The Scientific Method, Bias, and Reasoning 2 Truth is sought for its own sake. And those who are engaged upon the quest for anything for its own sake are not interested in other things. Finding the truth is difficult, and the road to it is rough.* 2.1 Introduction The hallmark of any successful crime scene investigation is a team effort whose director, like the maestro of an orchestra, is its leader. How well the team performs is related to how well its leader uses the scientific method, thinks critically and creatively, opens lines of communication with subordinates, and applies inductive, deductive, and abductive logic. If done properly the culture of science wraps its arms around the team and protects it from making critical mistakes, introducing subjectivity, and conducting a biased investigation. This is true because science itself is self-correcting. Mistakes, when found, are corrected, and biases, through team effort and the application of the scientific method, are avoided. As Chisum and Turvey wrote [2], Evidence interpretation is a complex process, and the less one understands about the nature of physical evidence, the principles of forensic science, analytical logic, and the scientific method, the simpler crime reconstruction may seem. (p. 92) When asked how the crime was solved or the most probative evidence located, no crime scene investigator would consciously say, ―I used the scientific method to solve this crime,‖ or ―The scientific method pin-pointed the most important evidence at that scene,‖ or ―My creative employment of induction and deduction led me to the killer,‖ or ―I ensured that I had an open line of communication with my subordinates.‖ That is not how science and scene investigation work. Most if not all crime scene investigators never consciously consider their cognitive thoughts, that is, the reasoning process they employ after or during the investigation. It becomes and should be second nature. Their approach is based on education, intuition, training, and experience. The scene scientist’s approach is different, however, because it is based on deductive and inductive reasoning buttressed by education, intuition, training, and experience. Chapter 1 suggested that crime scene investigation is not simply the application of science but actually a complex scientific endeavor. As such, it is, like all science, a step-wise * Stated by Alhazen (Ibn Al-Haytham) in his Critique of Ptolemy, translated by S. Pines, Actes X Congrès internationale d’histoire des sciences, Vol. I. Ithaca, 1962, as quoted in Ref. [1]. 32 Crime Scene Forensics process of knowledge and information-building about understanding what happened, who might have done it, and what probative evidence is available. It is also a problem-solving exercise. For example, why did this happen and in what sequence? Obtaining as precise an answer as possible illustrates why crime scene investigation is particularly suited to the application of the scientific method. If we delve further into the reasons, the explanation of why this is true is plainly clear. Just as scientists attempt to explain observations through a series of constantly tested and revised hypotheses, the ultimate feedback mechanism, using what is called the scientific method, so do scene scientists. 2.2 The Scientific Method: Scene Scientists and Scene Investigators Authors of science text books nearly invariably include a discussion of the scientific method, by typically listing the so-called steps inherent in the method, often offering an appropriate diagram spiced with an example or two. Over the years, there have been scholarly articles on the subject. As Cohen says [3], According to the currently fashionable view, it is of the very essence of the scientific method to distrust all reason to rely on the facts only. The motto, ―Don’t think; find out,‖ often embodies this attitude. The scientific method is supposed to begin by banishing all preconceptions or anticipations of nature. In the first positive stage it simply collects facts; in the second, it classifies them; then it lets the facts themselves suggest a working hypothesis to explain them. It is only in the last stage, in the testing or verifying of hypotheses (so as to transform them into established laws) that the rational deduction of consequences plays any part. Such deduction, it is maintained, brings us no new information. It only makes explicit what experience has already put into our premises. Most scientific texts list five to seven steps inherent in the scientific method. Edmund lists eleven on his Web site [4]. While his is one person’s opinion, students of science will recognize the following widely accepted list, which tracks Cohen’s explanation quoted above. Observe a phenomenon that has no good explanation (potential evidence, which we label as observable phenomena) Formulate a hypothesis Design an experiment(s) to test the hypothesis Perform the experiment(s) Accept, reject, or modify the hypothesis By itself, this list offers little insight to its application to a specific scene investigation. To illustrate, consider an example of a homicide scene examined applying the scientific method. The following are the gross observations—the macro scene (see Chapter 4). The micro scene is not considered in this discussion. A deceased male is lying face up on the floor in his bedroom, the victim of a single stab wound to the chest. He has an incised wound (a deep cut) on his right forefinger. His shirt, which is torn, has drip pattern in blood on the chest. The front door is open. 33 Crime Scene Forensics The living room window is open. A sneaker print in dust is on the floor under the window. Dried droplets—not blood—mar some of the sneaker print detail. A dry but diluted drop of blood is in the kitchen sink. A crumpled paper towel with dried blood sits on the kitchen sink. A shattered drinking glass is on the kitchen floor near the sink. A wicked storm with high winds passed through the town several hours earlier. All scenes have multiple gross observable phenomena (macro-scene elements), much of which has no immediate, simple, reasonable, or seemingly logical explanation. Additionally, each might have many explanations of which only one rests in the ground truth. Frustratingly, most explanations will never be known with absolute certainty because, other than the participants of the crime, investigators were not present when the event took place. Thus, the essence of all scene investigations is sorting fact from fiction, identifying what is crime-related and eliminating what is not. One can generalize the observable phenomena and place them into arguable categories. It would be instructive for novice investigators and students to place these observations into usable categories such as: People living their lives Activity of the crime that spawned the investigation Activity or events unrelated to neither of the above Unknown at this time All observable scene phenomena must be interpreted correctly, that is, put into a category correctly, or risk being misinterpreted. If misinterpreted, the result can be a botched investigation leading to the arrest and conviction of an innocent person. Scene investigators use their experience and intuition to understand these macro-scene elements. Certainly, using experience to interpret new phenomena is a normal extension of an engaged mind, and it is reasonable. It is, however, the seed of experienced-based bias, which is the natural consequence of living and working in the real world. Bias happens subconsciously and, while not necessarily bad if controlled, it can lead the scene scientist/investigator down an incorrect interpretive path. Called the ―I’ve seen it a million times‖ syndrome, everyone pigeonholes their experiences and past scene observable elements they use to interpret scene elements in each new setting. The result is a mixture of correct and incorrect interpretations of what the scene is trying to tell them. What differentiates scene investigators from scene scientists is how they use their experience and intuition to arrive at final interpretations or conclusions. Scene investigators trust their experience and draw final conclusions based on their observations over time. This seems reasonable but, in truth, is a myopic approach because the process automatically arrests the mental exercise once they have ―decided‖ what happened. The scene scientist, however, never offers a ―final‖ interpretation or conclusion until all hypotheses have been ―tested‖ and alternative explanations exhausted. The word ―final‖ is in quotes because even the final conclusion offered can change if new evidence is located. It is this ―testing‖ of alternate explanations that separates scene investigators from scene scientists. Table 2.1 illustrates the first thought processes leading to hypotheses based on observable phenomena at the scene. 34 Crime Scene Forensics Table 2.1 First Hypotheses Based on Scene Observations Gross Observable Phenomena Deceased on floor with single stab wound to chest Deep incised wound to right forefinger Drip pattern of dried blood on the shirt of the deceased Initial Interpretation (Hypothesis) Deceased stabbed by assailant and died quickly Defensive wound—deceased knew what was happening and was fighting for his life Result of stab wound or blood dripping from knife Storm passed through hours earlier Living room window is open No immediate causal relationship to crime Perpetrator entered the house through window Dry residue sneaker print on floor under window Dried droplets mar sneaker pattern Front door open Dried, diluted drop of blood in kitchen sink Perpetrator’s sneaker print Window was open before storm Perpetrator’s exit route Perpetrator was injured and went to sink to wash after committing crime Perpetrator used towel to clean and dry wound Altercation began in the kitchen with perpetrator struggling with deceased here Paper towel with dried blood on kitchen sink Shattered drinking glass on kitchen floor These explanations of observable phenomena seem reasonable and they may be all the explanation needed. However, a scene scientist considers these as testable hypotheses, each having an alternative explanation. Thus, this is the first round of observational explanations, some of which might be correct and others incorrect. If this is as far as an investigator goes, which is essentially jumping to conclusions without considering reasonable alternatives, the scene is open to missing important probative evidence. The next step requires the formulation of alternative hypotheses, which is the essence of the scientific method. Returning to the homicide scenario, the scene scientist formulates alternative scenarios. The result is shown in Table 2.2, an expansion of Table 2.1. Here, the third column offers alternative explanations of the macro-scene observation scenarios, different from those in the first column and also perhaps more reasonable. These alternative explanations also have merit but, regardless, the ground truth remains unknown. In fact, it might lie somewhere between the two hypotheses. Or, a third as yet unconsidered alternative might be closer to what actually happened. At this point, these are competing hypotheses. New evidence could trigger a third set of hypotheses or eliminate one of the original two. The point is that the best fact-fit can be only determined through experimentation, step 3 of the scientific method. For students and novice investigators, the concept of experimentation is often frightening because it forces them into unknown intellectual territory. Experimentation, however, does not necessarily require or necessarily imply actual laboratory experiments, although that is certainly a possibility. From a crime scene perspective, some experimentation can be done at the scene, which might result in a change of one of the hypotheses or even create new ones. Laboratory testing might be done on evidence collected and transported to the forensic laboratory. If laboratory experimentation is necessary, the best interpretation of what happened and the sequence of events would be put on hold. Table 2.3 proposes possible experiments (see the last column), not all of which are laboratory-based. The idea is not to design a way to prove the original or alternative hypotheses but to disprove them. In fact, the scientific method requires that the experiment’s intended design is to disprove projected hypotheses. This not-so-subtle fact is lost The Scientific Method, Bias, and Reasoning 35 Table 2.2 Alternative Scenarios Based on Observable Phenomena at the Scene Gross Observable Phenomena Initial Hypothesis Alternative Hypothesis Deceased on floor with single stab wound to chest Deep incised wound to right forefinger Deceased stabbed by assailant and died quickly Defensive wound—deceased knew what was happening and was fighting for his life Deceased fell on knife and died quickly Deceased cut finger picking up shards of glass from floor Drip pattern of dried blood on the shirt of the deceased Result of stab wound or blood dripping from knife No immediate causal relationship to crime Result of blood dripping from cut finger Perpetrator entered the house through window Deceased opened window to cool house before crime took place Perpetrator’s sneaker print Victim’s sneaker print Dried droplets mar sneaker pattern Window was open before storm Window was open before storm. Droplets might have happened during storm if storm blew open window Front door open Dried, diluted drop of blood in kitchen sink Perpetrator’s exit route Perpetrator was injured and went to sink to wash after committing crime Perpetrator used towel to clean and dry wound Altercation began in the kitchen with perpetrator struggling with deceased here Blown open by storm Deceased tried to clean wound after cutting finger on glass shards Deceased tried to stop bleeding with paper towel Deceased dropped glass when storm blew open front door Storm passed through hours earlier Living room window is open Dry residue sneaker print on floor under window Paper towel with dried blood on kitchen sink Shattered drinking glass on kitchen floor Storm blew open front door on most lay investigators [3]. It is also expected that these experiments might create new hypotheses, which triggers more testing. A time will come when all data have been analyzed, hypotheses tested and retested, and a point will be reached when no more testing seems reasonable or warranted. Then, a truth of some sort will emerge. This truth may not represent the actual ground truth, but will be closer than if the process had been stopped after the first set of hypotheses. The process may have led to new evidence that should have elicited additional questions and perhaps new hypotheses. In the example above, what would the investigative result have been if the investigator solely relied on the initial interpretation of the events? Possibly, the evidence collected and subsequently analyzed by a forensic laboratory would have represented that investigator’s experience-based bias, which would be myopic in scope. By considering all alternative scenarios, the scene scientist/investigator permits the self-correcting mechanism inherent in the scientific method to dictate the flow of the investigation and the evidence discovery process. This is why the scientific method, applied correctly, is the only way to eliminate bias in an arena where there are few, if any, checks and balances. By its very nature, the scientific method conducts those checks and balances through the hypothesis, experimentation, and feedback process. For science students, this process makes sense. For novice and many 36 Crime Scene Forensics Table 2.3 Proposed Experiments to Disprove Advanced Hypotheses Gross Observable Phenomena Initial Hypothesis Deceased stabbed by assailant and died quickly Alternative Hypothesis Deceased fell on knife and died quickly Deep incised wound to right forefinger Defensive wound— deceased knew what was happening Deceased cut finger picking up shards of glass from floor Drip pattern of dried blood on the shirt of the deceased Result of stab wound or blood dripping from knife Result of blood dripping from cut finger Storm passed through hours earlier Living room window is open No immediate causal relationship to crime Perpetrator entered the house through window Storm blew open front door Deceased opened window to cool house before crime took place Dry residue sneaker print on floor under window Perpetrator’s sneaker print Victim’s sneaker print Dried droplets mar sneaker pattern Window was open before storm Window was open before storm. Droplets might have happened during storm if storm blew open window Deceased on floor with single stab wound to chest Front door open Perpetrator’s exit route Blown open by storm Possible Experiments Examine report of the medical examiner Look for other signs of struggle Check fingerprints Examine medical examiner’s report Look for shards of glass with blood DNA analysis of blood Examine bloodstain pattern expert’s interpretation of drip pattern DNA testing on blood See entry ―Front door open‖ below Check for pry marks Check finger marks not matching deceased Check victim/other footprints on ground Check sneaker of victim Check storm wind direction • Check storm wind direction • Check jimmied or Dried, diluted drop of blood in kitchen sink Perpetrator was injured and went to sink to wash after committing crime Deceased tried to clean wound after cutting finger on glass shards Paper towel with dried blood on kitchen sink Perpetrator used towel to clean and dry wound Altercation began in the kitchen with perpetrator struggling with deceased here Deceased tried to stop bleeding with paper towel Deceased dropped glass when storm blew open front door Shattered drinking glass on kitchen floor broken locks Check impact area where door should have hit wall DNA analysis of blood Bloodstain pattern DNA of blood Check glass for blood Check for crushed shards on shoe of deceased The Scientific Method, Bias, and Reasoning 41 experienced investigators, this is a foreign concept. For scene investigators as opposed to scene scientists, the approach is not necessarily satisfying because it offers only a sketchy roadmap of how to conduct a competent scene investigation. This is the reality of the scientific method. Scientific processes are complex. Scene investigations, too, are complex and without question a scientific endeavor. Using a cut-and-dried method for conducting a scene investigation is ludicrous. The above homicide example was intentionally simplistic because it was intended to illustrate the scientific thought process. In the current paradigm of crime scene investigation (Chapter 1), police investigators apply scientific methods at crime scenes without using the scientific thought process. Like researchers, scene scientists create new understanding because they use what is known to uncover the truth of the unknown, that is, the truth underlying alleged facts. Unlike scene scientists, researchers question the unknown and use the scientific method to uncover more basic truths. Despite these disparate missions, scene scientists and basic researchers use a common approach to accomplish their goals. The foregoing discussion makes it seem as though scene scientists and scene investigators exist on different planets. Nothing could be further from the truth. The discussion is not meant to imply that scene scientists and police investigators do not and cannot play on the same field, even if their individual roles and missions are decidedly different. In fact, they are complementary. At the crime scene, the scene scientist conducts the business of science distinct from but complimentary to the investigative role of the police scene investigator. The scientific mind places a premium on precision, objectivity, repeatability, experimentation, uncertainty management, peer review, and experience in order to arrive at reliable interpretations of clues left at the scene because these methods have a history of successfully relating information while correcting misunderstandings. The scene scientists’ approach is radically different from that of the scene investigator, whose mission is to find and arrest the perpetrator. The scene scientist has no mission or allegiance except to the facts. The scene investigator searches for facts (evidence) to fit a scenario or an ―idea‖ of what happened based on experience or on information relayed by messengers—witnesses, other officers, and so on, as the cartoon above implies. Still, they play on the same team. One is the pitcher and the other the catcher. Is the scene scientist the pitcher or the catcher? The reader should decide. 2.3 Bias in Scene Investigations Scene scientists judge facts, understanding that one must define facts carefully; some facts are hard and others are soft. The competent scene scientist remains loyal to the hard facts, such as scientific data, observable phenomena, and so on, and seriously considers only undeniably supportable soft facts, such as witness statements, and so on. Each new fact must pass muster by testing against an appropriate hypothesis. Only when all of the facts are known can a scenario of what happened be reliably expressed. New facts that do not fit must have an explanation, which means a new or modified hypothesis. Without this process, investigations are prone to bias. According to Dror, an expert studying human behavior, and Rosenthal [5] Human judgments are affected by a variety of factors. These effects stem from our initial perceptual mechanisms to higher cognitive functions. Given such variability and individual differences, the question arises: How reliable are human judgments? The variability across 38 Crime Scene Forensics individuals reflects that people are different; they have different past experiences, mental representations and abilities, as well as different motivations, personalities, and so forth. (p. 900) Scene investigators will likely bristle at the suggestion that they are biased in their work. They correctly believe crime scene investigation is a critical process and, as such, they strive to find evidence to support a successful prosecution. They would likewise challenge the suggestion that they do not actively search for exculpatory evidence because their subconscious minds are focused on prosecutions. This should not come as a surprise. Scene investigators in the United States mostly represent the investigative arm of law enforcement and, thus, are an integral part of the prosecution team. Their mission is to find probative evidence to prove someone committed the crime, which they do through a process of deductive reasoning, where, if their premises are true, the conclusion must also be true. By definition, then, they are as biased as the crime scene expert hired by the defense team. The prosecutor, who is an integral player and arguably the most critical member of the prosecution’s adversarial team, is by definition also biased. But the prosecutor, as an advocate, is supposed to be biased; so is the defense attorney. The scene scientist who responds to the scene and who is likely employed by the prosecution’s team must resist the inherent biases and should have no stake in the investigative process. The reason is that science brings objectivity to the investigative process. That is not to imply that a scene scientist cannot be swayed by reason. Science researchers use the scientific method to eliminate bias. If the researcher conducts experiments to arrive at a preconceived result, the process is not science. Researchers work at a frontier of the unknown, an area of uncertainty, where the work is a continual process of discovery propelled by the scientific method as the guiding principle. The scientific community conducts research and presents and/or publishes the work in scientifically acceptable venues. There, the review process takes place and sanctions occur, albeit usually at a later time. The researcher is, in essence, saying, ―Look what I’ve done. Take your best shot.‖ This is the process by which science tests itself and reveals errors before they are widely disseminated. In this way, too, science finds scientific fraud and thus is selfcorrecting through the process of peer review, publication, and collegial interactions (e.g., sharing at conferences). Unlike law enforcement, the culture of science encourages and rewards critical questioning of past results and its colleagues. The scientific culture encourages cautious, precise statements and frowns on statements that venture beyond facts; it is acceptable for colleagues to challenge one another, even if the challenger is junior. This is not what happens in the scene investigative arena. The issue is that each scene investigation should be thought of much like a research project. At crime scenes, scene scientists/investigators are working at the frontier of the unknown, of uncertainty. But unlike scientific research, there is no fall-back position and rarely peer review. In fact, most criminal cases have no contemporary review process, no peer review, and thus no mechanism to uncover shoddy, biased, or fraudulent work. The scientific process is the antithesis of what happens in real-life crime scene investigations. Unfortunately, the following are real-life events: Planting evidence at a crime scene to point to a defendant. Collecting evidence without a warrant by claiming exigent circumstances. Ignoring evidence at a scene that might exonerate a suspect. Failing to report a colleague, superior, or subordinate for inappropriate activity. The Scientific Method, Bias, and Reasoning 41 Many types of bias exist to which investigators and scientists are prone. Examples are shown in Table 2.4. Forensic examples were added to illustrate relevance. With respect to bias in the forensic sciences, which is equally true for crime scene investigation, Risinger and Saks [6] wrote, When everyone from Nobel prize winners to average citizens . . . take steps to make sure their judgments are not distorted by extraneous context information, then it is hard to conceive of what it is that makes forensic scientists think they are immune from the same effects. (p. 51) Of particular relevance is that these ideas with respect to crime scene investigation are not new. May wrote in 1936 [7], and Cooley and Turvey [8] reported later, . . . often the most significant bit of evidence is overlooked or misinterpreted because someone has jumped to a premature conclusion.... To face a crime with an open mind—a mind willing to believe and disbelieve even its own senses, sometimes willing to admit and desert one lie of investigation for another, is one of the most difficult tasks of the detective. (p. 71) Uncertainty is the breeding ground for bias and two factors are at work: ambiguity and subjectivity. In the former, if the evidence or the circumstances are incomplete, murky, or equivocal, the scene scientist—or the crime scene investigator—is free to develop opinions, which can represent the analyst’s personal bias, for example, slanting interpretations toward the prosecution’s view of a case or searching for scene evidence based on an arrest or a colleague’s description of the assailant. When subjectivity is the issue, scene interpretations rest on an examiner’s experience or beliefs which results in bias. Prejudices and biases occur during decision-making situations where the decisions represent a gray area, typically where there is ambiguity and subjectivity [8,9]. In black-and-white situations, decisions are typically straightforward and easy: Where do we cordon off the broadcast and print media during the investigation? Who on the team is the spokesperson for the media? Where is the command center? When should we look for fingerprints? Such groundball questions have definite answers. Table 2.4 Forensically Important Categories of Bias Bias Name Bandwagon effect (also groupthink, herd behavior, manias) Bias Description Forensic Example Tendency to do or believe things because many other people do The prosecutor or detectives are certain a suspect is the killer because he is a ―bad guy.‖ At the scene, this knowledge could sway the course of the investigation especially if the scene investigators know who the ―bad guy‖ is. Confirmation effects The tendency to search for or interpret information in a way that confirms one’s preconceptions The crime scene investigator who has a preconceived idea of what happened or who committed the crime and searches the scene to uncover evidence to support that notion. Observer-expectancy effect (also observer effects, context effects, expectancy effects) The expectation of a given result with the unconscious manipulation of an experiment or misinterpretation of data in order to find it An analyst expects a result but does not get it, and then creates scenarios so that the correct results are not obtained. The crime scene investigator ignores evidence that does not fit a specific theory of the case. 40 Crime Scene Forensics Some decisions are not straightforward, however: What does the bloodstain spatter pattern on a wall mean? What technique will best visualize latent prints on a door knob— super glue fuming or dusting with powders? How should we enhance a wet residue footwear impression? Can we lift it? Is it really a wet residue print? How do we know? Which enhancement chemicals should we use? Should we do it at the scene or should we try to collect it? What is the bullet path through the car? Where was the shooter standing? No one corners the market on making bad decisions, certainly not scene scientists, forensic scientists, or crime scene investigators. Everyone has biases and everyone makes decisions based upon them. But bias does not belong at crime scenes or in the forensic laboratory. Thus, it must be controlled. The first example comes not from a crime scene investigation but from what has been termed the ―Gold Standard‖ [10] of forensic science—DNA. 2.3.1 Case Example 1 An example of ambiguity involved a decision made with respect to the reporting of DNA statistics. The deceased was found naked on his bedroom floor. The crime scene unit (laboratory scientists) found a hair on the body of the deceased, and the laboratory’s microscopic analysis reported that the hair was different from that of both the deceased and the defendant. This must have been a blow to the prosecution because it meant that physical evidence found at the scene did not belong to the person the prosecutor had on trial: believed to be a lone killer. The ensuing DNA analysis was critical for the prosecution because, had the DNA conclusively matched either the defendant or the victim, the microscopic analysis would not have mattered. The first analytical decision the laboratory made after the microscopic examination was incorrect. At that time, and at the time of writing this text, the first line of DNA attack after microscopic analysis of hair was to perform mitochondrial DNA typing. Instead, the laboratory chose to analyze for nuclear DNA, the results of which were not a typical DNA match; according to the laboratory, the DNA results matched the victim but the statistics were not stellar: 1 in 493 or almost 1 in 500. Still, the statistics clearly favored the prosecution’s view of the case. A closer look at the statistics showed that the laboratory tailored the statistics to favor the prosecution, whether intentionally or not. Had the DNA calculations been made in an unbiased way, the statistics would have been 1 in 6, a huge difference in determining whose hair it was or was not [11]. The second example is a crime scene example [12]. 2.3.2 Case Example 2 The brutal murder of a 20-year-old woman, presumably by her ex-boyfriend is an example of extreme on-scene bias and incompetence. The prosecutions alleged factual history follows: Ex-boyfriend breaks down glass door to the house to come after his exgirlfriend, who is dating someone else. He grabs a chef’s knife from a knife holder in the kitchen and chases his exgirlfriend to an upstairs bathroom. He butchers the ex-girlfriend, slashing her head and throat with the knife. He stabs himself in the chest and leaves the bathroom, goes down the stairs and falls at the foot of the stairs next to the kitchen where he remains until found by the police. The Scientific Method, Bias, and Reasoning 41 The ex-girlfriend’s sister calls her father who calls the police. The father is first on the scene. He finds his daughter and then the exboyfriend. He begins kicking the ex-boyfriend in the head. One of the officers on the scene is the uncle of the deceased. He does not leave the scene. The father is still at the scene. The on-scene investigation is incomplete, presumably because the police conclude that the ex-boyfriend is the murderer. In addition to the above, the on-scene work was shoddy or simply not considered important since the ―actor‖ was in custody. They photographed evidence of the scene without scales and without using a tripod. Most photographs were taken at an angle. Bloody footprints in the bathroom were not photographed properly nor were they enhanced (see Figure 2.1). Bloody footprints in the hallway outside the bathroom and in the kitchen were not enhanced. No on-scene comparisons were made of the class characteristics of the bloodstained footwear impression prints to the shoes of the deceased, the exboyfriend, or the father. No fingerprinting was performed at the scene. The ex-boyfriend’s clothing and shoes were not examined at the scene or by the crime laboratory for glass shards. The bloodstain spatter patterns in the bathroom were not interpreted. A bloody knife found in the bathroom was processed by the police latent print laboratory using Amido black. Stained friction ridge detail is observed, but the laboratory reports an inability to raise latent prints (see Figure 2.2). BPA of the staining on the knife does not support the contention that the exboyfriend stabbed himself after murdering the ex-girlfriend. Figure 2.1 Bloodstained footprint at scene. (Photograph by Robert C Shaler) 42 Crime Scene Forensics Figure 2.2 Amido black stained bloody knife handle. (Photograph by Robert C. Shalerj So how can biases be eliminated or at least minimized at the crime scene? One way is to change the existing crime scene paradigm (see Chapter 1). The reason is that science is self-correcting because of the inherent challenges built into the scientific method and the need for peer review. Such a process does not exist during or after a crime scene investigation. However, if each crime scene is considered a research project unto itself, the selfcorrecting process must be completed within a timeframe dictated by the case situation. Certainly investigators can and do return to crime scenes, but unlike scientific research, where additional experiments can be run, when the crime scene is released, it is finished. How does one integrate the self-correcting aspect of science into the scene investigation process? Science does it by challenging hypotheses developed by the scientific method and by peer review. Challenging investigative hypotheses at a crime scene is difficult because scene investigators are usually from the same agency, and attempting this may require challenging a supervisor. In a paramilitary organization such as the police, this can lead to professional suicide. The ideal is a team leader who is willing and open to having intellectual discussions concerning alternative hypotheses during the investigation. Hopefully, the discussion would be openly challenging. Theoretically, this ideal is certainly possible, and in some jurisdictions it may well happen. How well it works will depend on the team leader because the personality of each team is different, and each team leader has different relationships with subordinates. The openness of such discussions depends on the ego, self-esteem, and self-assuredness of the team leader. If the team leader’s ego and/or decisions are not at least open for discussion, his/her evolving emotions may get in the way of a competent investigation, as the team leader’s opinions and ideas will become the final arbiter in how the investigation proceeds. Figure 2.3 illustrates how a meaningful investigation can be severely compromised. The team leader is not open to suggestions that might call into question his authority or his ego. The dark arrows indicate closed or minimally open lines of communication. In these situations, opposing hypotheses may not be seriously considered or might even be considered a threat to the team leader’s authority, even though the purpose is to solve the crime. This emotional blockage of information between the team leader and subordinates is illustrated in Figure 2.3. The Scientific Method, Bias, and Reasoning 45 Team leader Hypotheses Subordinate investigator Figure 2.3 Truncated information flow between subordinates and team leader. However, a scientist at the scene can change the dynamics of the team’s personality. The scientific presence automatically brings a sense of objectivity that should not be challenged. Certainly there will be personality conflicts and differences in authority, but the scientist should not be the team leader’s subordinate. In time, conflicts will be minimized as scientists and investigators establish a healthy working relationship. Individual team members might feel apprehensive about challenging their superior, but the scientist should not, as the palm print example in Chapter 1 illustrated. When the scientist enters the investigative arena, Figure 2.3 changes and should appear more like Figure 2.4. The green arrows show open lines of communication. The yellow arrows represent a partially open communication path. The dark arrows, as in Figure 2.3, indicate minimal effective communication, such as the team leader instructing subordinates with respect to the scene investigative process. The subordinate-to-team leader openness might still be truncated, but the scientist and the team leader should have an open dialogue. In fact, the scientific presence can open lines of communication such that an open forum can occur among all scene investigators: scientist, team leader, and subordinates. Certainly this is the ideal. It opens the possibility that the scene will be processed as it should: as a research project amid uncertainty and chaos. Figure 2.4 Lines of communication among team members. Information flow Criminalist Criminalist Hypotheses Hypotheses Team leader Hypotheses Subordinate investigator Team leader Hypotheses Subordinate investigator 44 Crime Scene Forensics The forgoing underscores the self-correcting, bias-mitigating capabilities of the scientific approach in crime scene investigations. Without a scientific presence at the scene, the investigation is prone to bias, error, and subjectivity, none of which has a place in an environment in which ambiguity and uncertainty prevail. Since many if not most scene investigations have no contemporaneous peer review process, the scientific approach is the only way to protect the integrity of the investigation. 2.4 Reasoning There is little doubt that a form of reasoning takes place during any crime scene investigation. And since crime scene investigation is a scientific process and the direct application of the scientific method, there must be logic and reasoning. Is it a deductive or an inductive process? To explore these possibilities, definitions are in order. Deductive reasoning works from the general to the more specific and is often referred to as a ―top-down‖ process of logic. It begins with a theory and moves to a testable hypothesis. Testing or verification requires observations or experiments that address the hypothesis which is ultimately either confirmed or not. This feels like what happens at crime scenes. Concerning deductive criminal profiling, Turvey says [13], The advantages of the Deductive Criminal Profiling model are very important. This model requires specialized education and training in forensic science, crime scene reconstruction, and wound pattern analysis. . . . Deductive Criminal Profiles tend to be more specific than Inductive Criminal Profiles, assisting greatly in the major goal of the profiling process, which is to move from a universal set of suspect characteristics to a more unique set of suspect characteristics. Inductive reasoning begins with an observation and moves to a testable tentative hypothesis, which is more open-ended. This also feels like what happens at crime scenes. From there, a theory is derived, which must be confirmed. In short, it moves from the specific to the general and is thought of as a ―bottom-up‖ process of logic or reasoning. Inductive criminal profiling, Turvey [13] says, is: The process of profiling criminal behavior, crime scenes, and victims from the known behaviors and emotions suggested by other criminals, crime scenes, and/or victims. In essence, as the term suggests, this is reasoning from initial statistical data to specific criminal offender behavior. In any event, Inductive Criminal Profiling is generally the result of some kind of statistical analysis, or finds it’s reasoning in cases outside of the case at hand. Both deductive and inductive reasoning are illustrated in Figure 2.5. Crime scene investigations require both types of reasoning simultaneously. And by examining the above closely, it certainly appears that an overlap exists. It might be possible to assemble both into a singular concept that continuously cycles from theories to observations and back again to theories. In fact, researchers often observe patterns in data that lead to the development of new theories—an inductive process. All branches of natural science use inductive reasoning and establish theories and laws of nature based on observation and on deductive reasoning to draw conclusions. There are circumstances where observations come first. One might observe certain facts that form a pattern that eventually leads to tentative hypotheses, which are tested to form The Scientific Method, Bias, and Reasoning 45 Theory Hypothesis Observation Confirmation Theory Tentative hypothesis Pattern Observation Figure 2.5 Illustration of deductive and inductive reasoning. the basis of a theory. Still, the theory must be confirmed, which would be abduction. Gudwin wrote [14], We propose understanding abduction not as the process of anomaly detection (which would be deduction), neither as the process that generates hypothesis (which would be induction) but as the test selection of those hypotheses. In this context, confirmation is a process that invokes another type of reasoning— abductive reasoning. A combined process of inductive, deductive, and abductive reasoning appropriate for crime scene investigation is illustrated in Figure 2.6. From a practical perspective, does defining how reasoning fits the crime scene investigation help? Many believe the process is intuitive and subject to experience. The truth is that reasoning is applied but likely not as a conscious thought process. The scientist does this as a matter of education, training, and experience. Investigators—the good ones—do it as a matter of intuition, training, and experience. Figure 3.7 (Chapter 3) is solely academic and illustrates the intellectual mechanism of how crime scene reasoning progresses. As these mental processes are at work at the crime scene, there is little doubt that the investigation is not a scientific endeavor. Thus, the scientific method is the best tool to ensure a successful scene investigation. As Sherlock Holmes said [15], In solving a problem of this sort, the grand thing is to be able to reason backward. That is a very useful accomplishment, and a very easy one, but people do not practice it much. In the everyday affairs of life it is more useful to reason forward, and so the other comes to be neglected. There are fifty who can reason synthetically for one who can reason analytically. (p. 83) The Scientific Method, Bias, and Reasoning 45 Fi gu re 2. 6 Observation Pattern Tentative hypothesis Theory Ind uct ive /de du cti ve/ ab Abductive reasoning du cti ve Confirmation rea so nin g as applied in crime scene investigations. References 1. S. Sambursky (ed.) 1974. Physical Thought from the Presocratics to the Quantum Physicists . UK: Pica Press, p. 139. 2. W. J. Chisum and B. Turvey. 2007. Practice standards for the reconstruction of crime. In: W. J. Chisum and B. E. Turvey (eds), Crime Reconstruction. New York, NY: Elsevier Academic Press, p. 92. 3. M. R. Cohen. 1931 [1921]. Reason and Nature: An Essay on the Meaning of Scientific Method, 3rd printing edn. 4. N. W. Edmund. The Scientific Method Today. Available at: http://www.scientificmethod.com/ index.html (accessed February 7, 2009). 5. I. Dror and R. Rosenthal. 2008. Meta-analytically quantifying the reliability and biasability of forensic experts. Journal of Forensic Sciences , 53(4):900– 903. Available at: http://www. cci-hq.com/Dror_JFS_expert_reliability_and_biasability.pdf (accessed February 8, 2009). 6. D. M. Risinger, M. J. Saks, W. C. Thompson, and R. Rosenthal. 2002. The Daubert/Kumho implications of observer effects in forensic science: hidden problems of expectation and suggestion. California Law Review, 90(1):1–56. 7. L. S. May. 1936. Crime Nemesis. New York, NY: The Macmillan Company. 8. C. M. Cooley and B. E. Turvey. 2007. Observer effects and examiner bias: psychological influences on the forensic examiner. In: W. J. Chisum and B. E. Turvey (eds), Crime Reconstruction. New York, NY: Elsevier Academic Press, pp. 58–60, 71. 9. V. J. Phillips and M. Saks. 2001. The application of signal detection theory to decision making in forensic science. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 46:294–298. 10. M. J. Saks and J. J. Koehler. 2005. The coming paradigm shift in forensic identification science. Science, 309:892–895. 11. Testimony of Robert C. Shaler in State of New York v. Daniel Pelosi. 12. Commonwealth of Pennsylvania v. Frederick Cleveland. 13. B. E. Turvey. 1998. Deductive criminal profiling: Comparing applied methodologies between inductive and deductive criminal profiling techniques. Knowledge Solutions Library , Epub. Available at: http://www.corpus-delicti.com/Profiling_law.html 14. R. R. Gudwin. On the generalized deduction, induction and abduction as the elementary reasoning operators within computational semiotics. Available at: ftp://ftp.dca.fee.unicamp. br/pub/docs/gudwin/publications/isas98_3.pdf (accessed February 9, 2009). 15. A. C. Doyle. 1887. A Study in Scarlet, Part 2, Chapter 7, p. 83.