Literature and language 35 SPEECH PERCEPTION. STEREOTYPES OF SPEECH BEHAVIOR N.L. Ovshieva Elista, Kalmikiya We are affected by our own tereotypes, social standards and expectations of other people while processing speech in face-to-face iteraction. As Edwards has remarked, "we do not react to the world on the basis of sensory input lone but, rather, in terms of what we perceive that input to mean. This is the foundation of all our social cons group relationships, and it is a foundation that reflects - in an ongoing fashion our accumulated social knowledge" [1, p. 101]. Utterance interpretation takes place so fast and so spontaneously that we are usually not aware of how we recover the message we make. This is achieved through the activation and implementation of categorical thinking. Speech perception, in this sense, is the filter that is culturally specific and - within social groupings - individualized to a greater or lesser extent. Thus, bottom-up processing of acoustic information directly interacts with a higher level information related to people's socially constructed stereotypes of gender, sex, occupation or cultural norms. Consequently, because our knowledge of the world guides what we see and hear stereotypes have a great deal of potential to influence speech perception. tructions, of all our individual and As such, people's schematic preconceptions drive their evaluations of others and their reactions in face-to-face interaction. Speech perceivers regularly construct and use stereotypes (e.g. categorical representations) in their dealing with others. That domination of schematic thinking in speech perception process has its output in various stereotypes of inter-actants' speech behavior. As people are inevitably exposed to the cultural transmission of stereotypic ideas during childhood socialization social stereotypes (characteristics ascribed "to people on the basis of their group memberships" [2, p. 3] are activated automatically upon detection of a person's group membership. Stereotype activation in the domains of race, gender, age, occupation and cultural norms has resulted in social (race, gender, age, occupational) and cultural stereotypes of speech behavior. Social identity theory is the theoretical basis for the description of inter-actants' evaluative and stereotyped automatic reactions in face-to-face interaction. According to social identity theory, an important component of the self-concept is derived from memberships in social groups and categories. As the researchers in the sphere of social cognition observed, "social identities are cognitively represented as group prototypes that describe and prescribe beliefs, attitudes, feelings and behavior that optimize a balance between minimization of in-group differences and maximization of inter-group differences" [3, p, 228]. Thus, social identities should influence behavior through the mediating role of group norms people will be more likely to engage in a particular behavior if it is in accord with the norms of a behaviorally relevant group membership, particularly if the identity is a salient basis for self-definition [3, p. 228]. If the group membership is not salient, then people's behavior and feelings should be in accord with their own personal and idiosyncratic characteristics rather than group norms [3, p. 228]. 36 There is an important point of difference between identity theory and social identity theory. Identity theory focuses on role identities - such as mother, academic and blood-donor whereas social identity theory focuses on identities that emanate from group memberships. It may be that self- (or role) identities can be distinguished from both personal and social identities. Role identities are conceptualized as individual-level identities or 'me's' because they reflect the definition of self as a person who performs a particular social role, whereas group-level identities are conceptualized as 'we's1 because they reflect identifications of the self with a social group or category. As collective identities, both role and social identities are contrasted with personal identities, where the self is defined in terms of unique and idiosyncratic characteristics [3, p. 230]. Thus, from evaluative and stereotyped reactions of interlocutors in face-to-face interaction we draw inferences about facets of their identity in stereotypical group terms. Our background knowledge - that stereotype-based material that is acknowledged to reside in long-term memory - evokes expectations about how a speaker "should" behave according to his or her status as a role or group member. Let's look at the examples illustrating how in people's responses their attitudinal evaluations are externalized. An interesting area of investigation is the study of cultural norms. For example, the British culture places a very high value on the Maxim of Tact and speakers are supposedly well known for the use of'yes, but...' replies Let's look at a conversation between two people in phatic communion. Phatic communion as a type of speech may be regarded as a mode of action, for "each utterance is an act serving the direct aim of binding hearer to speaker by a tie of some social l sentiment or other" [4, p. 151]. Cultural stereotype -inquiries about health, comments on weather, affirmations of some supremely obvious state of things is designed not to inform or express any thought, but to connect people in action. English-speakers who meet each other but have nothing in particular to say will begin talking about the weather or exchanging phrases of politeness, since silence is considered unacceptable: 'I'm sure he benefits from your visits though. Mags ... you will come around to dinner soon, won't you?' ' Certainly. You know my routine well enough. There's Keep Fit tomorrow of course; I'll probably stop in and work on my Thai course Wednesday; Thursdays I'm usually free' ' Well, I'll be in touch,' said Brenda, apparently backing away from a firm date. 'That would be super!'Margaret said, grateful that nothing specific was arranged. [5, p. 15] In this communication situation Brenda, Margaret's colleague, feels uncomfortable at seeing her: Margaret's father is seriously ill and her friend has abandoned her. To get over the strange and unpleasant tension they feel she asks her round. Just for show Margaret picks out the day when she is free. linally they exchange phrases of eness, but actually they intend to d this meeting. hus, the community members share ;ain norms for interacting (i.e. erstand what behavior norms are >ropriate) which implies shared ,erstanding as well. Let's consider the imple when the speakers do not iform to cultural stereotypes: Eventually she could not stand her ecision any longer. 'Colette!' Her flat mate came into the bedroom, ui?1 'I can't decide between these trousers id the skirt... It's so hopeless! I can irely go on the street with all these orgeous Parisian women making me iel like a tramp!' Colette was thrown. She had always sgarded Margaret as one of those itellectual English roses for whom coking glamorous is an act of treason against some kind of unspoken code of ordinariness. 'It looks very nice with the irousers...' she said, a little unconvincingly. 'I wish I could go out and spend a fortune on clothes!' The French woman does not spend a fortune, Margaret - she would add a Hermes silk scarf, some earrings and perhaps a new handbag - as they say in the American magazines: Accessorize!' [5, p. 149] Here Margaret behaves counter to a common stereotype of English women (as one of those intellectual English roses for whom looking glamorous is an act of treason against some kind of unspoken code of ordinariness). She's trying to accommodate herself to a stereotype associated with French women (these gorgeous Parisian women making me feel like a tramp). Margaret is made self-conscious, for she is aware of this difference (I can't decide between these trousers and the skirt). At home, in England the 'intellectual rose' could conform to ordinariness, for English ladies delight in their frumpiness, but it is not expected here. A cultural stereotype of French women is externalized through the article (The French woman = the stereotypical French woman). Thus, cultural female stereotypes with regard to couture (i.e. the fabrication of clothes) are contrasted here. Condescending, giving advice to everybody as a person who is superior in knowledge, or experience, or judgement to them is an occupational stereotype associated with a teacher: "What do you think I should do?" He hesitated, then smiled. "Find someone like me? Who isn't married? If that doesn't sound too impossibly vain." She tied a final bow in the tags of black ribbon. Literature and language 3 7 "And Henry?" "Not even a Rembrandt has the right to ruin someone else's life." "I'm not sure it isn't ruined already." "That's self-pity. Not the real you." "Cowardice." "Also not the real you." [6, p. 122] In this communication situation David behaves like a teacher. He cannot help advising Diana (indirect speech act of advice is represented as eiicitation). David knows he is being didactic, can tend to be too pedantic. He is modifying her attitude (self-evaluation), correcting twice. He is aware of the danger of behaving too stereotypically, so he is trying to teach her in a gentle, delicate way. But he cannot help repeating the same words (speech act of evaluation), encouraging her not to give up. The categorization of people according to their sex is one of the most primary judgments that human perceivers make of each other. Gender information guides human behavior and judgment. Accordingly we react to people as we see fit, based on these expectations: I felt that I was not carrying out my embassy with any great skill. I was distinctly nettled. 'Hang it all, one can't leave a woman without a bob' 'Why not?' 'How is she going to live?' 'I've supported her for seventeen years. Why shouldn't she support herself for a change?' 'She can't.' 'Let her try.' [7, p. 48] Here gender stereotype "a man cannot leave a woman' influences the speaker's perception of Charles. Social expectations about gender have an effect on his evaluation of Charles' behavior (indirect speech act of reproach). A softening device "one can't" is used to establish a stereotype, where «one» stands for all. 38 The Caspian Sea Humanities and sports You have to conform to a stereotype "A gentleman can behave towards the lady". Otherwise, this behavior is not acceptable in the society. Here there is another stereotype: "A woman has no means to support herself". The implication of the social gender stereotype operating in the nineteenth century is as follows: "Nobody should do that." Age stereotypes may cause misunderstanding between interlocutors, for teenagers are egocentric and are apt to fall prey to the idea that their understanding is, in fact, the only sort possible: "Where you girls from?" I asked her. She didn't answer me, though. She was busy looking around for Peter Lorre to show up, I guess. "Where you girls from?" I asked her again. "What?" she said. "Where you girls from? Don't answer if you don't feel like it. I don't want you to strain yourself." "Seattle, Washington," she said. She was doing me a big favor to tell me. "You're a very good conversationalist," I told her. "You know that?" "What?" [8, p. 89] In this communication situation Holden is trying to start a talk with the girl he has invited to a dance. He is annoyed (if you don't feel like it). She is not even willing to strain herself to answer, though she should p to him. Here is a stereotypical behavior of a young girl preoccupied with her own thoughts. Impolite behavior is more obvious in teenagers who are self-centered, egocentric, who have not practiced social conventions well enough So she is exhibiting teenage rudeness. Holden is being sarcastic (You're a very good conversationalist). ay attention The issues discussed in this article indicate the necessity of a new theory of speech perception. Current speech perception theories that do not allow for the influence of stereotype information cannot offer a fully inclusive account of speech perception process. Developing such a theory necessarily involves the integrative approaches and data from a multitude of research areas including knowledge about socially constructed, culture-specific identity and resulting stereotypes. Our assumptions (stereotypes) that are not manifest are externalized through our behavior. And conversely, we behave according to expectations of others, trying to conform to stereotypes. Thus, from our stereotypical behavior one can draw inferences about facets of our identity and our stereotypes of thinking that have been termed figuratively "perceptual spectacles". 1. Edwards, J Refining our understanding of language attitudes // Journal of Language and Social Psychology. 1999. Vol. 18. No 1. P. 101 110. 2. Oakes P., Haslam A., Turner J Stereotyping and Social Reality. Oxford: Blackwell, 1994. 3. Terry D., Hogg M., White K. The Theory of planned behaviour: Selfidentity, social identity and group norms //British Journal of Social Psychology 1999. Vol 38. P. 225-244 4 Malinovsky, B. Phatic communion //Communication in Face - to -Face Interaction: Selected Readings /Ed. by J. Laver, S. Hutcheson. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books Ltd., 1972. P. 146-152. 5. Urban M. The Linguist. London: Headline Book Publishing. 1998. 6. Fowies J. The Ebony Tower. M. 2000. 7. Maugham W.S. The Moon and Sixpense. M., 2000. 8. Salinger J. The Catcher in the Rye. M., 1979. Каспий. Гуманитарные науки и спорт: Международный сборник научных трудов/ Ассоциация университетов прикаспийских государств. Астрахань, Изд-во АГПУ, 2002. Вып.III. С.-35-39.