Blackwell Publishing LtdOxford, UKOBRObesity Reviews1467-7881© 2007 Queen’s Printer and Controller of HMSO; published with permission; Journal compilation © 2007 The International Association for the Study of Obesity? 20078123125Review ArticleSocial psychological factors in tackling obesity G. R. Maio et al. obesity reviews Social psychological factors in tackling obesity G. R. Maio, G. G. Haddock and H. L. Jarman School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Keywords: Attitudes, ambivalence, obesity, implicit. Cardiff, UK Accepted 28 November 2006 Address for correspondence: Professor GR Maio, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF10 3AT, UK. E-mail: maio@Cardiff.ac.uk OnlineOpen: This article is available free online at www.blackwell-synergy.com Background: the role of ambivalence in healthy behaviour In the fight to reduce rising levels of obesity, a persistent obstacle is the psychological conflict between things people want to eat – for instance, fatty or sweet foods – and good health. People have very mixed feelings and beliefs about healthy lifestyle options. They experience positive sensations from eating foods that provide excessive calorie and salt content, yet finding the time to exercise (which in itself they may find unpleasant) is difficult. People know that rich foods are bad for them but, on the other hand, they also know that moderate exercise is good for them (1). No one escapes the psychological conflict, or ‘ambivalence’ that ensues. Yet, public information campaigns insist on simply reminding people to avoid certain foods and to exercise, rather than helping to resolve this deep-seated ambivalence. Ambivalence is an important issue because prior experimental evidence from the field of social psychology reveals it to be an important obstacle to healthy behaviour. Healthrelated ambivalence makes it psychologically difficult to resist the temptations of unhealthy choices. Several studies illustrate the effort required to overcome unhealthy temptations (2). In one study, young teenagers were given the opportunity to eat a large quantity of crisps, which they consumed with glee. The experimenter then presented them with a jug of Quench, a ‘new’ bright-red drink, along with a written description of its positive and negative attributes. The thirsty participants were told that they could have some of the drink, but it would be better to wait until later because supplies were short. The participants then sat alone with the drink for 10 min. During this obesity reviews (2007) 8 (Suppl. 1), 123–125 time, the experimenter unobtrusively videotaped them and gave them an additional questionnaire to assess the ways in which they had coped with their temptation to drink the beverage. Analyses revealed that the participants tried a range of strategies to avoid drinking the beverage. Some participants moved the tasty-looking juice away from them. Others tried to focus attention on other materials that were available in the laboratory (e.g. magazines) or tried to highlight the negative (not positive) aspects of the drink in the questionnaire that the experimenter had given to them. In addition, participants even created negative attributes of the drink (e.g. that it was ‘too sweet’) that were never even mentioned in the initial, brief description. These behaviours betray a high amount of psychological effort aimed at combating temptation. More important, this effort occurred more strongly when the experimenter emphasized that the participants were free to choose whether or not to consume the beverage. Freedom of choice makes it more difficult to resist temptation. Other research and theory indicate that stress (3,4) and habit formation also impede the ability to resist temptation (5). Yet, choice, stress and habit are all inescapable or difficult to reduce. Methods to reduce health-related ambivalence So how can we help people cope with this ambivalence? Unfortunately, there is good reason to doubt the efficacy of interventions that simply provide new persuasive information. The problem is that people who are high in ambivalence towards an issue carefully scrutinise any information that can help resolve their conflict (6,7). As a result, ambiv- This paper was commissioned by the Foresight programme of the Office of Science and Innovation, Department of Trade and Industry © 2007 Queen’s Printer and Controller of HMSO; published with permission Journal compilation © 2007 The International Association for the Study of Obesity. obesity reviews 8 (Suppl. 1), 123–125 123 124 Social psychological factors in tackling obesity G. R. Maio et al. alent individuals are more likely to pick out flaws in the messages when the messages are overly simplistic (e.g. ‘just say no’) and form more negative attitudes and behaviours towards the healthy options after detecting the flaws. Indeed, a series of studies sponsored by the Economic and Social Research Council have demonstrated this type of backfire when anti-racism messages are presented to people who are highly ambivalent towards ethnic-minority groups (8). A more basic problem with a persuasive information approach is that it may fail to positively affect nonconscious evaluations of health-related behaviours. Indeed, the research on anti-racism messages revealed backfire effects of persuasive messages on implicit measures of attitude, which assess evaluations that people are unable or unwilling to retrieve from memory (9–11). Implicit measures do not ask people to directly report their attitudes but, instead, assess their attitudes without their immediate awareness or control (e.g. using subliminal presentation of stimuli). Importantly, responses on implicit measures are often discordant with self-reported (explicit) attitudes. For example, a person may explicitly report that she dislikes chocolate cake because of its perceived health impact, yet she exhibits a strong desire to eat the cake on an implicit measure (compare this with the work of Roefs et al. (12)). This difference is important because it is the scores on the implicit measures that tend to best predict actual spontaneous behaviour (13,14). Thus, it is vital that interventions manage to influence the associations tapped by the implicit measures and not just the attitudes obtained by self-report measures. It is likely that this non-conscious attitude change requires repeated and creative interventions to change attitudes – even a single powerful message might not be enough to elicit long-term non-conscious change (15). Potential clues to addressing this problem are revealed by important evidence that ambivalence has unique neurological properties. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging brain imaging techniques, Cunningham et al. found that regions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), specifically the ventrolateral PFC, are more active when people make evaluations of objects that elicit ambivalence, over and above the activity associated with emotional valence and intensity, in regions such as the amygdala (16). This pattern indicates that ambivalence has unique neuropsychological substrates, which may help us understand the effects of ambivalence on behaviour. In fact, such evidence may help to solve an interesting behavioural puzzle about ambivalence. Specifically, ambivalence tends to elicit polarized responses to the objects of the ambivalence (i.e. extremely positive or extremely negative) (1), despite eliciting enhanced scrutiny of relevant persuasive messages as described above (7). In other words, ambivalent people tend to respond in extreme ways that obesity reviews are consistent with salient situational factors, despite resisting direct social influence. One potential resolution is that the polarization and resistance effects occur over different timescales. The studies showing response polarization have used very obvious and immediate cues (e.g. mood; good behaviour) that may be salient within a ‘teachable moment’, when a person is still processing their conflicting evaluations. In contrast, the persuasive messages require longer periods of attention and deliberation, and they may fail, partly because they are not comprehended early enough during the person’s thoughts about an object, and before the person has begun to resolve their ambivalence in a particular direction. Neurological scanning can be used to better track the time course of ambivalence (e.g. using event-related potential or magnetoencephalography) and to test whether situational cues have a different impact during the activation of brain regions that are associated with ambivalence than after this activation has subsided. Such evidence may reveal ways in which interventions can be shaped to use, rather than challenge, ambivalence. For example, it may be possible to develop signage or messages that give instant pictorial cues to healthy behaviour immediately after eliciting feelings of ambivalence about a behaviour. These approaches may be more successful than those that use drawn-out print messages for ambivalent topics. Conclusion Ambivalence and difficulties in trying to resolve it are important in the health context. It is important to develop interventions that are capable of affecting the non– conscious associations that plausibly direct many unhealthy behaviours, and the tools are present to test this possibility. There are also powerful tools (e.g. brain imaging techniques) for looking more directly at the neurological structure and consequences of health-related ambivalence. The use of such tools can coincide with attempts to explore the ways in which social psychological approaches to healthy behaviour change can be adapted to suit public health interventions. We believe that this agenda should be an important aim for future research. Conflict of Interest Statement No conflict of interest was declared. References 1. Bell DW, Esses VM. Ambivalence and response amplification: a motivational perspective. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2002; 28: 1143– 1152. 2. Lima MV. Self persuasion strategies to resist temptation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cardiff University, UK, 2005. This paper was commissioned by the Foresight programme of the Office of Science and Innovation, Department of Trade and Industry © 2007 Queen’s Printer and Controller of HMSO; published with permission Journal compilation © 2007 The International Association for the Study of Obesity. obesity reviews 8 (Suppl. 1), 123–125 obesity reviews Social psychological factors in tackling obesity 3. Kruglanski AW, Webster DM Motivated closing of the mind: ‘seizing’ and ‘freezing’. Psychol Rev 1996; 103: 263–283. 4. Muraven M, Baumeister RF. Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychol Bull 2000; 126: 247–259. 5. Wood W, Quinn JM, Kashy DA. Habits in everyday life: thought, emotion, and action. J Pers Soc Psychol 2002; 83: 1281– 1297. 6. Jonas K, Diehl M, Bromer P. Effects of attitudinal ambivalence on information processing and attitude-intention consistency. J Exp Soc Psychol 1997; 33: 190–210. 7. Maio GR, Bell DW, Esses VM. Ambivalence and persuasion: the processing of messages about immigrant groups. J Exp Soc Psychol 1996; 32: 513–536. 8. Maio GR, Haddock GG, Watt SE, Hewstone M. Implicit measures and applied contexts: an illustrative examination of anti-racism advertising. In: Petty RE, Fazio RH, Brinol P (eds). Attitudes: Insights from the New Wave of Implicit Measures (in press). 9. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JKL Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998; 74: 1464–1480. G. R. Maio et al. 125 10. Schuette RA, Fazio RH. Attitude accessibility and motivation as determinants of biased processing: a test of the MODE model. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 1995; 21: 704–710. 11. Wittenbrink B, Judd C, Park B. Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. J Pers Soc Psychol 1997; 72: 262–274. 12. Roefs A, Stapert D, Isabella LAS, Wolters G, Wojciechowski F, Jansen A. Early Associations with food in anorexia nervosa patients and obese people assessed in the affective priming paradigm. Eat Behav 2005; 6: 151–163. 13. Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Gaertner SL. Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol 2002; 82: 62–68. 14. Dovidio JF, Kawakami K, Johnson C, Johnson B, Howard A. On the nature of prejudice: automatic and controlled processes. J Exp Soc Psychol 1997; 33: 510–540. 15. Wilson TD, Lindsey S, Schooler TY. A model of dual attitudes. Psychol Rev 2000; 107: 101–126. 16. Cunningham WA, Johnson MK, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Banaji MR. Neural components of social evaluation. J Pers Soc Psychol 2003; 85: 639–649. This paper was commissioned by the Foresight programme of the Office of Science and Innovation, Department of Trade and Industry © 2007 Queen’s Printer and Controller of HMSO; published with permission Journal compilation © 2007 The International Association for the Study of Obesity. obesity reviews 8 (Suppl. 1), 123–125