The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at http://www.emerald-library.com/ft IJSIM 12,3 234 Received March 2000 Revised December 2000 Accepted February 2001 The contribution of emotional satisfaction to consumer loyalty Yi-Ting Yu Monash University, Singapore Alison Dean Monash University, Churchill, Australia Keywords Loyalty, Customer satisfaction, Customer loyalty, Higher education Abstract Many customer satisfaction studies have concluded that there is a significant relationship between customer satisfaction and loyalty, but this finding has been questioned in that most of the studies focus on measuring the cognitive component of customer satisfaction. This study includes the cognitive component, but focuses on the affective component. It explores the role of emotions in satisfaction, and then compares the predictive ability of the cognitive and affective elements. Key findings are that both positive and negative emotions, and the cognitive component of satisfaction correlate with loyalty. Regression analysis indicates that the affective component serves as a better predictor of customer loyalty than the cognitive component. Further, the best predictor of both overall loyalty and the most reliable dimension of loyalty, positive word of mouth, is positive emotions. Thhe theoretical and practical implications of these findings are discussed. Introduction It may appear unnecessary to study the relationship between customer satisfaction and customer loyalty as many studies have confirmed that there is a significant positive relationship between these two variables (see Colgate and Stewart, 1998; Hocutt, 1998; Patterson and Spreng, 1997). However, many of the satisfaction-loyalty relationship studies were done when the development of the satisfaction construct was at an early stage, and customer satisfaction was still seen as an ``elusive construct'' (Rosen and Surprenant, 1998). More recently, scholars comment that it is inappropriate to ignore the emotional component of satisfaction, and hence the reliability findings of the previous studies are questioned (Liljander and Strandvik, 1997; Peterson and Wilson, 1992; Stauss and Neuhaus, 1997; Wirtz and Bateson, 1999). Consequently, this paper reports on a study which aims to: . explore the role of emotions in customer satisfaction; and . re-test the satisfaction-loyalty relationship when the emotional component is included. International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 12 No. 3, 2001, pp. 234-250. # MCB University Press, 0956-4233 First, we review the recent literature on customer satisfaction and customer loyalty, and develop research propositions consistent with the research aims. The research design is then described, and the results discussed. The paper concludes with implications and recommendations for future research. A brief review of customer satisfaction research Emotional In the early stages of services research, researchers attempted to diminish the satisfaction and confusion between customer satisfaction and service quality by determining consumer loyalty whether there is any distinction between them, and by exploring antecedents (Cronin and Taylor, 1992; Oliver, 1993a). Oliver (1993a, p. 76) distinguishes between the two constructs by suggesting that satisfaction is ``potentially all 235 salient dimensions'', it requires experience-dependency and involves emotions. In his study, Oliver reverses the previous notion that satisfaction is the antecedent of quality and claims that quality is the antecedent of satisfaction. Spreng and Mackoy's (1996) study further tests and confirms Oliver's conceptual model. Lately, customer satisfaction has been commonly accepted as a different construct from service quality and the emphasis has been on studying the relationships between them (Shemwell et al., 1998; Taylor and Baker, 1994). With consistent findings that service quality and satisfaction are different constructs, and that service quality leads to customer satisfaction, the research interest moved to studying the linkages between customer satisfaction, service quality and customer loyalty/retention. While the direct relationship between customer satisfaction and loyalty has been shown to be complex and asymmetric (Bloemer and Kasper, 1995; Mittal and Lassar, 1998; Oliver, 1999), and some research has shown that switching behavior and repurchase intentions are not consistent with satisfaction levels (Stauss and Neuhaus, 1997), a number of studies suggest that there is a significant positive relationship between customer satisfaction and customer loyalty/retention (Anderson and Sullivan, 1993; Cronin, Brady and Hult, 2000; Shemwell et al., 1998; Taylor and Baker, 1994). Hence an overall research proposition is suggested as follows. Research proposition 1: There is a significant positive relationship between customer satisfaction and customer loyalty. Before proceeding with further literature, we now briefly discuss the terms satisfaction and loyalty and define them for the context of our study. Drawing on the work of leading researchers in the services field for over 20 years, Roest and Pieters (1997) developed a nomological net to distinguish service quality and customer satisfaction. In doing so, they define satisfaction, as a relative concept that involves both cognitive and affective components, is consumerrelated (rather than product-related), mainly transactional, and incorporating an appraisal of both benefits and sacrifices. However, Roest and Pieters also state that ``. . . eventually, satisfaction may become or influence product attitude, which may be regarded as an aggregated but not relativistic construct involving a readiness to act'' (1997, p. 345). In summary, we note the distinction between transaction-specific and overall satisfaction, and we adopt the broader definition of satisfaction whereby the overall measure is an aggregation of all previous transaction-specific satisfaction, and involves both cognitive and IJSIM 12,3 236 affective components. Recently, the overall measure has been shown to be a better predictor of repurchase intentions (Jones and Suh, 2000). Loyalty is interpreted as true loyalty rather than repeat purchasing behaviour, which is the actual rebuying of a brand, regardless of commitment (Bloemer and Kasper, 1995). True loyalty, in this context, encompasses a nonrandom, behavioural response which results from evaluation processes that result in commitment (Bloemer and Kasper, 1995). This is in contrast to spurious brand loyalty which is a function of inertia. However, loyalty is a multi-dimensional construct and includes both positive and negative responses (Zeithaml et al., 1996). In this study, loyalty is to an educational provider and is therefore service loyalty, rather than brand loyalty as has been developed in relation to goods. In comparison to brand loyalty, service loyalty studies are under-represented in the literature (Bloemer et al., 1999; Javalgi and Moberg, 1997). Having established our overall research proposition as above, we acknowledge that it would be meaningless to re-test the same proposition without incorporating recent developments in the satisfaction literature. In particular, it is argued that satisfaction includes both cognitive and emotional components. The cognitive component refers to a customer's evaluation of the perceived performance in terms of its adequacy in comparison to some kind of expectation standards (Liljander and Strandvik, 1997; Oliver, 1980; Wirtz, 1993). The emotional component consists of various emotions, such as happiness, surprise and disappointment (Cronin et al., 2000; Liljander and Strandvik, 1997; Oliver, 1993b; Stauss and Neuhaus, 1997). It is important to note that the emotional component is a form of affect, and is a response to service delivery. In this study, ``consumption emotions are the affective responses to one's perceptions of the series of attributes that compose a product or service performance'' (Dube and Menon, 2000, p. 288). Such emotions are usually intentional (that is, they have an object or referent) and are different to the concept of mood, which is a generalised state induced by a variety of factors, and is usually diffused and non-intentional (Bagozzi et al., 1999). Emotions and mood (and attitudes) are all elements of a general category for mental feeling processes, referred to as ``affect'' (Bagozzi et al., 1999). The emotional component in the satisfaction judgment is therefore independent from the overall affective sense present in the respondent at the time of the service (de Rutyer and Bloemer, 1998). The cognitive and emotional components of satisfaction are now considered separately. Reflections on the cognitive component in customer satisfaction studies As indicated above, expectancy disconfirmation theory is the dominating model for measuring customer satisfaction (Brookes, 1995). That is, satisfaction is determined by the confirmation or disconfirmation of expectations with perceptions of the perceived performance on various service items (Danaher and Haddrell, 1996). The multi-item disconfirmation model has been applied in many customer satisfaction studies, and has been proven to be very useful Emotional (Danaher and Haddrell, 1996; Wirtz and Bateson, 1999). Further, when satisfaction and compared to other approaches, its benefits outweigh its main shortcoming, consumer loyalty which is its conceptual overlap with service quality. Such benefits include higher reliability, convergent and discriminant validity, face validity, managerial value and a lower skewness problem (Danaher and Haddrell, 1996). 237 In relation to the overlap between service quality and satisfaction, the multiitem disconfirmation model is very similar to the most famous service quality measurement scale, SERVQUAL. As both scales use disconfirmation of expectations, it is doubtful whether the results of some previous satisfaction studies show satisfaction or service quality. However, the two constructs employ a different definition of expectations (Zeithaml et al., 1993), and have a conceptual distinction in that satisfaction is an ``experience-dependency'' construct and service quality does not require experience (Danaher and Haddrell, 1996; Oliver, 1993a). If the scale seeks respondents' assessment of their ``perceived service experience'', it is alleged that it is essentially measuring satisfaction rather than service quality (Danaher and Haddrell, 1996). This study therefore employs the multi-item disconfirmation scale to measure the cognitive component of satisfaction. Many previous satisfaction studies, which focus on the cognitive component, suggest that there is a positive relationship between satisfaction and loyalty (see Andreassen and Lindestad, 1998; Colgate and Stewart, 1998; Danaher and Haddrell, 1996; Mittal et al., 1998; Taylor and Baker, 1994), and we therefore propose the following: Research proposition 1a: There is a significant positive relationship between the cognitive component of customer satisfaction and customer loyalty. However, focusing only on the cognitive component of satisfaction neglects an important element, namely emotions, and may be insufficient to obtain a comprehensive picture of consumer responses. The emotional element is now pursued. Affective measures in customer satisfaction Although there is still debate about whether satisfaction is itself an emotional construct or a cognitive construct which includes an emotional component (Babin and Griffin, 1998; Bagozzi et al., 1999; Crooker and Near, 1998), it appears that emotions may be one of the core components of satisfaction (Dube and Menon, 2000; Westbrook and Oliver, 1991). Further, it is suggested that emotions may distinguish customer satisfaction from service quality (Oliver, 1993a). Recent studies recognize that emotion is a core attribute in satisfaction and suggest that customer satisfaction should include a separate emotional component (Cronin et al., 2000). Stauss and Neuhaus (1997) argue that most satisfaction studies only focus on the cognitive component, and that the omission of the affective component is one of the main issues in satisfaction IJSIM 12,3 238 research. Their proposition is supported by Liljander and Strandvik (1997), who argue that customer satisfaction includes both affective (or emotional) and cognitive components. Further, Stauss and Neuhaus (1997) suggest that it is inappropriate to assume that consumers experience the same emotions and cognition when they give the same score for their overall satisfaction level. We therefore propose to include a separate emotional component in satisfaction, as the major contributor to the affective element. It is proposed that one's emotions have an influence on behavior. This is due to human nature, in that one responds to an event in certain ways to maintain a positive emotion, such as happiness, and to avoid a negative emotion, such as depression. Specifically, a person's positive emotions tend to link to his/her decisions to stay or continue with what he/she has been doing. Conversely, negative emotions tend to link to the opposite decisions, such as to leave and discontinue involvement (Bagozzi et al., 1999). Positive emotions may also lead one to share the positive experience with others, while negative emotions may result in complaining behavior (Bagozzi et al., 1999; Liljander and Strandvik, 1997). Supported by the previous findings that there is a connection between emotions and behavior (Bagozzi et al., 1999), and Stauss and Neuhaus' (1997) study, which found that there is a significant relationship between emotions and loyalty, we propose the following: Research proposition 1b: There is a significant positive relationship between the affective component of customer satisfaction and customer loyalty. The better predictor of customer loyalty: cognitive or affective? While general research conclusions suggest that there is a positive relationship between customer loyalty and both the cognitive and emotional components of satisfaction, there is a lack of empirical evidence to determine which of the components serves as a better predictor of satisfaction. This is particularly important, as the cognitive component of satisfaction alone has failed to serve as an effective predictor of customer loyalty (Stauss and Neuhaus, 1997). To date there are very few affective/emotional scales specifically developed to measure customer satisfaction emotions. Stauss and Neuhaus (1997) developed a scale, originally with four dimensions, namely, optimism/ confidence, steadiness/trust, disappointment/indecision, and protest/ opposition. Stauss has now extended this to five with the addition of indifference/resignation (personal communication, 1999). In 1997, Liljander and Strandvik (1997), based on the previous literature, developed a more comprehensive emotional scale that includes seven emotional attributes: happy, hopeful, positively surprised, angry, depressed, guilty and humiliated. Liljander and Strandvik also suggest that customer satisfaction emotions can be divided into two groups: positive emotions and negative emotions. Positive emotions include happy, hopeful and positively surprised, while negative emotions include angry, depressed, guilty and humiliated. Although there is no apparent consensus about the best way to measure the emotional component, in traditional literature, positive and negative emotions are often used to Emotional compare effects (Crooker and Near, 1998). satisfaction and There is consensus among researchers that loyalty is a complex construct, consumer loyalty evident in the variety of perspectives that have been used to study it (Javalgi and Moberg, 1997). These perspectives include behavioral, attitudinal and cognitive processes, however, the early customer loyalty studies focus mainly 239 on the behavioral perspective and then later shift to an attitudinal approach (de Ruyter et al., 1998). Based on the attitudinal approach, customer loyalty can be studied via its dimensions, such as word-of-mouth, complaining behavior and purchase intention. However, there are different findings in relation to loyalty dimensions, even when the same loyalty scales are employed. Parasuraman et al. (1994) developed a loyalty scale and found that loyalty consists of loyalty to company, propensity to switch, willingness to pay more, external response to problem and internal response to problem. De Ruyter et al. (1998) later adopted the same scale but found that loyalty consists of three dimensions: preference, price indifference and dissatisfaction response. However, the same authors suggest that the necessary elements to operationalise loyalty are captured in the ``behavioral intentions battery'', refined by Parasuraman and his coworkers. (Bloemer et al., 1999; Zeithaml et al., 1996). This study therefore adopts and customises this scale to explore the relationship between customer satisfaction, including both cognitive and emotional components, and loyalty. Methodology Sample The subjects in this study were on-campus undergraduate students in business and economics at a large university in Australia. Convenience sampling was employed and self-administered surveys were used to collect the data. A total of 320 surveys were distributed and 122 valid returns were obtained, giving a response rate of 37.5 per cent. The average age of respondents was 24. The respondent profile was female (55.8 per cent), average age 24 years, and comprising 57.9 per cent Australian and 42.1 per cent international students. This cohort was considered representative by the head of the school. Scales employed: customer satisfaction cognitive component Seven-point Likert scales were employed to measure both customer satisfaction and loyalty. The scale employed to measure the cognitive component of customer satisfaction focused on educational service attributes, and was customised from the instrument developed by Dean (1999). It is based on the multi-item disconfirmation model, and uses a single column format. The scale includes six groups of service attributes: course structure, teaching, lecturer's interaction and support, administration support, feedback and assessment, and physical presentation. A typical item reads, ``The written feedback on assignments . . . 1 (failed to meet my expectations) . . . to 7 (far exceeded my expectations)''. At the end of each group, an overall value for satisfaction with the focus of the items was obtained. IJSIM 12,3 240 Scales employed: customer satisfaction affective component To gain insights into the affective component, we used the emotional scale developed and tested by Liljander and Strandvik (1997). The scale was adopted in its entirety. A typical item reads ``So far, my overall studying experience at (the provider institution) makes me feel . . . happy''. Responses are on a Likert scale from 1 (never) . . . to 7 (often). Liljander and Strandvik were unable to study the relationship between the customer satisfaction emotional component and customer loyalty because, in the industry that they used, customers do not have the choice to switch to another brand or another service provider. Our study therefore builds on their work in pursuing the relationship. Scales employed: customer loyalty In terms of customer loyalty, Parasuraman et al.'s (1994) ``Reconfigured behavioral-intentions battery'', subsequently refined by its authors (Zeithaml et al., 1996) and also used by de Ruyter et al. (1998), and Bloemer et al. (1999) was adopted and customised for this study. The original scale had 13 items (discussed in relation to Table II), and five components: loyalty to company, external response to problem, propensity to switch, willingness to pay more, and internal response to problem. In their paper, Parasuraman et al. (1994) note that their scale requires refinement particularly for the latter three of these components, but that the ``loyalty'' component demonstrated excellent internal consistency while the ``external response'' was adequate according to the criteria of Nunnally (1978). Results and discussion The scales Prior to exploring the findings with respect to the research aims, we include some discussion about the scales that we used, their reliability and the factor patterns that they produce. Normality In all cases, negative questions were reverse coded for consistency. Normality tests, based on the different components in the survey, indicated adequate results. Of particular interest is the degree of skewness as customer satisfaction studies tend to be positively skewed (Coakes and Steed, 1999). The overall scales did indicate some positive skewing (0.22 and 0.23 for emotional and cognitive scales) but, given the nature of the study, these values were considered adequate to continue with the analysis. Reliability The reliability of the scales was established by utilising Cronbach's alpha. The cognitive component, emotional component and overall loyalty component had alpha scores of 0.94, 0.80 and 0.77 respectively, all indicating acceptable values (Nunnally, 1978). Factor analysis: emotions scale Emotional It was intuitively expected that the emotions scale would divide into two factors, satisfaction and representing positive and negative emotions. Principal components analysis with consumer loyalty varimax rotation confirmed this expectation, with the two factors explaining 46.9 per cent and 19.1 per cent of the variance respectively (see Table I). However, feelings related to anger loaded more highly with positive emotions than with 241 negative. This result is not easily explained but it is possible that, as anger is usually directed at someone, its interpretation is confused. Or anger might represent a third dimension of the emotional component and need to be further explored. This possibility is consistent with the model conceptualised by Dube and Menon (2000) which suggests that negative emotions have three components, ``other-attributed'', ``self-attributed'' and ``situation-attributed''. In their model, the ``other-attributed'' component is related to anger. Another possibility is that both positive and negative emotions can exist at the same time at a high level, thus anger loads on both factors. It is suggested that customers may have a zone of tolerance for negative emotions and that, within this zone, their negative emotions do not affect their positive emotions, so that both positive and negative emotions can exist at the same time (Liljander and Strandvik, 1997). In this study, a possible interpretation is that the respondents have simultaneously experienced high levels of positive emotions and anger. When anger is dropped from our scale, the reliability only decreases by 0.1, so it is retained in our current analysis. However, its role in customer satisfaction provides an interesting focus for future work on the emotions scale. Factor analysis: cognitive scale The dimensions of the cognitive scale, the educational service attributes have been developed and tested previously (Dean, 1999). However, as a number of items were added to the original scale, to account for the different educational context (on-campus classes versus distance education), a factor analysis was performed. Principal components analysis with varimax rotation identified Description 1. Positive emotions Happy Hopeful Positively surprised 2. Negative emotions Angrya Depresseda Guiltya Humiliateda Factor loading 1 Reliability 2 0.794 0.778 0.810 0.852 Note: Factor loadings less than 0.35 have been omitted; 0.546 0.657 0.829 0.771 a reverse coded 0.77 0.75 Table I. Emotions scale: factor loadings and reliability values IJSIM 12,3 242 seven factors with eigenvalues greater than one, accounting for 69.1 per cent of the variance. The results are shown in Table II. The factors shown in Table II have loaded essentially as expected, except for the factor that we had labelled ``teaching'' which split across the factors we had labelled ``teaching'' and ``course structure''. On reflection, this result is readily Description 1. Feedback and assessment Clearness of criteria Written feedback Assignment return time Questions and subject aims Fairness 2. Physical environment Tables Heating Chairs Lighting Factor loading 1 2 3 6 7 0.362 0.372 0.879 0.817 0.811 0.594 0.803 0.792 0.720 4. Administration Effectiveness Efficiency Time student advisor spent Friendliness of staff Visual presentation of lectures 0.847 0.772 0.698 0.567 0.464 5. Learning materials Ease of reading Learning materials Clarity of objectives Arrival in timely manner 0.390 6. Course structure and content Variety Flexibility Ease of understanding Knowledge or skills taught Teaching techniques used 7. Technology Technology in the classroom 5 0.821 0.763 0.641 0.608 0.594 3. Interaction and support Feedback in class Interaction with lecturers Consultation times Table II. Cognitive scale: factor loadings 4 0.423 0.415 0.422 Note: Factor loadings less than 0.35 have been omitted. 0.439 0.356 0.815 0.675 0.630 0.597 0.463 0.382 0.662 0.628 0.623 0.602 0.464 0.755 explained as the items left in ``teaching'' are all related to ``learning materials'', Emotional and ``course structure'' is really ``course structure and content''. Finally, the satisfaction and single item relating to technology split out and formed a factor on its own. In consumer loyalty summary, the seven factors that contribute to the cognitive scale, and the percentage of variance that they explain, are feedback and assessment (11.2), physical environment (11.1), interaction and support (10.8), administration 243 (10.6), learning materials (10.1), course structure and content (9.7), and technology (5.5). As the overall cognitive scale demonstrated good reliability (0.94) and was used in its entirety for our analysis, we did not pursue further implications of the factor structure. However, the results provide a useful starting point for further scale development. Factor analysis: loyalty scale When the items in the loyalty scale were analysed, four factors emerged, as shown in Table III. These factors (positive word of mouth, complaining behavior, switching behavior, and willingness to pay more) accounted for 28.9 per cent, 19.2 per cent, 10.7 per cent and 9.3 per cent of the variance respectively. The first of our four factors contains four of the five items in Parasuraman et al.'s (1994) ``loyalty to company'' factor. The fifth item, ``Do Description 1. Positive word-of-mouth Say positive things about the course Recommend the course to someone else Encourage friends to apply for the same course Consider the same uni. as the first choice if pursue further study 2. Complaining behavior Complain to other students if experience problems Complain to external agencies if experience problems Complain to school staff if experience problems 3. Switching behavior Try to switch to another campus of the same university if experience problems Try to switch to another university if experience problems Study in another uni. If it offers a better price Try to study fewer subjects at this university 4. Willingness to pay more Continue the same course if the price increases Pay a higher price for the benefits currently received Note: Factor loadings less than 0.35 have been omitted Factor loading 1 2 0.887 0.954 0.933 0.544 3 Alpha 4 0.94 0.479 0.67 0.671 0.782 0.777 0.597 0.611 0.563 0.646 0.72 0.748 0.622 0.635 0.780 0.45 Table III. Loyalty scale: factor loadings and reliability values IJSIM 12,3 244 more business with XYZ in the next few years'' was not included in our study as we felt it would be confusing to respondents in the second or third year of their degree programs. We refer to the first factor as ``positive word of mouth'' to indicate its primary focus. Consistent with Parasuraman et al.'s (1994) study, the best reliability was demonstrated by this factor. In relation to the other factors, ``willingness to pay more'' has the same two items as Parasuraman et al. (1994) but is not internally consistent, while the items in ``external response to problem'' and ``propensity to switch'' have been customised and are shown in the manner in which they loaded in our study. We have changed the names to facilitate clarity. To conclude the discussion of the scales, we found that they have proven sufficiently reliable to work with, and the items have generally loaded as expected on the various dimensions. Having established that the instrument was adequate to pursue the aims of the study, we now report and discuss the findings with respect to the specific aims. The role of emotions To commence our investigation of the first aim, the role of emotions in the measurement of customer satisfaction, we first consider the correlation between the dimensions of loyalty and the cognitive and emotional components of satisfaction (see Table IV). Consistent with research propositions 1a and 1b, Table IV confirms that there is a significant correlation between the two major components of customer satisfaction (emotional and cognitive) and loyalty. However, there is a higher correlation between overall customer loyalty and the emotional component than the cognitive component, at the 0.01 significance level. Further, the emotional component has slightly higher correlation coefficients for positive word of mouth, switching behavior and willingness to pay more when compared to the cognitive component. The correlation coefficients for the positive and negative emotions suggest that positive emotions are associated with all dimensions of loyalty except ``complaining behavior''. This finding may be due to the emotions scale not covering all the emotions that significantly correlate with the various loyalty dimensions. Intuitively, we expect that positive and negative emotions would link to complaining behavior (Liljander and Strandvik, 1997) but, as well as the three positive emotions included, there are other positive emotions, such as Table IV. Correlation analysis results Overall loyalty Positive word of mouth Complaining behaviora Switching behaviora Willingness to pay more Notes: * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; a Emotional component Cognitive component Positive emotions Negative emotionsa 0.534** 0.590** 0.088 0.262** 0.350** 0.424** 0.517** 0.153 0.145 0.313** 0.516** 0.582** 0.007 0.246** 0.375** 0.404** 0.435** 0.133 0.206* 0.235* items reverse coded relief, elation and joy (Bagozzi et al., 1999), which were not included in the scale. Emotional There is no evidence to show that each emotion has the same influence on satisfaction and different responses, such as complaining behavior, but rather that different consumer loyalty emotions may trigger different behavioral intentions (see Stauss and Neuhaus, 1997). Bagozzi et al. (1999) suggest that emotions influence decision making, and 245 that positive emotions particularly link to one's intention to maintain an ongoing plan and share the outcome of a certain activity/event. This conclusion is consistent with our research findings that positive emotions significantly correlate with positive word of mouth (to share the positive experience), switching behavior (negatively correlated) and willingness to pay more (in order to stay where he/she is). The association between loyalty and negative emotions (which have been reverse coded) suggests that they have a significant impact on loyalty as well. Of particular interest is the lack of association between negative emotions and complaining behavior. Again, this may be due to the absence of specific negative emotions from the scale, such as regret and disappointment, which are more likely to cause complaining behavior (Zeelenberg and Pieters, 1999). The seven emotions in our emotions scale do not correlate with the respondents' complaining behavior in this study, and it is noteworthy that the findings about the relationship between regret and disappointment with complaining behavior are inconsistent in Zeelenberg and Pieters' (1999) studies. These results emphasise the need to further explore the possible negative emotions that may influence complaining behavior. They also indicate that education providers need to seek out negative responses as these are unlikely to be voluntarily provided by students. The satisfaction-loyalty relationship revisited The second major aim of our study was to re-test the satisfaction-loyalty relationship when the emotional component of satisfaction is included. In particular, we were keen to establish the best predictors of loyalty and so we used regression analyses to explore the possible relationships. Best predictors of overall loyalty To gain a feel for the relative importance of the cognitive and emotional components in predicting customer loyalty, in the first regression we used overall loyalty as the dependent variable, with the cognitive and emotional components as independent variables. The adjusted R2 = 0.336, and F(2, 98) = 26.325, sig = 0.000. The standardized beta coefficients are shown in Table V Cognitive component Emotional component Beta t sig. 0.179 0.482 1.917 5.149 0.058 0.000 Table V. Standardized beta coefficients (dependent variable: overall loyalty) IJSIM 12,3 246 and, while only 33.6 per cent of the variance in loyalty is explained, the findings indicate that the emotional component is an important factor in explaining loyalty, apparently more important than the cognitive component. To substantiate the finding that inclusion of the emotional component leads to better results in explaining loyalty when compared to using the cognitive component alone, we performed another regression with overall loyalty as the dependent variable and only the cognitive component as the independent variable. In this case, the adjusted R2 = 0.172, and F(1, 101) = 22.151, sig = 0.000. The beta value for the cognitive component equalled 0.424, t = 4.706, sig = 0.000. This result suggests that the emotional component is an important predictor of loyalty, and is consistent with the suggestions of Liljander and Strandvik (1997) that customer satisfaction is better explained when emotions are included. The next question of interest is the relative effect of positive and negative emotions. To explore this, another regression analysis was performed, again using overall loyalty as the dependent variable, but including the factor scores for the two types of emotions. While the same variance is explained, positive emotions emerge as the best predictor of overall loyalty (beta = 0.336, t = 3.153, sig = 0.002) with negative emotions also significant (beta = 0.232, t = 2.488, sig = 0.015) and the overall cognitive assessment no longer significant. One would expect that having positive emotions towards ``my overall studying experience at service provider'', would result in more loyalty to that service provider. Our finding supports this assumption and also indicates that there is a negative association for negative feelings (as responses were reverse coded). How students feel about their studying experience is therefore highly relevant to the messages they are likely to give to others, and the personal responses they are likely to make. Best predictors of word of mouth behavior As indicated in Table IV, positive word of mouth has the highest correlation with the components of customer satisfaction and the highest reliability of the four loyalty dimensions. Further, switching costs are high and price regimes are generally inflexible in education, so we decided to conduct a further regression analysis using the components of satisfaction with positive word of mouth as the dependent variable. As there was no correlation between complaining behaviour and the components of satisfaction, we did not pursue its analysis. The results in Table VI indicate that positive emotions are an important predictor of ``positive word of mouth'', but that the students' cognitive Table VI. Standardized beta coefficients (dependent variable: word of mouth) Positive emotions Negative emotions Cognitive component Beta t sig. 0.369 0.152 0.263 3.720 1.735 2.882 0.000 0.086 0.005 assessment of satisfaction with educational attributes is also a significant Emotional predictor. The adjusted R2 = 0.395 and F(3, 100) = 23.402, sig = 0.000. Further, satisfaction and while the t value for negative emotions is not significant at the 95 per cent consumer loyalty confidence level, the result suggests that this is worthy of further investigation. When a further regression was run, using positive word of mouth as the dependent variable against the cognitive component only, the result confirmed 247 that, in this study, positive emotions are a better predictor of positive word of mouth than the cognitive element. In particular, for the latter regression, the adjusted R2 = 0.260 and F(1, 104) = 37.855, sig = 0.000. In general then, if emotions were not included in the scale, and only the cognitive component used to measure satisfaction, a comprehensive illustration of satisfaction is not gained. Consequently, it is suggested that an emotional scale needs to be included as part of customer satisfaction measurement. Implications of the study The main theoretical implication of this study is that the emotional component of satisfaction, which has not been considered in some of the recent customer satisfaction studies, serves as a better predictor of loyalty than the cognitive component. In particular, positive emotions are positively associated with positive word of mouth and willingness to pay more, and negatively associated with switching behaviour. Similarly, negative emotions are negatively related to positive word of mouth and willingness to pay more, and positively associated with switching behaviour. However, as this study represents a relatively small sample in one industry, these results require further investigation and verification. As there is a significant relationship between customer satisfaction (especially the emotional component) and customer loyalty, and based on the assumption that it is cheaper to retain existing customers than attract new customers (see Alford and Sherell, 1996), it appears that managers need to reemphasise how customers ``feel'' about their experiences of service delivery. In particular, they should try to achieve some balance in their pursuit of satisfaction information. It seems feasible that, in endeavouring to adopt measurement practices that are scientific and rigorous, managers may not provide sufficient opportunity for comments with an affective base and, consequently, fail to recognise the power and importance of emotions. An obvious extension of this is that, in retaining or enhancing customer loyalty, organizations need to explore and, as far as possible, manage the emotional components. Future research It is suggested that expectations, perceived performance and/or satisfaction level shift over time (Patterson et al., 1998; Peterson and Wilson, 1992). In this study, emotions in customer satisfaction were measured at one specific point in time and, hence, the result is only true for the time of completion of the questionnaire. As the delivery of higher education is an extended service IJSIM 12,3 248 encounter, the endurance of loyalty resulting from satisfaction may be challenged (Oliver, 1999). Future longitudinal studies could explore this issue. Demographic backgrounds also need to be considered. In particular, it is suggested that different cultural backgrounds may affect one's beliefs and behaviors (Hofstede, 1994; Winsted, 1997). That is, future research could compare the sample being studied based on their demographic backgrounds to see if the affective component still serves as a better predictor for customer loyalty. As in other satisfaction studies, there are still some methodological issues that need to be addressed. These include the use of bipolar construct questions, and positively skewed responses (Peterson and Wilson, 1992). Finally, there is a need to develop and refine the emotion scales. This provides scope for much interesting work. References Alford, B. and Sherell, D. (1996), ``The role of affect in consumer satisfaction judgments of credence-based services'', Journal of Business Research, Vol. 37, pp. 71-84. Anderson, E.W. and Sullivan, M.W. (1993), ``The antecedents and consequences of customer satisfaction for firms'', Marketing Science, Vol. 12, Spring, pp. 125-43. Andreassen, T.W. and Lindestad, B. (1998), ``Customer loyalty and complex services: the impact of corporate image on quality, customer satisfaction and loyalty for customers with varying degrees for service expertise'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 7-23. Babin, B.J. and Griffin, M. 1998), ``The nature of satisfaction: an updated examination and analysis'', Journal of Business Research, Vol. 41, pp. 127-36. Bagozzi, R.P., Gopinath, M. and Nyer, P.U. (1999), ``The role of emotions in marketing'', Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 27 No. 2, pp. 184-206. Bloemer, J.M.M. and Kasper, H.D.P. (1995), ``The complex relationship between consumer satisfaction and brand loyalty'', Journal of Economic Psychology, Vol. 16, pp. 311-29. Bloemer, J., de Rutyer, K. and Wetzels, M. (1999), ``Linking perceived service quality and service loyalty: a multi-dimensional perspective'', European Journal of Marketing, Vol. 33 No. 11/ 12, pp. 1082-106. Brookes, R. (Ed.) (1995), Customer Satisfaction Research, ESOMAR, Amsterdam. Coakes, S.J. and Steed, L.G. (1999), SPSS: Analysis without Anguish, John Wiley & Sons, Chichester. Colgate, M. and Stewart, K. (1998), ``The challenge of relationships in services: a New Zealand study'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 9 No. 5, pp. 454-68. Cronin, J.J. Jr and Taylor, S.A. (1992), ``Measuring service quality: a reexamination and extension'', Journal of Marketing, Vol. 56, July, pp. 55-68. Cronin, J.J. Jr, Brady, M.K. and Hult, G.T.M. (2000), ``Assessing the effects of quality, value and customer satisfaction on consumer behavioral intentions in service environments'', Journal of Retailing, Vol. 76 No. 2, pp. 193-218. Crooker, K.J. and Near, J.P. (1998), ``Happiness and satisfaction: measures of affect and cognition?'', Social Indicators Research, Vol. 44, pp. 195-224. Danaher, P.J. and Haddrell, V. (1996), ``A comparison of question scales used for measuring customer satisfaction'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 7 No. 4, pp. 4-26. de Ruyter, K. and Bloemer, J. (1998), ``Customer loyalty in extended service settings: the interaction between satisfaction, value attainment and positive mood'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 10 No. 3, pp. 320-36. de Ruyter, K., Wetzels, M. and Bloemer, J. (1998), ``On the relationship between perceived service quality, service loyalty and switching costs'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 9 No. 5, pp. 436-53. Dean, A.M. (1999), ``Issues and challenges in delivering HR programs by distance education'', Asia Pacific Journal of Human Resources, Vol. 37 No. 1, pp. 20-38. DubeÂ, L. and Menon, K. (2000), ``Multiple roles of consumption emotions in post-purchase satisfaction with extended service transactions'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 11 No. 3, pp. 287-304. Hocutt, M.A. (1998), ``Relationship dissolution model: antecedents of relationship commitment and the likelihood of dissolving a relationship'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 189-200. Hofstede, G. (1994), Uncommon Sense about Organizations, SAGE, Thousand Oaks, CA. Javalgi, R.G. and Moberg, C.R. (1997), ``Service loyalty: implications for service providers'', The Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 11 No. 3, pp. 165-79. Jones, M.A. and Suh, J. (2000), ``Transaction-specific satisfaction and overall satisfaction: an empirical analysis'', Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 14 No. 2, pp. 147-59. Liljander, V. and Strandvik, T. (1997), ``Emotions in service satisfaction'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 8 No. 2, pp. 148-69. Mittal, B. and Lassar, W.M. (1998), ``Why do customers switch? The dynamics of satisfaction versus loyalty'', The Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 12 No. 3, pp. 177-94. Mittal, V., Ross, W.T. Jr and Baldasare, P.M. (1998), ``The asymmetric impact of negative and positive attributed-level performance on overall satisfaction and repurchase intentions'', Journal of Marketing , Vol. 62 No. 1, pp. 33-47. Nunnally, J.C. (1978), Psychometric Theory, 2nd ed., McGraw-Hill, New York, NY. Oliver, R.L. (1980), ``A cognitive model of the antecedents and consequences of satisfaction decisions'', Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. XVII, November, pp. 460-9. Oliver, R.L. (1993a), ``A conceptual model of service quality and service satisfaction: compatible goals, different concepts'', Advances in Services Marketing and Management, Vol. 2, pp. 65-85. Oliver, R.L. (1993b), ``Cognitive, affective, and attribute bases of the satisfaction response'', Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 20, December, pp. 418-30. Oliver, R.L. (1999), ``Whence consumer loyalty?'', Journal of Marketing, Vol. 63, pp. 33-44. Parasuraman, A., Zeithaml, V.A. and Berry, L.L. (1994), ``Moving forward in service quality research: measuring different customer-expectation levels, comparing alternative scales, and examining the performance-behavioral intentions link'', Working Paper: Report number 94-114, Marketing Science Institute. Patterson, P.G. and Spreng, R.A. (1997), ``Modeling the relationship between perceived value, satisfaction and repurchase intentions in a business-to-business, services context: an empirical examination'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 8 No. 5, pp. 414-34. Patterson, P., Romm, T. and Hill, C. (1998), ``Consumer satisfaction as a process: a qualitative, retrospective longitudinal study of overseas students in Australia'', Journal of Professional Services Marketing, Vol. 16 No. 1, pp. 135-57. Peterson, R.A. and Wilson, W.R. (1992), ``Measuring customer satisfaction: fact and artifact'', Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 20 No. 1, pp. 61-71. Emotional satisfaction and consumer loyalty 249 IJSIM 12,3 250 Roest, H. and Pieters, R. (1997), ``The nomological net of perceived service quality'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 8 No. 4, pp. 336-51. Rosen, D.E. and Surprenant, C. (1998), ``Evaluating relationships: are satisfaction and quality enough?'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 103-25. Shemwell, D.J., Yavas, U. and Bilgin, Z. (1998), ``Customer-service provider relationships: an empirical test of a model of service quality, satisfaction and relationship-oriented outcomes'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 155-68. Spreng, R.A. and Mackoy, R.D. (1996), ``An empirical examination of a model of perceived service quality and satisfaction'', Journal of Retailing, Vol. 72 No. 2, pp. 201-14. Stauss, B. and Neuhaus, P. (1997), ``The qualitative satisfaction model'', International Journal of Service Industry Management, Vol. 8 No. 3, pp. 236-49. Taylor, S.A. and Baker, T.L. (1994), ``An assessment of the relationship between service quality and customer satisfaction in formation of consumers' purchase intentions'', Journal of Retailing, Vol. 70 No. 2, pp. 163-78. Westbrook, R.A. and Oliver, R.L. (1991), ``The dimensionality of consumption emotion patterns and consumer satisfaction'', Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 18, June, pp. 84-91. Winsted, K.F. (1997), ``The service experience in two cultures: a behavioral perspective'', Journal of Retailing, Vol. 73 No. 3, pp. 337-60. Wirtz, J. (1993), ``A critical review of models in consumer satisfaction'', Asian Journal of Marketing, Vol. 2, December, pp. 7-22. Wirtz, J. and Bateson, J.E.G. (1999), ``Consumer satisfaction with services: integrating the environment perspective in services marketing into the traditional disconfirmation paradigm'', Journal of Business Research, Vol. 44 No. 1, pp. 55-66. Zeelenberg, M. and Pieters, R. (1999), ``Comparing service delivery to what might have been: behavioral responses to regret and disappointment'', Journal of Service Research, Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 86-97. Zeithaml, V.A., Berry, L.L. and Parasuraman, A. (1993), ``The nature and determinants of customer expectations of service'', Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 21 No. 1, pp. 1-12. Zeithaml, V.A., Berry, L.L. and Parasuraman, A. (1996), ``The behavioral consequences of service quality'', Journal of Marketing, Vol. 60 No. 2, pp. 31-46.