1 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION Past research on the mother-child dyad has been extensive, portraying mothers as nurturers and caregivers of children (Culp, Schadle, Robinson, & Culp, 2000), but it has not explored the role of fathers in depth. However, with the rise of dual-earner families, many fathers are now sharing caregiving responsibilities with mothers (Culp et al., 2000; Lamb, 2000; Marsiglio, Amato, Day, & Lamb, 2000; Maurer & Pleck, 2006) in addition to their traditional roles as financial providers (Lamb, 2000; Maurer & Pleck, 2006). Although research on father involvement is still sparse, emerging studies show fathers’ influences on children’s academic outcomes, future employment, and overall social competence (Culp et al., 2000); thus, fathers are now recognized as influential in their children’s cognitive and socio-emotional development (Tamis-LeMonda, Shannon, Cabrera, & Lamb, 2004). However, the emerging research on fathers was done primarily with European American families in the United States, but little is known about fathers from different cultural contexts. Thus, the current study focuses on Chinese American fathers and their influence on children’s social and emotional development. 2 Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between father involvement in Chinese American families and children’s socio-emotional development. According to Jankowiak (1992), traditional Chinese fathers are emotionally reserved in comparison to European American fathers and interact with their children primarily when disciplinary action is needed. Chinese American fathering, however, may differ across individuals due to acculturation factors. The United States consists of a diverse array of cultures; however, very little is known about parenting across cultures. Parenting undoubtedly differs across cultures due to varying beliefs, values, and goals. For example, cross-cultural research suggests that American parents value autonomy whereas Chinese parents value social cohesion and interpersonal relations (Wang & Fivush, 2005). To better understand childrearing and child development in the United States, researchers must look beyond the role of mothers as caretakers and examine other ethnic groups besides European Americans in order to recognize the importance of paternal involvement and its effect on children. The proposed study focuses on Chinese American fathers and explores their influence on the socio-emotional development of their school-aged children. 3 Significance of the Study Father-child relationships have a profound influence on child development, but have often been overlooked. Previous studies found that children who experienced high father involvement in childcare tended to have fewer external behavioral problems (Culp et al., 2000) and better psychological adjustment when maturing to adulthood (Lewis & Lamb, 2003). In earlier research, fathers who engage in stimulating physical play situations with their children have been seen as large contributors to their children’s socialization (Boyum & Parke, 1995; Carson & Parke, 1996; Lamb, 2000). Recent research has also shown that fathers’ emotional displays of positive affect correlates with children’s social competency and peer acceptance (McDowell & Parke, 2005). There is no doubt that father-child interactions are important to children’s development. Despite what little is known in the area of paternal involvement, even less is known about Chinese fathers raising children in the United States. Because Asian Americans are the fastest growing ethnic group in the United States, it is important to increase the body of knowledge about these families (Sue, Nakamura, Chung, & YeeBradbury, 1994). Little research has been done on Chinese-American parental involvement, let alone father-child interactions. Past research has portrayed Chinese fathers as authoritarian in parenting style (Chao, 1994), but little research has been done on Chinese American fathers. Findings in Hong Kong showed an association between fathers’ harsh parenting and children’s depressive symptoms (Shek, 2008). Thus, more research is needed to understand the subtleties and complexities of Chinese American 4 fathering, as the fathers are significant contributors to children’s social and emotional development. Subsequently, this study examined the relationship of Chinese American father involvement with the following areas: (a) their acculturation levels (b) the quality of their children’s peer relationships, and (c) their children’s levels of self-esteem. The results of the study may be useful in empowering Chinese American fathers to take active roles in their interactions with children and to help educators be culturally sensitive to children of differing ethnicities. Methods Participants For this study, 31 Chinese-American families were recruited from Chinese churches in three California cities: Pleasanton, Sacramento, and Los Angeles. All participating families had at least one child between the ages of seven and 11 years old, with both biological parents residing with the child in the home. All parents included in this study had resided in the United States for at least 10 years. Fathers and mothers from this sample, if not born in the US, were born in Hong Kong, China, or Taiwan, with the exception of one father born in Peru, one mother born in Jamaica, and one mother born in Cambodia. 5 Measures Survey measures were used to gather information on acculturation, paternal involvement, children’s peer relationships, and children’s self-esteem. Paternal involvement was evaluated under five subscales of the Paternal Involvement and Childcare Index (PICCI): general involvement, childcare responsibilities, socialization responsibilities, influence in child-rearing responsibilities, and time-availability. Fathers were requested to fill out the demographic questionnaire (Appendix A), which included embedded acculturation measures, as well as the PICCI (Appendix B). Mothers were asked to fill out two questionnaires, the Quality of Child’s Friendship (Appendix D) and Behavior with Peers (Appendix E), on their children’s peer relationships. Children were asked to fill out the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory (CSEI), which measured selfesteem under four subscales: general self-esteem, social self-esteem, home and parents self-esteem, and school or academic self-esteem. The CSEI can be found in Appendix C. Correlational analyses were then conducted to determine the relationships between acculturation and father involvement, father involvement and children’s peer relationships, and father involvement and children’s self-esteem. Definition of Terms Father involvement can be defined with the following dimensions: engagement, accessibility, responsibility, warmth, and control (Hofferth et al., 2007; Lamb, 2000; Parke, 2000; Saleh, Buzi, Weinman, & Smith, 2005). Engagement is defined by direct 6 interactions with the child through caregiving activities and shared activities such as play (Lamb, 2000; Parke, 2000). Accessibility is defined by the time and availability the father has to interact with the child (Hofferth et al, 2007; Lamb, 2000). Responsibility concerns managing children’s physical needs such as housing, physician visits, education, and making sure the child is fed (Hofferth et al, 2007; Lamb, 2000; Parke, 2000). Warmth, derived from Baumrind’s (1971) conceptualization of parenting styles, can be defined as responsiveness to children’s needs. Lastly, control is described by restrictions placed on children’s behaviors and parents’ level of demandingness (Baumrind, 1991; Hofferth et al., 2007). Father involvement in caregiving has shown positive relationships in children’s cognitive (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2004) and socio-emotional development (Carson & Parke, 1996; Lewis & Lamb, 2003); thus the current study examined the relationship between Chinese American father involvement in childcare and children’s social and emotional development. Acculturation is the degree to which an individual living in a new country adopts the values and practices of the new culture. Berry, Trimble, and Olmeda (1986) described four types of acculturation: assimilation, marginalization, separation, and integration. Assimilation is discarding one’s native culture and identifying with the new culture. Marginalization is defined as rejecting both the native and new culture. Separation is rejecting the new culture and adhering to the native culture, and lastly, integration is blending together the native and new cultures. Self-esteem can be defined as an individual’s perception of his or her own value and worth (Coopersmith, 1981; Rosenberg, 1965). Coopersmith (1981) makes four 7 distinctions in children’s self-esteem: their general self-esteem, social self-esteem, selfesteem at home, and self-esteem in the school setting. Self-esteem may vary depending upon the different contexts the children face. Thus, this study uses the four subcategories of self-esteem that Coopersmith identified to obtain an overview on children’s selfperceptions in different situations. Peer relationships are defined as the acquaintance with people of similar age within a particular context (Berndt, 1996). Two different facets of children’s peer relationships were considered in the current study: behavior with peers and friendship quality. The former references adaptive and maladaptive behavior with other children in the general peer context. The latter refers to relations between friends. Both behavior and friendship quality are divided into positive and negative qualities. Elements of positive peer behavior include taking turns, being kind, and cooperating with others. Negative peer relationships are characterized by behaviors such as threatening others, being bossy, or purposely excluding others. Friendships develop from peer relationships. Friendship is defined as a reciprocated, intimate, and loyal relationship with another individual in which both people fulfill their social need for companionship and acceptance (Buhrmester, 1990). When looking at a child’s particular relationship with another individual, this study examined both positive and negative friendship qualities. Positive qualities of friendship included the child working well with his or her best friend, helping the friend, and the two taking each other’s suggestions into consideration. Negative qualities of friendship 8 include using verbal aggression, criticizing the friend, and protesting when the friend tries to take control. Limitations of the Study The results obtained from a sample size of 31 families require caution in generalizing to the entire Chinese American population. Additionally, recruitment originated from the church setting, so variability among the sample regarding childrearing practices may be limited as churchgoers may value certain dimensions of father involvement over others. There has not been much attention to father involvement in the Chinese community in the past, as most childrearing tasks are usually designated to the mother. Traditionally, fathers were typically less involved in childcare responsibilities; therefore calling father’s attention to father involvement in this study may have invoked feelings of shame or inadequacy. As a result, fathers may have felt restricted in the responses they provided as they may have felt that they were being judged or criticized for their parenting, although the researcher reassured fathers prior to the start of the study that this was not the case. Despite these limitations, the results obtained are valuable as they provide a window into the lives of Chinese American families, thus supporting the importance of the role of fathers in the family dynamic when fathers have been undervalued in the past. 9 Organization of the Study Chapter One contained an overview of the research study, and provided information on the purpose of the study, its significance, methodology, definition of terms, and limitations. Chapter Two presents a literature review of the following four areas: (a) The history of father involvement in the US and China, (b) the theoretical background related to father involvement, (c) the role of fathers in children’s social and emotional development, and (d) the socio-emotional issues facing Chinese American children. Chapter Three provides a detailed description of the methodology used in the research study, which includes information on participants, procedures, survey measures, and the data analysis. Chapter Four presents the results of correlational analysis used to examine the associations between father’s acculturation and father involvement, father involvement and children’s peer relationships, and father involvement and children’s self-esteem. Lastly, Chapter Five discusses the implications of the study’s major findings, its limitations, and future research that can stem from this study. 10 Chapter 2 REVIEW OF LITERATURE Children’s social-emotional development involves a complex network of interactions between children and their families, peers, and the environment. For example, research on parent-child interactions suggests that parents influence children’s emotional regulation, thus affecting their social competence and peer acceptance (McDowell & Parke, 2009; Rah & Parke, 2008). In addition, fathers may play a role in children’s social-emotional development that is distinct from that of mothers. For example, research shows that fathers’ own affect and emotional control predicts children’s externalizing behaviors with their peers (McDowell & Parke, 2005). Also, father’s involvement in play is positively related to children’s cognitive, language, and social-emotional development (Carson & Parke, 1996; Lewis & Lamb, 2003; TamisLeMonda et al., 2004). However, most of these studies were conducted on European American families and may not be representative of ethnic minority families that have differing cultural values. Thus, further research examining father-child interactions in other cultural contexts is crucial to the field of child development. The present study examined Chinese American father involvement in childrearing and its relation to children’s social and emotional development. Chinese American families are important to study because they are a growing subcultural group in the United States. Recently, there has been an increase in research on Chinese parenting, but 11 there is still limited research on Chinese American families, and particularly on Chinese American fathers. One important topic to investigate is the role of acculturation in Chinese American parenting: specifically, relations among acculturation, father involvement, and children’s social-emotional development. The present study examined three aspects of father involvement in Chinese American families. First, it examined the relation between Chinese American fathers’ acculturation levels and their involvement with their children. Second, it explored relations between father involvement and children’s quality of peer relationships. Third, it examined the relation between father involvement and children’s self-esteem. This study was informed by the literatures on fathering in American society, Chinese and Chinese American families, and culture and parenting. The review of the literature is organized into the following four areas: (a) the history of fathering in US and China, (b) the theoretical background related to father involvement, (c) the role of fathers in children’s social-emotional development, and (d) specific cultural factors that may affect Chinese American children’s social-emotional development. Father Involvement in the US and China The role of fathers in children’s development was largely overlooked in earlier developmental research. Only recently has there been an increase in studying the quality of father involvement and its direct effect on child outcomes. Father involvement, in this current study, includes fathers’ direct interactions with their children, availability to 12 attend to their children, childcare responsibilities, parental warmth, and parental control (Hofferth et al., 2007; Parke, 2000). However, it is important to note that conceptions of father involvement have changed over time and vary with culture. The following section will briefly address father involvement in the US, father involvement in China, and current definitions and conceptions of father involvement in child development literature. Father Involvement in the US In the United States, notions of ideal father involvement have changed over the years in accordance with the values of the time. During the Puritan times, the father’s role was to teach morals through discipline (Lamb, 2000). Fathers did not take part in childrearing activities with young children unless discipline was involved. Fathers, however, often modeled their workmanship for the older children (usually sons), as sons often served as apprentices. During the industrial times, the source of income shifted from home-industry, or agriculture, to out-of-home business in the mills and factories. As a result, fathers became the “breadwinners” and were valued primarily in this role (Lamb, 2000; Marsiglio, Amato, Day, & Lamb, 2000). Fathers would leave the home to earn their wages elsewhere, while mothers raised children at home, educating, disciplining, and socializing them. After World War II and the feminist movement, the criteria for judging a good father shifted from the emphasis on breadwinning and disciplining children to nurturing children. Specifically, women entered the workforce, and childrearing responsibilities were then shared with fathers. Nowadays, many women are financial contributors to their 13 families, and thus, are not as readily available to participate in 100% of childrearing activities; hence, fathers are now expected to share in those childrearing responsibilities. The societal expectations of involvement in childrearing now includes these dimensions: engagement with children during play or one-on-one interactions, accessibility in terms of time-availability for children, responsibility in childcare, warmth in interaction style, and control in terms of discipline (Hofferth et al., 2007; Parke, 2000). Father Involvement in China The role of fathers has also been shifting in Chinese societies. In ancient China, fathers were seen as stern disciplinarians. Most families lived in agrarian villages, and parenting followed the motto, “strict father, kind mother,” (Shwalb, Nakazawa, Yamamoto, & Hyun, 2004). Parenting practices were influenced by Confucian principles of social harmony, in which each person fulfills his or her role in the social hierarchy (Bond & Hwang, 1986). Those who were subordinate were expected to show respect and humility toward elders, while the elders were responsible for teaching and disciplining the younger members of the society (Chao, 1994). Hence, fathers took their moral obligation as the stern disciplinarian seriously (Shek, 2008). Since 1979, China instituted the one-child policy, which transformed father-child relationships in China. In the past, fathers were traditionally more involved in the upbringing of boys than girls, because boys carried on the family bloodline and were culturally valued more than daughters (Jankowiak, 1992; Shwalb, Nakazawa, Yamamoto, & Hyun, 2004). Currently, with families only allowed one child in the family, much 14 focus and attention is placed on that one child. As a result, families have been taking the necessary measures (e.g., abortion) to increase the likelihood of having a son, as can be seen by the disproportionate birthrate for males compared to female babies (Shwalb, Nakazawa, Yamamoto, & Hyun, 2004). As a result, contrary to their roles as disciplinarians, fathers unexpectedly began to spoil their children by giving into their whims (Shwalb, Nakazawa, Yamamoto, & Hyun, 2004). Thus, the fathering role has been evolving with the changing social cultural context. Chinese fathers are not only disciplinarians now, but their parenting style more likely includes warmth and sharing of caregiving responsibilities since mothers are now commonly working outside the home (Chang, Schwartz, Dodge, & McBride-Chang, 2003; Costigan & Dokis, 2006; Jankowiak, 1992). Fathering has been transformed in both Eastern and Western cultures; however, the values and conceptions of fathering in the US and China are different. There is limited research regarding fathering in China and its effect on child outcomes, and even less research done on Chinese immigrant families in the US. Definitions and Conceptions of Father Involvement in Child Development Literature The father involvement construct referred to in this study focuses mainly on positive paternal involvement and can be operationally defined as a father’s engagement and accessibility in terms of amount of time spent with the child, responsibility in childcare activities, warmth in parenting style, and monitoring of children’s behavior (Hofferth, 2003; Yeung, Sandberg, Davis-Kean, & Hofferth, 2001). There are multiple 15 pathways in which father involvement influences child outcomes, both indirectly and directly. Fathers may indirectly affect child outcomes through their employment status, attitudes about fathering, and relationship with the child’s mother. Additionally, there may be a bidirectional influence between fathering and child outcomes, as certain behaviors exhibited by the child may prompt either greater or lesser involvement from fathers (Pleck, 2007). On the other hand, fathers may directly influence their children through their everyday interactions with them. Currently, the role of fathers in the US has been redefined from that of a breadwinner to that of an involved co-parent (Pleck & Pleck, 1997). Research since the 1980s began to show the positive relationship between this kind of father involvement and child outcomes, and began to recognize fathers as important contributors to child development (Pleck, 2007). Despite this increasing research, the social cultural context in which children are developing is changing and the role of fathers is complex. For example, within the last decade, there have been increases in divorce rates, single parenting, step parenting, and cultural diversity in the US (Cabrera, Tamis-LeMonda, Bradley, Hofferth, & Lamb, 2000). Children are thus being exposed to complicated family structures that consist of different father types. As a result, these different father types will in turn influence children’s developmental outcomes (Cabrera et al., 2000). Thus, there is even more of a pressing need to research the influence of fathers within the ever-changing social context, as relationships become increasingly complex, and culturally diverse concepts of parenting are being integrated. 16 Factors Affecting Father Involvement As the social cultural context changes in the United States, there are several factors that affect fathers’ involvement with their children. In particular, fathers’ income, education, and age are factors that influence their level of involvement with their children. Currently, men and women in the United States are getting married at a later age. The mean age of first marriage for men, according to the U.N. Department of Economic and Social Affairs Population Division (2008), is 28 years old, and the mean age of marriage for women is 26 years old. Men and women across the United States are obtaining higher levels of education, which correlates with getting married later in life and earning higher wages with a college degree. Father’s Income and Education. An overarching factor that that pervades all dimensions of father involvement is age, income and educational level. Researchers found that common barriers to father-child involvement are: “lack of maturity, lack of economic support, unemployment, and low educational attainment” (Saleh, Buzi, Weinman, & Smith, 2005, p. 514). The ability of the father to provide financial support influences not only father-child relations, but also mother-child relations. As a result, they both heavily predict children’s developmental well-being. Father’s income is an important predictor of children’s development because more often than not, income level is contingent upon the father’s age and educational attainment. Those with higher levels of education also tend to be older. Father’s income, age, and education levels can indirectly influence a child’s cognitive and language development by affecting the mother’s relationship with the child (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2004). Economic resources 17 can also determine how invested a father is to his child. Such investment may lead to increased warmth in the father-child relationship and affect the degree of control the father has over his child. Landale and Oropesa (2001) studied the role economic status played in Puerto Rican father-child relationships. The majority of Puerto Rican children in the study were born out of wedlock. Often times, non-resident fathers would have little contact with their children if they could not contribute economically. Those who were not financially invested in their children could not be engaged, accessible, or responsible for their children. The fathers who lived with their children’s mother tended to be older, employed, and capable of contributing financially to the children’s upbringing, which had a positive correlation to child involvement. Father’s Age. In general, father’s age was positively associated with involvement in childrearing responsibilities. Heath (2001) examined fathers who had children after the age of thirty-five. The researchers hypothesized that late-time fathers would spend more time with their children, have higher expectations of their children’s behaviors, be more nurturing, and be less controlling than on-time fathers. All four of the hypotheses were confirmed and the differences between on-time and late-time fathers were attributed to the fact that late-time fathers were established in their work, have fewer children, and tended to have higher levels of education. In order to better understand the importance and complexity of father involvement, the next section reviews literature on the theoretical background related to father involvement. 18 Theoretical Background Related to Father Involvement Researchers have started to recognize the need to study father involvement and its effect on child outcomes. There are numerous theories that explain how fathers can influence child development. Particularly, there are three theoretical perspectives to employ when focusing on an immigrant population, such as Chinese American fathers, in which the host culture is vastly different from the immigrant’s culture of origin. The following subsections will review parenting under three theoretical perspectives: Bronfenbrenner’s Bioecological Systems Theory, the cultural value systems of Individualism and Collectivism, and lastly, Acculturation Theory. Bronfenbrenner’s Bioecological Systems Theory Parenting plays a large role in children’s well being, as the effects of parenting permeate children’s cognitive, social, and emotional development. The importance of father involvement and its effect on child development can be better understood by first looking at the dynamic reciprocal relationship between the environment and the child, also known as proximal process interactions. Proximal process interactions drive development and become increasingly complex as the child matures and continues to learn from people, objects, and symbols in the environment (Bronfenbrenner, 1994; Pleck 2007). Bronfenbrenner describes an ecological model that consists of various levels of reciprocal influence that can be visualized as nested systems, with the innermost level being the microsystem. 19 Microsystem. Microsystems are the layer of the environment closest to the child and the area of most direct influence on children’s development. In this layer, the child builds relationships with various partners, such as with his or her parents, peers, teachers, and other adults. Proximal process, which is the reciprocal interaction between the child’s microsystem partners and the child, becomes progressively complex as the child ages and learns new things. These increasingly complex interactions between the child and his or her microsystem partners drive the child’s development into adulthood (Pleck, 2007). Residential fathers are often categorized under this layer and function as one of the child’s microsystem partners. Proximal process interactions between father and child are unique because fathers differ from mothers in their interaction style, which consequently affects child’s development. Specifically, father involvement in Western culture has been positively associated with children’s cognitive, language, and socio-emotional development (Carson & Parke, 1996; Lewis & Lamb, 2003; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2004). Fathers, unlike mothers, often engage in rough-and-tumble play, which through proximal processes, promote children’s emotional regulation (Parke, 2002; Pleck, 2007). Hence, fathers are important figures in children’s early development, as their direct interactions with them influence their children’s later developmental outcomes. Mesosystem. Connecting the microsystems are the mesosystems, which are the connections between the child’s microsystem partners, or the people directly involved with the child, for example, the relationship between the child’s parent and teacher or the relationship between the child’s parents. Fathers indirectly influence children’s development through this layer of the Bioecological System. For example, research 20 shows that fathers who are more involved in children’s education (e.g., volunteering in school functions or participating in parent-teacher conferences) send children the message that school is important; thus children of involved fathers show increased attendance, fewer discipline problems, and higher academic achievement in school (McBride, Schoppe-Sullivan, & Ho, 2005). Other research has shown that the father’s relationship with the child’s mother, who is another microsystem partner, is an important component to father involvement. Pleck and Hofferth (2008) have found that low marital conflict was associated with increased father involvement, and Cabrera et al. (2000) found that marital harmony influences the quality of parent-child relationships. Research reveals that a father’s involvement with his children can be mediated through mother’s involvement, such as “maternal gatekeeping”; hence, the relationship between the child’s mother and father can indirectly influence child outcomes. In other words, the mother is more likely to allow the father to be involved if her relationship with the father consists of low conflict (Pleck & Hofferth, 2008). Thus, fathers are important contributors to the mesosystem, in that fathers’ relationships with their children’s other microsystem partners have been associated with positive child outcomes. Exosystem. Encircling the mesosystems are the exosystems, in which the child is not directly involved. This level accounts for the relationships between the people directly involved with the child and those individuals who do not directly interact or even know the child, such as the relationships between the child’s parent and parent’s coworker, or the relationship between the child’s teacher and his or her spouse. The exosystem includes such factors as the father’s social network, education, and income. 21 Specifically, Tamis-LeMonda et al., (2004) found that fathers who were educated and employed indirectly influenced child’s development by enhancing mother-child relationships. Additionally, indirect factors such as a father’s education and income have also been positively related to children’s success at school, peer relationships, and cognitive development (Marsiglio, Amato, Day, & Lamb, 2000; Saleh, Buzi, Weinman, & Smith, 2005; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2004). Macrosystem. The next level, the macrosystem, includes larger cultural values, attitudes, ideologies, and expectations, as well as social policies. In the case of Chinese American children, the macrosystem identifies individualism as a key societal value while their family promotes a different cultural orientation – one that values collectivism. One way of defining culture is through examining its core societal values (Cooper & Denner, 1998). Children grow up within the expectations of their culture, which in turn, shapes their behavior and relationships with their microsystem partners. However, when discussing Chinese American children, culture is not so clearly defined for them. Their microsystem partners consist of individuals with differing cultural ideals. For example, when at home, their mother may downplay their children’s successes and focus on their misdeeds to help their children improve in those areas; whereas, at school, teachers may praise the children for their accomplishments and then downplay failures with the purposes of enhancing self-esteem (Miller, Cowan, Cowan, & Hetherington, 1993; Wang & Fivush, 2005; Wang & Leichtman, 2000). As a result, children may be confused as to how they are supposed to act, as the developmental goals and value systems of the authority figures in their lives may differ. It will be important to research how cultural 22 values affect parental involvement as Parke (2000) has shown that a father’s ethnic identity, whether it be more American or more Chinese, critically shapes how he parents his children. Chronosystem. The last level in Bronfenbrenner’s Bioecological Systems Theory is the chronosystem, which can include life transitions such as going to school, entering puberty, moving, marriage, divorce, and much more (Bronfenbrenner, 1986; Bronfenbrenner, 1994; Pleck, 2007). Research has shown that as the child gets older, the level of parental involvement changes. For example, the increased age of children has been correlated with fathers’ decreased involvement (Pleck & Hofferth, 2008). As children enter into adolescence, they become increasingly independent, and parents become less involved in the childcare (e.g., bathing, dressing, putting the child to bed, etc.) and more involved in monitoring children’s behaviors. Another aspect of the chronosystem includes large societal changes, such as the shifts seen in the history of fathering. As the social-cultural contexts changes with the times, so do expectations of what constitutes an involved father. During Puritan times, an involved father was defined as that of a disciplinarian; whereas, in current day, with women in the workforce, involved fathers are defined as engaged, accessible, responsible in childcare duties, warm in interaction style, and authoritative in disciplining children (Hofferth et al., 2007; Parke, 2000). Bronfenbrenner’s Bioecological Systems outlines the overarching importance of parenting within the context of the United States, as this theory defines an individual’s development through the interactions between the person and environment. When 23 considering Chinese American families, the focus of the current study, it is important to address variations between US and Chinese cultures. Thus, the identified “cultural syndromes” of individualism and collectivism will be discussed next. A cultural syndrome refers to the shared values (e.g., attitudes, beliefs, norms, etc.) of a group of individuals who live in the same time period, speak the same language, and live in the same geographic location (Triandis, 1993; Triandis, 1996). These cultural syndromes define culture through pre-established core societal values that then shape the behavior of the individual of that culture (Cooper & Denner, 1998). The following section will discuss the “cultural syndromes” of individualism and collectivism that describe the United States and Chinese culture, and how children’s development is affected. Individualism and Collectivism Culture is intimately linked with developmental outcomes. The environment in which a child develops contains meaningful social values and practices that are transmitted from one generation to the next (Cooper & Denner, 1998; Super & Harkness, 2002). Traditionally speaking, Western cultures are seen to promote developmental goals of independence and autonomy, whereas Eastern cultures are seen to promote collectivistic ideals of social and group harmony (Cooper & Denner, 1998; TamisLeMonda et al., 2008). However, this view that these two value systems are mutually exclusive is an over-simplification of culture. In fact, when examining parenting goals across cultures on a micro-level, researchers found a coexistence between the two value systems (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008). 24 On a macro-level, individualistic cultures contain four implicit autonomy-related parenting goals that are promoted in the individuals of that society: personal choice, intrinsic motivation to achieve personal goals, self-esteem, and self-maximization (i.e., striving to reach one’s full potential). In these cultures, these parenting goals are perceived to promote independent and healthy individuals (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008). However, as the social-cultural context continues to evolve due to globalization and technological advances, boundaries between individualism and collectivism are blurred (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008). Chinese American children are raised in an environment that embraces and expects these overarching individualistic standards, but their parents, depending upon their level of assimilation in the US, are socializing them, to varying degrees, with collectivist ideals. In contrast to individualistic societies, collectivistic cultures promote relatedness and interdependence upon other members of that society. The implied parenting goals of this culture includes goals to reinforce the importance of close relationships, adapting to the larger group, and respecting and obeying those who are in authority (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008). Particularly, parenting in China was heavily influenced by Confucian principles that stated that people are defined by their relationships, and that those relationships contain a hierarchical order; preserving social harmony was of utmost importance (Bond & Hwang, 1986). However, Chinese American children are not being raised under pure individualistic or collectivistic ideals. Specifically, immigrants may adopt new parenting goals upon entering a new country, and each family unit employs these ideals to varying degrees (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008). Hence, Chinese American 25 children do not have a clearly defined macrosystem level, so research on their developmental outcomes is of particular interest. Chinese American parents employ parenting goals that blend individualistic and collectivistic ideals. The goals may be conflicting, additive, or functionally dependent (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008). Firstly, autonomy, an attribute that is usually associated with individualism, can coexist with relatedness in a conflicting manner. For example, Chinese immigrant parents are expected to display the collectivistic parenting goals of relatedness. However, they may show concern for children’s academic achievement (autonomy) if their child spends too much time with friends (relatedness) instead of studying (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008). Hence, the value systems are conflicting as well as coexisting. Autonomy and relatedness can also coexist in additive ways. For example, in a study by Wang and Tamis-LeMonda (2003), Chinese American mothers reported endorsing qualities in their children that demonstrated a coexistence of collectivistic and individualistic characteristics such as: obedience, cooperation, politeness, assertiveness, diligence, leadership, and striving toward personal success. Lastly, these two value systems can coexist in a fashion where one value system is functionally dependent on another system. For example, Chinese parents may encourage their children to be autonomous by achieving individual success so that they may one day give back to their family group (Yu & Yang, 1994). Cooper and Denner (1998) note the importance of social-identity as the key to being functional members of society. Since Chinese Americans are, in fact, living in a country generally promoting independence, those who adapt to this social identity will 26 have a greater chance of being better adjusted socially and emotionally. Cooper and Denner (1998) explain that, “individual competence is defined not in universal terms but within the cultural and historical contexts in which children develop” (p. 568). Because Chinese American children are raised in a cultural context different from that of their European American peers, Chinese American children may have more challenges in their socio-emotional development. Hence, one variable of the current study is examining the acculturation level of Chinese American fathers and how that affects father involvement and child social and emotional outcomes. Hopefully, examining fathers’ acculturation level will provide insight into the cultural values underlying Chinese American father involvement and how it affects children’s peer relationships and self-esteem. Acculturation Theory Acculturation, another form of within-group variability, is the degree to which an immigrant adapts to a new host culture (Pope-Davis, Liu, Nevitt, & Toporek, 2000; Weaver & Kim, 2008). Acculturation has been closely linked to parental child-rearing beliefs, which ultimately influences children’s development and psychological well-being in adolescence (Farver, Xu, Bhadha, Narang, & Lieber, 2007). There are two common models of acculturation theory, the unidirectional model and the bi-directional model. The unidirectional model is described as linear and oversimplified, as it portrays acculturation as a process of relinquishing the native culture in exchange for the new host culture. The bi-directional model is more widely accepted and takes into account the complexity of acculturation and recognizes that acculturation can be nonlinear (Flannery, 27 Reise, & Yu, 2001). Berry, Trimble, and Olmeda (1986) described four types of acculturation under the bi-directional model. The first one being assimilation, which is the discarding of one’s native culture and identifying with the new culture. The second type is marginalization, described as rejecting both the native and new culture. The third type is separation, which is the rejection of the new culture and adherence to the native culture. Lastly, integration is one type of acculturation that blends the native and new cultures. Research has shown the integration type of acculturation results in less conflict and anxiety within immigrant families. (Berry, Kim, & Boski, 1988; Berry, 1997). Chinese parents who immigrate to the United States are often faced with pressures to acculturate to the Western style of living. Depending on the family, acculturation levels vary across families and within the family unit itself. Often, there is a dissonance within parent and child relationships as children usually acculturate faster than their parents (Portes, 1997; Kwak, 2003). Weaver and Kim (2008) studied Chinese American parents and adolescents in Northern California and found that, when parents and children both had a bi-cultural profile, the children reported fewer depressive symptoms. This is consistent with the findings from past research that integration was associated with more positive psychological adjustment in adolescents. However, children who had a bicultural profile but had parents who were more Chinese-oriented, reported less supportive parenting and more depressive symptoms. The researchers also found that language (i.e., the ability of the parent and child to converse with each other) was the key predictor of Chinese American adolescents’ well-being in this study. Likewise, a study by Schofield, 28 Parke, Kim, and Coltrane (2008) with Mexican American families found that the acculturation gap within the parent-child relationship may serve as a stressor, but the quality of relationship may serve as a buffer. This is consistent with the Chinese American study that the effects of an acculturation gap can be counteracted by the quality of relationship between parent and child. Thus, the current study examined fathers’ acculturation and its correlation to father involvement. Acculturation is a concept that gives insight into immigrant families’ parenting values. Every individual within the family unit acculturates differently. Fathers may assimilate to the new culture, reject both the new and old culture, adhere to their native culture, or blend the two cultures together. Thus, depending upon their cultural outlook, fathers may hold values that affect their involvement with their children. Consequently, their involvement then affects children’s social and emotional outcomes. The following section will address parent and child relationships and its association with children’s social and emotional development. Role of Fathers in Children’s Social and Emotional Development Both mothers and fathers play distinct roles within the family, and the proximal process interactions that take place between the parent and child leads to different outcomes (Finley, Mira, & Schwartz, 2008; Pleck, 2007). The following section will briefly discuss the literature on how fathers’ role in children’s social and emotional development differs from mothers’ role, particularly in play interactions and 29 communicative style and the influence fathers have on children’s peer relationships and self-esteem. Influence of Fathers on Children’s Peer Relationships Fathers’ involvement in various aspects of childcare uniquely affects children’s social and emotional development and relationship with peers. Mothers and fathers differ in their play interactions with their children and their communication style with their children (Lanvers, 2004; Leaper, Anderson, & Sanders, 1998; MacDonald & Parke, 1984; McBride, Dyer, Liu, Brown, & Hong, 2009; Russell & Russell, 1987; TamisLeMonda et al., 2004). As a result, these differences in parental roles have an influence in children’s social and emotional outcomes, such as children’s ability to regulate their emotions, communicate with a variety of individuals, and make friends in school. These two differences will be discussed in further detail in the following subsections. Parental Differences in Play Styles. Past research has shown that mothers tend to be involved in all aspects of childcare, including play (Finley, Mira, & Schwartz, 2008; Russell & Russell, 1987). In contrast, fathers were rated as less involved in childcare responsibilities compared to mothers, except for the subscale of play interactions. Fathers and mothers spent about an equal amount of time playing with their children, but the type of play differed between fathers and mothers (Carson & Parke, 1996; Lewis & Lamb, 2003; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2004). Fathers engage in more rough and tumble play, especially with sons, while mothers’ play is less physically intensive and more verbally directive (Flanders, Leo, Paquette, Pihl, & Seguin, 2009; MacDonald & Parke, 1984). 30 Research done by Paquette (2004) suggests that children learn to regulate their emotions during rough and tumble play, especially when fathers were the more dominant figure during play. In those rough and tumble play sessions, fathers often controlled when their child had the upper hand, and was able to set limits on their child’s behavior. Physical play elevates children’s emotional arousal, which increases children’s aptitude to learn the emotional displays exhibited by their fathers in that situation. Mothers’ displays of emotion, however, were not found to significantly influence children’s emotional expression since mothers did not typically engage in that level of physical play with children (Boyum & Parke, 1995; Carson & Parke, 1996). A study by Flanders et al. (2009) found that fathers who were the dominant play partner had children who were better able to manage their emotions with their peers, and were rated by their teachers to be more popular than those children with fathers who were not as dominant. Those fathers who were more verbally directive during play had children who were less popular and scored lower on social competency scales, but mothers’ verbal directedness during play predicted children’s popularity (MacDonald & Parke, 1984). Fathers’ positive and negative affect during play is also an important component to children’s social and emotional development in terms of peer competency. Fathers who display higher intensity of positive affect with their children have children who were more prosocial in their peer relations, and daughters who were more liked by peers (Boyum & Parke, 1995). Fathers’ own emotional displays predicted children’s peer acceptance and social competence. Carson and Parke (1996) found that fathers who responded to children with negative affect during play had children who exhibited high 31 levels of externalizing behaviors and were more likely to be rejected by their peers. However, fathers who displayed more positive affect had children who mirrored those behaviors and were consequently better accepted by peers (McDowell & Parke, 2009; McDowell & Parke, 2005; Boyum & Parke, 1995; Rah & Parke, 2008). Although both mothers and fathers have differing play styles with their children, fathers are unique contributors to children’s social and emotional development. Parental Differences in Communication Style. Research indicates that mothers and fathers also differ in their communicative style with their children. For example, Lanvers (2004) found that mothers tend to talk with children more frequently than fathers do, and to use more supportive speech and engage in child-initiated topics. Fathers, on the other hand, tend to deliver more monologues on their own topic, deliver more commands to their children, and use directives in their speech, often asking fewer questions (Andersen, 1996; Leaper, Anderson, and Sanders, 1998; Lanvers, 2004). Communicative differences between mothers and fathers have been recognized for decades. Gleason (1975) proposed a Bridge Hypothesis that was developed on the premise that fathers were less sensitive to their children’s communicative intentions than mothers; thus, children had to work harder to communicate with their fathers. As a result, children were able to develop communication skills that could be generalized, or “bridged” to the outside world of peers (Mannle & Tomasello, 1987). Although this view has been debated, current research verifies that communicative styles do in fact, differ between fathers and mothers. McDowell, Parke, and Wang (2003) studied mothers’ and fathers’ advice-giving styles and discovered that fathers’ advice-giving predicted 32 children’s social competence over that of mothers’ advice giving. Fathers were more informative and directive in their speech, and tended to reserve advice giving until situations in which children needed advising. This is consistent with the study by Steinberg and Silk (2002) that found that children would seek out their mother’s support during emotional situations and seek out their fathers when they need information or material assistance. Thus, communicative patterns between mothers and fathers differ, but are important to children’s social and emotional outcomes. The current study was not able to examine fathers’ communication in detail, but had survey measures with aspects of communication embedded in them. Fathers influence children’s peer relationships indirectly through assisting in their children’s development of social competency. This is done through fathers’ play interactions, emotional displays, and communication style. Research has shown that fathers’ rough and tumble play interactions with their children help them regulate their emotions and understand social boundaries. Consequently, the children are then able to generalize this skill to their peer relationships. Literature in father involvement also showed that children learn emotional displays from their fathers and would model those displays during similar play situations with their peers. Lastly, fathers’ unique communication style contributes to children’s social competency, as children often seek out their fathers’ advice when they need information or material assistance. Thus, the current study assesses peer skills and friendship quality with respect to father involvement. 33 Influence of Fathers on Children’s Self-Esteem Self-esteem can be defined as an individual’s assessment of his or her own personal worth and perceptions of competency (Rosenberg, 1965; Coopersmith 1981). Self-esteem is an important construct to study in children’s social and emotional development, as low self-esteem is associated with negative child outcomes such as depression and substance abuse (Arbona & Power, 2003). Currently, there is limited data on how father involvement relates to children’s self-esteem. Earlier research done on young adults has shown the relationship between perceived parental warmth and control and higher self-esteem (Diener, Isabella, Behunin, & Wong, 2008; Parish & McCluskey, 1993); however, the samples were restricted to European American families. Researchers have proposed that self-esteem is socialized through parent-child interactions; but research is currently lacking in addressing the effects of other factors of father involvement on children’s self-esteem. Research done by Marin, Bohanek, and Fivush (2008) showed the different ways parents scaffold their children’s emotional understanding in conversations about past emotional events. Parents who were able to help their children understand aversive experiences had children with higher self-esteem and emotional regulation. However, earlier research has shown that fathers spoke less, in general, to their children than mothers (Lanvers, 2004), which may in turn affect the number of opportunities fathers can scaffold with their children about emotionally salient events. In fact, most research in the area of self-esteem has focused on mother-child attachment (Marin, Bohanek, & Fivush, 2008), thus there is a need to study father involvement in relation to self-esteem. 34 The current study correlated children’s self-esteem with peer relations in order to provide a comprehensive overview of children’s social and emotional development as well as supplement the literature on Chinese American fathering. Little is known about Chinese American fathers’ involvement and their children’s developmental outcomes. Chinese American children face numerous issues that correspond with being children of immigrant parents, and those issues are discussed in the next section. Issues Facing Chinese American Children Little research has been done on Chinese American families and children’s peer relationships and self-esteem. The research that does persist pertains to Asian American parental involvement in children’s academic achievement and the socialization goals of Asian mothers. However, there has been increasing research done in China and Hong Kong in regards to parenting styles and children’s emotional regulation. This research provides some insight into the dynamics of Chinese families living in the United States, as there may be some cultural practices and beliefs that have been carried over by immigrant parents. Many concerns arise for Chinese American children with immigrant parents, as their social and emotional development is affected by their relationship with their parents. Research has shown that children often acculturate to their new host culture faster than their parents. Children who identified more with Western culture, but had parents who were more Chinese oriented, had more conflicts with their parents, showed more depressive feelings, and had less motivation to do well in school (Costigan & 35 Dokis, 2006). Thus, more research on the Chinese American population is needed, as Chinese American children face numerous challenges that will later affect their social and emotional adjustment. The next subsection provides a brief overview on what little is known about Chinese American parenting style and its implications on children’s social and emotional development. Chinese American Parenting Styles Chinese American parenting style has been characterized as authoritarian by Western scales (Steinberg, Lamborn, Dornbusch, & Darling, 1992). However, these Western scales did not account for other factors involved in Chinese parenting. Chinese parenting, which can be deeply rooted in Confucian principles, focuses on teaching children to preserve the social hierarchy and group harmony. As a result, often times, Chinese parenting is perceived as controlling and directive in style (Chao, 1994). Chao (1994) identifies two Confucian concepts that better explain Chinese parenting rather than the Western definition of authoritarian: chiao shun, meaning training, and guan, meaning governance, care, and love – both of which include some authoritarian characteristics. Depending upon Chinese American parents’ acculturation levels, parents may vary in how closely they adhere to the Chinese way of childrearing. In traditional Chinese culture, parents often use shaming to teach modesty and humility (Huntsinger & Jose, 2009). With European American families, shaming tactics led to children’s increased aggression and depressive outcomes (Alessandri & Lewis, 1993). However, shaming did 36 not lead to those outcomes in Chinese families because it was counterbalanced with the principles of group harmony. In fact, Chinese American fathers who scored high on chiao shun, or training, had children who with fewer behavioral problems years later (Huntsinger & Jose, 2009). Thus, more research is needed to study Chinese American fathers’ parenting and involvement, since acculturation may be a mediating factor that influences how children are raised, which subsequently affects children’s developmental outcomes. Struggles with Ethnic Identity American culture, in contrast to Chinese culture, generally emphasizes independence by focusing more on an individual’s qualities and uniqueness rather than a connected and relational being (Cooper & Denner, 1998). This poses a problem for Chinese-American children who are reared in an environment that does not prioritize focusing on the self. However, when they attend the American school system, they are expected to be independent and self-reliant. Thus, maintaining an ethnic identity is difficult, and consequently, many Asian Americans suffer from depression. Emotions are de-emphasized in Chinese culture, but are relevant and valued in American culture (Soto, Levenson, & Ebling, 2005). Often times, these children are not taught how to cope with negative affect because the traditional Chinese-American parents’ response is to have their children suppress any negative emotions and avoid conflict (Chan & Leong, 1994). Chinese men are culturally expected not to express emotions since men should “drop blood but not tears” (Shek, 2000, p. 143). Chinese-American children exist at the juncture 37 of two vastly different cultures, which makes it difficult for these children to truly identify their ethnic identity. As a result, children may not know to what standard to evaluate their own personal worth and feelings of competence and significance, which may potentially lead to decreased self-esteem (Alvarez & Helms, 2001). Struggles with Self-Esteem Promotion of children’s self -esteem is a parenting goal commonly found in the US, but not one that is generalized across cultures. In fact, self-esteem is a term that does not even exist in the Chinese language. Miller, Wang, Sandel, and Cho (2002) compared European American and Taiwanese mothers’ beliefs about self-esteem. They found that the American mothers were concerned about what would lower their children’s selfesteem and self-perceptions and what they would need to do, as parents, to enhance selfconcept. After defining the term to the Taiwanese mothers, the researchers found that the Taiwanese mothers did not see self-esteem as a prioritized socialization goal. They thought too much praise and encouragement, practices commonly used by American mothers to enhance their children’s self-esteem, would end up crippling the children by giving them too much pride and inadvertently teaching them to disrespect authority. Instead, the Taiwanese mothers prioritized familial relationships, respect, and conforming to the collective. Asian-Americans live in a country that emphasizes self-esteem, but they may not receive much validation or encouragement in their home setting because selfesteem is not a major cultural value. Particularly, Chinese fathers are traditionally reserved in their emotional displays and may not teach children to value their self-esteem, 38 which may pose as a challenge to the social and emotional well-being of Chinese American children living in the United States. The Present Study In summary, studies on European American father-child relationships have been shown to influence children’s cognitive, language, and socio-emotional development. However, European American families are not the only ethnic group in the United States, thus do not give us a complete look at child development in this country. To better understand child development of a diverse US population, one must look at father involvement in the perspective of non-European cultures. Chinese-Americans make up a large percentage of the United States’ immigrant population. Little research has been done on Chinese-American parental involvement, let alone father-child interactions. Past analyses have shown the importance of acquiring a social identity and Chinese-American children are often at odds because the ideals promoted in their home life differs dramatically from the expectations promoted in their society. Three hypotheses are tested in this study: (a) Chinese American fathers who are more acculturated to American culture will score higher on the American scales of paternal involvement, (b) higher scores on father involvement scales will positively correlate with children’s socioemotional adjustment in terms of peer relations quality and (c) higher scores of father involvement will be a positively correlated to children’s self-esteem. 39 Chapter 3 METHODS This quantitative study focused on father involvement in Chinese American families and its relation to school-age children’s socio-emotional development. Specifically, this study examined the relation of Chinese American father involvement with the following areas: (a) their acculturation levels (b) their children’s quality of peer relationships and (c) their children’s levels of self-esteem. To investigate these three areas, correlational analyses were conducted and comparisons made between father groups that were more acculturated and those that were less acculturated. Fathers, mothers, and children provided data for this correlational study. Father involvement was measured by responses on the Paternal Involvement and Child Care Index (PICCI), and their acculturation level was measured by the number of years they resided in the US, as well as their ability to speak, read, and write the Chinese language. Children’s peer adjustment was measured through two questionnaires that were completed by the mother, the Quality of Child’s Friendship Questionnaire (QCFQ) and Behavior with Peers Questionnaire (BPQ). Children’s emotional adjustment was measured by their responses on the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory (CSEI). 40 Participants Thirty-one Chinese-American two-parent families with children aged 7 to 11 years were recruited from Chinese churches in Sacramento, Pleasanton, and Los Angeles. Both biological parents of the child were asked to participate in this study. Only those parents who had resided in the United States for at least 10 years, identified themselves as Chinese American, and were fluent in the English language, were included in this study. Both parents were told that the purpose of the study was to uncover fathers’ general role in Chinese families and examine the relationship between their involvement and children’s social and emotional development. Participating families were not reimbursed for their time, but were thanked and entered into a raffle drawing for a dinner gift certificate. Fathers and mothers in this sample were mainly born overseas: 22 fathers and 25 mothers were born in Hong Kong, mainland China, or Taiwan. Eight fathers and four mothers were born in the United States, one father was born in Peru, one mother was born in Jamaica, and one mother was born in Cambodia. On average, 61% of fathers and 40% of mothers had resided in the United States for more than 21 years, the other 39% and 60% resided in Hong Kong, China, or Taiwan, immigrating to the United States within the last 20 years. Twenty-one fathers were between the ages of 41 and 50 years, seven fathers were between the ages of 31 and 40 years, and three fathers were between the ages of 51 and 60 years old. Thirty fathers held Bachelor’s degrees or higher. 41 Seventeen of the 31 fathers could speak, read, and write Chinese fluently. Three fathers did not speak Chinese at all, and 10 fathers were unable to read or write any Chinese. There were 21 mothers between the ages of 41 to 50 years, and 10 mothers between the ages of 31 and 40. Twenty-five mothers held Bachelor’s degrees or higher. Twenty-one mothers were fluent in speaking, reading, and writing Chinese. Four mothers did not speak any Chinese, and eight mothers were reported to not be able to read or write Chinese. Twenty-one families reported to have some form of Chinese spoken in the household: 21 children (68%) of children were somewhat fluent in speaking, reading, and writing Chinese, and only four children (13%) were fully fluent in speaking Chinese, and two children (6%) were fully fluent in reading and writing Chinese. Six children (19%) were unable to speak any Chinese, and eight children (26%) were unable to read or write any Chinese. Procedures Recruitment The researcher, who is of Chinese decent, recruited and contacted families by visiting Chinese Christian churches in three California cities: Pleasanton, Sacramento, and Los Angeles. When visiting those sites, the researcher handed out recruitment forms as well as sent mass emails to members of those churches. Families interested in participating filled out recruitment forms or notified the researcher verbally or through email. The researcher also enlisted the help of friends and family who were active in the 42 Chinese community to help recruit additional participants. Many mothers in the original 35 families expressed interest and were eager to participate. However, for four families, the mother and child completed their surveys, but the father refused to complete forms. These four families were excluded from the study, resulting in 31 families that completed data for analysis. Data Collection Survey packets were either hand-delivered or emailed to the participating families. The families were then able to complete the surveys at their own convenience. Mothers and fathers were required to sign a consent form for themselves and their participating children prior to the start of the study. Fathers were asked to fill out a demographic survey and a parent involvement measure, the PICCI. Mothers were asked to fill out the QCFQ and the BPQ questionnaire. The child was asked to fill out the CSEI. The researcher then returned one week later to the families’ homes or used email correspondence to collect the completed questionnaires. Upon collecting the surveys, the families were thanked for their participation. Measures Paternal Measures Demographic Data. Fathers completed this general self-report questionnaire that contained open-ended questions and multiple-choice questions requesting basic 43 demographic information including age, the number of biological children living at home, years of education, and employment. Also, the questionnaire requested information about acculturation, including: (a) the number of years the father had resided in the United States and (b) father’s fluency in speaking and reading the Chinese language. The demographic questionnaire is presented in Appendix A. Paternal Involvement. Paternal Involvement was assessed using the Paternal Involvement and Child Care Index (PICCI) (Radin, 1982). This survey was administered in English and was completed by fathers. This instrument is a Likert-scale questionnaire that has been used extensively in child development research to measure paternal involvement in terms of task sharing in child rearing (Radin, 1982). The questionnaire’s measurement of father involvement has been comprehensive enough to be used crossculturally with African-American families, Israeli families, Puerto Rican families, and Indian families (Suppal & Roopnarine, 1999). This measure assesses paternal involvement under five subscales: general involvement (e.g., “How involved are you in caring for your children?”), childcare responsibilities (e.g., “How frequently do you feed the children?”), socialization responsibilities (e.g., “How often do you help children with personal problems?”), influence in child-rearing decisions (e.g., “”Who generally makes decisions about when children should be disciplined?”), and time-availability (e.g., “How often are you away from home on weekends?”). Fathers’ scores on each subscale were totaled for an overall score that ranged between 0 and 72 (Radin, 1982). There were two items that measured general involvement, and scores ranged from 0 to 24. For the childcare, socialization responsibilities, and time availability subscales, four questions 44 were used in each subscale, and had scores ranging from 1 to 12 in each sub-domain. Fathers’ involvement in decision-making was measured through two items, and scores also ranged from 1 to 12. Higher scores indicated more perceived paternal involvement (Culp et al, 2000). Radin (1982) found test-retest reliability to be very good. The PICCI is provided in Appendix B. Child Measures Self Esteem. The Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory (CSEI), a widely used measure in child development research, was used to assess general attitudes towards oneself, attitudes towards peers, parents, school, and personal interests. The current study used a specialized school version meant for children ages 8 to 15-years old. This measure contains 58 general statements in which the child selects whether the favorable or unfavorable aspects mentioned are "like me" or "not like me" (Coopersmith, 1981). Statements include the following: “I have a low opinion of myself,” “I’m a lot of fun to be with,” and “I never worry about anything.” This measure has also been used in a variety of cultural samples, including Turkish, Australian, Vietnamese Australian, and Latino students (Lane, White, Henson, 2002; Torres, Solberg, & Carlstrom 2002; Verkuyten, 1994). The CSEI scores each child on four aspects of their self-esteem: their general selfesteem, social self-esteem, self-esteem at home, and self-esteem in the school setting. Scores in each subdomain were summed, and higher scores indicated higher self-esteem. The CSEI also contains a “lie scale” as a validity indicator to help determine whether or 45 not the participant purposely tried to score high on the questionnaire. The “lie scale” was summed separately (range from 0-8), and adjusted in the final self-esteem scores. Johnson, Redfield, Miller, and Simpson (1983) evaluated the construct validity of the CSEI and discovered that it had convergent validity with two other self-concept surveys, the Piers-Harris Children’s Self-Concept Scale and the Coopersmith Behavioral Academic Scale. Chiu (1985) evaluated the CSEI in seven classrooms. Cronbach alpha was .85. See Appendix C for questionnaire. Peer Relations. Mothers were asked to complete two different questionnaires on their child’s peer relationships and the quality of those relationships via a Likert-type scale. First, mothers completed the Quality of Child’s Friendship Questionnaire (QCFQ) (Appendix D), a measure used in the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. For this measure, mothers selected how strongly they agreed to 20 statements such as, “My child and my child’s very best friend work well together,” and “My child and my child’s very best friend accuse each other of unfairness.” The questions were adapted from another measure, the Quality of Classroom Friends (Clark & Ladd, 2000). Clark and Ladd (2000) examined the construct validity of this measure and found that children’s numbers of friends was positively correlated with peer acceptance and friendship harmony, and was negatively correlated with peer conflict. Items were separated into positive (10 items) and negative (10 items) friendship. Total scores for each scale were obtained by taking the mean of relevant items. Cronbach alpha was .68 and .70 for positive and negative scales respectively. 46 To assess children’s social behavior with peers, mothers completed the Behavior with Peers scale also obtained from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. Mothers were to mark whether each of the 43 statements listed as “not true,” “sometimes true,” or “often true” of their child. Statements included, “My child disrupts peers’ activities,” and “My child is kind toward peers.” Items on this questionnaire were derived from Ladd and Profilet’s (1996) Child Behavior Scale (Cronbach alpha = .89 to .96), Kochenderfer and Ladd’s (1996) Peer Victimization Scale (Cronbach alpha = .74), and Crick’s (1996) Children’s Social Behavior Scale – Teacher Form (Cronbach alpha = .94). Items were separated into positive and negative peer behavior, and summed for total positive (Cronbach alpha = .69) and negative (Cronbach alpha = .68) peer behavior scores. See Appendix E for questionnaire. 47 Chapter 4 RESULTS Correlational analyses were conducted to examine the relationships among Chinese American fathers’ involvement and (a) their acculturation levels, (b) their children’s quality of peer relationships, and (c) their children’s self-esteem. Descriptive and correlational data on fathers’ involvement and acculturation levels are presented first. This is followed by descriptive data on children’s peer relationships, friendship qualities, and lastly, correlations between father involvement and self-esteem and quality of peer relations are presented. Chinese American Father Involvement and Acculturation Levels Descriptive Data Father Involvement and Acculturation Father involvement was measured through five subscales of the PICCI: (a) general involvement, (b) childcare responsibilities, (c) socialization responsibilities, (d) influence in decision-making, and (e) time-availability to spend with the children. On the subscale of general involvement, Chinese American fathers’ scores (M = 14.0; SD = 4.62) ranged from 1.24 (low involvement) to 24 (high involvement), with a total possible score of 24. The remaining subscales of childcare involvement (M = 2.43; SD =1.89), socialization (M = 3.27; SD = 1.74), decision-making (M = 4.70; SD = 2.03), and time 48 availability (M = 8.45; SD = 1.36), had scores that ranged from 1 (low involvement) to 12 (high involvement), with a total possible score of 12 in each domain. As shown in Table 1, the five subscales of the PICCI were intercorrelated. General involvement scores were positively correlated with fathers’ childcare involvement scores (r = .37, p < .05). Higher scores in childcare involvement were associated with higher scores on fathers’ socialization involvement (r = .42, p < .05). Lastly, father involvement in the socialization category was positively correlated with father’s involvement in the decision making process (r = .57, p < .01). Father’s time availability was not significantly correlated with any of the other father involvement subscales. Acculturation was indexed through three variables: fathers’ number of years living in the United States, their reported fluency in speaking the Chinese language, and their reported fluency in reading and writing Chinese. Most fathers had resided in the US for more than 10 years but less than 40 years; eight fathers were born in the US. Fathers ranged from being fully fluent in speaking, reading, and writing the Chinese language to not fluent at all. The number of years fathers reported residing in the US was negatively correlated with their fluency in reading and writing Chinese (r = -.79, p < .01); however; it was not significantly correlated to Chinese fathers’ fluency in speaking Chinese. Relations Between Acculturation and Father Involvement Table 1 shows Pearson correlations between fathers’ acculturation and father involvement. There were two significant relationships found between the father involvement and acculturation variables. First, fathers’ number of years of residence in 49 Table 1 Pearson Correlations Between Fathers’ Acculturation and Father Involvement 1. Years in the U.S. 2. Speak Chinese 3. Read Chinese 4. General Involvement 5. Childcare 6. Socialization 7. Decisions 8. Availability *p < .05; ** p < .01 1 2 -- -.09 -- 3 4 5 6 -.79** -.00 .02 .32 .42* -.19 .28 -.17 -.29 .37* .16 -.13 -- .01 -.10 -.30 -.30 .22 -- .37* .34 .09 -.08 -- .42* .10 .16 .57** -.06 -- 7 -- 8 .08 -- 50 the US was positively correlated with their reported decision-making responsibilities (e.g., decisions about when children should be disciplined and when children were old enough to try new things) (r = .42, p < .05). Second, fathers’ fluency in speaking Chinese was positively correlated with their socialization scores (e.g., setting limits for children’s behaviors and helping children with their personal problems) (r = .37, p < .05). Fathers’ involvement in childcare and their time availability were not significantly correlated to any acculturation measures used in this study. Fathers’ age was also not significantly correlated to any father involvement measures. Children’s Social and Emotional Development Descriptive Data on Children’s Social-Emotional Scores Children’s social relationships were examined through two survey measures, the BPQ and the QCFQ. These measures yielded four composite scores: Positive and Negative Peer Behavior and Positive and Negative Friendship Quality. For positive peer behavior, there was not much variability in scores (M = 1.65; SD = .28), as scores ranged from 1 to 2 on a scale of 0 to 3. There was also little variability on the negative peer behavior scores (M = .23; SD = .23), as scores ranged from .03 to .97. On the positive friendship quality measures, scores ranged from 2.09 to 5 and with a mean score of 3.39 (SD = .59). On the negative friendship quality measures, scores ranged from 1.11 to 4.89 (M = 2.31; SD = .84). 51 The CSEI yielded four subscales: (a) general self-esteem, (b) social self-esteem, (c) self-esteem at home with parents, and (d) self-esteem in the school setting. Refer to Table 3 for descriptive data. The subscales of the CSEI were correlated with each other (r’s ranging from .53 to .77, p < .01). Correlations Between Father Involvement and Social-Emotional Measures Correlational analyses were employed to examine relationships between father involvement and children’s peer behavior, friendship qualities, and self-esteem. As Table 2 shows, there were significant correlations of father involvement with children’s friendship quality measures but not with peer relation measures. Unexpectedly, father involvement in childcare responsibilities was negatively correlated with both children’s positive friendship qualities (r = -.37, p < .05) and negative friendship qualities (r = -.52, p < .05). Additionally, fathers who scored higher on their availability to their children had children with less positive friendship qualities (r = -.41, p < .05). Positive friendship qualities were significantly positively correlated to negative friendship qualities (r = .46, p < .01), but no significant relationships were found between quality of friendships and children’s positive and negative peer relations. Table 3 displays the correlations between Chinese American father involvement variables and children’s levels of self-esteem. Father involvement was not significantly correlated with children’s self-esteem with exception to father’s availability being positively correlated with children’s social self-esteem (r = .41, p < .05). 52 Table 2 Pearson Correlations between Chinese American Father Involvement and Children’s Peer Relationships 1. General Involvement 2. Childcare 3. Socialization 4. Decisions 5. Availability 6. Positive Friendship Qualities 7. Negative Friendship Qualities 8. Positive Peer Relations 9. Negative Peer Relations *p < .05; ** p < .01 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 -- .37* .34 .09 -.08 -.31 -.34 -.02 .06 -- .42* .10 .16 -.37* -.52** -.04 -.16 -- .57** -.06 .19 -.08 .29 .02 -- .08 .21 .08 .31 -.34 -- -.41* -.21 .08 -.21 .46** .16 .01 -.19 .25 -- -.24 -- -- -- 53 Table 3 Pearson Correlations between Chinese American Father Involvement and Children’s Self-Esteem 1. General Involvement 2. Childcare 3. Socialization 4. Decisions 5. Availability 6. General Self-Esteem 7. Social Self-Esteem 8. Home and Parents SelfEsteem 9. Social/ Academic SelfEsteem *p < .05; ** p < .01 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 -- .37* .34 .09 -.08 -.01 .06 .20 -.31 -- .42* .10 .16 -.11 .12 .17 -.24 .57** -.06 .03 -.23 -.05 -.32 .08 .14 -.06 -.20 -.11 -- .09 .41* .12 -.11 -- .58** .77** .75** -- .53** .56** -- .60** -- -- 9 -- 54 Chapter 5 DISCUSSION Conceptions of father involvement have changed over time in the United States, and fathers’ involvement in childrearing is becoming the norm. Fathers are no longer seen as just breadwinners, but are being recognized to be unique contributors to children’s development. Despite the research that is currently emerging on fathers, much is still unknown, especially regarding fathers of various cultural groups. The present study aimed to supplement the literature on fathers, particularly Chinese American fathers, since Chinese are a fast growing ethnic group in the United States. The following sections will discuss the findings of the current study on the relationships between Chinese American fathers’ involvement and (a) their acculturation levels, (b) their children’s quality of peer relationships, and (c) their children’s self-esteem. Acculturation and Father Involvement Fathers’ acculturation did not relate to their general involvement in their children’s lives. However, fathers who resided in the US for a longer amount of time were more involved in decision-making for their children than were those fathers who had lived in the US for a shorter amount of time. Decisions included when the children should be disciplined as well as when children were old enough to try new things. 55 Fathers’ role as disciplinarians is shared by both American and Chinese cultures (Costigan & Dokis, 2006). However, in Chinese culture, children were not given many opportunities to try novel activities on their own, as this was not normative in Chinese parenting. Chinese parents were often characterized as being directive toward their children, instructing them on what they are to do, as this adheres to the Confucian concepts of training (chiao shun) and governance (guan) (Chao, 1994). Thus, fathers who were more acculturated to the US would be more prone to be involved in the decisionmaking process, especially in the area of children trying new things, since developing independence is valued by Western culture (Wang & Fivush, 2005). Interestingly, fathers who were more fluent in speaking the Chinese language (less acculturated) were more involved in the socialization aspects of childcare. Socialization factors included helping the children with personal problems and helping them learn, as well as disciplinary actions of teaching appropriate behavior (e.g., punishing the children and setting limits on their behavior). Because the PICCI was meant as a tool to evaluate only fathers, socialization included many aspects of discipline, which is in line with the expectations of the role of fathers across cultures (Suppal & Roopnarine, 1999). Thus, socialization on this measure was significantly correlated with the decision-making subdomain, as many of the describing characteristics of socialization were related to discipline. This particular subdomain of socialization may have corresponded more with Chinese cultural values and expectations than it did with American values. Results indicated that Chinese fathers who were more fluent in speaking the Chinese language might have had more of a Chinese orientation than 56 American orientation (Chao & Tseng, 2002). As a result, these fathers may take more responsibility than mothers in disciplining children, overseeing their behavior, and governing their education (Kwak, 2003). Thus, the findings that the fathers who were more fluent in speaking Chinese were more involved in their children’s socialization is consistent with the findings of Chao and Tseng (2002), in that those with Chinese orientation valued education, training, and governing their children. Correlations Between Father Involvement and Peer Relationship Measures Father involvement was correlated with children’s behavior with peers, but no significant correlations were found. The BPQ assessed children’s social skills within their peer group. Originally, teachers were meant to fill out the BPQ, but for the purposes of this study, the mother completed it. Thus, this may explain why there were no significant findings in this area since mothers typically did not have the opportunities to observe their child’s general behaviors with peers in the home environment. However, the QCFQ posed different results. Unlike the BPQ, which was designed to be a teacher-reported measure, the QCFQ was intended to be completed by mothers. The QCFQ asked mothers to think about their child’s specific relationship with his or her best friend. Thusly, significant correlations were found between father involvement and children’s friendship quality scores from the QCFQ. Father involvement in childcare responsibilities, such as having sole responsibility for the children, feeding them, playing with them, making sure they are bathed and 57 dressed, and putting them to sleep, was significantly negatively correlated to negative friendship qualities. Fathers who were more involved in childcare were associated with children who displayed fewer negative friendship qualities such as, being verbally aggressive, ignoring other’s suggestions, criticizing others, and protesting when others control play. This is consistent with research done by Paquette (2004), which found that children learned emotional displays from their fathers during play scenarios. Particularly, fathers who were effectively able to set limits on their children’s behaviors during play scenarios had children who displayed more social competence with peers. The results of the current study supports the idea that fathers who were more involved in childcare responsibilities had children who displayed fewer negative friendship characteristics. Interestingly, father involvement in childcare responsibilities was also negatively correlated with positive friendship qualities. Positive friendship qualities included children working well with each other, negotiating peacefully, using kind words, and respecting others’ preferences and attitudes. The survey used to measure children’s quality of friendships was designed for American families and may not be applicable to children with Chinese backgrounds. In traditional Chinese culture, fathers were directive in their interactions with children and did not express emotions verbally (Jankowiak, 1992; Shek, 2000). Parents often used verbal directions to instruct their children on what they are to do, as reflected in the Chinese child-rearing ideologies of chiao shun and guan. As a result, skills such as respecting a peer’s preferences, using nice words, and working with each other, were not skills socialized within the home. Instead, parenting efforts were diverted to teaching children to be respectful and subordinate to those in 58 authority (Chao, 1994; Suizzo, 2007). Thus, fathers who were more involved in childcare responsibilities may have spent more time reiterating the hierarchical relationships within the home, and did not directly teach children how they were to act with people who were not within the social hierarchy, such as their equals, or peers. Additionally, fathers who were more available to spend time with their children also had children who displayed fewer positive friendship qualities. Chinese fathers who were available might have utilized that time to teach children their role in the social hierarchy as well as focused heavily on children’s academic achievement (Chao, 1994; Leung, Lau, & Lam, 1998). Also, Chinese fathers may have been more available to fulfill their role as being the disciplinarian. In a study by Chang, Schwartz, Dodge, and McBride-Chang (2003), Chinese adolescents perceived their fathers’ parenting as harsh, and reported higher occurrences of aggression within their interactions with peers. As a result, if fathers were to spend more time within the home, children may have also perceived their fathers’ parenting as harsh, and may not have had the opportunity to learn the positive social skills valued on the American scales of friendship quality. Thus, more research is needed to understand Chinese American fathers’ role within the home in order to understand children’s social and emotional outcomes. The results of this study showed that father involvement in general, is a complex construct that is culturally specific. General father involvement, as measured by the PICCI, may not be a comprehensive enough measure to capture the subtleties of Chinese American parenting. Instead, subdomains of father involvement showed more decisive results since they were better able to describe characteristics of Chinese parenting. For 59 example, Chinese American fathers’ involvement in childcare was associated with positive children’s friendship quality, which was in line with McDowell and Parke’s (2005) study that showed that fathers who played with their children were able to model appropriate behaviors that children could later emulate when around their friends. Father involvement in time availability has been associated with positive outcomes for children in European American families in the past (Heath, 2001; Landale & Oropesa, 2001; Saleh et al., 2005; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2004). However, the case was the opposite in Chinese American households. It was not necessarily beneficial for Chinese fathers to have increased availability for children at home because Chinese fathers have often been perceived as strict, reserved, and directive (Shek, 2000). Correlations Between Father Involvement and Children’s Self-Esteem There were no significant correlations found between fathers’ general involvement and children’s general self-esteem. However, the availability subdomain of the PICCI showed significant results to children’s friendship qualities. In the previous sections, results indicated that fathers’ availability was negatively correlated to children’s display of positive friendship qualities; however, fathers’ availability was positively correlated to children’s social self-esteem. Social self-esteem was defined as children’s perception of worth about themselves when they are in the community around other individuals (Coopersmith, 1981). Children who scored high in this area typically agreed with the following statements: “I’m a lot of fun to be with,” “I’m popular with kids my 60 own age,” “I’m easy to like,” and “Kids usually follow my ideas.” This shows children’s self perceptions are not directly related to the friendship quality scale used in this study. Although traditional Chinese families did not recognize self-esteem as an important value to cultivate in their children, they did teach their children the importance of preserving social harmony (Miller, Wang, Sandel, & Cho, 2002). Even though Chinese parents may not deliberately teach their children how to interact with peers, they ingrained the principle of social harmony in their children. Results show that Chinese American children may not be outright in their display of positive friendship qualities, but they perceive that they are liked and accepted by their peers. Boucher, Peng, Shi, and Wang (2009) found that Chinese Americans possessed inconsistent self-esteem, where their actions differed from their self-perceptions. Thus, father involvement, in the area of time availability, is important to the development of children’s social self-esteem. In past research on European American families, fathers’ general involvement was associated with children’s increased social competence and peer acceptance, which led to children’s increased self-esteem (McDowell & Parke, 2005). However, results of this study showed that Chinese American fathers’ increased involvement, particularly in time availability, did not follow this same pathway. Chinese American father who were more available to spend time with children had children who displayed fewer positive friendship qualities, but had social self-esteem that was unaffected. The measures used for this study were created for the European American population; thus, results differed when used on Chinese Americans who have different cultural values and practices. This study adds to the literature that Chinese parents, who may be perceived as authoritarian in 61 parenting style, did not necessarily have children with poorer outcomes. The Confucian ideals of social harmony and respect for those in authority may have mediated the effects of father involvement on children’s self-esteem. Limitations of the Study The results of the study showed that Chinese American father involvement is complex, and the current measures did not take into account cultural ideas such as the preservation of social harmony or compliance with the social hierarchy. However, the findings still posed significant results in that Chinese American fathers played significant roles in children’s social and emotional outcomes. Future research should include more culturally sensitive tools to better understand this fast growing ethnic group. The following subsections discuss in further detail, limitations to the current study. Sample Size and Participants This study was limited by several characteristics of the sample. First, due to the small sample size, many correlations were not statistically significant. For example, the number of years fathers resided in the U.S. was positively correlated to their involvement in socializing their children, but only approached statistical significance (r = .319, p = .08). Likewise, fathers’ involvement in decisions was positively correlated to children’s school self esteem (r = .316, p = .08) and children’s positive peer relationships (r = .306, 62 p = .09). A larger sample might have yielded a clearer pattern of results on father involvement in Chinese American families. Second, participants were recruited from Chinese churches from Sacramento, the Bay Area, and Los Angeles. As a result, many participants may uphold certain parenting values based upon their specific religious beliefs, and results may not be generalized to the entire Chinese American community. Additionally, there may be differences between Chinese families in the California and other regions of the US. Future research would benefit from a sample of Chinese American families from differing religious affiliations and regions of the US. Third, the researcher found it difficult to recruit families in which the father was willing to participate. During the recruitment process, mothers showed more interest in participating, and fathers commonly refused. On four occasions, fathers verbally agreed to participate, but did not complete their survey measures, although the mother and child completed theirs. As a result, those fathers who agreed to participate may have been more involved in their children’s upbringing than those fathers who refused to participate. Therefore, results cannot be fully generalized to all Chinese American families in the US. Measures The study was also limited by characteristics of the survey measures used. This study only took into account father involvement and did not take into consideration mother involvement. Mothers may mediate many father involvement variables by being the maternal “gatekeeper” to their children (Cabrera et al., 2000). Depending upon the 63 relationship between mothers and fathers, mothers may place restrictions on when fathers can come into contact with the children. This would then have a bearing on the results of this study. Acculturation was an important variable to this study. A limitation of the present study is that a formalized acculturation measure was not used to evaluate Chinese American fathers’ acculturation to the U.S. This study oversimplified acculturation and did not measure the subtleties of acculturation as mentioned in Berry, Trimble, and Olmeda’s (1986) work. The current study was not able to determine what type of acculturation (assimilation, marginalization, separation, or integration) characterized the fathers. Instead, the study used demographic survey measures as an estimation of acculturation. In the future, a standardized measure of acculturation would be useful to examine acculturation more in depth and how it relates to fathers’ parenting beliefs and involvement. To determine children’s social and emotional outcomes, two questionnaires on peer relationships and a questionnaire on self-esteem was used, and results were generalized to describe children’s social and emotional development. Complexities of children’s social and emotional development, such as externalizing behaviors, social competency, and emotional regulation were variables that were not taken into account. The Behavior with Peers Questionnaire was originally designed to be completed by teachers, but was designated to the mothers for simplicity purposes. As a result, mothers had limited ability to rate their children’s general interactions with other children, as they were not usually in the school setting. This may explain the non-significant results with 64 this measure. Additionally, the measures used were not specific for children of an ethnic background. As the social cultural context continues to change, there is a need for the development of culturally sensitive measurement tools. Many cultural factors that were specific to the Chinese population were not captured in the surveys used. In the future, more of a qualitative approach in which families are interviewed may be beneficial in capturing the subtleties of the family structure in that culture. Particularly, children’s own perspectives on their fathers’ involvement will be useful to understanding children’s social and emotional outcomes. Although there are numerous limitations to this study, the results are an important contribution to the field of child development. Fathers, who have been overlooked in the past, and whose roles have been considered second to mothers, are in fact monumental contributors to children’s developmental outcomes. Although little is known on Chinese American fathers, this study shows that Chinese American fathers are nonetheless important figures, and their involvement affects children’s social and emotional outcomes. In conclusion, future research should focus on more cultural samples of fathers as well as include comparative studies between various groups, such as Chinese families compared to Chinese American families to keep with the changing shifts in the social cultural contexts of the US. 65 APPENDICES 66 APPENDIX A Demographic Questionnaire This survey asks about you and your spouse. For questions about your spouse, please answer them to the best of your knowledge. All responses will be kept confidential and anonymous. Please remember that your participation is completely voluntary. You are not required to answer all questions and may choose to discontinue your participation at any time. However, any amount of participation will be beneficial and greatly appreciated. You 1. Where were you born (country)? a. If not born in the US what age were you when you immigrated to the US? 2. As of today, how long have you resided in the United States? 3. What country were your parents raised in? 4. Are your parents residing in the US? If yes, how long have they been living in the US? Circle one: Yes Your Spouse No Circle one: Yes If Yes: ____________ 5. How fluent are you in speaking the Chinese language? (0=not at all; 4=very fluent) 6. How fluent are you in reading and writing the Chinese language? (0=not at all; 4=very fluent) 7. What is your age? 8. What is the highest level of education you have completed? 9. What is your religions affiliation? (If you don’t have one, say “none”) 10. What is the primary language spoken in your household? 11. How many children are residing in your home? 12. How fluent is your child in speaking the Chinese language? (0=not at all; 4=very fluent) 13. How fluent is your child in reading and writing the Chinese language? (0=not at all; 4=very fluent) No If Yes: ____________ 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 a. 18-21 b. 22-25 c. 26-30 d. 31-40 e. 41-50 f. 51-60 g. 61 or over h. 18-21 i. 22-25 j. 26-30 k. 31-40 l. 41-50 m. 51-60 n. 61 or over 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 Demographic Questionnaire 1 of 1 67 APPENDIX B Paternal Involvement in Child Care Index (PICCI) N. Radin Please complete the following questions to the best of your ability. The purpose of this study is by no means an evaluation or judgment of any individual or individual family. Rather, it is aimed to uncover fathers’ general role in the family. Please remember that your participation is completely voluntary. You are not required to answer all questions and may choose to discontinue your participation at any time. However, any amount of participation will be beneficial and greatly appreciated. 1. How involved are you in caring for your children? (Please mark one) a. Very involved b. Involved c. Neutral d. Uninvolved e. Very uninvolved 2. Not counting the hours your youngster is in a school or center, with a sitter, or asleep for the night, what percentage of the remaining time are you the child’s primary caregiver? (Prime caregiver is meant the person who must be available to attend to the child’s needs). ________ % You ________ % Your spouse How frequently are the following parenting tasks done in your family? (circle one response) Frequently Sometimes Infrequently 3. Feeding the children 4. Having sole responsibility for the children 5. Punishing the children 6. Setting limits for the children’s behavior 7. Helping children with personal problems 8. Bathing and dressing the children 9. Putting the children to bed 10. Helping children to learn 3 2 1 3 2 1 3 2 1 3 2 1 3 2 1 3 2 1 3 2 1 3 2 1 What percentages of these tasks are done by: Self Spouse Other PICCI 1 of 2 Taken from Handbook of Family Measurement Techniques 68 11. Who in your family generally makes decisions about when children should be disciplined? a. Husband always b. Husband more than wife c. Husband and wife equally d. Wife more than husband e. Wife always 12. Who in your family generally makes decisions about when children are old enough to try new things? a. Husband always b. Husband more than wife c. Husband and wife equally d. Wife more than husband e. Wife always How available are you to the children? 13. Away from home and children weeks and months at a time 14. Away from home days at a time 15. Away from home on weekends 16. Out in the evening at least 4 nights a week 17. Out in the evening at least 2 nights a week 18. Misses supper with children at least 2 nights a week 19. Has breakfast during the week with children and family 20. Home during the week for lunch 21. Home afternoons when children come home from school 22. Home all day during the week with children and family How available is your spouse to the children? Frequently Sometimes Infreq. Frequently Sometimes Infreq. 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 2 3 PICCI 2 of 2 Taken from Handbook of Family Measurement Techniques 69 APPENDIX C Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory CSEI-SC For use by Lillian Wu only. Received from Mind Garden, Inc. on September 2, 2007 Please read through the statements and check whether the statement is “like you” or “unlike you.” Like Me Unlike Me Statements 1. Things usually don’t bother me. 2. I find it very hard to talk in front of the class. 3. There are lots of things about myself I’d change if I could. 4. I can make up my mind without too much trouble. 5. I’m a lot of fun to be with. 6. I get upset easily at home. 7. It takes me a long time to get used to anything new. 8. I’m popular with kids my own age. 9. My parents usually consider my feelings. 10. I give in very easily. 11. My parents expect too much of me. 12. It’s pretty tough to be me. 13. Things are all mixed up in my life. 14. Kids usually follow my ideas. 15. I have a low opinion of myself. 16. There are many times when I’d like to leave home. 17. I often feel upset in school. 18. I’m not as nice looking as most people. 19. If I have something to say, I usually say it. 20. My parents understand me. 21. Most people are better liked than I am. 22. I usually fell as if my parents are pushing me. 23. I often get discouraged at school. 24. I often wish I were someone else. 25. I can’t be depended on. 26. I never worry about anything. 27. I’m pretty sure of myself. 28. I’m easy to like. 29. My parents and I have a lot of fun together. CSEI-SC 1 of 2 70 Like Me Unlike Me Statements 30. I spend a lot of time daydreaming. 31. I wish I were younger. 32. I always do the right thing. 33. I’m proud of my school work. 34. Someone always has to tell me what to do. 35. I’m often sorry for the things I do. 36. I’m never happy. 37. I’m doing the best work that I can. 38. I can usually take care of myself. 39. I’m pretty happy. 40. I would rather play with children younger than I am. 41. I like everyone I know. 42. I like to be called on in class. 43. I understand myself. 44. No one pays much attention to me at home. 45. I never get scolded. 46. I’m not doing as well in school as I’d like to. 47. I can make up my mind and stick to it. 48. I really don’t like being a boy/girl. 49. I don’t like to be with other people. 50. I’m never shy. 51. I often feel ashamed of myself. 52. Kids pick on me very often. 53. I always tell the truth. 54. My teachers make me feel I’m not good enough. 55. I don’t care what happens to me. 56. I’m a failure. 57. I get upset easily when I’m scolded. 58. I always know what to say to people. CSEI-SC 2 of 2 71 APPENDIX D Quality of Child’s Friendship Questionnaire Please answer how your child and his or her closest friend get along by agreeing or disagreeing with the following statements. Please remember that your participation is completely voluntary. You are not required to answer all questions and may choose to discontinue your participation at any time. However, any amount of participation will be beneficial and greatly appreciated. My child and my child’s very best friend… 1. Work well together 2. Ignore each other’s suggestions 3. Are very competitive with one another 4. Negotiate peacefully to settle issues 5. Take turns effectively 6. Are verbally aggressive with each other 7. Say they like each other or are friends 8. Reach agreement easily 9. Get mad at each other a lot 10. Readily comply with one another’s request 11. Say “I hate you” or “I’m not going to play with you” when angry at one another 12. Show a pattern where one child dominates the other 13. Endorse one another’s attitudes and preferences 14. Pick each other as partners 15. Help each other out 16. Criticize each other 17. Notice and respond to each other’s protests and complaints 18. Share readily with each other 19. Protest when the other child attempts to control the play 20. Accuse each other of unfairness Strongly Disagree Disagree Agree Strongly Agree 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 1 2 3 4 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 1 2 2 3 3 4 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 1 1 2 2 2 3 3 3 4 4 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 QCFQ 1 of 1 Taken from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care 72 APPENDIX E Behavior with Peers Questionnaire Please describe your child’s behavior with peers. Ratings should be based on your observation of the child in the care setting, both indoors and outdoors, and anywhere else you have observed your child interacting with peers. Circle the number of the description that best applies. Please remember that your participation is completely voluntary. You are not required to answer all questions and may choose to discontinue your participation at any time. However, any amount of participation will be beneficial and greatly appreciated. 1. Tends to react to other children’s distress by teasing them or making things worse 2. Not chosen as playmate by peers 3. Likes to be alone 4. Keeps peers at a distance 5. Peers avoid this child 6. When mad at a peer, gets even by excluding the peer from the group 7. Seems concerned when other children are distressed 8. Is an aggressive child 9. Taunts and teases other children 10. Often unoccupied 11. Threatens other children 12. Spreads rumors or gossips about some peers 13. Takes turns with play materials 14. Kind toward peers 15. Can be trusted, is dependable 16. Listens to classmates 17. When angry at a peer, tries to get other children to stop playing with the peer 18. Is excluded from peers’ activities 19. Compromises in conflict with peers 20. Is ignored by peers 21. Is cooperative with peers 22. Loses temper easily in conflicts with peers 23. Argues with peers 24. Friendly toward other children 25. Annoys or irritates other children 26. Is a solitary child 27. Disrupts peers’ activities 28. 0 1 2 When mad at a peer, ignores the Not True Sometimes True Often True 0 1 2 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 0 1 2 0 1 2 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 0 1 2 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 73 peer or stops talking to the peer 29. Shows concern for moral issues (e.g., fairness, welfare of others) 0 1 2 BPQ 1 of 2 Taken from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care 30. Is ridiculed by peers 31. Avoids peers 32. Offers help or comfort when other children are upset 33. Withdraws from peer activities 34. Will continue to bother or hurt other children even when they are clearly upset 35. Is bossy toward peers 36. Threatens to stop being a peer’s friend in order to hurt the peer or to get what is wanted from peer 37. Is picked on by other children 38. Is called names by peers 39. Is pushed around by other children 40. Peers say negative things about him/her to other children 41. Is teased or made fun of by peers 42. Is hit or kicked by other children 43. Tries to exclude certain peers from peer group activities Not True Sometimes True Often True 0 0 0 1 1 1 2 2 2 0 0 1 1 2 2 0 0 1 1 2 2 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 2 2 2 2 0 0 0 1 1 1 2 2 2 74 BPQ 2 of 2 Take from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care 75 REFERENCES Alessandri, S.M., & Lewis, M. (1993). Parental evaluation and its relations to shame and pride in young children. Sex Roles, 29, 335-343. Alvarez, A.N. & Helms, J.E. (2001). Asian American acculturation and ethnic/racial identity: Research innovations in the new millennium. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 7(3), 217-231. Andersen, E.S. (1992). Speaking with Style: The Sociolinguistic Skills of Children. London, UK: Routledge. Arbona, C., & Power, T.G. (2003) Parental attachment, self-esteem, and antisocial behaviors among African American, European American and Mexican American adolescents. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50, 40-51. Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology Monograph, 4, 1-103. Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on adolescent competence and substance use. Journal of Early Adolescence, 11(1), 56-95. Berndt, T.J. (1996). Exploring the effects of friendship quality on social development. In W.M. Bukowski, A.F. Newcomb & W.W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 346-365). New York: Cambridge University Press. Berry, J.W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychiatry: An International Review, 46, 5-68. Berry, J., Kim, U., & Boski, J. (1988). Psychological acculturation of immigrants. In Y. Kim & W. G. (Eds.), Cross-cultural adaptations: Current approaches (pp. 62-87). Newbury Park, California: Sage. Boucher, H., Peng, K., Shi, J., & Wang, L. (2009). Culture and implicit self-esteem: Chinese are “good” and “bad” at the same time. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 24-45. Boyum, L.A,, & Parke, R.D. (1995). The role of family emotional expressiveness in the development of children’s social competence. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 593-608. 76 Berry, J.W., Trimble, J.E., & Olmedo, E.L. (1986). Assessment of acculturation. In W.J. Lonner & J.W. B. (Eds.), Field methods in cross-cultural research (pp. 291324). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Bond, M.H., & Hwang, K.K. (1986). The social psychology of the Chinese people. In M.H. Bond (Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people, (pp. 213-266). New York: Oxford: University Press. Bronfenbrenner U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723-742. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1994). Ecological models of human development. In T. Husen & T.N. Postlethwaite (Ed.), The international encyclopedia of education. (2nd ed., pp. 1643-1647). New York: Elsevier Science. Buhrmester, D. (1990). Intimacy of friendship, interpersonal competence, and adjustment during preadolescence and adolescence. Child Development, 61, 1101-1111. Cabrera, N.J., Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Bradley, R.H., Hofferth, S., & Lamb, M.E. (2000). Fatherhood in the twenty-first century. Child Development, 71(1), 127-136. Carson, J. L., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in parent-child interactions and children's peer competency. Child Development, 67, 2217-2226. Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K.A., McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology, 17(4), 598-606. Chan, S., & Leong, C.W. (1994). Chinese families in transition: Cultural conflicts and adjustment problem. Journal of Social Distress and the Homeless, 3(3), 263-281. Chao, R.K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65, 1111-1119. Chao, R., & Tseng, V. (2002). Parenting of Asians. In M.H. Bornstein (Series Ed.), Handbook of parenting, Vol. 4: Social conditions and applied parenting (2nd ed., pp. 59-93). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Chiu, L.H. (1985). The reliability and validity of the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory – Form B. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 45(4), 945-949. 77 Clark, K.E., & Ladd, G.W. (2000). Connectedness and autonomy support in parent-child relationships: Links to children’s socio-emotional orientation and peer relationships. Developmental Psychology, 36, 485-498. Cooper, C.R., & Denner, J. (1998). Theories linking culture and psychology: Universal and community-specific processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 559-584. Coopersmith, S. (1981). The antecedents of self-esteem. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. (Original work published 1967). Costigan, C.L., & Dokis, D.P. (2006). Relations between parent-child acculturation differences and adjustment within immigrant Chinese families. Child Development, 77(5), 1252-1267. Crick, N.R. (1996). The role of overt aggression, relational aggression, and prosocial behavior in the prediction of children’s future social adjustment. Child Development, 67(5), 2317-2327. Culp, R.E., Schadle, S., Robinson, L., & Culp, A.M. (2000). Relationships among paternal involvement and young children’s perceived self-competence and behavioral problems. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 9(1), 27-38. Diener, M.L., Isabella, R.A., Behunin, M.G., & Wong, M.S. (2008). Attachment to mothers and fathers during middle childhood: Associations with child gender, age, and competence. Social Development, 17, 84-101. Farver, J.M., Xu, Y., Bhadha, B.R., Narang, S., & Lieber, E. (2007). Ethnic identity, acculturation, parenting beliefs, and adolescent adjustment: A comparison of Asian Indian and European American families. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 53(2), 184-215. Finley, G.E. Mira, S.D., & Schwartz, S.J. (2008). Perceived paternal and maternal involvement: Factor structures, mean differences, and parental roles. Fathering, 6(1), 62-82. Flanders, J.L., Leo, V., Pihl, R.O., & Seguin, J.R. (2009). Rough-and-tumble play and the regulation of aggression: an observational study of father-child play dyads. Aggressive behavior, 35(4), 285-295. Flannery, W., Reise, P., & Yu, J. (2001). An empirical comparison of the uni0drectional and bi-directional models of acculturation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1035-1045. 78 Gleason, J.B. (1975). Fathers and other strangers: Men’s speech to young children. In D.P. Dato (Ed.), Theory and Application (pp. 289-297). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press. Heath, D.T. (2001). The impact of delayed fatherhood on the father-child relationship. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 155(4), 511-530. Hofferth, S.L. (2003). Race/Ethnic differences in father involvement in two-parent families: Culture, context, or economy? Journal of Family Issues, 24(2), 185-216. Hofferth, S.L., Cabrera, N., Carlson, M., Coley, R.L., Day, R., Schindler, H. (2007). Resident father involvement and social fathering. In S.L. Hofferth & L.M.C. (Eds.), Handbook of measurement issues in family research (pp. 335-374). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers. Huntsinger, C.S., & Jose, P.E. (2009). Relations among parental acceptance and control and children’s social adjustment in Chinese American and European American families. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(3), 321-330. Jankowiak, W. (1992). Father-child relations in urban China. In B.S. Hewlett (Ed.), Father-child relations: Cultural and biosocial contexts (pp. 345-363). Adline DeGruyter: New York. Johnson, B.W., Redfield, D.L., Miller, R.L., & Simpson, R.E. (1983). The Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory: A construct validation study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 43(3), 907-913. Kochenderfer, B.J., & Ladd, G.W. (1996). Helping children become more prosocial: Ideas for families, schools, and communities. Young Children, 51(2), 62-70. Kwak, K. (2003). Adolescents and their parents: A review of intergenerational family relations for immigrant and non-immigrant families. Human Development, 46, 15-136. Ladd, G.W., & Profilet, S.M. (1996). The child behavior scale: A teacher-report measure of young children’s aggressive, withdrawn, and prosocial behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32(6), 1008-1024. Lamb, M.E. (2000). The history of research on father involvement: An overview. Fatherhood: Research, Interventions and Policies, 23-42. Lane, G.G., White, A.E., & Henson, R.K. (2002). Expanding reliability generalization methods with KR-21 estimates: An RG study of the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 62, 685-711. 79 Landale, N.S., & Oropesa, R.S. (2001). Father involvement in the lives of mainland Puerto Rican children: Contributions of nonresident, cohabiting and married fathers. Social Forces, 79(3), 945-968. Lanvers, U. (2004). Gender in discourse behaviour in parent-child dyads: A literature review. Child: Care, Health & Development, 30(5), 481-493. Leaper, C., Anderson, K.J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents’ talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34, 327. Leung, K., Lau, S., & Lam, W.L. (1998). Parenting styles and academic achievement: A cross-cultural study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 157-172. Lewis, C., & Lamb, M.E. (2003). Fathers’ influences on children’s development: The evidence from two-parent families. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 18(2), 211-228. MacDonald, K., & Parke, R.D. (1984). Bridging the gap: Parent-child play interaction and peer interactive competence. Child Development, 55, 1265-1277. Mannle, S., & Tomasello, M. (1987). Fathers, siblings, and the bridge hypothesis. In K.E. Nelson & A.V. (Eds.), Children’s language, Vol. 6, (pp. 23-42). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Marin, K.A., Bohanek, J.G., Fivush, R. (2008). Positive effects of talking about the negative: Family narratives of negative experiences and preadolescents’ perceived competence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18(3), 573-593. Marsiglio, W., Amato, P., Day, R.D., & Lamb, M.E. (2000). Scholarship on fatherhood in the 1990s and beyond. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 1173-1191. Maurer, T.W., & Pleck, J.H. (2006). Fathers’ caregiving and breadwinning: A gender congruence analysis. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 7(2), 101-112. McBride, B.A., Dyer, J., Liu, Y., Brown, G.L., Hong, S. (2009). The differential impact of early father and mother involvement on later student achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2), 498-508. McBride, B.A., Schoppe-Sullivan, S.J., & Ho, M. (2005). The mediating role of fathers’ school involvement on student achievement. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26, 201-216. 80 McDowell, D.J., Parke, R.D., & Wang, S.J. (2003). Differences between mothers’ and fathers’ advice-giving style and content: Relations with social competence and psychological functioning in middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49(1), 55-76. McDowell, D.J., & Parke, R.D. (2005). Parental control and affect as predictors of children’s display rule use and social competence with peers. Social Development, 14, 440-457. McDowell, D.J., & Parke, R.D. (2009). Parental correlates of children’s peer relations: An empirical test of a tripartite model. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 224235. Miller, P.J., Sandel, T.L., Liang, C.H., & Fung, H. (2002). Self-esteem as folk theory: A comparison of European-American and Taiwanese mothers’ beliefs. Special Issue: “Parental ethnotheories: Cultural practice and normative development” in Parenting: Science and Practice, 2, 209-239. Miller, N.B., Cowan, P.A., Cowan, C.P., & Hetherington, E.M. (1993). Externalizing in preschoolers and early adolescents: A cross-study replication of a family model. Developmental Psychology, 29(1), 3-18. Parish, T.S., & McCluskey, J.J. (1993). Parenting styles, young adults’ self-concepts, and evaluations of parents. The School Community Journal, 3(2), 85-88. Paquette, Daniel (2004). Theorizing the father-child relationship: Mechanisms and developmental outcomes. Human Development, 47, 193-219. Parke, R.D. (2000). Father involvement: A developmental psychological perspective. Marriage and Family Review, 29, 43-58. Parke, R.D. (2002). Fathers and families. In M.H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol 3. Status and social conditions of parenting. (2nd ed., pp. 27-73). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Pleck, E.H., & Pleck, J.H. (1997). Fatherhood ideals in the United States: Historical dimensions. In M.E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development. (3rd ed., pp. 33-48). New York: Wiley. Pleck, J.H. (2007). Why could father involvement benefit children? Theoretical perspectives. Applied Developmental Science, 11(4), 196-202. Pleck, J.H., & Hofferth, S.L. (2008). Mother involvement as an influence on father involvement with early adolescents. Fathering, 6(3), 267-286. 81 Pope-Davis, D.B., Liu, W.M., Nevitt, J., & Toporek, R.L., (2000). The development and initial validation of the multicultural environmental inventory: A preliminary investigation. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 6, 57-64. Portes, A. (1997). Immigration theory for a new century: Some problems and opportunities. International Migration Review, 31, 799-825. Radin, N. (1982). Primary caregiving and role-sharing fathers of pre-schoolers. In M.E. Lamb (Ed.), Nontraditional families: Parenting and child development (pp. 173204). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Rah, Y., & Parke, R.D. (2008). Pathways between parent-child interactions and peer acceptance: The role of children’s social information processing. Social Development, 17, 341-357. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Russell, G., & Russell, A. (1987). Mother-child and father-child relationships in middle childhood. Child Development, 58, 1573-1585. Saleh, M.S., Buzi, R.S., Weinman, M.L., Smith, P.B. (2005). The nature of connections: Young fathers and their children. Adolescence 40(159), 513-523. Schofield, T.J., Parke, R.D., Kim, Y., & Coltrane, S. (2008). Bridging the acculturation gap: Parent-child relationship quality as a moderator in Mexican American families. Developmental Psychology, 44(4), 1190-1194. Shek, D.T.L. (2000). Differences between fathers and mothers in the treatment of, and relationship with, their teenage children: Perceptions of Chinese adolescents. Adolescence, 35(137), 135-146. Shek, D.T.L. (2008). Parental behavioral control and parent-child relational quality predictors of perceived parental knowledge in Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 36, 332-343. Shwalb, D.W., Nakazawa, J., Yamamoto, T., & Hyun, J. (2004). Fathering in Japanese, Chinese, and Korean Cultures. In M.E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (pp. 146-181). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Soto, J.A., Levenson, R.W., & Ebling, R. (2005). Cultures of moderation and expression: Emotional experience, behavior, and physiology in Chinese Americans and Mexican Americans. Emotion, 5(2), 154-165. 82 Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S.D., Dornbusch, S.M., & Darling, N. (1992). Impact of parenting practices on adolescent achievement: Authoritative parenting, school involvement, and encouragement to success. Child Development, 63, 1266-1281. Steinberg, L., & Silk, J. (2002). Parenting adolescents. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Volume 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 103-133). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Sue, S., Nakamura, C.Y., Chung, R.C.-Y., &Yee-Bradbury, C. (1994). Mental health research on Asian Americans. Journal of Community Psychology, 22, 61– 67. Suizzo, M. (2007). Parents’ goals and values for children: Dimensions of independence and interdependence across four U.S. ethnic groups. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(4), 506-530 Super, C.M., & Harkness, S. (2002). Culture structures the environment for development. Human Development, 45, 270-274. Suppal, P., & Roopnarine, J.L. (1999). Paternal involvement in child care as a function of maternal employment in nuclear and extended families in India. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 40, 731-744. Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Shannon, J.D.,Cabrera, N.J., & Lab, M.E. (2004). Fathers and mothers at play with their 2-and 3-year olds: Contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Development, 75, 1806-1821. Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Way, N., Hughes, D., Yoshikawa, H., Kahana Kalman, R., & Niwa, E.Y. (2008). Parents’ goals for children: The dynamic coexistence of individualism and collectivism in cultures and individuals. Social Development, 17, 183-207. Torres, J., Solberg, V., & Carlstrom, A. (2002). The myth of sameness among Latino men and their machismo. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 72(2), 163-181. Triandis, H.C. (1993). Collectivism and individualism as cultural syndromes. CrossCultural Research, 27, 155-180. Triandis, H.C. (1996). The psychological measurement of cultural syndromes. American Psychologist, 51(4), 407-415. U.N. Department of Economic and Social Affairs Population Division (2008). World population prospects: The 2008 revision population database. Retrieved October 18, 2009 from http://esa.un.org/unpp/. 83 Verkuyten, M. (1994). Self-esteem among ethnic minority youth in Western countries. Social Indicators Research, 32(1), 21-47. Wang, Q. & Fivush, R. (2005). Mother-child conversations of emotionally salient events: Exploring the functions of emotional reminiscing in European-American and Chinese families. Social Development, 14(3), 473-495. Wang, Q., & Leichtman, M.D. (2000). Same beginnings, different stories: A comparison of American and Chinese children’s narratives. Child Development, 71(5), 13291346. Wang, S., & Tamis-Lemonda, C.S. (2003). Do child-rearing values in Taiwan and the United States reflect cultural values of collectivism and individualism? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 629-642. Weaver S.R., & Kim, S.Y. (2008). A person-centered approach to studying the linkages among parent-child differences in cultural orientation, supportive parenting, and adolescent depressive symptoms in Chinese American families. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 36-49. Yeung, W.J., Sandberg, J.F., Davis-Kean, P.E., & Hofferth, S.L. (2001). Children’s time with fathers in intact families. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 136-154. Yu, A.B., & Yang, K.S. (1994). The nature of achievement motivation in collectivistic societies. In U. Kim, H.C.T., C.K., S.C.C., & G.Y. (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism: Theory, method, and applications (pp. 239-250). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.