Perceived Risk and Severity after Product-harm Crises

advertisement
Perceived Risk and Severity after Product-harm Crises: The role of Product
Involvement, Product Type and Crisis Response Strategies
Abstract
A growing body of studies have focused on consumers’ reactions after product-harm crises.
However, the differential effect of product characteristics such as product involvement and
product type has attracted little attention. In this study, we investigate the influence of product
involvement, product type (hedonic vs. utilitarian) and crisis response strategies on risk
attitude and risk perception and perceived severity. The results of an experiment indicate that
after product-harm crises, consumers have higher risk perception for high involvement
products. Moreover, consumers have higher risk perception and higher perceived severity
after product-harm crises involving utilitarian products than those regarding hedonic ones.
However, a boundary condition was found for the effects of product involvement and product
type on the perceived severity, which is higher for high involvement hedonic products
(perfume). Moreover, our research suggests that the worst crisis response strategy is
involuntary recall, since it leads to higher risk perception and enhances consumers’ risk
aversion. Finally, the findings reveal that purchase intention of hedonic products facing a
crisis is explained by risk attitude and risk perception, while that of utilitarian products is
explained by risk perception and the perceived severity. Our findings contribute to a better
understanding of consumers’ reaction after product-harm crises.
Keywords: Risk, Severity, Product Type, Crisis Response Strategies
Track: Consumer Behaviour
1. Introduction
Product-harm crises can be defined as discrete, well publicized occurrences wherein
products are found to be defective or dangerous (Siomkos & Kurzbard, 1994). A crisis, by
definition, generates substantial risk and this is why academic research has emphasised and
has empirically supported the idea that product-harm crises can seriously harm a firm’s
performance. As far as consumers are concerned, we know that they have a tendency to
reduce the risks during the decision process (Mitchell, 1992), while they are perceived as
higher ones after a crisis situation. As it has been suggested, involvement can affect consumer
behaviour (Ray, 1973; Peter & Olsen, 1987) and influencing consumers’ perceived risk is one
way to do that (Pennings et al., 2002). In addition, if we examine the relationship between risk
and involvement, we can understand the specific roles played by each construct in influencing
different consumer behaviours (Dholakia, 2001). Therefore, the aim of this study is to provide
a better understanding of why consumers behave as they do after a product harm crisis by
examining the impact of product involvement, product type (hedonic – utilitarian) and crisis
response strategies on risk perception and risk attitude.
2. Conceptual Framework & Hypotheses
2.1 Product involvement
Most researchers agree that the level of involvement can be understood by the degree of
personal relevance or importance or interest (e.g. Zaichkowsky, 1985; Laurent & Kapferer,
1985; Mittal, 1995). Thus, involvement can be defined as “a person's perceived relevance of
the object based on inherent needs, values, and interests” (Zaichkowsky, 1985). After
reviewing literature, it has been revealed that involvement, in the context of crisis situations,
has attracted surprisingly poor attention, although, according to some authors, involvement
can be a key factor for the outcome of a crisis (McDonald & Hartel, 2000; Coombs &
Holladay,2005). Yet, only few researchers have examined how consumer’s involvement
influences consumers’ perceptions of organizational reputation (Choi & Lin, 2009),
credibility of the organisation (Arpan & Roskos-Ewoldsen, 2005), attribution of blame
(McDonald & Hartel, 2000, Choi & Lin, 2009), consumers’ perceived risk (Pennings et al.,
2002), emotions (McDonald & Hartel, 2000, Choi & Lin, 2009) and purchase intentions
(McDonald & Hartel, 2000; Arpan & Roskos-Ewoldsen, 2005).
2.2 Risk
The two-dimensional model of risk, proposes that risk is the resultant of the perceived
importance of avoiding negative purchase consequences and consumer’s subjective feeling of
uncertainty about purchase consequences (Cox, 1967 c.f Venkatraman, 1989). Research has
addressed the level of risk as a critical factor in consumer behaviour (Dowling and Staelin,
1994). As already mentioned, a crisis undoubtedly engenders considerable risk and this is
why researchers have stressed that the effective and proactive crisis management should
include risk analysis (Pearson and Clair, 1998). In this paper, instead of the concept of
perceived risk, the concept of risk perception was used, in an attempt to disaggregate risk into
its two different components, risk perception and risk attitude. Risk perception reflects
consumers’ interpretation of the probability to be exposed to the content of the risk and it can
be defined as consumer’s assessment of the uncertainty of the risk content inherent in a
particular situation (Pennings et al., 2002). Risk attitudes are a person’s overriding tendencies
toward risk, in various risky situations and they refer to how willing a person is to accept that
risk (Schroeder et al., 2007).
2.3Risk and involvement
While there is no doubt that involvement and risk are closely related, there is some
ambiguity regarding how these constructs influence one another. Bloch and Richins (1983)
regard involvement as a variable of the internal state and they have developed the core
concept in terms of importance and perceived risk. Although both constructs, risk and
1
involvement, incorporate the notion of a product category being important to consumers
(Bloch and Richins, 1983), it appears that involvement cannot simply be equated with
perceived risk (Laurent and Kapferer, 1985; Chaffee and McLeod's, 1973). Additionally, one
broad conceptual distinction arises, given that the perceived risk considers only the negative
consequences resulting from the purchase and use of the product, while the level of product
involvement is affected to a great extent by the positive consequences as well (Dholakia,
2001).
The consensus seems to be that risk influences involvement (Chaffee & McLeod, 1973;
Laurent & Kapferer, 1985; Rothschild, 1979). Extensive research on consumer involvement
has identified the product characteristics as one of the three factors determining consumers’
level of involvement (Engel, Blackwell & Miniard, 1993) and the existence of some
perceived risk renders products more involving (Jacoby & Kaplan 1972 c.f. Craig-Lees, Joy
& Browne, 1995). As Sirgy (1982) points out, consumers’ product involvement is a
consequence of multiple factors, like the importance of the product to the consumer and risk
perception. Similarly, Laurent and Kapferer (1985) propose the perceived risk associated with
product purchase (which in turn has two sub facets: the perceived importance of negative
consequences in case of a poor choice and the perceived probability of making such a
mistake) as one of four facets/ antecedents of involvement, with the other three being the
importance, the hedonic (pleasure) and sign (symbolic) value of a product. Mittal and Lee
(1989) argue that the risk facet in the Laurent and Kapferer scale is not proper for the
conceptualization of involvement; it is one of its antecedents. In line with Laurent and
Kapferer (1985), who showed that risk importance and risk probability are two facets of the
involvement construct, are the findings of the survey of Pennings et al. (2002); according to
them, involvement [measured by a proxy question (Sudman & Wansink, 2002)] is negatively
related to consumer risk aversion and consumer risk perception in a significant way,
indicating that the higher the involvement, the lower the risk aversion and risk perception
when it comes to eating beef. Furthermore, based on Jones and Davis (1965, c.f Lee, 2004), it
could be argued that the more severe the crisis, the more personal involvement/ relevance is
aroused in the public. Consequently, we hypothesize that:
H1: The higher levels of product involvement will lead to (H1a) lower risk aversion
(higher risk attitude), (H1b) lower risk perceptions regarding using the product in crisis
and (H1c) higher perceived crisis severity.
2.4 Risk and Product Type
Quester and Lim (2003) suggest that consumers’ perceptions can differ with respect to
different products. Research shows that we can classify the products into two product types,
hedonic and utilitarian, implying that involvement can vary depending on the characteristics
of each product (Laurent and Kapferer, 1985; Vaughn, 1986). Hedonism and utilitarianism
are not the two ends of a one-dimensional scale (Okada, 2005) but products vary in their
perceived level of hedonic and utilitarian attributes (Batra & Ahtola, 1990). Hedonic goods
differ substantially from utilitarian goods. The latter are easy to compare prior to consumption
(Addis & Holbrook, 2001), since the attributes of utilitarian goods are more functional and
tangible, in contrast to hedonic products’ attributes which are more intangible and symbolic
(Kahnx et al., 1997). If consumers are more uncertain of the precise value of hedonic goods
than of utilitarian ones, the decision may be seen as a choice between a certain (utilitarian)
and an uncertain (hedonic) outcome (Dhar and Wertenbroch, 2000). This indicates that
hedonic goods are highly risky for consumers (Clement, Fabel & Schmidt-Stolting, 2006). If a
product is highly considered as luxury, then the perception of handled risk in terms of
choosing brands in that product class will be higher as well (Chaudhuri, 1998). Hedonic and
utilitarian categories are considered, in general, as quintessential product types, akin to the
notions of luxuries and necessities (Bearden and Etzel, 1982 c.f Chaudhuri, 2001). In
2
addition, it has been shown that the higher the perceived risk, the insecurity, or the uncertainty
regarding a product, service, or tourist destination, the lower its demand. More specifically,
tourism demand presents a higher elasticity index per level of perceived risk than any other
industry because of the intrinsic hedonistic (pleasure-giving) or eudaimonistic (happinessgenerating) benefits that consumers ascribe to its products and services (Gonzalez- Herrero
and Pratt, 1998). We therefore assume that hedonic and utilitarian products function quite
differently with regard to the perceived risk.
H2: A product which is seen as superior on a hedonic dimension will have a more
positive effect on (H2a) risk aversion (negative risk attitude), (H2b) risk perceptions and
(H2c) perceived severity than a product which is seen as superior on a utilitarian
dimension.
2.5 Risk and Crisis Response Strategies
Several studies have focused on crisis response strategies and have revealed their
importance to controlling and minimizing consumer’s negative reactions. Overall, research
has shown that the more accommodative the crisis response strategy is, the more positive the
consumers’ reactions toward the company are (Lee, 2004). Although crisis response strategies
have attracted the attention of researchers, their interaction with perceived risk hasn’t.
According to Stockmyer (1996), after a tampering incident, the constructive action from the
company’s part is not significantly associated with a reduction in risk. On the other hand,
Siomkos and Kurzbard (1994) showed that consumers regard the defective product as really
dangerous, when the company involuntarily recalls it. In this study the typology of crisis
response strategies of Siomkos & Kurzbard’s has been used (1994) (accommodative
strategies super effort
voluntary recall
involuntary recall
denial defensive
strategies). Therefore, we form the following hypotheses:
H3: Consumers will have a relatively small degree of risk perception and risk aversion
associated with the defective product, when the company’s response lies higher in the
“company response continuum”.
2.6The Impact of Risk Attitude, Risk Perception and Severity on Purchase Intention
The perceived risk appears to be a factor with significant impact on purchase intentions
following a product tampering incident (Stockmyer, 1996). Pennings et al. (2002) have shown
that buying a crisis-affected product is considered as highly risky. Furthermore, it has been
found that changes in beef consumption were strongly related to the perceived risk of “mad
cow disease” (Setbon et al., 2005). Moreover, Dawar and Pillutla, (2000) argue that
purchasers of the brand in crisis are more sensitive to the response of the firm than to the risk
of the defective product itself, contrary to purchasers of other brands who appear to focus
more on product risk perceptions than on the firm’s response. The perceived danger of the
defective product is a significant predictor of behavioural intentions, that is the higher the
danger perceived, the more unfavourable the behavioural intentions toward the company (de
Matos and Rossi, 2007). It has been argued that any element contributing to an increase in the
perceived risk of a touristic product affects negatively consumer’s purchasing behaviour,
something that clearly applies to business crises as well (Gonzalez- Herrero and Pratt, 1998).
What is more, Jones and Davis’s (1965, c.f Lee, 2004) argue that a high crisis severity may
trigger negative emotional reactions and send out the alarm that being a costumer of that
company involves risks. Additionally, both types of risk (perception and attitude) and the
perceived severity can influence purchase intention. Nevertheless, no research has explored
the effects of risk and severity on purchase intentions, in relation with product type (hedonic
vs. utilitarian) in a crisis context. Therefore, we pose the following research question:
RQ: In which way different levels of risk perception, risk attitude and perceived severity
affect consumer’s purchase intentions in a product-harm crisis for utilitarian and hedonic
products?
3
3. Methodology
To test the hypotheses, we used a 2 Product Involvement (High and Low) x 2 Product
Type (Hedonic and Utilitarian) x 4 Crisis response strategy (Denial, Voluntary Recall,
Involuntary Recall and Super Effort) full factorial between-subject experimental design. The
manipulations were embedded within descriptions of a hypothetical product harm crisis
scenario, presented in a fictitious newspaper article (Appendices: Table 1). A pre-test was
conducted in order to select the four investigated products (high involvement utilitarian, high
involvement hedonic, low involvement utilitarian, low involvement hedonic). Each
respondent was asked to answer the ten-item PII scale for product involvement
(Zaichkowsky, 1994) and the five-item scale for product type (Vaughn, 1986) referring to one
product from an array of twenty product categories. The pre-test results revealed that the
personal computer/laptop is perceived as high involvement utilitarian product, the insecticide
as low involvement utilitarian product, the perfume as high involvement hedonic product and
finally the beer as low involvement hedonic product. In this research, already established
scales were used. Risk attitude and risk perception ware measured with the scale of Pennings
et al. (2002), having a Cronbach’s alpha of .712 and .847 respectively. The perceived severity
of the crisis was measured by using the scale of Silvera et al. (2009), with Cronbach’s alpha
of .928. A survey instrument was created and distributed through an online survey, by using a
snowball sampling technique. Respondents were randomly divided in 16 treatments. A total
of 410 individuals participated voluntarily in this study. However, 33 participants missed to
indentify the right crisis response strategy and thus were excluded from the sample, resulting
in a sample 377 respondents. In terms of sex, 72 % of the respondents were female. In terms
of age group, 37.7 % of the respondents were between 18 and 24 years of age and 44 % in
the 25-34 age group.
4. Results
The manipulation checks provide strong evidence that the subjects did not have any
problem identifying intended situations (product involvement: F=122.138; p<0.001 and
hedonic or utilitarian product type: F=197.677; p<0.001). Due to the correlations of risk
attitude with risk perception and the perceived severity (-.530 and -.289), a MANOVA was
conducted in order to examine the research hypotheses. Product involvement, product type
and crisis response strategy were used as independent variables while risk attitude, risk
perception and perceived severity as dependent ones. Tests were run to ensure that all the
assumption of MANOVA were satisfied. An approximate z-value based on the skewness was
used to test univariate normality (Tabachnik and Fidell, 1996). Our results revealed only
minor deviations from the normality assumption. Box's M test was used for the assumption of
multivariate homogeneity of variance and the corresponding p-value was not significant
(Box's M: 124,95; F = 1,31; p > 0,01).
Firstly, the overall model shows three significant main effects of product involvement
(F=2.629, Wilks’ Lamda=.97, p < .05), hedonic-utilitarian product type (F=6.954, Wilks’
Lamda=.94, p < .001), crisis response strategy (F=2.007, Wilks’ Lamda=.95, p < .05) and a
two-way significant effect of product involvement and product type (F=7.283, Wilks’
Lamda=.94, p < .001). MANOVA results indicate that there are no three-way interactions
among the three independent variables. Univariate F tests were used to determine the specific
dependent variables that contribute to the significant overall effect. The univariate F tests
revealed that product involvement has a significant main effect on risk perception (F=7.835, p
<.01) but no significant effect on risk attitude and severity. More specifically, risk perception
is higher for the low involvement products (e.g. insecticide & beer) than for the high
involvement products (M low involvement =4.80 vs. M high involvement =4.35). Thus, only
H1b is supported. Regarding the hedonic-utilitarian distinction, the product type does have
significant main effects on risk perception (F=10.533, p <.001) and the perceived severity
4
(F=9.825, p <.01).However, contrary to our expectations, respondents have higher risk
perception (Mutilitarian =4.84 vs. Mhedonic =4.32) and higher perceived severity
(Mutilitarian =5.53 vs. Mhedonic =5.11) after product-harm crises involving utilitarian
products than hedonic ones. Therefore, H2a, H2b and H2c are not supported.
In terms of crisis response strategies, it has been found that the response of the company
has a significant main effect on risk attitude (F=3.155, p <.05) and risk perception (F=3.521, p
<.05). More specifically, Figure 1 and 2 (see Appendices) show that risk attitude is at the
lowest level (M denial = 2.76, M involuntary recall = 2.38, M voluntary recall = 2.55, M
super effort = 2.94) and risk perception at the highest (M denial = 4.43, M involuntary recall
= 4.94, M voluntary recall = 4.68, M super effort = 4.25) when the company is forced by the
government to recall the harmful products. However, as revealed by Tukey HSD Post Hoc
Test, there are significant differences only between super effort and involuntary product
recall. Consequently, only H3a and H3b are supported. Finally, a two way interaction of
product involvement and hedonic-utilitarian product type is significant for the perceived
severity (F=8.974, p =0.01). It seems that the severity is lower when the product under crisis
is a high involvement hedonic product (e.g. perfume), contrary to other combinations of
product involvement and hedonic-utilitarian product type (M high involvement-hedonic =
4.85, M high involvement-utilitarian = 5.66, M low involvement-hedonic = 5.37, M low
involvement-utilitarian = 5.39).
In order to explore the impact of risk perception, risk attitude and perceived severity on
purchase intentions for hedonic and utilitarian products we used separate regression analyses
for each product. The results indicate that risk attitude is a significant predictor only for
hedonic products (perfume: B=.401, p=.001 & beer: B=.278, p=.017) contrary to the
perceived severity which predicts the purchase intentions only for utilitarian products (laptop:
B=-.263, p=.020 & insecticide: B=-.280, p=.002). Finally, risk perception has significant
impact on purchase intention for both types of product (perfume: B=-.447, p=.000 & beer:
B=-.359, p=.001 laptop: B=-.255, p=.010 & insecticide: B=-.623, p=.000).
5. Discussion
This study makes an important contribution to the product-harm crisis theory and to
existing research because it is among the first exploring the role of product type (hedonic vs.
utilitarian) and the impact of product involvement and crisis response strategies on risk
attitude, risk perception and perceived severity. Contrary to our expectations, it was found
that respondents have higher risk perception and perceived severity after product-harm crises
involving utilitarian and not hedonic products. This could be explained by the fact that
according to Chaudhuri (1998) it seems reasonable to assume that important products are
likely to be positively associated with a high level of perceived risk, in terms of possible
product failure, etc. Furthermore, via our study we strongly support the argument that
products of high involvement are perceived as more risky than low involvement ones,
something which is consistent with previous researches. It has been found in previous studies
that product importance is related to the perceived risk (Laurent and Kapferer, 1985;
Zaichkowsky, 1985) and the importance of a product increases the level of perceived product
risk (Bettman, 1973). Another interesting result is that there is higher perceived severity for
high involvement hedonic products. Finally, our research suggests that the worst crisis
response strategy is that of involuntary recall, leading not only to higher risk perception but to
enhancement of the risk aversion of consumers as well. Our findings are confined by the usual
limitations of an experimental research. Furthermore, there is the limitation of the nonprobabilistic sampling process. Finally, a study using scenarios compromises realism. Future
5
studies should empirically test the effect of involvement on other product categories in a crisis
context, in order to verify the product type effect.
Appendices
Table 1: Scenarios for High involvement Utilitarian product
“Best Computers” is a big multinational company in producing computers. Unfortunately,
few days ago, the lithium rechargeable battery of a specific model was overheated and caused
an explosion. The explosion of the battery caused injuries and burns at 11 users of “Model 2”
who were end up to a hospital.
Scenario 1: Super effort
“Best Computers” company responded to these events saying that it was a shock for the
company to hear of a potential connection between the hospitalization of those people and the
company’s products and expressed her deep sorry and sympathy to the victims and their
families. “Best Computers” company stated that what is most important now is the welfare
those immediately affected by this unfortunate incident as consumers safety has always been
first priority for the company. To emphasize this, “Best Computers” ordered the immediate
recall of 1.500 laptops with a specific serial number. Also the defective products were
replaced free of charge and company provided all the information consumers need to know
about the faulty product and the ways that they can reimbursed. Additionally, a 24-h call
center provided support and was responsible for giving out any information requested.
Finally, “Best Computers” compensated the victims and reassured all customers that steps
were taken to ensure that a similar occurrence will not happen in the future”.
Scenario 2: Voluntary Recall
“Best Computers” company responded to these events saying that it was a shock for the
company to hear of a potential connection between the hospitalization of those people and the
company’s products and expressed her deep sorry and sympathy to the victims and their
families. “Best Computers” company stated that what is most important now is the welfare
those immediately affected by this unfortunate incident as consumers safety has always been
first priority for the company. To emphasize this, “Best Computers” ordered the immediate
recall of 1.500 laptops with a specific serial number. Also the defective products were
replaced free of charge and the company provided/disclosed all the information consumers
need to know about the faulty product and the crisis incident.
“Best Computers” company responded to these events saying that it was a shock for the
company to hear of a potential connection between the hospitalization of those people and the
company’s products and expressed her deep sorry and sympathy to the victims and their
families. Beyond of this “Best Computers” recalled 1.500 laptops a few days later, after the
Ministry of Health ordered so.
“Best Computers” company responded to these events saying that it was a shock for the
company to hear of a potential connection between the hospitalization of those people and the
company’s products and expressed her deep sorry and sympathy to the victims and their
families. Beyond of this “Best Computers” company denied the responsibility for the incident.
The company argued that all the appropriate controls had been made and the laptop’s batteries
were not found defective. “Best Computers” company argued that the product is safe and a
recall was unnecessary. Such kinds of incidents could happen to any product regardless of
what kinds of precautions are taken
6
Figure 1: Risk Attitude Means for each Crisis Response Strategy
Figure 2: Risk Perception Means for each Crisis Response Strategy
7
References
Addis, M. & Holbrook, M. B. (2001). On the conceptual link between mass customization
and experiential consumption: An explosion of subjectivity. Journal of Consumer Behaviour,
1, 50–60.
Arpan, L. M., & Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. R. (2005). Stealing thunder: Analysis of the effects of
proactive disclosure of crisis information. Public Relations Review, 31, 425–433
Batra, R., & Ahtola, O. T. (1990). Measuring the hedonic and utilitarian sources of consumer
attitudes. Marketing Letters, 2, 159–70.
Bearden, W.O., Lichtenstein, D.R., & Teel, J.E. (1984). Comparison Price, Coupon and
Brand Effects on Consumer Reactions to Retail Newsaper Advertisiments. Journal of
Retailing, 60, 11-34
Bettman, J. R. (1973). Perceived Risk and Its Components: A Model and Empirical Test.
Journal of Marketing Research, 10 184-90.
Bloch, P. H. & Richins, M. L. (1983). A theoretical model for the study of product
importance perceptions. Journal of Marketing, 47, 69-8 1.
Brown T. J., Barry T. E., Dacin P. A., & Gunst R. F. (2005). Spreading the Word:
Investigating Antecedents of Consumers' Positive Word-of-Mouth Intentions and Behaviors
in a Retailing Context. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 33,123-138.
Chaffee, S . M. & McLeod, J. M. (1973). Consumer decisions and information use. In S.
Ward & T. S. Robertson (Eds.). Consumer behavior: Theoretical sources (pp. 385-415).
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc.
Chaudhuri, A. (1998). Product class effects on perceived risk: The role of emotion.
International Journal of Research in Marketing, 15,157–168.
Chaudhuri, A. (2001). A study of emotion and reason in products and services. Journal of
Consumer Behaviour, 3, 267-279.
Choi, Y., & Lin Y., (2009). Consumer response to crisis: Exploring the concept of
involvement in Mattel product recalls. Public Relations Review, 35, 18–22.
Claeys, Cauberghe & Vyncke (2010). Restoring reputations in times of crisis: An
experimental study of the SCCT and the moderating effects of locus of control. Public
Relations Review, 36, 256–262.
Clement, M. Fabel, S. & Schmidt-Stolting, Ch. (2006). Diffusion of Hedonic Goods: A
Literature Review. International Journal on Media Management. 8, 155-163.
Coombs, W. T., & Holladay, S. J. (2005). An exploratory study of stakeholder emotions:
Affect and crises. Research on Emotion in Organizations, 1, 263–280.
Craig-Lees, Margaret, Sally Joy and Beverly Browne (1995), Consumer Behaviour, Brisbane:
John Wiley and Sons.
Dawar, N. & Pillutla, M.(2000). Impact of product-harm crises on brand equity: the
moderating role of consumer expectations. Journal of Marketing Research, 27, 215-26.
De Matos, C.A. & Veiga, R.T. (2005). How to Deal with Negative Publicity: The Importance
of Consumer Involvement. Brazilian Administration Review, 2, 57-72.
De Matos, C.A., & Rossi, C.A.V., (2007). Consumer reaction to product recalls: factors
influencing product judgement and behavioural intentions. International Journal of Consumer
Studies 31, 109-116.
8
Dhar, R., & Wertenbroch, K. (2000). Consumer choice between hedonic and utilitarian goods.
Journal of Marketing Research, 37, 60–71.
Dholakia U.M., (2001). A motivational process model of product involvement and consumer
risk perception. European Journal of Marketing, 35, 1340-1360.
Dick, A.S. & Basu, K. (1994). Customer loyalty: toward an integrated conceptual framework.
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Sciences, 22, 99-113.
Doney, P.M, & Cannon J.P. (1997). An Examination of the Nature of Trust in Buyer-Seller
Relationships. Journal of Marketing, 61, 35-51.
Dowling, Grahame R. and Richard Staelin (1994). A Model of Perceived Risk and Intended
Risk-Handling Activity. Journal of Consumer Research, 21,119-34.
Engel, J., Blackwell, R. D., & Miniard, P. W. (1986). Consumer behaviour. New York: CBS
College Publishing.
Gonzalez- Herrero A, & Pratt B. C.(1998). Communication Marketing Crises in Tourism:
Strategies in the United States and Spain. Public Relations Review, 24, 83-97.
Griffin, M., Babin, B. J., & Darden, W. R. (1992). Consumer assessments of responsibility for
product-related injuries: The impact of regulations, warnings, and promotional policies.
Advances in Consumer Research, 19, 870-877.
Kahnx, B. E., Ratner, R. K., & Kahneman, D. (1997). Patterns of hedonic consumption over
time. Marketing Letters, 8, 85–96.
Kassarjian, H.H. & Kassarjian W.M. (1979). Attitudes Under Low Commitment Conditions.
In Attitude Research Plays for High Stakes eds. John C. Maloney and Bernard Silverman
Chicago: American Marketing Association.
Laurent, G., & Kapferer, J. (1985). Measuring consumer involvement profiles. Journal of
Marketing Research, 49, 41-53.
Lee, B.K. (2004). Audience-Oriented Approach to Crisis Communication: A Study of Hong
Kong Consumers’ Evaluation of an Organizational Crisis. Communication Research, 31, 600618.
McDonald, L., & Hartel, C. E. J. (2000). Applying the involvement construct to
organizational crises. In Visionary marketing for the 21st century: Facing the Challenge,
Australian and New Zealand Marketing Academy Conference Proceedings, 799–803.
Australia: Griffith University.
Mitchell, V.W., (1992). Understanding consumers’ behaviour: Can perceived risk theory
help?. Management Decision, 30, 26–31.
Mittal, B. (1995). A comparative analysis of four scales of consumer involvement.
Psychology & Marketing, 12, 663 – 682.
Mittal, B., & Lee, M. (1989). A causal model of consumer involvement. Journal of Economic
Psychology, 10,363-389.
Okada, E. M. (2005, February). Justification effects on consumer choice of hedonic and
utilitarian goods. Journal of Marketing Research, 42, 43–53.
Parthasarathy M. & Forlani D.( 2010). Do Satisfied Customers Bad-Mouth Innovative
Products?. Psychology & Marketing, 27, 1134–1154.
Pearson, C.M. & Clair, J.A. (1998). Reframing Crisis Management. The Academy of
Management Review, 23, 59–76.
9
Pennings, J.M.E., B. Wansink, & Meulenberg M.T.G (2002). A Note on Modeling Consumer
Reactions to a Crisis: The Case of the Mad Cow Disease. International Journal of Research
in Marketing,…, 1991-100.
Peter, J. P., & Olsen, J. C. (1987). Consumer behaviour: Marketing strategy perspectives.
Homewood, IL7 Irwin.
Peter, J. P., & Olson, J. C. (1990). Consumer behaviour and marketing strategy (2nd ed.)
Boston: Irwin.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion: Classic and contemporary
approaches. Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown.
Quester & Lim (2003). Product involvement/brand loyalty: is there a link?. Journal of
Product and Brand Management, 12, 22-38.
Ray, M. L. (1973). Marketing Communications and the Hierarchy-of-Effects. In P. Clarke
(ed.). New Models for Mass Communication Research, (pp. 147-76). CA: Sage Publications.
Richins, & Root-Shaffer, (1988). The Role of Involvement and Opinion Leadership in
Consumer Word-of-Mouth: An Implicit Model Made Explicit. Advances in Consumer
Research, 15, 32-36.
Rothschild, M. L. (1979). Advertising strategies for high and low involvement situations, in J.
Maloney & B. Silverman (Eds.). Attitude research plays for high stakes (pp. 74-93) Chicago,
IL: American Marketing Association.
Schroeder T. C., Tonsor, G.,T., Pennings, J.,M.E, & Mintert, J., (2007). Consumer Food
Safety Risk Perceptions and Attitudes: Impacts on Beef Consumption across Countries. The
B.E. Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy Contributions, Vol 7, Issue 1, Article 65
Setbon, M., Raude, J., Fischler C., & Flahault, A., (2005). Risk Perception of the “Mad Cow
Disease” in France: Determinants and Consequences. Risk Analysis, 25, 11589.
Siomkos, G.J. & Kurzbard, G. (1994). The hidden crisis in product-harm crisis management.
European Journal of Marketing, 28, 30-41.
Sirgy, M. Joseph (1982). Self-Concept in Consumer Behavior: A Critical Review. Journal of
Consumer Research, 9 , 287-300.
Stockmyer, J. (1996). Brands in Crisis: Consumer Help for Deserving Victims. Advances in
Consumer Research, 23, 429–35.
Sudman, S., & Wansink, B. (2002). Consumer panels. (2nd ed.). Chicago, IL: American
Marketing Association.
Vaughn, R.(1986).How advertising works: A planning model revisited. Journal of Advertising
Research, 26, 57-66.
Venkatraman, M. (1989).Involvement and Risk. Psychology and Marketing, 6, 229-247.
Wright, P. L. (1974). Analyzing media effects on advertising responses. Public Opinion
Quarterly, 38, 192–205.
Zaichkowsky, J. L. (1994). Research Notes: The Personal Involvement Inventory: Reduction,
Revision and Application to Advertising. Journal of Advertising, XXIII, 59-70.
Zaichkowsky, J. L.(1985). Measuring the involvement construct. Journal of Consumer
Research, 12, 341–352.
10
Download