The Effect of Video Games on Iranian EFL Learners' Vocabulary

advertisement
 61
The Reading Matrix © 2013
Volume 13, Number 1, April 2013 The Effect of Video Games on Iranian EFL Learners’ Vocabulary Learning
Sedigheh Vahdat
Shahid Chamran University
Amin Rasti Behbahani
Islamic Azad University
ABSTRACT
This study explored the effect of video games as a new tool for Iranian EFL (English as a foreign
language) vocabulary learning. To conduct the study, 40 intermediate EFL learners, both male
and female, were chosen through a TOEFL proficiency test. The participants were divided into
two groups (10 males and 10 females in each): a control group and an experimental group.
While the control group studied vocabulary via traditional classes, the experimental group
experienced vocabulary learning (the same words) via a video game called Runaway: A Road
Adventure. The results of the study revealed that learning vocabulary via video games is
beneficial, and that males are more inclined toward video-game learning than females.
INTRODUCTION
Vocabulary is the main building block of language learning. The words arrange together
on a grammar base and form the whole building of message. One cannot learn language without
vocabulary (Kang, 1995). Second or foreign language learners experience their greatest
inadequacy in vocabulary, and in spite of the many methods used to help learners acquire it, no
strategy has emerged as the best. Therefore, the research continues toward the most effective
path to vocabulary building.
Computers have changed people’s lives, and video games have been part of that
revolution. The video-game phenomenon has recently caught the attention of researchers from
varied disciplines (e.g., psychology and education) who have sought to utilize it as a tool for
advancement and improvement. Most of the studies have resulted in positive and beneficial
findings. Video games are another avenue for “experimentation in a safe virtual environment”
(Kirriemuir, 2003, p. 7).
62
LITERATURE REVIEW
Importance of Vocabulary Learning
One thing that students, teachers, materials writers, and researchers all agree on is that
vocabulary acquisition is an essential part of mastering a second language. However, the best
means of achieving vocabulary learning is still unclear, partly because it depends on a wide
variety of factors (Schmitt, 2008).
Nation and Kyongho (1995) categorized vocabulary into two main groups: generalservice vocabulary (frequently used words) and special-purpose vocabulary (words for academic
purposes). They believe that this classification is important because different types of words
require different instructional processes. Kang (1995) found that one of the best ways to enhance
second language vocabulary learning is to use a context-embedded approach in which new words
are presented in context. In his study, target English words, with their meanings and one or more
example sentences, were presented to learners via computer audio. Learners could hear the target
words as many times as desired. Zhang and Anual (2008) conducted a study to show the role of
the size of vocabulary knowledge in reading comprehension. They found that for students to be
able to read texts containing low-frequency vocabulary, knowledge of 2000 to 3000 highfrequency words is necessary. Schmitt (2008) states that, in the current research on second
language vocabulary learning, a large vocabulary is needed in order to function in English:
nearly 8000-9000 word families for reading, and perhaps as many as 5000-7000 word families
for speaking.
Video-Game Studies
The possibility of the pedagogical use of video games has recently caught experts’
attention in the field of education. Many researchers concur with Piaget (1962) who believed that
play is crucial to learning new skills, and recently, studies and projects, organizations, and books
have emerged exploring new uses for game-based technologies in learning (Squire, 2008).
Schlimme (2002) believes that video games provide a context in which participants can discuss
scenarios and outcomes in order to facilitate their understanding of other concepts and can
improve children’s reading, spelling, and spatial abilities and critical-analyzing techniques.
Schlimme also maintains that some simulation video games present players with unfamiliar
words which are needed in order to succeed in the game, and, therefore, the players’ vocabulary
levels may increase as a result.
Johnson et al. (2004) employed software called the Tactical Language Training System to
enhance communicative competencies of Arabic language learners. The software provided
integrated training in culture and spoken foreign language. Results of their study showed that the
software would enable learners, even those with low levels of confidence, to acquire
communication skills in difficult languages (such as Arabic).
Shaffer, Squire, Halverson, and Gee (2005) discussed the psychological aspects of video
games: the virtual world makes it possible to develop situated understanding, effective social
practices, and shared values, and helps gamers acquire an epistemic view of the content. They
concluded that the epistemic frame making, as well as the integrated knowing and doing of the
virtual world, are powerful tools for learning.
DeHaan (2005) conducted research to examine the role of video games in improving
listening and reading in Japanese. The results of his study showed that video games enhance a
learner’s language acquisition, and when a student plays a game to learn language, s/he balances
63
play and learning. Furthermore, playing video games increases learners’ ability in listening and
reading comprehension. Finally, DeHaan found that a video game’s repetition, contextual clues,
controllability, and simultaneously presented aural and textual language, all contribute positively
to language learning.
Ang and Zaphiris (2006) analyzed studies related to video games and language learning.
Regarding the psychological concepts in language learning, they found that video games
contributed in ways that would be considered behavioral and constructivist in nature. Video
games in language learning use repetition (behaviorism) and processes of organization of thought
and adaptation (constructivism).
Turgut and Irgin (2009), in their study concerning learning English via computer games,
found that video games are effective toward learning sub-skills, especially vocabulary and
pronunciation. DeHaan, Reed, and Kuwada (2010) measured the effect of interactivity with a
music video game on second language vocabulary recall, and explored the degree to which
video-game interactivity would help or hinder the noticing and recall of second language
vocabulary. The results of the study revealed that video-game observers recalled more words
than the gamers.
Brown, Hall, Holtzer, Brown, and Brown (1997) conducted a study to examine the
gender difference in video-game performance. They concluded that males perform better than
females in video games, but that both genders improve significantly in their vocabulary skill with
video-game use. Lucas and Sherry (2004) examined gender differences in video-game use by
focusing on interpersonal needs for inclusion, affection, and control, as well as socially
constructed perceptions of gendered game play. Their results showed significant gender
differences: (1) young men were more likely to be players than women, (2) young women are
less motivated than young men by social-interaction gratification in playing video games, that is,
since male players are more eager in playing video games, they share more time playing with
peers than females do (social-interaction, here, indicates the amount of time a player spends
playing and interacting with another peer in the same game), (3) both young women and young
men are highly motivated to play video games with challenge defined as the “ability to beat or
control the game” (p. 508), (4) young women enjoy non mental-3D rotation games more than
mental-rotation games, that is, games in which the players must have the ability to imagine a 3D
visual of an entity (e.g., a house) in their mind and rotate that 3D visual in their minds to get a
specific information (e.g., remembering which side of the house had a pink window frame), (5)
young men prefer mental-rotation games, (6) young men are more motivated by competition than
young women are, and (7) young women are more motivated by challenge rather than by
competition, that is, unlike challenge, “it [competition] is against player’s personal best or the
game itself” (p. 510). Ferguson, Cruz, and Rueda (2008) tested the effect of video games on
visual memory between males and females, and concluded that playing violent video games
helps males improve their visual memory more than females.
THE STUDY
Although the effectiveness of video games on different areas of learning (as well as
vocabulary learning) has been the focus of several studies, the present study tries to answer the
following two questions: (1) To what extent do video games help learners acquire vocabulary
over those who acquire vocabulary without using video games? (2) Do females acquire
vocabulary via video games better, or do males?
64
Participants
Forty subjects (20 males and 20 females, ages 23 to 27) were selected by administering a
paper-based TOFEL proficiency test to determine the English-language proficiency level of 80
undergraduate, intermediate-level students majoring in English teaching at Azad University.
Participants whose scores ranged between one standard deviation above and below the mean
were considered. They were then placed either in the experimental group or the control group.
Each group contained two sub-groups of 10 males and 10 females each.
Instruments
This study employed three instruments. The first instrument was the simulated TOEFL
test mentioned above. The second instrument, designed by the researchers and containing forty
multiple-choice questions, was an achievement test. The reliability of the achievement test was
computed by two pilot studies: the split-half reliability method (treats the two halves of a
measure as alternate forms) was used in the first pilot study; the Spearman-Brown formula
(which predicts the reliability of a test after changing the test length) was used in the second pilot
study wherein a reliability of 0.803 was achieved. The third instrument was a Likert-scaled
questionnaire designed to determine the experimental groups’ view and experience of learning
via videogames.
Materials
Two types of materials were used in the study: the experimental group used video games
to learn new words, whereas the control group used five text-drill chapters designed by the
researchers. These chapters contained stories adapted from the video game’s plot, followed by
drills. The words that both groups were assigned to learn were based on the video game,
Runaway: A Road Adventure (produced in 2003 by a German studio called Pendulo). In this
game, the major character (a male) is controlled by a mouse click. Participants, as gamers, must
first find some needed objects in every chapter of the game and then combine them together to
create a self-made tool to help the game’s character overcome the problem he has encountered in
that chapter. By solving the problem in each chapter, the game continues, the gamer solves the
problem in that excerpt. Solving all problems in one chapter makes the game progress.
The video game was selected according to three criteria. First, Nunan (1999) maintains
that every task for learning, either the skills or sub-skills of language, should include
presentation, practice (repetition), and production (Nunan’s “three Ps”). The game, Runaway,
provides a list of needed objects and how to combine them to craft new tools. In each chapter,
hints are hidden and activated by a combination of codes. The researchers provided these hints in
print to the participants and added an Oxford dictionary definition and an example for each
object (e.g., a definition for the object “stapler” followed by an example) in English to fulfill
Nunan’s first P: Presentation. Participants had to understand and use the definitions and the
examples to be able to not only conjure the image of the objects in their minds but also find them
in the chapters of the game. For example, they had to understand the definition provided for
catapult and use the example to make the definition crystal clear in their minds to recall the
picture and then find it in the game. This situation helps them to repeat the definitions given and
activate the images of the objects being sought in a particular chapter. The gamer’s behavior
fulfills Nunan’s second P: Practice or repetition. Utilizing the found objects, combining them,
65
and crafting new tools based on the hints provided, forces participants to use the newly learned
vocabulary items, thereby fulfilling Nunan’s third P: Production.
Second, Lucas and Sherry (2004) reported that the most popular genre of video games is
adventure, and according to Gamefaqs.com (a website regarding information about video
games), Runaway is categorized under the adventure genre. Third, Chang, Hsu, and Chao (2008)
maintain that video games designed for game-based learning should allow each player to
examine and move objects in the scenario by clicking and holding their mouses. In addition,
players should have backpacks to collect items in the game, and an inventory window should
display what items the players own, a common feature in many adventure games. Runaway is
one such adventure video game allowing players to examine and move objects around by
clicking and storing them in the inventory. The game not only presents new vocabulary items, it
also provides ample situations for gamers to practice and produce the newly learned vocabulary
items.
Procedure
The participants assigned to the experimental group learned how to play the video game
for vocabulary learning, whereas the participants of the control group met in a traditional class to
practice and drill the same vocabulary. At week five (120 hours), the achievement test was
administered to both groups. In addition, the experimental group received a questionnaire which
sought to capture the group’s views about learning via video games.
Data Analysis
Following the scoring of the tests, the results were put under a series of statistical
analyses (SPSS, version 16) to provide answers to the study’s two research questions. Inferential
statistics, such as t-tests and correlation co-efficiency formulas, were used to test the hypotheses.
To test the first hypothesis (H01: Video games have no effect on new vocabulary acquisition),
the researchers extracted the participants’ scores on three phases of the t-test analyses. At the first
phase, the mean of the males from both groups was compared; the t-critical was 2.101, and the
t-value was 6.707 (p < 0.05) which is higher than t-critical at the significance level of 0.05
(α < 0.05) with df (18). It can be surmised that the males of the experimental group outperformed
the males of the control group.
Table 1. Comparison of Males in Both Groups
Groups N Mean EG Males 10 CG Males 10 17.65 14.35 Std.
Deviation 1.131 1.081 Std. Error
Mean .357 .341 df t-value 18 6.707 t-critical Sig. (2-tailed) 2.101 .000 Note. EG: experimental group, CG: control group
At the second phase, the mean of the females of the experimental group was
compared with that of the females of the control group by a t-test. Statistics showed that the
t-value was significant at α < 0.05 with df (18) because the t-value (i.e., 3.111, p < 0.05) was
higher than the t-critical (2.101). Thus, it can be claimed that, in the achievement test, the
females in the experimental group performed better than the females in the control group.
66
Table 2. Comparison of Females in Both Groups
Groups N Mean EG Females CG Females 10 10 15.85 13.75 Std.
Deviation 1.491 1.532 Std. Error
Mean .471 .484 df t-value 18 3.111 t-critical Sig. (2-tailed) 2.101 .000 At the third phase, the mean of the experimental group was compared with the mean of
the control group (in other words, their overall performances were t-tested). As indicated in the
following table, the t-value is significant at α < 0.05 with df (38), and the t-value (5.869) is also
higher than the t-critical (2.042). As a result, it can be claimed that all participants in the
experimental group, both males and females, outperformed all participants in the control group.
Table 3. Experimental Group Compared to Control Group
Groups N Mean EG Total CG Total 20 20 16.75 14.05 Std.
Deviation 1.585 1.326 Std. Error
Mean .354 .296 df t-value 38 5.869 t-critical Sig. (2-tailed) 2.042 .000 The results of the three phases were significant; consequently, these results reject the first
null hypothesis (video games have no effect on new vocabulary acquisition). Thus, it can be
concluded that those who learned new English vocabulary via video games outperformed those
who learned them in a traditional way.
To test the second hypothesis (H02: There is no significant difference between males and
females in acquiring new vocabulary via video games), the researchers used a correlation
coefficient formula. According to the statistics presented in the following table, gender and
learning via video games are highly correlated (r-value = 0.969) at α < 0.05, and their
relationship is also positive. Their correlation is significant (p <0.05) because the r-critical with
df (18) for the 2-tailed analysis is 0.443, which is lower than the r-value (0.969). Thus, the
second null hypothesis is rejected (there is no significant difference between males and females
in acquiring new vocabulary via video games). The mean of the experimental group males was
higher than the mean of the experimental group females. As presented in Table 4, the correlation
coefficient is positive. As a result, we can conclude that, because the performance of the males
was better than the performance of the females, there is a significant effect of gender on
vocabulary learning via video games. Table 5 reflects the participants’ views on using a video
game for learning.
Table 4. Male/Female Correlation Coefficient
EG
Males EG
Females Pearson Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) N Pearson Correlation Sig. (2-tailed) N EGMales 1 10 .969 .000 10 EGFemales .969 .000 10 1 10 df r-critical 18 0.443 67
Table 5. Experimental Group Questionnaire
Questions 1. I’m so motivated when I try to learn via video games. 2. When I face a word that I don’t know, I’m so motivated to
learn it. 3. Video games make vocabulary learning more boring. 4. Learning vocabulary via video games is stressful. 5. Facing new words incidentally helped me to get them
better. 6. The virtual world of the game helped me to get the meaning
of new words. 7. The meaning of the words became crystal clear by supports
from the virtual world of the game. 8. When gaming, I felt like I was in an English- speaking
country. 9. I learned new words visually. 10. When I recall the words I learned via the video game, I
remember their images too. 11. Having fun didn’t lessen my learning. 12. Having fun motivated me to continue my learning for long
hours. 13. Sound bites, pictures, and graphics helped me to get the
meaning better. 14. The video game provided opportunities for deepening my
word knowledge. 15. The video game provided opportunities for developing
fluency with known vocabulary. 16. Repeated exposure provided by the video game helped me
learn better. 17. The video game helped me to learn word features, like
collocation. 18. Video games provide opportunities for the intentional
learning of vocabulary. 19. Video games made learning engaging. 20. I don’t feel the pressure of learning when I learn via video
games. 21. I don’t get nervous when I don’t know the meaning of a
word in a video game. 22. I think video games make language learning more
interesting. 23. Words in the video game were more related to male
occupations. 24. The main character of the game was a boy so I didn’t care
as much about the video game. 25. If the main character were a girl, I would care more about
the game. 26. The video game was male-oriented. Strongly
No
Strongly
Disagree Disagree Idea Agree Agree 0% 0% 15% 40% 45% 5% 15% 5% 55% 20% 70% 85% 20% 15% 5% 0% 5% 0% 0% 0% 5% 10% 0% 20% 65% 0% 5% 0% 25% 70% 0% 5% 5% 80% 10% 0% 0% 0% 15% 85% 5% 0% 15% 50% 30% 0% 5% 5% 15% 75% 5% 0% 20% 45% 30% 0% 0% 5% 35% 60% 10% 5% 35% 55% 0% 20% 5% 5% 25% 45% 5% 15% 15% 45% 20% 10% 10% 0% 30% 50% 0% 15% 5% 50% 30% 5% 0% 5% 5% 85% 15% 20% 5% 35% 25% 10% 10% 0% 30% 50% 0% 15% 50% 35% 0% 5% 0% 10% 30% 55% 20% 5% 5% 55% 15% 65% 20% 0% 15% 0% 15% 15% 55% 0% 15% 5% 0% 15% 35% 45% 68
The data from the experimental group questionnaire was analyzed and converted into
statistical results (the Likert-scaled answers were converted into percentages). The results show
that the 67.5% of the gamers were satisfied with learning vocabulary via the video games.
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION
In this study, we explored the effect of video games on vocabulary learning for male and
female EFL learners. As the results of this study show, video-game based learning has a strong
effect on acquiring new vocabulary, which confirms what Schlimme (2002) and Turgut and Irgin
(2009) predicted. In this study, statistical results revealed that using a video game as a tool for
vocabulary learning can be much more useful than using a traditional approach. The t-value in
each t-test revealed a greater value than the t-critical; comparing the overall performance of the
learners revealed 5.869 for the t-value, which is significantly higher than 2.042 for the t-critical.
Such a statistical difference indicates a significant effect of the video game on vocabulary
acquisition.
However, one key question remains: How did the video game help gamers to outperform
traditional learners? Shaffer et al. (2005) write that the virtual world created in video games is a
powerful tool because it simulates a social context, and helps gamers to become masters of their
learning using the learning environment provided by the virtual world in the video games.
Furthermore, the virtual world, with pictures, voices, and graphics, also helps make an authentic
context for learning. In other words, what is going to be taught or learned can also happen in a
real-life like situation that provides situated understanding for learners. The results of the
questionnaire (specifically, questions 6 and 7, with an average of 88.3% agreement) support this
idea. The virtual environments of video games offer enormous potential in education; ironically,
learning and assessment can be placed in a real-world context easier in a video game than in a
standard classroom.
Another beneficial aspect of video games is skill learning. Gass and Selinker (as cited in
Turgut and Irgin, 2009) maintain that the natural repetition in games allows a language learner to
be continuously exposed to the target language, creating more opportunities for acquisition to
occur. Furthermore, according to Turgut and Irgin, the sense of winning increases both the
motivation and awareness of the gamers, and the average of 81.3% agreement for questions 1,
11, and 22 (which were designed to elicit ideas about motivation and fun) supports this idea.
Finally, another advantage of using video games is its degree of interactivity. According to
DeHaan et al. (2010), a defining characteristic of video games is “the extent to which users can
participate in modifying the form and control of a mediated environment in real time” (p. 75).
They state that video games incorporate various technological and pedagogical elements to both
entertain and train the player, and when a degree of interactivity between players and these
elements takes place, learning happens.
The results of this study also revealed that there is a highly positive correlation between
gender and learning vocabulary via video games. The obtained value for the correlation
coefficient was 0.969, which indicates a powerful correlation. It must be noted that the main
character of the game was a male who left his doctoral scholarship in physics to save a beautiful
girl’s life. In doing so, he engages in male-oriented actions. Having a male-dominated
atmosphere might have had an effect on the study’s results in view of the second hypothesis.
Conversely, the game’s story—a love story adventure—could equally lower the effect.
Although many studies have explored the consistent gender differences in video-game
preferences and play patterns, the origin of the differences remains unexplained (Funk &
69
Buchman, 1996). Brown et al. (1997) claim that women are more competitive than men, and they
respond to challenging tasks better than men. Quaiser-Pohl, Geiser, and Lehman (2006) wrote
that video-game preferences are a very important issue in video games and gender studies, and
claim that boys are more used to playing computer games than girls. According to their findings,
another reason for the gender difference could be the genre itself. According to Ferguson, Cruz,
and Rueda (2008), the main reason for gender differences in video games is a difference in men
and women in visuospatial ability (the visual perception of the spatial relationships of objects).
They added that the higher the visual-memory recall, the better a video game player’s
performance would be. In their study, it was found that the male players had a better visualmemory recall (or visuospatial ability) than female players because they outperformed female
players in a violent video game.
There are aspects of this study that limit its generalizability, but which also provide focus
for future research. For example, the sample was small. Larger, and perhaps cumulative, groups
of students are needed to make the findings more robust. In addition, the video game covered
only a small vocabulary corpus. Therefore, further studies are needed using a video game
containing a larger vocabulary corpus.
The results of this study have implications for designers of educational video games,
teachers, and students. Language teachers are advised to use video games in their classrooms,
especially for vocabulary building. Students could use a variety of media to autonomously learn
a foreign language, and as video games continue to gain popularity, it seems likely that learners
will import or download foreign-language video games, using video games as an effective media
for self study.
Sedigheh Vahdat holds a doctorate in applied linguistics from the Allameh Tabatabaee
University, Tehran. She is now an Assistant Professor of TEFL at Shahid Chamran University.
She is interested in EFL teaching and testing in the Iranian context, EFL reading, and
computer-assisted instruction.
Email: royavahdat@hotmail.com
Amin Rasti Behbahani holds a Master’s Degree (of upper second class) in TEFL from Islamic
Azad University, Ahwaz Faculty of Science and Research. He is interested in computerassisted instruction, video-game assisted learning, vocabulary acquisition, EFL reading, and
literature.
Email: amin.rasti@live.com
70
REFERENCES
Ang, C. S., & Zaphiris, P. (2006). Developing enjoyable second language learning software
tools: A computer game paradigm. London: City University.
Brown, R. M., Hall, L. R., Holtzer, R., Brown, S. L., & Brown, N. L. (1997). Gender and video
game performance. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 36(11/12), 793-812.
Chang, H. B., Hsu, H. H., & Chao, L. R. (2008). Interactive video game platform for gamebased learning. In F. Li, J. Zhao, R. Lau, Q. Li, & D. McLeod (Eds.), Advances in webbased learning (pp. 332-240). Berlin: Springer Heidelberg.
DeHaan, J. W. (2005). Acquisition of Japanese as foreign language through a baseball video
game. Foreign Language Annals, 38(2), 278-282.
DeHaan, J. W., Reed, W. M., & Kuwada, K. (2010). The effects of interactivity with a music
video game on second language vocabulary recall. Journal of Language Learning &
Teaching, 14(2), 74-94.
Ferguson, C. J., Cruz, A. M., & Rueda, S. M. (2008). Gender, video game playing habits, and
visual memory tasks. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 58, 279-286.
Funk, J. B., & Buchman, D. D. (1996). Playing violent video and computer games and
adolescent self-concept. Journal of Communication, 46(2), 19-32.
Johnson, W. L., Beal, C., Fowles-Winkler, A., Lauper, U., Marsella, S., Narayanan, . . . &
Vilhjálmsson, H. (2004). Tactical language training system: An interim report. In J. C.
Lester (Ed.), Intelligent tutoring system: Lecture notes in computer science (Vol. 3220,
pp. 336-345). Berlin: Springer Heidelberg.
Kang, S. (1995). The effects of context-embedded approach to second language vocabulary
learning. System, 23(1), 43-55. doi:10.1016/0346-251x(94)00051-7
Kirriemuir, J. (2003, March). The relevance of video games and gaming consoles to the higher
and further education learning experience. JISC, 3(1). (Techwatch Report, TSW 2002)
Retrieved February 17, 2012, from
http://academia.edu/2888127/The_relevance_of_video_games_and_gaming_consoles_to
_the_higher_and_further_education_learning_experience
Lucas, K., & Sherry, J. L. (2004). Sex differences in video game play: A communication-based
explanation. Communication Research, 31(5), 499-523.
Nation, P., & Kyongho, H. (1995). Where would general service vocabulary stop and special
purposes vocabulary begin? System, 23(1), 35-41.
Nunan, D. (1999). Second language teaching & learning. Florence, KY: Heinle & Heinle.
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. NY: Norton.
Quaiser-Pohl, C., Geiser, C., & Lehman, W. (2006, February). The relationship between
computer-game preference, gender, and mental-rotation ability. Personality and
Individual Differences: The Official Journal of the International Society for the Study of
Individual Differences (ISSID), 40, 609-619. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.07.015
Schlimme, M. (2002, May). Video games: A source of benefit or addiction? (Unpublished
student paper). Serendip (a web publication of Bryn Mawr College). Retrieved July 25,
2010, from http://serendip.brynmawr.edu/bb/neuro/neuro02/web3/mschlimme.html
Schmitt, N. (2008). Review article instructed second language vocabulary learning. Language
Teaching Research, 12(3), 329-363.
Shaffer, D. W., Squire, K. R., Halverson, R., & Gee, J. P. (2005). Video games and the future of
learning. Phi Delta Kappan, 87(2), 104-111. Retrieved February 17, 2012, from
http://website.education.wisc.edu/kdsquire/tenure-files/23-pdkVideoGamesAndFutureOfLearning.pdf
71
Squire, K. D. (2008). Video game-based learning: An emerging paradigm for instruction.
Performance Improvement Quarterly, 21(2), 7-36. doi:10.1002/piq.20020
Turgut, Y., & Irgin, P. (2009). Young learners’ language learning via computer games.
Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 1(1), 760-764.
Zhang, J. L., & Anual, S. B. (2008). The role of vocabulary in reading comprehension: The case
of secondary school students learning English in Singapore. Journal of Regional
Language Center Journal, 39(1), 51-76. doi:1177/0033688208091140
Download