cheating repertoire” as circumstances become aligned with that behavior. This is seen in the specific case of Internet based plagiarism. Coincident with the stabilization of information resources on the Internet (access to Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 10 papers, articles, moderated journals, etc.) we see a rise in the level of on-line plagiarism as compared to traditional forms of plagiarism. This implies that the alignment of the technical prowess of the student, the availability of appropriate technology, and opportunities to capitalize on the cheating behavior increases the likeliness that a student may choose to act. One plausible way to explain the tendencies toward digital cheating is to consider the situated nature of the act of cheating (Brown, Collins & Duguid, 1989). From this perspective, cheating occurs in-the-moment, and is not subject to much if any reasoning, planning, or reflection. From this ecological view (Young, 2004) cheating is embedded in the momentary context and driven by the invitations given by the affordances that contemporary technologies provide (Wilson, 2002). Students’ goals emerge in the moment and the “online thinking” in conjunction with the cheating act is uninformed by the “offline thinking” associated with reasoning and judgment. It is through the ecopsychological perspective that we are studying technologies’ role in providing the affordances for cheating that certain students benefit from, based on the effectivities that they have evolved based on their experience with technology. In this context, we postulate that for most instances of cheating (e.g., test cheating, copying homework, etc), technological affordances are not appropriately aligned with situations in which cheating may take place or are being restricted (e.g., policies against the in-class use of a cell phone) so as to minimize those opportunities. This could be used in devising ways of reducing the number of reported cases of digital cheating. Clearly, the survey data does indicate that when technological affordances are available in a potential cheating situation, the cheating behavior flourishes. Existing Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 11 research (McCabe, 2005) combined with data derived in this study indicate that the specific case of Internet plagiarism is pervasive. This particular case merits further study. It would appear digital plagiarism is clearly aligned with the eco-psychological perspective in that it reflects “on-line” thinking, or thinking in the moment. The data also suggests that even though most students agree that plagiarism is cheating and most students see themselves as fair and honest, a greater percentage of students engage in this form of cheating than in any other. This would suggest that either morale neutralization strategies have been more effective on this cheating behavior than others, or that the act of Internet plagiarism does not involve much morale reasoning at all. It is clear that cheating in all its forms is a problem for educators. As the rise of the use of technology continues (e.g., on-line classes and testing, distributed learning environments, etc.) more opportunities to misuse technology in the academic setting will arise. It is important to better understand the trends in digital cheating such that methods can be devised to curtail or discourage those behaviors. Our research confirms that cheating with technology is a phenomenon still in its infancy. Additional research efforts are underway by this team to further explore the impact of digital cheating on teaching and classroom methodologies, the effect of morale judgment on digital cheating, and ways to effectively combat digital cheating in an ever increasing technology educational environment. Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 12 References Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. Beck, L., & Ajzen, I. (1991). Predicting dishonest actions using the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 285-301. Brown, J. S., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educational Researcher, 18(1), 32-42. CAI Research. (2002). The Center for Academic Integrity. Duke U., Durham. http://www.academicintegrity.org/cai_research.asp. Dewey, J. (1922). Human nature and conduct: An introduction to social psychology. New York: The Modern Library. Diekhoff, G. M., LaBeff, E. E., Clark, R. E., Williams, L. E., Francis, B., & Haines, V. J. (1996). College cheating: Ten years later. Research in Higher Education, 37(4), 487-502. Haines, V. J., Diekoff, G. M., LaBeff, E. E., & Clark, R. E. (1986). College cheating: Immaturity, lack of commitment, and the neutralizing attitude. Research in Higher Education, 25(4), 342-354. Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral development: The nature and validity of moral stages. San Francisco: Harper & Row. Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 13 Kohlberg, L., & Candee, D. (1984). The relationship of moral judgment to moral action. In W. M. Kurtines & J. L. Gewirtz (Eds.), Morality, moral behavior, and moral development. New York: Wiley. Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of a topographical psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. McCabe, D. (2001). Survey of academic behaviors. In J. M. Stephens (Ed.). Stanford, CA. McCabe, D. (2005). New CAI Research. Retrieved July 29, 2005, from http://www.academicintegrity.org/cai_research.asp McCarroll, C. (2001). Beating Web cheaters at their own game. The Christian Science Monitor Retrieved July 29, 2005, from http://www.csmonitor.com/2001/0828/p16s1-lekt.html Sykes, G. M., & Matza, D. (1957). Techniques of neutralization: A theory of delinquency. American Sociological Review, 22, 664-670. Thomas, K. (2001). Net makes cheating as easy as ABC. USA Today, June 19, 2001. [Retrieved online on October 19, 2003 from: http://www.usatoday.com/news/nation/2001-03-20-cheat.htm.] Wilson, M. (2002) Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(4), 625-636 Wright, R. E. (1994). Logistic regression. In G. Grimm & P. R. Yarnold (Eds.), Reading and understanding multivariate statistics (pp. 217-244). Washington: American Psychological Association. Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 14 Young, M. F. (2004). An ecological psychology of instructional design: Learning and thinking by perceiving-acting systems. In D. H. Jonassen (Ed.), Handbook of Research for Educational Communications and Technology (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 15 Tables Table 1 Student’s Ranking of Which Digital Technologies Can Be Used For Cheating Technology Percentage Agree Digital Cellular Phone 93.2 Instant Messaging PDAs Laptops Internet E-mail Calculators Digital watches Thumb drives Digital recorders Digital Cameras MP3 players GPS Other 89.8 85.6 82.4 82.2 80.5 68.6 62.7 61.9 55.9 55.1 41.5 15.3 8.5 Table 2 Mean Proportions and Paired Sample t Statistics for Student Engagement in Conventional versus Digital Forms of Cheating Behavior Cheating Behavior Variable Copied homework Unpermitted collaboration t-Statistic 1.91 2.72* Conventional .23 .47 Digital .16 .35 Plagiarized a few sentences .38 .51 - 3.62* Plagiarized a complete paper .04 .03 1.00 Used “cheat sheets” or stored notes during a test or exam .19 .07 3.45* Copied from someone else during a test or exam .14 .09 1.51 OVERALL .70 .66 1.22 Note. Bonferroni adjustment was used to control for inflation in Type 1 error associated with multiple comparisons: alpha = .05/6 = .008 * p < .008 \ Digital Technology and Academic Integrity 16 Table 3 Percentages of Student’ Self-Reported Engagement in Conventional and/or Digital Forms of Six Cheating Behaviors Variable Copying homework Unpermitted collaboration Cheating Behavior Conventional Digital 11.2 4.3 18.1 6.0 Neither 72.4 47.4 Bo 12. 28. Plagiarizing a few sentences 47.0 1.7 14.8 36. Plagiarizing a complete paper 94.8 2.6 0.9 1.7 Using “cheat sheets” or stored notes during a test or exam 79.1 13.9 1.7 5.2 Copying from someone else during a test or exam 81.7 9.6 4.3 4.3 OVERALL 25.0 9.5 5.2 60. Note. McNemar’s Test was used to determine statistical significance of differences in cell counts. Underlined percentages represent those cases in which the observed count significantly exceeded the expected count. Table 4 Correlation Matrix, Means, and Standard Deviations Variable 1. Conventional Cheating 2. Digital Cheating 1 – .70*** 2 3 - .09 - .12 – 4. Grade Level - .08 - .03 .08 6. Judgment of Responsibility 5 6 – 3. Sex (female=1) 5. Technological Ability 4 – .13 .19* - .43*** .09 – - .27** - .27** .20* - .05 - .12 – 7. Neutralization of Responsibility .28** .35*** - .11 - .10 .12 -.43*** 8. Digital Cheating More Acceptable .26** .39*** - .24** - .10 .16 -.50*** M 0.70 0.66 0.41 2.91 3.46 4.01 SD 0.46 0.48 0.49 1.00 0.71 0.80 Note. N = 113-116. * p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001