Teaching Street Crossings at Streets and Lanes Where There is No

advertisement
AER Orientation and Mobility Division:
Position Paper
Teaching Street Crossing at Streets and Lanes
Where There Is No Traffic Control
Originally approved by O&M Division membership through mail ballot, Spring 2008
Revised 2013
At crosswalks in streets or lanes where vehicles are not controlled by a signal or stop sign (e.g.,
no traffic control), pedestrians can cross either by relying on drivers to yield, or by crossing
when there is a gap in traffic long enough to allow time to cross. Orientation and Mobility (O&M)
programs must prepare consumers who are blind or visually impaired to cross at these
crosswalks by ensuring that they have the information, strategies, and skills necessary to
assess these situations. Strategies and skills include maximizing detection / assessment of
approaching vehicles, recognizing gaps in traffic that are long enough to cross and being able to
determine when you cannot do so, determining yields at the crossing point, and evaluating risk
of crossing--including the likelihood that vehicles that they didn’t detect when they started their
crossing could reach them and the likelihood that these drivers will yield to them. These
strategies and skills support informed decisions concerning street crossings, and are based on
information and knowledge about driver behavior, yielding likelihood, elements of risk, and
individual visual and auditory abilities.
Traditional O&M techniques for crossing at streets with no traffic control (Allen, Barbier, Griffith,
Kern, & Shaw, 1997; Hill & Ponder, 1976; Jacobson, 1993; LaGrow & Weessies, 1994) have
relied on the ability of pedestrians who are blind or visually impaired and have normal hearing to
be able to hear approaching vehicles well enough when it is quiet to be assured that, whenever
it is quiet and they hear no approaching vehicles, there is a sufficient gap in traffic. Although
this strategy of crossing when quiet is reliable in some situations, research and observations
indicate there are situations where it is not possible for a person with normal hearing and
walking speed to hear traffic with sufficient warning to be assured that there is a gap in traffic
long enough to cross even when it is quiet (Sauerburger, 1989, 1995, 1999, 2006, Snook-Hill &
Sauerburger 1996; Wall Emerson, Kim, Naghshineh, Pliskow, & Myers, 2011, Wall Emerson &
Sauerburger, 2008). More recent texts (e.g., Long & LaGrow, 2011) highlight the relative risks
of crossing with a yield or in a perceived gap when crossing at streets with no traffic control.
When relying on vision to gauge gaps in traffic, pedestrians who have low vision perform as well
as those with normal vision (Hassan & Massof, 2012) but all pedestrians tend to make better
crossing decisions when allowed to use both hearing and vision (Hassan, 2012).
Relying on drivers to yield carries some risk as a strategy, as motorists have been found to vary
from 17% to 97% in their yielding behavior, depending on factors such as the speed of vehicles,
the visibility of pedestrians, number of lanes, pedestrian behavior, presence of a long white
cane, and crossing treatments present (crosswalk style, warning flashers, etc. (Bourquin, Wall
Emerson, & Sauerburger, 2011; Fitzpatrick, Turner, Brewer, Carlson, Ullman, Trout, Park,
Whitacre, Lalani, & Lord, 2006; Geruschat & Hassan, 2005). However, pedestrians can perform
actions that will increase the likelihood of yielding. Presence of the long cane increased yielding
rates more than an orange vest or red flag when a pedestrian actively crossed a street
(Bourquin, Wall Emerson, & Sauerburger, 2011) and an assertive pedestrian who walks briskly
toward a crossing also achieved high yielding rates (Schroeder & Rouphail, 2010).
A goal of O&M programs is that consumers will have information about crossings with no traffic
control, including an understanding of the following:
• the fact that, although it is possible to hear or see vehicles well enough in some situations to
know it is clear to cross, there are other situations in which it is not possible to determine with
certainty that there is a gap in traffic long enough to cross, even when it is quiet and no
approaching traffic is visible/audible;
• the time they need to cross streets of various widths, taking into consideration their walking
pace;
• the effects of masking sounds or various lighting/visual conditions on their ability to hear or see
approaching traffic;
• the pedestrian and white cane laws regarding right-of-way;
• the likelihood that drivers of approaching vehicles will yield or avoid hitting them, including:
• visibility factors and the ability of drivers to see the pedestrian from a safe distance,
given the drivers’ need for perception/braking time
• the volume and speed of traffic
• the effect of various road conditions on drivers’ ability to yield
• information about the yielding behavior of drivers in the consumers’ area
• information about the existence of bad drivers, such as those who are distracted or driving
under the influence, etc.;
• alternatives to crossing streets independently.
In addition, among the goals of O&M instruction are that consumers will have the following:
• strategies to maximize their detection of approaching vehicles by audition and/or vision,
including an awareness of when it is appropriate to use which strategy;
• strategies that enable them to assess their ability to detect vehicles including being able to:
• analyze the width of the street and determine the time needed for crossing, taking into
consideration their walking pace;
• recognize situations where they cannot hear or see well enough to reliably predict gaps
in traffic;
• determine the probability that a vehicle that was undetected at the beginning of the
crossing could reach them before the crossing is finished;

strategies for increasing the likelihood of drivers’ yielding such as:


holding a cane prominently,
using an exaggerated arc to begin crossing,


stepping one step into a crossing and pausing,
facing the direction from which traffic will come in the nearest lane;
• techniques for those with sufficient functional vision to
• scan / glance efficiently in both or all directions to determine if there are approaching
vehicles;
• determine if approaching vehicles will allow enough time to complete a crossing;
• strategies to determine alternatives that may exist for crossing streets independently, and
implement them;
• strategies for advocating for features of accessibility and pedestrian safety as needed, such as
traffic calming features, traffic control, buildouts, and refuge islands to narrow the crosswalk, or
other intersection and environmental modifications.
References
Allen, W., Courtney-Barbier, A., Griffith, A., Kern, T., & Shaw, C. (1997). Orientation and
mobility teaching manual (2nd ed.). New York: CIL Publications.
Bourquin, E., Wall Emerson, R., & Sauerburger, D. (2011). Conditions that influence drivers’
yielding behavior for uncontrolled intersections. Journal of Visual Impairment &
Blindness, 105(12), 760-769.
Fitzpatrick, K., Turner, S., Brewer, M., Carlson, P., Ullman, B., Trout, N., Park, E., Whitacre, J.,
Lalani, N., & Lord, D. (2006). TCRP Report 112/NCHRP Report 562 – Improving
Pedestrian Safety at Unsignalized Crossings. Transit Cooperative Research Program
and
National Cooperative Highway Research Program, Washington, D.C.: Transportation
Research Board.
Geruschat, D. R. & Hassan, S. E. (2005). Driver behavior in yielding to sighted and blind
pedestrians at roundabouts. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 99(5), 286-302.
Hassan, S. E. (2012). Are normally sighted, visually impaired, and blind pedestrians accurate
and
reliable at making street crossing decisions? Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual
Science, 53(6), 2593-2600.
Hassan, S. E. & Massof, R. W. (2012). Measurements of street-crossing decision-making in
pedestrians with low vision. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 49, 410-418.
Hill, E. W., & Ponder, P. (1976). Orientation and mobility techniques: A guide for the
practitioner. New York: American Foundation for the Blind.
Jacobson, W. H. (1993). The art and science of teaching orientation and mobility to persons
with
visual impairments. New York, NY, AFB Press
LaGrow, S. J. & Weessies, M. (1994). Orientation and mobility: Techniques for independence.
Palmerston North, New Zealand: Dunmore Press.
Long, R. & LaGrow, S. J. (2011). Orientation and mobility: Techniques for independence,
second edition. Palmerston North, New Zealand: Dunmore Press.
Sauerburger, D. (1989). To cross or not to cross: Objective timing methods of assessing street
crossings without traffic controls. RE:view, Fall 1989, 153-161.
Sauerburger, D. (1995). Safety awareness for crossing streets with no traffic control. Journal of
Visual Impairment & Blindness, 89(5), 423-431
Sauerburger, D. (1999). Developing criteria and judgment of safety for crossing streets with
gaps
in traffic. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 93(7), 447-450.
Sauerburger, D. (2006). Instructional strategies for teaching judgment in detecting gaps for
crossing streets with no traffic controls. RE:view, 37(4), 177-188.
Schroeder, B. J. & Rouphail, N. M. (2011). Event-based modeling of driver yielding behavior at
unsignalized crosswalks. Journal of Transportation Engineering, 137, 455-465.
Snook-Hill, M. & Sauerburger, D. (1996). Teaching students to assess safety for crossing
streets
which have no traffic control. in Proceedings of International Mobility Conference VIII,
Tambartun National Resource Centre, Melhus, Norway, 535-540.
Wall Emerson, R., Kim, D. S., Naghshineh, K., Pliskow, J., & Myers, K. (2011). Examining the
impact of “quiet” vehicles on the performance of orientation and mobility tasks by
pedestrians who are blind. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 105(12), 745-759.
Wall Emerson, R. & Sauerburger, D. (2008). Detecting approaching vehicles at streets with no
traffic control. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 102(12), 747-760.
Download