Alex Storer whose TA is Rob humbly submits this essay regarding MUSICAL COGNITION for MCB 61 on April 29, 2002 1 As an intended music minor and an avid (yet rather inadequate) musician, it has become more and more obvious to me that the science of music is virtually ignored. Clearly, for such a universal standard to be in place regarding the precision and accuracy of certain musical concepts, a biological basis must underlie the artistry of music. In this paper, I will explore the neurological underpinnings of music and how they form a cogent system of building blocks that leads to the complexities seen in modern compositions. While the perception of music is a high level process and very difficult to comprehend as a cogent whole, each facet of musical perception can be at least partially deconstructed into neurological function, each of which is actually processed at a most basic level aside from the other aspects of the music. The essential elements of music can be isolated to several qualities that the human brain perceives. Perhaps the most simplistic of these is loudness, a measurement of the volume of a particular sound. Loudness is essential in music; changing the volume at specific times can change the mood of and meaning behind a piece. Louder sounds cause greater vibration in the basilar membrane, which is located in the cochlea. These vibrations cause the axons to fire more rapidly, which is in turn translated as a louder sound in the auditory pathways of the brain (Kolb, 2001). While little is known about the processing of loudness in the brain, studies have shown differences between musicians and non-musicians, implying that training can influence this part of musical perception. Specifically, musicians are able to maintain perception of loudness longer than nonmusicians, who will adapt to the loudness and perceive it as decreased over time (Chrystophe, 1995). This process is important for musicians to have a more accurate 2 assessment of their music as it is being played, and it’s understandable that this ability would be more fine-tuned for musicians. Rhythm is another important part of all types of music, and to understand the perception of music as a whole it is necessary to understand how these are processed. From a logical standpoint, rhythm involves mechanisms for timing, motor coordination and additional auditory processing. The cerebellum, basal ganglia and superior temporal gyrus provide the mechanisms necessary for analyzing duration of tones as well as motor timing necessary to create rhythmic sequences (Dennis, 2001). Rhythms, however, are not confined to only one part of the brain (Dennis, 2001). After losing full functionality in either hemisphere, patients’ abilities to detect and reproduce rhythms falter. Rhythm, rather than duration, however, is more of a high level process that cannot be quite so simply stated. In addition to the mere function of duration, how long a beat lasts, music implies the concept of meter, the organization of beats into logical groups. For example, quadruple meter implies that there are four beats of equal length in one measure, or grouping of notes. This system involves collaboration between temporal lobes. Perhaps the most well known element of music is pitch, the quality of a tone. From a physical standpoint, pitch is defined by the frequency of a sound wave. In music, relative pitch is used to compare notes in the scale and defines the building blocks of tonal progressions. These are defined in terms of ratios of the frequencies of pitches. A perfect fifth, such as from C to G, has a ratio of 3:2. Similarly, other relationships have frequency ratios in terms of relatively simple fractions (WWW 1). While the basic hearing of pitch takes place in the primary auditory cortex, the analysis of these frequencies takes place away from this, mostly in the temporal lobes. The most 3 noteworthy studies regarding musical perception lie in hemispherical analysis, perhaps because it is easiest to observe. The ability to experience melody seems to be focused on the right hemisphere of the brain, as is documented by Dennis & Hopyan. In fact, injections to temporarily disable right brain function show marked loss of melodic abilities without affecting rhythm or language (2001). Another fascinating attribute of pitch is the notion of absolute pitch. Most humans are not able to identify a given pitch by name, but the ability to do so is found in approximately 1 in 10,000 people. On a neurological level, absolute pitch is associated in some way with the planum temporale, and hemispherical asymmetries therein. Surprisingly enough, leftward asymmetries are common in musicians with absolute pitch. Normally, this would not seem reasonable due to the large number of musical functions associated with the right hemisphere of the brain (Keenan, 2001). This asymmetry is postulated to be a result of genetics, as the lateralization of the planum temporale can be seen even in utero. Another central cause of absolute pitch is musical training between the ages of 3 and 6, which attests to the mutability of the brain at young ages. It also spawns my general distaste towards my parents and their inability to provide me with suitable music lessons at an exorbitantly young age. While each one of these elements of music can be analyzed, it’s difficult to find material regarding how the brain pieces this information together to create a musical sensation. Instead, there are multiple approaches from the top down to approach music, focusing on theory and psychological or physiological effects of listening to it. It is postulated, however, that human ability to perceive music as a whole came with the development of a more extensive dopaminurgic system (Previc, 1999). Additional 4 complexities of musical perception are covered in the emerging field of neuromusicology, which plans to turn musical theory into neurological theory. Despite the lack of strong research at this point, it holds great promise for the future of neurological musical analysis. 5 References Christophe, M. Medial Olivocochlear System and Loudness Adaptation: Differences between Musicans and Non-musicians. Brain and Cognition 29: 127-136 (1995) Dennis, M. et al. Rhythm and Melody in Children and Adolescents after Left or Right Temporal Lobectomy. Brain and Cognition 47: 461-469 (2001). Keenan, J.P. et al. Absolute Pitch and Planum Temporale. NeuroImage 14: 1402–1408 (2001) Kolb, B. et al. An Introduction to Brain and Behavior. Worth Publishers (2001). Previc, H. P. Dopamine and the Origins of Human Intelligence. Brain and Cognition 41: 299-350 (1999) WWW 1. The Mathematics of Tuning and Temperament. <http://www.hlalapansi.demon.co.uk/Acoustics/MusicMaths/MusicMaths.html>