Optimizing the Contact Efficiency in Trade Shows through Chronic and Situational Regulatory Fit Abstract Gopalakrishna & Lilien (1995) developed a three-stage model of trade show performance, relying on different indices of performance at each stage: attraction, contact, and conversion efficiency. However, extant trade show literature neglects to address the relationship between exhibitors and visitors in a detailed way. Specifically, previous studies attribute superior trade show performance to plentiful resources and appropriate evaluations, but fail to address the influence of contact efficiency, which plays the consequential role for deciding the success or failure of a trade show performance. This study adopts the regulatory focus perspective and extends the literature by proposing a regulatory fit effect on trade show contact efficiency in terms of visitors’ chronic and situational regulatory focus. Overall, the empirical evidence supports chronic regulatory focus as having a greater influence on contact efficiency than situational regulatory focus. One major managerial implication for exhibitors is a possible reallocation of its trade show marketing resources to exploit greater contact efficiencies by anticipating visitors’ chronic regulatory focus. Key words: Trade show; Performance; Contact efficiency; Chronic regulatory focus; Situational regulatory focus. 1. Introduction Trade shows are important promotional tools for marketing many products and services. The Global Association of the Exhibition Industry (UFI) (2007) revealed that the exhibition industry is still expanding. Regional, national, and international trade shows constitute an international multibillion-dollar industry. The strong growth associated with the use of trade shows as a substantial part of business marketers’ communication mix has placed increased decision making pressure on management to determine the optimal number of shows to attend, which shows to attend, and how to accurately assess the success of participating in a show (Blythe, 2002; Godar & O'Connor, 2001; Smith, Hama, & Smith, 2003). Regardless of the importance of trade shows, the research on factors or relationships which influence trade show performance is still limited (Dekimpe, François, Gopalakrishna, Lilien, & Bulte, 1997; Hansen, 2002; Kerin & Cron, 1987; Li, 2007; Smith et al., 2003). Discussion about psychological factors involved in buyer/seller contact is especially lacking. As these factors exert a great influence, thorough research on buyers’ psychological states in trade show environments could be important for making trade shows more successful. Gopalakrishna & Lilien (1995) developed a three-stage model of trade show performance, relying on different indices of performance at each stage; attraction, contact, and conversion efficiency. The first stage of the model represents the “attraction efficiency” of the booth. The attraction efficiency measures how effectively the exhibitors attract target visitors. The second stage involves the contact established by the booth staff with the interested visitor. This stage involves the performance of the booth personnel, referred to as “contact efficiency.” In the third stage, establishment of sales leads were addressed. A lead is a potential sale as indicated by the visitor’s interest in a follow-up sales visit from the seller. Leads can originate from existing as well as from new customers. Effectiveness here is termed “conversion efficiency,” which reflects the ability of the salespeople to turn a contact into a lead. Among these, contact efficiency is not only the retention of attendees who are attracted by exhibitors, but also the further business opportunity through comfortable impersonal contact. The average contact at a busy show may last only 3 to 5 minutes and a salesperson might interact with 20 or more prospective customers in an hour (Resnick, 1991). Booth staff must effectively separate “lookers” from “buyers.” Most importantly, the goal is effective interaction with attendees in such a short period of time. This ultimately determines whether a buyer will seek ongoing contact with an exhibitor, thereby developing into a sales lead. Despite the importance of this process, however, previous literature had not put adequate focus on contact efficiency to discover how these short contact windows can be optimized to improve trade show performance. In this paper we therefore carefully examine contact efficiency, employing the theory of regulatory focus. We manipulated a trade show contact stage by creating two presentations for representatives of different firms; potential “buyers” were exposed to these presentations. By examining the choice of firm with which test participants express an interest in ongoing contact, we can explore how the chronic and situational regulatory fit effects on buyers affect contact efficiency. 2. Regulatory focus-chronic and situational 2 Higgins (1997, 1998) developed the regulatory focus theory, which describes important differences in the processes through which people approach pleasure and avoid pain. Chronic personality characteristics such as the extent as well as the accessibility of discrepancies between (1) the actual and ideal selves (as measures of a chronic tendency to self-regulation with a promotion focus), and (2) the actual and ought selves (as measures of a chronic tendency to self-regulation with a prevention focus) determine the latent activation levels of promotion and prevention mechanisms, respectively (Higgins, Klein, & Strauman, 1985; Higgins, Shah, & Friedman, 1997). Self-regulation mechanisms can be experimentally induced by (1) rendering positive (gain) or negative (loss) outcomes salient (Roney, Higgins, & Shah, 1995; Shah, Higgins, & Friedman, 1998), (2) activating knowledge structures that are related to nurturance needs or security needs (e.g., Friedman & Förster, 2001), (3) priming the standards (ideals vs. oughts) with which people try to bring themselves into alignment (Freitas, Liberman, & Higgins, 2002; Higgins, Bond, Klein, & Strauman, 1986), and/or (4) activating approach or avoidance motor actions (Forster, Higgins, & Idson, 1998; Friedman & Förster, 2001). Chronic regulatory focus Regulatory focus theory (see Higgins, 1997, 1998) proposes the existence of distinct regulatory systems that are concerned with acquiring either nurturance or security through goal attainment. Individuals’ self-regulation in relation to their hopes and aspirations (ideals) satisfies nurturance needs. The goal is accomplishment and the regulatory focus is promotional. Success and failure in a promotion focus are experienced as the presence of positive outcomes (gains) and the absence of positive outcomes (nongains), respectively. Individuals’ self-regulation in relation to their duties and obligations (oughts) satisfies security needs. The goal is safety and the regulatory focus is preventive. Success and failure in a prevention focus are experienced as the absence of negative outcomes (nonlosses) and the presence of negative outcomes (losses), respectively. 3. Chronic regulatory focus and contact efficiency Applied to the matter at hand, consumers’ attitudes toward a product are more favorable when its benefits fit their regulatory goals (Aaker & Lee, 2001; Cesario, Grant, & Higgins, 2004). Higgins (2000) has described how people actually assign greater monetary value to a chosen object when environmental cues induce a choice process which is more congruent with their dispositional tendencies toward either a promotion or prevention focus—independent of the value of the object itself. Also, decision makers with different regulatory orientations value their decisions more when they use decision strategies that suit their regulatory orientation (Avnet & Higgins, 2003; Camacho et al., 2003; Higgins et al., 2003). Since prevention-focused buyers’ behaviors are characterized by avoidance of losses and negative outcomes, they will typically be drawn by exhibits having a prevention-focus for a new product purchase. In contrast, promotion-focused buyers tend to focus on gains and thus may be relatively more interested in exhibits featuring a promotion-focus. H1. Promotion-focused buyers are more likely than prevention-focused buyers to effectively contact exhibitors whose presentations are promotion-focused. Conversely, prevention-focused buyers are more likely than promotion-focused buyers to effectively contact exhibitors whose presentations are prevention-focused. 3 4. Situational regulatory focus and contact efficiency Regulatory focus is determined both by situational and chronic factors (Higgins, 1997, 1998). Kark & Dijk (2007) proposed that situational regulatory focus can be defined by work context characteristics including industrial environment (dynamic or stable), and organizational structure (organic or mechanical). As inferred above concerning a person’s choice process, the buyer employs the self-regulatory behavior and adjusts the process of choosing a new supplier in accordance with situational factors in a trade show setting; that is, buyers would modify their choices in accord with situational regulatory focus, elicited by industrial and organizational contexts. As a result, situational regulatory focus effect should influence contact efficiency and we discuss the following situational regulatory foci, including external and internal factors, which affect contact efficiency in a trade show setting: 5.1. External index: Environmental uncertainty External factors affect whether people adopt more of a promotion versus prevention focus (Brockner & Higgins, 2001). One external index by which buyers make the contact choice is environmental uncertainty. In situations in which a firm’s best course of action is unclear, imitating the behavior of other firms within their network provides a low-cost solution to that uncertainty (Henisz & Delios, 2001). Cost means loss for buyers and uncertain environments tend to make buyers do the non-loss (prevention) strategy, as when they purchase directly from customers’ orders in order to curtail risk. On the contrary, in the stable environment, buyers use gain (promotion) strategies to obtain additional profit, as when they purchase in anticipation of customer demand. Thus, buyers in highly uncertain environments were motivated by prevention-focused presentations, and buyers in relatively certain environments were motivated by promotion-focused presentations. H2a. Buyers are more likely to maintain contact with exhibitors whose presentations are promotion-focused when environmental uncertainty is low. Conversely, buyers are more likely to maintain contact with exhibitors whose presentations are prevention-focused when environmental uncertainty is high. 5.2. Internal index: formalization Internal factors involving the buyer’s firm can also affect the buyer’s contact choice, and are also taken into account in our research. First we’ll look at formalization in the buyer’s firm. Contextual variables are likely influential (Roney et al., 1995); for example, organizational designs and structures that are characterized by a dynamic, change oriented, and organic structural form correlate to a promotion-oriented context. In comparison, organizational designs and structures that are characterized by a mechanistic and bureaucratic structure that stresses the importance of rules, regulations, stability, and standardization correlate to a prevention-oriented context (Kark & Dijk, 2007); that is, the degree of formalization can affect regulatory oriention. Hence, relying on the regulatory fit effect, it can be expected that buyers from highly formalized organizational contexts are naturally motivated by prevention-focused presentations, and buyers from relatively unformalized organizational contexts are motivated by promotion-focused presentations. H2b. Buyers from low formalized organizations can be expected to achieve higher contact efficiencies with exhibitors whose presentations are promotion-focused; 4 conversely, buyers from highly formalized organizations can be expected to achieve higher contact efficiencies with exhibitors whose presentations are prevention-focused. 5.3. Composite index: Performance improvement The firm’s past performance is attributed to external and internal causes (Walsh & Henderson, 1988). External causes include the strategies of competitors, customer requirements, and competitive environment; internal causes include quality management, supply base management, and customer relations practices (Tan, Kannan, Handfield, & Ghosh, 1999). Thus, performance improvement of the firm is the general reference of managers’ decision making. In instances involving some history of poor performance, a substantial reduction in research may be the best strategy to maintain public credibility; on the other hand, managers are prepared to reinvest heavily where there a history of successful performance (Walsh & Henderson, 1988). For example, the longer its tenure as a high-performing producer, the more a firm invests in quality-enhancement programs (Rob & Fishman, 2005). The reinvestment strategy is a typical feature of the promotion focus and the investment-reduction strategy is a typical feature of the prevention focus. Hence, higher investment in performance improvement is related to situational promotion regulatory focus and lower investment in performance improvement is related to situational prevention regulatory focus. Relying on the regulatory fit effect, buyers who emphasize high performance improvement are motivated to consider exhibitors’ promotion-focused presentations, and buyers who give relatively less emphasis on performance improvement are motivated to consider exhibitors’ prevention-focused presentations. H2c. Buyers emphasizing performance improvement can be expected to achieve higher contact efficiencies with exhibitors whose presentations are promotion-focused; conversely, buyers who place relatively less emphasis on performance improvement can be expected to achieve higher contact efficiencies with exhibitors whose presentations are prevention-focused. 5. Chronic and situational regulatory focus effect on contact efficiency The average contact at a busy show may last only 3 to 5 minutes and a salesperson might interact with 20 or more prospects/customers in an hour (Resnick, 1991). Researchers exploring psychological factors often approach time as a mental construction having only subjective meaning (Bergadaà, 1990). During the short contact periods involved in trade show contexts, buyers feel time pressure to decide whether to proceed from the attraction stage to the contact stage. This time pressure has been shown to restrict individuals’ information processing abilities (Suri & Monroe, 2003). Severe time constraints typically force the abandonment of established decision-making practice and canon, in favor of new approaches hastily adapted to the immediate circumstances (Hansen & Helgeson, 1996). Besides time constraint concerns, another important factor relates to the fact that this is, after all, the contact stage, with the limited goal of obtaining pledges for further contact; not commitment to purchase. 5 Under these circumstances of time constraint and decision-making short of final commitment, individuals are less likely to consider a great number of factors as they go about processing information, but instead focus on general characteristics to search out choices which are generally positive (Chaiken, Liberman, & Eagly, 1989). This restriction of decision making factors under time constraints, coupled with buyer awareness that decisions are not final, high pressure purchasing decisions, combine to suggest that the operative focus during the contact stage is the chronic regulatory focus. We go on to argue that people’s chronic regulatory focus (Higgins, 1997) plays an important role in directing people’s attention to information that fits their regulatory orientation. Hence, in light of these further observations on the nature of the contact stage, we expect buyers to make decisions more on the basis of chronic regulatory focus rather than situational regulatory focus in the contact stage. H3. Chronic regulatory focus has a more significant impact on contact efficiency in the trade show environment than situational regulatory focus. 6. Methodology 7.1. Outline We conducted two experiments to test our hypotheses. The first study attempts to develop stimuli for participants to provide evidence for our proposed chronic regulatory fit effect on contact efficiency. Specifically, Study 1 demonstrates that promotion (prevention)-focused buyers prefer ongoing contact with promotion (prevention)-focused exhibitors. Study 2 builds on this finding and shows the effect of chronic regulatory focus and three situational regulatory foci on contact efficiency. 7.2. Study 1 7.2.1. Stimuli development We manipulated regulatory focus via trade show display scenarios framed either in promotion or in prevention terms. Based on in-depth interviews with several trade show experts, we came up with a new product with the following attributes: (1) Requiring repurchase an average of six times per year, (2) each purchase involving an outlay of $10,000, (3) returning clients can pay cash on delivery (COD), while new buyers must pay cash in advance, and (4) development expense and mould tooling charge costs would be around $12,000; this is either “from scratch” in-house redesign or the expense for getting your old supplier to release the mould tooling specs. We designed two kinds of presentations by referring to Aaker & Lee (2001)’s manipulation. The promotion-focused presentation exhibitor, with the firm name “MHT,” emphasized the benefits that buyers could gain. The prevention-focused presentation exhibitor, with the firm name “GQD,” emphasized the problems that buyers could avoid. In our in-depth interviews with seasoned exhibitors, we discussed general inquiries posed by existing or new customer/attendees. They included eight prominently recurring issues: (1) supplier categories (“Are you the manufacturer or a trading company?”), (2) suppliers’ main markets (“Who do you sell to?”), (3) price 6 range, (4) delivery period (time from request to delivery), (5) exclusive agency (“Can I get privileged distribution rights for a geographical area?”), (6) supplier monthly capacity (“How many of these things do you make?”), (7) mould charge for custom models, and (8) payment terms; typically interest in some kind of a “credit line” arrangement. Among those issues, the most serious ones when new visitors consider potential supplier are price, payment, and exclusive agency. Accordingly, the detail statement of manipulation is as shown in Table 1. 7.2.2. Data collection and sample characteristics This study has defined its population as middlemen that participate in international trade shows to purchase from overseas suppliers and resell to their domestic or foreign markets. A survey of personnel with purchasing experience was conducted to obtain a base of potential participants for the present study. We primarily sampled from the 2007 Mido Optical Fair catalog, listing more than one thousand firms which attend international optical shows, like the Mido show (Milano, Italy), Silmo optical show (Paris, France), Hong Kong optical show (Hong Kong), etc. We made preliminary phone calls to identify participants willing to commit to our study before we dispatched questionnaires. Largely because of competitive data privacy concerns, our original base of 278 contactees got whittled down to 48 who were willing to follow through on data collection, and completed questionnaires for our study. 7.2.3. Procedure Participants would be measured for regulatory focus by “Selves Questionnaire” (Brockner, Paruchuri, Idson, & Higgins, 2002; Idson & Higgins, 2000). They were initially provided with a definition of their ideal and ought selves. Their ideal self was defined as the type of person they ideally would like to be, the type of person they hoped, wished, or aspired to be. Their ought self was defined as the type of person they believed they ought to be, the type of person they believed it was their duty, obligation, or responsibility to be. Self-congruency scores were calculated as the difference between self-guide extent ratings and respective actual ratings for ideal and ought self-guides separately (Idson & Higgins, 2000). Specifically, each actual/ideal extent rating was subtracted from its corresponding ideal extent rating. The resultant differences (one for each attribute) were then summed such that a more positive score means lower ideal congruency. Similarly, each actual/ought extent rating was subtracted from its corresponding ought extent rating and then summed. A more positive score means lower ought congruency. Then, participants were instructed to take a 15-minute break from our task to do something else unrelated to the experiment. Afterwards, participants were asked to imagine themselves as the buyers of our manipulated new product in the trade show. We presented participants with a list of features related to the presentation about price, payment terms, and exclusive agency as shown in Table 1. Participants are required to read this appeal and then complete their evaluation which was to indicate one exhibitor with whom they would like ongoing contact, and then rate the presentation on three seven-point scales, anchored at the ends by “bad/good,” “negative/positive,” and “unfavorable/favorable.” Finally, they were required to complete some demographic data and we gave them U.S. $35 coupon for their help. 7 We averaged participants’ evaluations of the exhibitors on the four items to form a brand attitude index for each of the two exhibitors (αMHT = .96 and αGQD = .93). Promotion-focused participants’ attitude toward exhibitor MHT is significantly higher than for GQD (M = 4.92 versus 3.53, p < .001), and prevention-focused participants’ attitude for exhibitor MHT is significantly lower than that for GQD (M = 4.18 versus 5.00, p < .001). 7.2.4. Measures This study sought to find the relationship between chronic regulatory focus and contact efficiency. We adapted the logistic model from Dhar & Nowlis (1999). The dependent variable was the ongoing contact choice of either MHT or GQD and was modeled as a function of the independent dummy variable, and the buyers’ chronic regulatory focus—promotion vs. prevention focus. Our logistic regression analysis in this and subsequent studies follows that of other research on similar choices (e.g., Dhar & Sherman, 1996; Nowlis & Simonson, 1997). In addition, in order to control the effect of certain objective firm factors on contact efficiency, such as “traditional vs. new-tech”, and “sales volume”, these were entered as control variables into the logistic regression equation in this study. 7.2.5. Results The logistic regression model had a predictive ability of 82.1% for contact efficiency. Consistent with Hypothesis 1, the coefficient for chronic regulatory focus was statistically significant (BCRF= 2.59, p<.01). These results provide evidence that effect of chronic regulatory on contact efficiency is significantly positive; that is, promotion- (prevention-) focused buyers prefer ongoing contact with exhibitors whose presentations are characterized by gain (non-loss). 7.2.6. Discussion Buyers who are motivated to process information within time constraints rely more on regulatory focus when choosing one exhibitor over another for ongoing business relations. However, as indicated in the literature review, contact efficiency is affected by not only chronic but also situational regulatory focus. Hence, we added four kinds of situational regulatory focus into our model to explore the relationship and to understand whether the model combing chronic and situational regulatory focus could have better predictive ability for contact efficiency. 7.3. Study 2 7.3.1. Data collection and sample characteristics Besides the sampling difficulties mentioned regarding Study 1, we also suffered additional sampling difficulties, because either (1) company policy would prohibit free expression of company organizational information, or (2) they felt that the overhead involved in providing required information for our study, over and above the actual business of managing their running shows, was onerous. In total, 146 8 completed questionnaires were returned and the resultant sample we felt adequately covered four major industries, consisting of mechanical/electrical appliances, chemical industry supplies, commodities, and optical import/manufacture. The large majority of the respondent firms (56.2%) had more than 51 employees, while nearly one-forth (26.0%) had 21~50 employees, and the rest (17.8%) had less than 20 employees. Besides this, more than half of the respondent firms’ (56.8%) volume was more than US$3.3 million, while nearly one-forth (28.8%) was less than US$1.6 million, and the remaining (14.4%) was US$1.6~3.3 million. The respondents consisted of a balanced mix of firm leaders with one-fourth of being owners, another fourth being executives, another fourth being department managers, and the rest being purchasing agents. The cross-validation methodology is employed to examine the predictive power of chronic and situational regulatory focus for contact efficiency in terms of sampling variation. One simple use of the cross-validation idea consists of randomly splitting a sample into two subsamples. We first randomly used 117 samples to build up our logistic model, and then used another 29 samples to cross-validate our model. 7.3.2. Procedure The participants for Study 2 were distinct from the Study 1 group, but were still drawn from the same sample frame. They were instructed to identify their chronic regulatory focus, as with the Study 1 group. Then, they were given a 15-minute break to do something unrelated to the experiment. After the break, they were asked about our sample firms’ situations including their impressions of environmental uncertainty, formalization, and performance improvement. Participants were then manipulated as in Study 1. They were then asked to imagine themselves as buyers in a trade show environment and were provided with presentations which related prices, payment terms, and exclusive agency terms for two exhibitors. They were required to read the presentations and complete evaluations indicating one exhibitor with which they would prefer ongoing contact. They were then asked to rate the presentations on three seven-point scales, anchored by “bad/good,” “negative/positive,” and “unfavorable/favorable.” Finally, they filled in some demographic data. As with the Study 1 group, we gave each of them a US$35 gift coupon and a nice gift for their help. Unlike most trade show studies that use field surveys or tap performance databases, this study is based on an experimental design. Therefore, the current study seeks to exclude unnecessary characteristics that could skew observed variances in contact efficiency apart from chronic and situational regulatory focus effects. Specifically, participants were asked to absorb and assume a baseline of organizational intrinsic and extrinsic factors applicable to our experimental shows under consideration, consisting of competition and performance figures from recent years. The participants were also asked to rate the firm’s trade show contact efficiency on two fronts: choice for ongoing contact and general attitude towards the firm. As with Study 1, we averaged participants’ evaluations for each of our two manipulated exhibitors (αMHT = .96 and αGQD = .95). Promotion-focused participants’ attitude toward exhibitor MHT is significantly higher than that for GQD (M = 5.15 versus 3.78, p < .001), and prevention-focused participants’ attitude for exhibitor MHT is significantly lower than that for GQD (M = 3.72 versus 4.85, p < .001). 9 7.3.3. Measures This study began by combining fieldwork on trade shows and insights proffered by the literature on the psychology of industrial marketing to specify the domain of each of the construct dimensions under investigation and to develop items that could serve as indicators of three situational regulatory foci. We maintained our Study 1 approach adapting a dependent variable as a dummy variable and the same logistic model as Study 1. The measures of three situational regulatory foci have acceptable reliabilities and psychometric properties. Table 2 shows correlations between these three variables. The specific operational terms used are shown in the Appendix. Hypotheses 2a~2c suggest that situational regulatory focus has influence on the buyer’s contact choice. Hypothesis 3 indicates that chronic regulatory focus has a more positive effect on contact efficiency than situational regulatory focus in the trade show environment. The dependent variable was the preference choice for further contact between MHT or GQD, and was modeled as a function of the following independent dummy variables: (1) the buyers’ chronic regulatory focus (promotionvs. prevention- focus), and (2) situational regulatory focus [uncertainty (high/low), formalization (high/low), and performance improvement (high/low)]. In addition, as in Study 1, we introduced control variables into the logistic regression equation. We used hierarchical regression to analyze the effect of chronic and situational regulatory focus on contact efficiency and used another sample to do cross-validation for our model. 7.3.3.1. Environmental uncertainty Environmental uncertainty is defined as the degree to which perceived instability of environments relates to a firm's business (Duncan, 1972; Tan & Li, 1996). We adapted the scale of environmental uncertainty from Fam & Yang (2006). In this study, respondents were asked to rate their perceived uncertainty for the following four items: (1) customers' buying habits, (2) nature of competition, (3) taste and preference of customers, and (4) market activities of competitors. Each statement was measured with a 7-point Likert scale with endpoints labeled as 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree. 7.3.3.2. Formalization Formalization is the most important measurement of organizational structure. The items of formalization used in our study were adapted from Ferrell & Skinner (1988). In order to get more objective data, we used “employees” in replace of “I.” Respondents were asked to rate their firm’s formalization for the following six items: (1) if a written rule does not cover some situation, employees make up informal rules for doing things as they go along, (2) there are many employees’ business practices that are not covered by some formal procedure, (3) usually, employees’ contact with this company and its representatives involves doing things “by the rule book”, (4) contact with this company and its representatives are on a formal preplanned basis, (5) employees ignore the rules and reach informal agreements to handle some situations, and (6) when rules and procedures exist in this company, they are usually written 10 agreements. Among these items, item (1), (2), and (5) are reversed. Each statement was measured with a 7-point Likert scale with endpoints labeled as 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree. 7.3.3.3. Performance improvement Performance improvement was measured by asking respondents to indicate the extent to which their performance had improved over the past two years with respect to 6 financial, market oriented, and operational measures of performance. Self-reporting in this domain has by now become a validated, established practice (e.g. Bourgeois & Eisenhardt, 1988; Conant, Mokwa, & Varadarajan, 1990; Powell, 1994). The six-item scale of performance improvement was adapted from Cohen (2001). In this study, respondents were asked to rate their firm’s performance improvements via the following six items: (1) sales growth, (2) profitability relative to competitors, (3) cash flow generating ability, (4) market share, (5) competitive position, and (6) operational efficiency. Each statement was measured with a 7-point Likert scale with endpoints labeled as 1 = strongly low to 7 = strongly high. 7.3.4. Results Logistic regression analysis was used to assess the effects of chronic and situational regulatory focus on the buyers’ choices between two exhibitors (see Table 3). The logistic regression model had a 79.5% predictive ability. Coefficient for chronic regulatory focus was still consistent with Hypothesis 1 (BCRF= 2.35, p<.001). Coefficient for uncertainty was consistent with Hypothesis 2a (BUN= -2.15, p<.05). This confirms that buyers prefer ongoing contact with exhibitors whose presentations are prevention-focused when they are in more uncertain environments. On the other hand, buyers preferred ongoing contact with exhibitors whose presentations were promotion-focused in less uncertain environments. The coefficient for formalization was not consistent with Hypothesis 2b (BFO= -.52, p>.28). The coefficient for performance improvement was consistent with Hypothesis 2c (BPE= 1.42, p<.05). This means that buyers preferred ongoing contact with exhibitors whose presentations are promotion-focused when those firms evince higher performance improvement. On the other hand, buyers preferred ongoing contact with exhibitors whose presentations were prevention-focused when those firms had lower performance improvement. These results are also consistent with Hypothesis 3, and provide evidence that the effect of chronic regulatory focus on contact efficiency is more positive than that of situational focus. We also did cross-validation for our logistic model by collecting another sample. The cross-validation model had a 83.3% predictive ability and coefficients for uncertainty and performance improvement were significant in our building model. These results provide additional evidence that a model incorporating chronic and situational regulatory focus has significant predictive power with respect to contact efficiency. 7.3.5. Discussion 11 The results suggest that the effect of situational regulatory focus on contact efficiency deserves further attention. Highly uncertain conditions produced more favorable evaluations for the exhibitor when his/her positioning addressed non-loss concerns. However, conditions of low uncertainty produced preference for exhibitors who provided presentations emphasizing gain. However, formalization does not appear to have a significant situational regulatory focus effect on contact efficiency. Performance improvement significantly stimulates the situational regulatory focus effect on contact efficiency. Clearly, in this essential contact stage in the trade show, chronic regulatory fit has the greatest impact on contact efficiency. 7. Conclusion The purpose of this article is to further the current state of knowledge about how fit influences decisions and to provide a better understanding of regulatory fit theory as a source of value that applies to contact efficiency in trade show environments. More specifically, this article proposes that if the exhibitor can anticipate the decision maker’s chronic regulatory state, it increases the level of intention toward a decision outcome. Moreover, we showed that the situational regulatory focus effect is not the only factor of significance in the contact stage. Our results have shown that contact efficiency is significantly impacted by environmental uncertainty and performance improvement, with the former more significant than the latter. Formalization, on the other hand, is not significant. Therefore, the external situational regulatory focus effect is more significant than the internal one during the contact stage. The structural dimensions of a given firm or situation have been suggested as impacting the positioning of the buying center (Johnston & Bonoma, 1981). Contact stage response in the trade show environment appears to be owned by individual buyers, not the buying center. Hence, organizational factors, like structure and power, do not affect the buyer’s decision in the contact stage. The average duration of the contact stage in trade shows is as short as that of a typical recruitment interview. This stage can be likened to a buyer’s “interview” with an exhibitor. Correspondingly, a survey of the literature on interviewing is suggestive. Interviewers’ pre-interview evaluations of applicants tend to be self-fulfilling (Dipboye, 1982). In other words, time limitations make interviewers more likely to choose target interviewees by their chronic conditions; less likely by the external situation. These findings are important because of the unstructured manner in which the selection interview is frequently conducted. The data gathered from such interviews are typically biased and often unrelated to future job performance (Huffcutt & Arthur Jr., 1994; McDaniel, Whetzel, Schmidt, & Maurer, 1994; Conway, Jako, & Goodman, 1995). These biases include interviewers’ tending to favor applicants who share their non-work-related attitudes, giving unduly high weight to negative information, and allowing undue factors, such as interview ordering, to influence evaluations. (Dipboye, 1982). It appears the same applies to the contact stage in trade show environments: Limited time in the contact stage makes the chronic regulatory focus effect more significant than the situational one on contact efficiency. The value-from-fit phenomenon has also been shown to occur by means of different types of choice strategies, such as progressive elimination strategy and full evaluation strategy, and by examination of content-based information versus feelings-based information (Avent & Higgins, 2006). Clearly, our results suggest that 12 the value derived from regulatory fit for contact efficiency in a trade show environment mainly comes from a progressive elimination strategy. Buyers make further contact decisions by chronic factors—regulatory focus, external situational factors—uncertainty—and composite situational factors—performance improvement—although they appear to negate organizational factors—like formalization. 8.1. Managerial implications Our results illustrate that contact efficiency is enhanced when the buyer’s chronic regulatory focus fits the exhibitor’s, and that external situational regulatory focus has influence on contact efficiency. From an exhibitor’s perspective, these findings have important implications for managers executing marketing strategies. First, our study suggests that the chronic regulatory fit effect makes the securing of pledges for future contact from buyers more efficient, i.e., the contact efficiency at the trade show is higher when promotion (prevention)-focused exhibitors are exposed to promotion (prevention)-focused buyers. Furthermore, high degrees of perceived uncertainty within a particular industry tend to further enhance the effectiveness of prevention-focused presentations, whereas perceived performance improvement tends to enhance the effectiveness of promotion-focused presentations. The effects of environmental uncertainty and performance improvement therefore open up opportunities for enhancing contact efficiency. Second, chronic regulatory non-fit situations diminish contact efficiency. When the exhibitor does not fit with the buyer in this regard, the best strategy is to change the exhibitor to achieve a better fit because doing so will tend to increase the buyers’ intention to ongoing contact with the exhibitor. However, if the exhibit cannot be changed, our findings suggest that compensation can be achieved through the application of external situational regulatory focus variables (environmental uncertainty and performance improvement due to external factors). More broadly, our work signals other opportunities to enhance contact efficiency. Any methods that stand to decrease buyer uncertainty are salutary. For example, better integrating industrial information at the trade show or adroitly addressing requests for particularly industry-savvy salespersons and managers can decreaser buyer uncertainty. Other methods, like providing bottom-line prices, promoting buyer-friendly payment terms, and messages that promote an image of industrial stability, can decrease uncertainty. On the other hand, there’s a workable angle involving enhancing uncertainty via the purposeful highlighting of industrial information that highlights fluctuating prices and other instabilities, as well as via supplier-oriented payment terms. Managerially, this exercise would exploit a chronic regulatory non-fit effect which can be cultivated in complementary future communications efforts. Third, our study highlights an approach to documenting the effectiveness of a communications activity, as applied to the trade show environment, in terms of contact efficiency. While the extent of impact may differ across firms and in varied specific situations, such an exercise provides a useful approach for managers to demonstrate accountability on the issue of contact efficiency by quantifying the contribution of a specific activity within a communications system as shown in Table 4. 8.2. Research limitations and future research 13 Our study has several limitations, which we believe will provide useful avenues for future research. First, this study considers the same new product exhibited by two similar firms. Although it is tempting to generalize our results to other firms or to other shows, to do so would be beyond the scope of our study. Replications of this exploratory work in different types of shows and with different types of industrial buyers would allow more dependable generalizations to be made. Second, we looked at the typical communications mix in a business-to-business marketing context; however, future studies may deem it necessary to include other elements such as advertising and direct mail. Third, our study focused on a new product to avoid the effects of prior, real-world marketing activities. Because we did not track the profits of other products exhibited at the show, we do not abstract possible positive or negative effects of the larger show performance aside from our test subjects. Furthermore, the effects of personal selling activities were not taken into account. Further research is needed to extend these considerations to situations involving existing products. Fourth, the measurement of sales effort was limited by our access to data. We believe a more direct form of measuring contact efficiency that includes separating the effects of culture and field would be valuable. Fifth, although it is not easy to measure individuals’ chronic regulatory focus, besides the “Selves Questionnaire” adopted by our study, other methods of measurement taken from past studies, like “Event Reaction Questionnaire” (Higgins, Friedman, Harlow, Idson, Ayduk, & Taylor, 2001) and “Promotion/Prevention Scale” (Lockwood, Jordan, & Kunda, 2002) should be considered. Sixth, future research about situational regulatory focus should include more factors, like economic climate, leadership behavior, and development expectation, etc. Our study just explored the effects of three representative factors on contact efficiency. Finally, our methodology is limited to dependent variables that necessarily only test the buyer’s perspective. Future studies which manipulate experimental groups of buyers should allow for the development of more robust models that take bilateral cognition into account. Appendix. Description of construct operational items used in this study Construct Environmental Uncertainty Formalization Performance improvement Item customers' buying habits nature of competition taste and preference of customers market activities of competitors legal, political and economic constraints If a written rule does not cover some situation, employees make up informal rules for doing things as they go along* There are many things in employees’ business that are not covered by some formal procedure for doing it* Usually, employees’ contact with this company and its representatives involves doing things “by the rule book” Contact with this company and its representatives are on a formal preplanned basis Employees ignore the rules and reach informal agreements to handle some situations* When rules and procedures exist in this company, they are usually written agreements Sales growth Profitablity relative to competitors Cash flow generating ability Market share Competitive position Operational efficiency 14 *reversed item References Aaker, J., & Lee, A. (2001). “I” seek pleasures and “we” avoid pains: the role of self-regulatory goals in information processing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 33–49. Avent, T., & Higgins, E. T. (2006). How regulatory fit affects value in consumer choices and opinions. Journal of Marketing Research, 43, 1-10. Avnet, T., & Higgins. E. T. (2003). Locomotion, assessment, and regulatory fit: value transfer from “How” to “What.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 525–530. Bergadaà, M. (1990). The role of time in the action of the consumer. Journal of Consumer Research, 17, 289–302. Blythe, J. (2002). Using trade fairs in key account management. Industrial Marketing Management, 31(7), 627-635. Bourgeois, L. J., & Eisenhardt, K. M. (1988). Strategic decision processes in high velocity environments: four cases in the microcomputer industry. Management Science, 34(7), 816-835. Brockner, J., & Higgins, E. T. (2001). Regulatory focus theory: implications for the study of emotions at work. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86(1), 35-66. Brockner, J., Paruchuri, S., Idson, L. C., & Higgins, E. T. (2002). Regulatory focus and the probability estimates of conjunctive and disjunctive events. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 87, 5–24. Camacho, C. J., Higgins, E. T., & Luger, L. (2003), Moral value transfer from regulatory fit: What feels right is right and what feels wrong is wrong. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(3), 498–510. Cesario, J., Grant, H., & Higgins, E. T. (2004). Regulatory fit and persuasion: transfer from “Feeling Right.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 388–404. Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context in Unintended Thought, ed. James S. Uleman and John A. Bargh, New York: Guilford, 212-252 Cohen, J. F. (2001). Environmental uncertainty and managerial attitude: effects on strategic planning, non-strategic decision-making and organizational performance. South Africa Journal of Business Management, 32(3), 17-31. Conant, J. S., Mokwa, M. P., & Varadarajan, P. R. (1990). Strategic types, distinctive marketing competencies and organizational performance: a multiple measures-based study. Strategic Management Journal, 11(5), 365-383. Conway, J. M., Jako, R. A., & Goodman, D. F. (1995). A meta-analysis of interrater and internal consistency reliability of selection interviews. Journal of Applied Psychology, (October), 565-579. 15 Craig, C. S., & Douglas, S. P. (2000). Configurational advantage in global markets. Journal of International Marketing, 8(1), 6-26. DeCanio, S. J., Dibble, C., & Amir-Atefi, K. (2000). The importance of organizational structure for the adoption of innovations. Management Science, 46(10), 1285-1299. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum. Dekimpe, M. G., François P., Gopalakrishna S., Lilien G. L., & Bulte C. V. (1997). Generalizing about trade show effectiveness: a cross-national comparison. Journal of Marketing, 61, 55-64. Dhar, R., & Nowlis, S. M. (1999). The effect of time pressure on consumer choice deferral. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(4), 369-384. Dhar, R., & Sherman, S. J. (1996). The effect of common and unique features in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 23, 193-203. Dickson, V., & Lere, J. C. (2003). Problems evaluating sales representative performance? Try activity-based costing. Industrial Marketing Management, 32(4), 301-307. Dipboye, R. L. (1982). Self-fulfilling prophecies in the selection-recruitment interview. Academy of Management Review, 7(4), 579-586. Duncan, R. (1972). Characteristics of organizational environments and perceived uncertainty. Administrative Science Quarterly, 2, 409 – 43. Fam, K., & Yang, Z. (2006). Primary influences of environmental uncertainty on promotions budget allocation and performance: a cross-country study of retail advertisers. Journal of Business Research, 59(2), 259-267. Ferrell, O. C., & Skinner, S. J. (1988). Ethical behavior and bureaucratic structure in marketing research organizations. Journal of Marketing Research, 25, 103-109. Fink, R. C., Edelman, L. F., & Hatten, K. J. (2006). Relational exchange strategies, performance, uncertainty, and knowledge. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 14(2), 139-153. Forster, J., Higgins, E. T., & Idson, L. C. (1998). Approach and avoidance strength during goal attainment: Regulatory focus and the “goal looms larger” effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1115–1131. Freitas, A. L., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2002). Regulatory fit and resisting temptation during goal pursuit. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 291–98. Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion and prevention cues on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1001–1013. Galaskiewicz, J., & Wasserman, S. (1989). Mimetic processes within an interorganizational field: an empirical test. Administrative Science Quarterly, 34(3), 454–479. Garbarino, E. C., & Edell, J. A. (1997). Cognitive effort, affect, and choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 147-158. 16 Godar, S. H., & O’Connor, P. J. (2002). Same time next year—buyer trade show motives. Industrial Marketing Management, 30(1), 77-86. Gopalakrishna, S., & Lilien, G. L. (1995). A three-stage model of industrial trade show performance. Marketing Science, 14(1), 22-42. Hansen, D. E., & Helgeson, J. G. (1996). Choice under strict uncertainty: processes and preferences. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66(2), 153-164. Hansen, K. (2002). Measuring performance at trade shows: scale development and validation. Journal of Business Research, 57, 1-13. Henisz, W. J., & Delios, A. (2001). Uncertainty, imitation, and plant location: Japanese multinational corporations, 1990-1996. Administrative Science Quarterly, 46(3), 443–475. Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: value from fit. American Psychologist, 55, 1217–1230. Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300. Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 1–46. Higgins, E. T., Bond, R. N., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1986). Self-discrepancies and emotional vulnerability: How magnitude, accessibility, and type of discrepancy influence affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 5–15. Higgins, E. T., Friedman, R. S., Harlow, R. E., Idson, L. C., Ayduk, O. N., & Taylor, A. (2001). Achievement orientations from subjective histories of success: promotion pride versus prevention pride. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 3-23. Higgins, E. T., Idson, L. C., Freitas, A. L., Spiegel, S., & Molden, D. C. (2003). Transfer of value from fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1140–1153. Higgins, E. T., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1985). Self-concept discrepancy theory: a psychological model for distinguishing among different aspects of depression and anxiety. Special issue: Depression. Social cognition, 3, 51–76. Higgins, E. T., Shah, J., & Friedman, R. (1997). Emotional responses to goal attainment: Strength of regulatory focus as moderator. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 515–525. Huffcutt, A. I., & Arthur Jr., W. (1994). Hunter and Hunter (1984) revisited: interview validity for entry-level jobs. Journal of Applied Psychology, (April), 184-190. Hutchinson, K., Alexander, N., Quinn, B., & Doherty, A. M. (2007). Internationalization motives and facilitating factors: qualitative evidence from smaller specialist retailers. Journal of International Marketing, 15(3), 96-122. Idson, L. C., & Higgins, E. T. (2000). How current feedback and chronic effectiveness influence motivation: everything to gain versus everything to lose. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 583-592. 17 Idson, L. C., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2000). Distinguishing gains from nonlosses and losses from nongains: a regulatory focus perspective on hedonic intensity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 252-274. Johnston, W. J., & Bonoma, T. V. (1981). The buying center: structure and interaction. Journal of Marketing, 45, 143– 156. Kark, R. & Dijk, D. V. (2007). Motivation to lead, motivation to follow: the role of the self-regulatory focus in leadership processes. Academy of Management Review, 32(2), 500-528. Kerin, R. A., & Cron, W. L. (1987). Assessing trade show fuctions and performance: an exploratory study. Journal of Marketing, 51, 87-94. Li, L. Y. (2007). Marketing resources and performance of exhibitor firms in trade shows: a contingent resource perspective. Industrial Marketing Management, 36(3), 360-370. Li, L. Y. (2006). Relationship learning at trade shows: its antecedents and consequences. Industrial Marketing Management, 35(2), 166-177. Locke, E. A. & Latham, G. P. (1990). Work motivation and satisfaction: light at the end of the tunnel. Psychological Science, 1(4), 240-246. Lockwood, P., Jordan, C. H., & Kunda, Z. (2002). Motivation by positive or negative role models: regulatory focus determines who will best inspire us. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(4), 854-864. Markman, A. B., Baldwin, G. C., & Maddox, W. T. (2005). The interaction of payoff structure and regulatory focus in classification. Psychological Science, 16(11), 852-855. McDaniel, M. A., Whetzel, D. L., Schmidt, F. L., & Maurer, S. D. (1994). The validity of employment interviews: a comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, (August), 599-616. Nowlis, S. M., & Simonson, I. (1997). Attribute-task compatibility as a determinant of consumer preference reversals. Journal of Marketing Research, 34, 205-218. Orth, U. R. (2005). Consumer personality and other factors in situational brand choice variation. Brand Management, 13(2), 115-133. Pawar, B. S., & Eastman, K. K. (1997). The nature and implications of contextual influences on transformational leadership: a conceptual examination. Academy of Management Review, 22(1), 80-109. Porter, M. E. (1986). Changing patterns of international competition. California Management Review, 27(2), 9-40. Powell, T. C. (1994). Untangling the relationship between strategic planning and performance: the role of contingency factors. Canadian Journal of Administrative Science, 11(2), 124-138. Prahalad, C. K., & Doz, Y. L. (1987). The multinational mission: balancing local demands and global vision. New York: The Free Press. Resnick, K. (1991). 8 steps to scale success. Business Marketing, (Fall), A20-21. Rob, R., & Fishman, A. (2005). Is bigger better? Customer base expansion through word-of-mouth reputation. Journal of Political Economy, 113(5), 1146-1162. 18 Roney, C. J. R., Higgins, E. T., & Shah, J. (1995). Goals and framing: How outcome focus influences motivation and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1151–1160. Shah, J., Higgins, E. T., & Friedman, R. (1998). Performance incentives and means: How regulatory focus influences goal attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 285–293. Shamir, B., & Howell, J. M. (1999). Organizational and contextual influences on the emergence and effectiveness of charismatic leadership. Leadership Quarterly, 10(2), 257-283. Smith, T. M., Hama, K., & Smith, P. M. (2003). The effect of successful trade show attendance on future show interest: exploring Japanese attendee perspectives of domestic and offshore international events. Journal of Business & Industrial Marketing, 18(4/5), 403-418. Suri, R., & Monroe, K. B. (2003). The effects of time constraints on consumers judgments of prices and products. Journal of Consumer Research, 30, 92-104. Tan, J, & Li, M. (1996). Effects of ownership types on environment – strategy configuration in China’s emerging transitional economy. Advance International Company Management, 11, 217– 50. Tan, K. C., Kannan, V. R., Handfield, R. B., & Ghosh, S. (1999). Supply chain management: an empirical study of its impact on performance. International Journal of Operations & Production Management, 19(9/10), 1034-1052. The Global Association of the Exhibition Industry (UFI) (2007). http://www.ufinet.org/. Van-Dijk, D., & Kluger, A. N. (2004). Feedback sign effect on motivation: Is it moderated by regulatory focus? Applied Psychology: An International Review, 53(1), 113-135. Wallace, J. C., Chen, G., & Kanfer, R. (2005). Development and validation of a work-specific measure of regulatory focus. Paper presented at the 20th Annual Conference of the Society for Industrial/Organizational Psychology, Los Angeles, CA. Walsh, J. P., & Henderson, C. M. (1988). An attributional analysis of decisions about making commitments. Journal of Social Psychology, 129(4), 533-549. Wang, J., & Lee, A. Y. (2006). The role of regulatory focus in preference construction. Journal of Marketing Research, 43, 28-38. Wernerfelt, B. (1984). A resource-based view of the firm. Strategic Management Journal, 5(2), 171-180. Zahay, D., & Griffin, A. (2004). Customer learning processes, strategy selection, and performance in business-to-business service firms. Decision Sciences, 35(2), 169–203. Table 1 Manipulation of presentation with representatives of two firms 19 Presentation with MHT’s representative Presentation with GQD’s representative price You: Your price is a little higher than GQD’s. MHT: As we know, our price could be the same as our competitor; moreover, you will get 5% discount if you can buy in total amount $200,000 a year. Accordingly, our price should be lower than others. payment You: How about your payment term? I know the general term for new supplier is requested by cash in advance. MHT: If you agree to pay 30% deposit about $3,000 after receipt of our sales confirmation, then you can pay the balance one month after receipt of goods. exclusive agency You: May I have this model exclusively in our country? MHT: That’s great. If you would like to share our research expense and model tooling charge about $5,000, then we will keep it for you. Table 2 Inter-correlation for key study constructs Variable Environmental uncertainty X1 Formalization X2 Performance improvement X3 You: After I check the quotation with MHT, your price is a little higher. GQD: It looks like true, but we actually don’t charge 3% handling charge even you ordered amount is below the minimum quantity. You can compare it with other competitors. Don’t worry about our prices. You: How about your payment term? Our suppliers give us cash on delivery. We do this business over 25 years. GQD: As you know, the general payment term for new buyer is cash in advance, but you can make it by cash on delivery if you would like to pay 1% interest rate each mouth. You: Do you offer this model to other importer in our country? Could you keep it exclusively for our firm? GQD: Yes, we can discuss your request. Our condition is that we need to collect the research expense and model charge about $6,000 first; then, we will return that amount whenever your ordered quantity reaches $60,000. X1 X2 -0.03 -0.16 -0.14 Table 3 20 X3 Results of the hierarchical logistic regression analysis Study 1 Study 2 Dependent variable: contact Controls Full model Controls Full model choice: MHT=1, GQD=2 β β β β Control variables S.E. S.E. S.E. S.E. Industry 0.23* 0.12 0.33* 0.17 -0.05 0.06 -0.13* 0.08 Turnover -0.47* 0.27 -0.55* 0.32 -0.31** 0.13 -0.41** 0.16 β β β β Independent variables S.E. S.E. S.E. S.E. Chronic regulatory focus: 2.59*** 0.96 2.35*** 0.58 promotion=1, prevention=2 Environmental uncertainty: -2.15** 0.93 high=1, low=2 Formalization: high=1, low=2 -0.52 0.49 Performance improvement: 1.42** 0.60 high=1, low=2 -2 log-likelihood 44.44 34.68 147.29 111.64 Cox and Snell R2 0.19 0.37 0.05 0.31 Nagelkerke R2 0.26 0.50 0.07 0.42 Model χ2 8.36 18.12 6.22 41.87 Percent correctly classified 66.7 82.1 64.3 79.5 ***p<0.01; **p<0.05; *p<0.1 β, unstandardized regression coefficients; S.E., standard error of the coefficients Table 4 Four communication strategies of exhibitors with buyers 21 Promotion-focused exhibitor Prevention-focused buyer Promotion-focused exhibitor Promotion-focused buyer 1. 2. Change the exhibitor Decreasing-uncertainty strategy (1) providing bottom-line price (2) providing buyer-oriented payment term (3) providing industrial stable messages 3. Improving-performance strategy (1) new product can improve productivity (2) new product can be produced quickly Prevention-focused exhibitor Promotion-focused buyer 1. 1. 2. 1. 3. 2. Decreasing-uncertainty strategy (1) providing bottom-line price (2) providing buyer-oriented payment term (3) providing industrial stable messages Improving-performance strategy (1) new product can improve productivity (2) new product can be produced quickly Prevention -focused exhibitor Prevention -focused buyer Change the exhibitor Increasing-uncertainty strategy (1) providing fluctuating price (2) providing supplier-oriented payment term (3) providing industrial unstable messages stabilizing-performance strategy (1) new product has stable market (2) new product has standard to produce 22 2. Increasing-uncertainty strategy (1) providing fluctuating price (2) providing supplier-oriented payment term (3) providing industrial unstable messages stabilizing-performance strategy (1) new product has stable market (2) new product has standard to produce