Submission to the Society of Energy Professionals and the Power Workers’ Union Comment on the Canadian Nuclear Safety Commission discussion paper Fitness for duty: Proposals for strengthening alcohol and drug policy, programs and testing By Scott Macdonald August 10, 2012. My background Attached is my curriculum vitae, which provides a summary of various accomplishments over my career. My educational background includes degrees in Psychology (BSc, University of Victoria), Criminology (MA, University of Toronto) and Epidemiology and Biostatistics (PhD, University of Western Ontario). In 1978, I started work as a Research Assistant at the Addiction Research Foundation in Toronto. In 1985, I became a Scientist with responsibility for independent research. I remained at the Addiction Research Foundation (which later became the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health) until 2005, when I took on a new position as Assistant Director at the Centre for Addictions Research of BC and Associate Professor, School of Health Information Science, University of Victoria. In 2009, I was promoted to a Professor. At all these positions, I conducted social epidemiological research related to substance use and abuse. A primary focus of my research has been on the relationships between substance use and injuries. These studies include injuries/accidents in the workplace and crashes. I have over 20 peer-reviewed publications on this subject and 8 peer-reviewed grants and contracts, specifically on this topic. I have considerable expertise in drug testing programs, which includes 18 published works, a peer reviewed grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and contracts with the International Labour Office and Transport Canada. As well, I have conducted program evaluation studies of workplace programs. Social epidemiology is the study of the causes of diseases that have a social dimension. This discipline focuses on assessing the distribution and determinants of health-related states or events, including injuries, in populations and recommending approaches to alleviate these health problems (Last, 1995). A major purpose of epidemiology is to collect and analyze data that provide an accurate assessment of risks associated with diseases. Analytic epidemiology involves the assessment of risks of diseases from various exposures – for example, the risk of job accidents from substance use. 2 An emphasis of epidemiology is on methodological issues in the collection of data that could potentially bias findings, such as: • Measurement error – Are the instruments/scales valid and reliable and measure what we wish to measure? • Internal validity – Does the study design allow strong inferences of cause and effect? • External validity – Do the results of the study apply to the “real world”? • Selection bias – Does the study have systematic differences in how data was collected in the cases (e.g. injury subjects) and control (e.g. noninjury subjects) that may inflate or deflate the estimates of risks associated with exposure (e.g. alcohol or drug use)? • Confounders – Are there other third variables that may better explain the findings? • Threats to internal validity (i.e. the degree to which we can make strong inferences between cause and effect). Threats to internal validity represent various explanations of how the results of a study might not be internally valid, based on the designs. These include potential flaws, such as selection (differences found are due to fundamental differences in the group), history (competing interventions caused the change), experimenter bias, testing effects (both practice effects and Hawthorne effect) and statistical regression. Introduction This report contains my opinions of the discussion paper document in response to requests from Sonia Pylyshyn at the Society of Energy Professionals and Emily Lawrence, Counsel for the Power Workers' Union. I was asked to comment on different 3 drug testing approaches - specifically their accuracy and what they show. I was also asked to report on the scientific literature on drug use and safety risk, issues of deterrence, and evaluation studies of drug testing (with emphasis on job accidents). Finally, I was asked to comment on studies showing possible negative consequences of drug testing and alternatives to drug testing. In this review, I will first examine the accuracy, strengths and limitation of different biochemical approaches for determining drug use (Section 1). In the following section, research on the acute effects of different drugs will be briefly reviewed (Section 2). Next, will be an analysis of studies that address the issue of whether drug users represent a safety risk in the workplace (Section 3). Then the question of whether drug testing is a deterrent is addressed (Section 4). In the final sections, studies on the effect of drug testing on safety (Section 5), potential negative consequences of testing (Section 6) and alternative approaches (Section 7) are addressed. The report culminates in recommendations with respect to the Nuclear Commissions discussion document. My main conclusions are as follows. The discussion document contains recommendations that will not achieve the stated goal of identifying individuals who are unfit for duty. Commonly used biochemical drug tests cannot identify whether employees are under the influence of a drug at the time of the test and therefore cannot be used to identify those unfit for duty. As well, drug testing has not been scientifically shown to improve work safety. In most Canadian workplaces, drug use at work represents an insignificant problem. 1. Strengths and limitations of different drug testing approaches. An important issue is the accuracy of the drug tests in detecting the drugs in question. Sensitivity and specificity are terms often used to describe the accuracy of individual tests. Sensitivity refers to the proportion of people who test positive divided by the number of true drug positives. Specificity refers to the proportion of people who test negative divided by the number of true drug negatives. Potential errors that can occur are false positives (indicating an individual has used a drug but he/she has not) or false 4 negatives (indicating an individual has NOT used a drug but he/she has). These types of errors are typically determined using studies, where drug use of individuals is known, and compared against results from drug testing. Two major analytic approaches Two main analytic approaches exist for assessing drug use: (1) immunoassay or (2) mass spectrometry in combination with gas/liquid chromatography (MS/GC). Immunoassay screens use antibodies to detect the presence of a drug or its metabolites (Dyer & Wilkinson, 2008). Immunoassays lack accuracy and are unable to definitively identify the presence or absence of a specific drug, hence their use as a screening device only (Dyer & Wilkinson, 2008). Although false negatives are more common, false positives can also occur where a molecule with a similar structure to the drug of interest is present (Dyer & Wilkinson, 2008). Most immunoassay tests are conducted in laboratory conditions and therefore results are not immediate. An exception is point of collection tests, an immunoassay approach that can provide findings of drug use (typically from saliva) within 10 minutes. There is approximately 12 point of collection tests on the market but none provide a high degree of accuracy (Dyer & Wilkinson, 2008). In a recent study, Blencowe et al. (2011) compared the accuracy of the point of collection tests with laboratory methods using MS/GC. While accuracy varied across devices, none of the tests had perfect sensitivity for any drug. The average sensitivity for amphetamines was 60% and specificity was 97%. Accuracy was particularly poor for cannabis at 73%, with average sensitivity of 38% and specificity of 95%. For cocaine, these tests had excellent specificity of close to 100%, but average sensitivity across all tests was only 36%. The gold standard of drug tests is MS/GC - considered near perfect for drug detection and identification (Jaffee et al, 2008) and can be conducted with any type of biological sample. This approach requires sophisticated laboratory equipment. Immunoassay screens are often used as a screening device with confirmation of positive tests with MS/GC. However, the immunoassay approach with confirmation with MS/GC still likely results in some false negatives, as confirmation tests will only be 5 conducted on samples that initially test positive with immunoassay screens (known for low sensitivity). Since the accurate testing procedures require laboratory conditions, chain of custody procedures are required to ensure that biological samples are not adulterated or misplaced. Proper quality control involves documentation of specimen collection to the final report (Normand et al., 1995; p.184-190). Positive drug tests using MS/GC still can result from licit sources. For example, it is possible that a positive test for morphine could be due to certain poppy seed cakes (Cone and Huestis, 2007). A positive test for cannabis could be due to pharmaceutical THC (Sativex) (Cone and Huestis, 2007) or over the counter hemp oil products (U.S Department of Transportation, 2004). Both drug testing methods have limitations as in most instances (described under biological specimens) cannot be used to determine whether individuals are under the influence of the drug or have a drug problem. Different biological samples Different types of biological samples can be used to test for alcohol and drugs. For the detection of drugs, hair, urine, saliva or blood can be used. Urine testing is the most common approach used in Canadian workplaces, and more recently saliva testing is being used. Hair testing is used by some employers the United States to detect prior drug over a long time frame (months) but since it is not common in Canadian workplaces, it will not be reviewed here. For alcohol detection, breathalyzer tests are most common. In this section, first the general strengths and limitations of urine and saliva testing will be reviewed. Since the Commission’s recommendations describe fitness for duty as impairment due to the presence of alcohol and/or drugs, in the next section, comparisons are drawn between normal periods where people are under the influence of different drugs and the detection windows for these drugs with urine or saliva tests. Finally, the strengths and limitations of breath and blood tests are reviewed. Urine tests detect inert metabolites associated with ingestion of each drug being tested and not the psychoactive ingredients of each drug. These metabolites are 6 eliminated at different rates, depending on the drug and usage (Moeller, Lee, & Kissack, 2008). For each drug, the detection periods for urine far exceed the periods for which individuals could be considered under the influence and in every case, exceed the detection times for saliva tests (with the exception of PCP where no studies were found). Impairment, dependence, or frequency of use cannot be determined by urine tests (Kapur, 1994). The most common procedure in the workplace is to first conduct a immunoassay screen, and positive tests are verified with MS/GC procedures. Urine tests are invasive because visual monitoring of urination is needed to prevent adulteration of the samples. Saliva tests for drugs detect the psychoactive components (i.e. the parent drug), with the exception of cocaine where both the parent drug and inert metabolite can be detected (Drummer, 2006). Although saliva tests detect the psychoactive components of the drug, conclusions of impairment cannot be made based on test results (Kadehjian, 2005; Dyer and Wilkinson, 2008). A positive saliva test can only be used to determine prior use of drugs within certain detection periods that vary, depending on the drug. The detection time (i.e. the time from last use to the test) for all drugs will vary among individuals regardless of the testing technology, depending on the dose taken, frequency of use, route of administration, duration of use, tolerance, metabolic rate and cut-off levels of the tests (Verstraete, 2004; Dyer and Wilkinson, 2008). Saliva tests can also be used to detect alcohol levels within two hours after consumption, which closely correlate to those in blood (Center for Substance Abuse Treatment, 2006). However, since the oral cavity is easily contaminated; the time window for detecting ethanol in saliva is short (Center for Substance Abuse Treatment, 2006) and saliva tests are rarely used for alcohol. Comparisons of time periods for being under the influence of various drugs and the detection windows for urine and saliva tests. Table 1, column 1, shows the time periods in which a person consuming the drug under typical conditions would normally be considered under the influence of each drug. For smoking cannabis, the maximum period of being under the influence is estimated to be about 4 hours (Grotenhermen, 2003; Ramaekers et al., 2004). Typically, a peak high 7 is experienced within 30 minutes which lasts approximately 2 hours Grotenhermen, 2003). After eating the drug, a peak high typically is noted between 1 to 3 hours which declines to low levels within 6 hours (Grotenhermen, 2003). For snorting cocaine, a high will typically last 15 to 30 minutes and for oral ingestion about 1 to 2 hours (U.S. Department of Transportation, 2004). For amphetamines, typical effects last 4 to 8 hours, with residual effects possible up to 12 hours (U.S. Department of Transportation, 2004). Peak effects for morphine occur under 60 minutes, and residual effects up to 6 hours; whereas for heroin, peak effects occur under 2 hours and overall effects dissipate after 5 hours (U.S. Department of Transportation, 2004). Column 2, Table 1, shows the detection periods after last use for saliva tests. Detection periods with saliva tests vary, depending on the reference source and other variables, such as the cut-off values and the analyte being targeted. Regardless of reported differences in maximum detection times among authors, the detection period with saliva tests is longer than the period that individuals can be considered under the influence for every drug. For cannabis, the detection period from last use is about 34 hours (Verstraete, 2004). Dyer and Wilkinson (2008) differentiate between a single use (4 to 14 hours) and chronic use (4 to 30 hours); however, test results cannot be used to differentiate occasional from heavy users. For amphetamines, the detection time indicated noted by Verstraete is 20 to 50 hours, whereas for Dyer and Wilkinson it is less than 24 hours. Also, differences exist for cocaine, depending on whether the parent drug (5 to 12 hours) or metabolite (12 to 24 hours for benzoylecgonine) is being detected (Verstraete, 2004). Research on detection times for PCP is not available (Cone and Huestis, 2007). The tests are a poor instrument for assessing workplace performance. Column 3 of Table 1 shows the detection periods for urine tests. For every drug, the detection periods for urine exceed those of saliva tests. Breath tests (i.e. breathalyzers) can only be used to detect alcohol. They measure the concentration of ethanol in the breath, which is highly correlated to the level of ethanol in the blood (Drummond & Ghodse, 1999). The advantages of using breathalyzers are that they provide rapid results, are non-invasive, and provide accurate information on impairment by alcohol (Drummond & Ghodse, 1999). Sources of error in 8 breathalyzer use include failure to follow manufacturer’s instructions for regular calibration and ensuring that alcohol has not been consumed within the last 15 minutes (including alcohol-containing mouthwashes), smoking cigarettes or exposure to certain types of paints (Drummond & Ghodse, 1999). Aside from these potential limitations, false positives are unlikely (Hannuksela et. al., 2007). Blood tests are another approach that can be used for the detection of drugs; however, they are invasive. There currently is no accepted relationship between drug concentrations in the blood (alcohol excepted) and degree of impairment. Preliminary consensus cut-off levels in the blood for cannabis impairment have been prepared by a consensus group (Grotenhermen et al, 2007), but there are no internationally accepted standards. For other drugs, no accepted standards for impairment levels have been proposed. 2. The acute effects of drugs Experimental laboratory studies have been conducted to understand how the level of drugs in the human body is related to performance. The acute effects of drugs (i.e. observed when the psychoactive ingredients are present) are best assessed in experimental studies by measuring the performance of subjects randomly assigned to drug and no drug conditions. A clear conclusion drawn from a multitude of laboratory studies is that the pharmacological properties and effects on psychomotor performance for the drugs vary considerably. Research shows the acute effects of some drugs (i.e. cannabis, PCP and opiates) can negatively affect performance, such as the ability to operate equipment (McKim, 1986). Some improvement in cognitive skills, such as visual spatial and attention has been noted with methamphetamines (Hart et al., 2012). An important limitation of experimental studies is that the results may not be applicable to real world conditions; that is, the tasks used in a laboratory setting may not reflect activities conducted in the workplace. For example, most laboratory studies measure the effects of a drug on peak performance as opposed to typical performance. In some studies participants are asked to drive around an obstacle course as quickly as possible and deficits have been observed. In real driving situations these deficits might not be severe enough to increase collision risk because people do not need to drive as fast as 9 possible. Alternatively, those who use drugs may be unlikely to use during activities such as driving or while at work. 3. Do drug users represent a safety risk in the workplace? Given that drug tests are only useful for detecting drug users (as opposed to detecting impairment), one question posed by many is whether drug users are more likely to have accidents and injuries than non-users. Two main types of epidemiological studies have been conducted to answer this question: (1) those comparing accidents or collisions of people who test positive for drugs versus those who test negative; and (2) those comparing accident rates of people who self-report drug use versus those who do not. These studies have been conducted on two population groups, employees and drivers. Conclusions from these studies are summarized below. Studies of employees Some epidemiological studies were found that compared on the job accident rates of employees who tested positive versus negative for drug use. Only one study was found where those testing positive had significantly higher accident or injury rates than negatives (Zwerling et al., 1990). In this study, U.S. postal workers who tested positive for marijuana were significantly more likely to have reportable accidents and work injuries. Those who tested positive for cocaine had significantly more injuries, but no statistical difference was found for job accidents. After two years of follow-up, risks of adverse outcomes declined among drug positives (Ryan et al., 1992). The authors acknowledge a limitation of their study is that alcohol use associated with drug use was not investigated. In addition, the results may not be replicable in other countries or for other types of workers. Other studies failed to show a relationship between positive drug tests and accidents or injuries. In a longitudinal study of 5,465 job applicants, Normand et al (1990) found no significant differences between drug positives and negatives in terms of job injuries. Similarly, Crouch et al (1989) found that drug positives did not have higher rates of job accidents than drug negatives. Overall, there is not enough research 10 evidence to conclude that those who test positive for drugs represent an increased risk for work injuries or accidents, which is a fundamental requirement of causation. Some studies have compared job injuries of drug users and non-users as determined through self-reports. A U.S. study of 1,740 employed adults indicated that drug users were 1.7 times more likely to be involved in injuries on the job (Hingson et al., 1985). More recently, Shipp et al. (2005) found the odds of workplace injury increased with the frequency of drug use for marijuana and cocaine. By contrast Hoffman and Larison (1999) did not find a relationship between occupational injuries and use of either marijuana or cocaine. In a survey of Ontario adults using similar methods, those who used either illicit or licit drugs in the prior year were significantly more likely to have job injuries than non-users (Macdonald, 1995). However, additional analyses controlling for potential confounding variables, pointed away from illicit drug use as a causal agent in job injuries. The existence of a statistical relationship between drug use and job injuries should not be interpreted that drug use causes job accidents/injuries. For example, illicit drug use is related to age and sleep problems, which in turn are significantly related to job injuries (Macdonald et al., 2009). Other researchers have suggested that risk taking behaviour is a plausible explanation for the relationship between drug use and accidents; we do not know whether drug users might have elevated rates of job accidents because they use drugs or because they tend to be risk takers, a characteristic also related to job accidents (Newcomb, 1994; Spicer, Miller & Smith, 2003). In summary, some studies found drug users have a higher risk of job accidents, while other studies failed to find significant relationships. All studies failed to show that drug use was a cause of workplace accidents, as statistical relationships between drug users and accidents/injuries might be explained by other variables. One study suggested the relationship between drug use and job injuries could be explained by other factors correlated with drug use, such as sleep problems, smoking, shift work, and age (Macdonald, 1994). In most studies, several drugs with different pharmacological properties were grouped into one category (presumably to increase statistical power); 11 consequently, very little is known about specific drugs (see Hingson et al., 1985, Normand et al., 1990; Crouch et al., 1989). The methods and operational definitions of drug use used varied considerably, making it difficult to meaningfully compare the results of the studies. To assess causality, the strength and significance of a statistical relationship between positive tests and job accidents/injuries should first be determined. This is typically accomplished by calculating odds ratios (OR) where the proportion of those who test positive in the accident/injury group is compared to the proportion in a noninjury control group. Factors such as sex, age or risk taking propensity (called confounders), should be ruled out as possible explanations for the results found. Confounders are typically addressed through matching of cases and controls in the design of a study, or through stratification or multivariate statistics in the analysesprocedures rarely used in the drug testing and job accident research. Studies of drivers Such studies can be found in the traffic safety literature, and are reviewed below. These studies have failed to find a relationship between positive tests results and collisions. The best methodological studies using oral fluid or urinalysis techniques to test drivers are analytic epidemiological studies that utilize either the case-control method (Ferrara et al., 1994; Marquet et al., 1998) or methods used to ascribe crash responsibility (Longo et al., 2000ab). Statistical significance was found in only one study with one drug - cannabis (Marquet et al., 1998). A study by Longo et al (2000ab) used drug tests of 2,500 injured drivers. Blood samples were analyzed to determine the concentration of each drug present and culpability for the crashes was assessed. The authors did not find that drivers who tested positive for cannabis were any more likely to be culpable for their crash than drug-free drivers. Many of the studies cited above also investigated whether the presence of cocaine metabolites was significantly related to the likelihood of crashes; but none found a statistical relationship. Thus epidemiological studies have failed to demonstrate a statistical relationship between drug positives and driver related 12 collisions, a fundamental requirement of causation. One reason for the failure to find significance could be the inaccuracy of the tests, which cannot detect impairment by drugs. In stark contrast to these studies, analytic epidemiological studies on alcohol impairment and collisions are persuasive that alcohol is a cause of collisions (see Macdonald, 1997 for a summary of this evidence). 4. Is drug testing a deterrent for drug use and drug related job accidents? Quest diagnostics is the largest provider of drug testing in the United States and conducted about 6.4 million tests in 2011 (Quest Diagnostics Incorporated, 2012). The proportion of employees who test positive has dropped considerably from 13.6% in 1988 to 3.5% in 2011. This decline might be interpreted as an indication of a deterrent effect of drug use among employees in companies where testing is conducted. However, other explanations are possible. As noted by Walsh (2008), there has been an exponential growth of alteration in substitution products developed to produce negative results, which might help to explain the decline. The most experienced users may be the most likely to use such methods. Although overall research indicates that companies that drug test generally produce a lower proportion of positive tests over time, this does not mean that fewer people are impaired by drugs at work. If some employees change their drug using behaviours, the intervention may have a greater effect on recreational users, who are unlikely to use at work. Testing does not appear to have had any impact on overall drug use in the US population. US surveys indicate a 30% increase in drug use between 1988 and 2004 (Walsh, 2008). Various explanations are possible for this discrepancy. First, employees who use drugs may choose to work in companies that do not test for drug usage. The lower rate of positive tests for preemployment testing (3.5%) is suggestive that companies that drug test may deter prospective employees from applying to vacant openings. Research has not shown that the decline in positive rates corresponds to safer workplaces. One approach that could be used to assess the likely impact on workplace safety is to compare the percentage of employees who test positive from random testing 13 to those who test positive from post-accident testing. If drug use is a major cause of workplace accidents then one would expect that the percentage of employees testing positive after accidents would be much higher than those from random tests. From 2009 to 2011, 5.3% of the US employees tested positive with post-accident testing (Quest Diagnostics Incorporated, 2012). This compares with random testing at 5.4% in 2009, 5.3% in 2010, and 5.2% in 2011. Given that the percentages are nearly identical suggests that the causal impact of drugs on workplace accidents is negligible. The aforementioned evidence does not indicate that reductions in positive test results over the past decade have translated into fewer job accidents. Although research has shown that per se laws for alcohol (i.e. laws prohibiting driving at certain BAC levels) and strong enforcement have been beneficial in reducing collisions caused by alcohol (Mann et al., 2001), it is not a good analogy to suggest that drug testing will achieve the same result among employees as there are many differences between the two. First, there is a substantial difference between the prevalence of use for alcohol and illicit drugs in our society. For example the most recent Canadian survey shows that 78% of Canadians used alcohol in the past 12 months, which compares to 9.1% for cannabis, .9% for cocaine and the numbers were too small to report for methamphetamine (Health Canada, 2011). Second, these aforementioned percentages represent overall usage and not use at work. Alcohol and drug use in the workplace is much less likely than during leisure hours (Frone, 2006). Finally, the epidemiological research shows a strong link between BAC levels and collision risk. Virtually every large case-control study conducted has shown a strong relation between blood alcohol content (BAC) levels and the likelihood of collision. For example, compared to drivers with BAC = 0, the adjusted relative risk of collisions was 1.4 for drivers with a BAC= 0.05%, 2.7 for drivers with BAC = 0.08%, 22.1 for BAC = 0.15%, and 81.8 for BAC = 0.20% (Bloomberg et al. 2009). By contrast, comparable road research using urinalysis has failed to find any relationship at all (see review of driver studies above). 14 5. Does drug testing improve workplace safety? Many studies of the impact of drug testing on job accidents have methodological limitations (Macdonald, 1997; Kraus, 2001). Kraus (2001) noted that “despite the extensive use of and management support for worksite based drug testing, the published evidence for the effects, such as reduced injury or accident rates lacks scientific detail. Better studies and careful reassessment of the issue appear warranted.” Numerous studies have a methodological limitation in that other safety initiatives were implemented at the same time as a drug testing program. Therefore, the specific impacts of drug testing on injury/job accident rates cannot be separated from these other initiatives. Several studies are illustrative of this flaw. Taggert (1989) reported substantial reductions in job accidents subsequent to drug testing being instituted; however, this study has been severely criticized because major safety improvements occurred at the same time that testing was implemented, making any conclusions unreliable (Jones, 1990). In another study, Ozminkowski et al. (2003) concluded that there was a significant relationship between testing and decreased injury rates, but they did not control for other safety initiatives. Similarly, another study failed to examine the impact of other safety programs and additionally had an extremely low response rate of 17% (Gerber and Yacoubian, 2001). Finally, a study of Peer Care training and random drug testing found reduced injuries (Miller et al., 2007) but the specific effects of drug testing could not be separated from other simultaneous interventions. Additionally, industries that had not introduced drug testing had similar reductions. Jacobson (2003) found a significant reduction in truck fatalities in 13 U.S. states that mandated drug testing compared to other states but there are other plausible explanations for this finding other than drug testing. For example, the states with workplace drug testing were twice as likely to have Employee Assistance Programs compared to non-testing mandated states. In a study by Wickizer et al., (2004) occupational injury rates of companies enrolled in a drug free work program were compared to 20,500 companies without 15 these programs. The drug free programs included formal written substance use policies, drug testing and Employee Assistance programs. Although the authors indicate the drug free program was associated with selective reductions in work injuries, we cannot conclude that the reduction were due to drug testing because the programs included other interventions. Furthermore, companies that enrolled in the program had substantially higher injury rates (21.18 per 100 person years after intervention) than the non-intervention group (13.82 per 100 person years). In another study by Morantz and Mas (2008), occupational injury claims of Divisions of a large retail chain that implemented post-accident drug testing is compared to Divisions without testing before and after program implementation. Although declines were noted for some types of compensation claims in Divisions with testing, the rate of claims was still higher in the testing. Divisions (than non-testing) for 3 of 4 compensation indicators. For example, a rate of 9.63/100,000 hours first aid reports was found in the after period among drug testing Divisions compared with 5.54/100,000 hrs in the non-testing Divisions (see Table 1, p.264). Such a finding raises the issue of regression to the mean, where sites with higher claims rates were chosen for implementation of post-accident testing. Reductions to the level of non-testing Divisions may have occurred without implementation of the testing programs. Furthermore, other studies using a cross-sectional design have been used; however, conclusions drawn from such designs should be considered tentative, due to the existence of possible alternative explanations for the findings. For example, in a study by Carpenter (2007), data from two U.S. national household surveys of 57,397 people was analyzed to assess whether drug testing was related to lower rates of marijuana use. Companies with drug testing programs, employee assistance programs, and written policies on substance use had lower prevalence of drug use among employees than those without these policies. Separation of the unique effects of drug testing from other programs was not possible. Feinauer & Havlovic (1993) found no differences in workplace accidents and injuries at companies with drug testing compared to 36 non-testing businesses. They 16 report significant reductions in accident and illness rates for post-accident testing but no change for reasonable cause testing. A limitation of this study was that data was obtained through a self-administered questionnaire raising issues of both accuracy of the data and possible selection bias (companies with lower incident rates may be more likely to respond). A common practice of employers is to dismiss employees who test positive for drugs. Interestingly, no evaluation study has been located in the literature on the performance of such employees prior to their dismissal compared to matched control subjects. Little research has been conducted that compares the different situations that employees could be tested. As pointed out in the report by the Canadian Nuclear Safety Commission, the courts have found that random alcohol testing is acceptable but random drug testing is not acceptable. No persuasive evidence is provided in the document to indicate potential benefits in terms of workplace safety. In light of the material reviewed in this document, the preponderance of the evidence does not indicate that measurable improvements in safety will be achieved with random testing. One conclusion drawn from existing research is that there is no credible evidence that drug testing programs reduce job accidents. Although some studies have noted reductions of work accidents/injuries following the implementation of drug testing programs, others have not. Methodological flaws are common – in particular, history (competing safety initiative at the same time as the drug testing program may have caused the change), and statistical regression (companies tend to implement drug testing when work accidents/injuries are particularly high) are common. 6. Potential negative impacts of drug testing Few studies exist that investigate the potential negative consequences on employee behaviour or attitudes related to drug testing. Employee drug testing has been theorized to be an instrument of social control, a technique for defining and responding to the use of illegal drugs (Brunet, 2002). Conflict theorists see drug testing is an issue of power rather than one that relates directly to drug use in the workplace 17 (Gerber et al., 1990). Most of the social control literature on drug testing is theoretical, and empirical studies that substantiate these theories could not be found. In experimental studies, subjects had a more positive attitude and intentions toward companies that did not have drug testing programs than ones that did (Crant and Bateman, 1990). A possible impact of testing, given this study is a decline of the total number of job applicants (Macdonald and Wells, 1994). Others have argued that testing can produce declines in employee morale or undermine labour-management relations (see Macdonald and Wells, 1994). Overall, too little research has been conducted to provide conclusions regarding negative impacts of drug testing on employees. 7. Alternative approaches for addressing workplace substance use An approach implemented in Canada for impaired driving by drugs is the Drug Recognition Expert (DRE) Program, also known as the Drug Evaluation Classification (DEC) Program. This program has evolved since initially being developed in the late 1970's by the Los Angeles Police Department, (Beirness et al., 2007; Porath-Waller, et al., 2009). A standardized procedure has been developed to identify both individuals under the influence of drugs, and the type of drug causing the observable impairment (Beirness et al., 2007; Porath-Waller, et al., 2009). The process involves a series of physical and psychomotor tests, and concludes with toxicological testing of a sample of a blood, urine or oral fluid (Beirness et al., 2007; Porath-Waller, et al., 2009). The procedures have been applied to other areas where identification of the drug-impaired individual is desirable (Porath-Waller, 2009). Beirness et al (2007) conducted a review of existing studies evaluating the DRE program. Laboratory studies of subjects administered impairment doses of different drugs produced ranges (depending on the study) in terms of sensitivity of DRE assessments: (cannabis – 30.4 to 53.1%, cocaine – 13.2% (one study), amphetamines – 4.2 to 10%, opiates – 65.4 to 75.9%, PCP – 75.3%) and specificity (cannabis – 59.1 to 86.4%, cocaine – 61.1%, amphetamines – 79.2 to 91%, opiates – 90.3 to 97.9%, PCP – 89.5%) (see Beirness et al., 2007). After initial behvaioural assessments of drug impairments, drug tests can be conducted. The DRE approach is advantageous over 18 drug testing for assessing fitness of duty in that behavioural symptoms consistent with impairment are used rather than prior drug use. In the early 1990’s the Ontario Law Reform Commission (1992) recommended behavioural performance testing, such as computerized tests, to assess psychomotor skills in safety sensitive positions, rather than drug testing. In relation to work, a possible advantage of these tests is they can pick up a wider variety of performance deficits beyond those that are drug related, such as those caused by fatigue, and thus could potentially be more directly linked to overall fitness for work. At that time, a review of computerized behavioural testing approaches was conducted by Butler and Tranter (1994). Behavioral performance tests have been developed to assess various dimensions of human functioning, such as psycho motor skills, memory, divided attention, decision-making and critical tracking, which are often used to assess drug related deficits in laboratory studies. The importance of each dimension for the workplace will depend largely on task requirements and the specific job. The degree to which the test results accurately relate to varied complex requirements of different jobs is not adequately known, with the possible exception of some specialized positions (e.g. airline pilot). Additionally, practice effects with repeated administrations tend to create improvements of scores so the cut-off levels change over time. Another issue is that some individuals do better than others on these tests, necessitating the development of different baseline measures for each person. These variations in standards across and within individuals are inconsistent with the principle of specific requirements for each job. Employee assistance programs (EAPs) are another approach used by a large proportion of Canadian employers to improve employee health and well-being, and to reduce performance problems (Macdonald et al., 2006). A few reviews have been conducted on the effectiveness of EAPs in achieving these objectives (McLeod, 2010; Macdonald et al., 1998, Normand et al., 1994) and all the reviews arrived at similar conclusions. Although definitive conclusions cannot be drawn based on the methodology of the studies in the heterogeneity of EAPs themselves, the 19 preponderance of the evidence suggests that these programs are beneficial in alleviating psychological problems and can have a positive effect on work performance. 8. Overall assessment of the policy proposal In the discussion paper by the Canadian Nuclear Safety Commission, fitness for duty is described as the capacity of employees to effectively perform job duties without risk to their own or others' health and safety. Several components of fitness are described as medical, psychological, occupational fitness and behavioural-performance. With respect to alcohol or drugs, nuclear power plant licensees would be required to take steps to prevent workers from bringing or consuming alcohol or illicit drugs at work, or working while under the influence of alcohol or any drug that impairs their ability to perform his or her duties safely. These goals are reasonable as the acute (i.e. immediate effects) of alcohol and many psycho-active drugs cause decreases in job performance. They further suggest that biochemical substance testing should be used to determine impairment due to the presence of alcohol and/or drugs (Section 3.0). Unfortunately, drug tests with saliva or urine cannot accurately determine whether employees are under the influence during work hours and therefore do not closely measure fitness for work. Furthermore, the drug tests cannot be used to assess whether employees have a substance use problem or dependence. The discussion paper provides no justification of random drug testing and given the research evidence outlined in this report, there is not a strong evidence base to support this recommendation. The exception is the breathalyzer for alcohol, which is an accurate device to assess whether employees are under the influence of alcohol at the time of the test and could be used randomly, if there is a legal justification for doing so. Saliva and urine tests, and the use of random drug testing in particular, will not achieve the goals of assessing fitness for duty in relation to substance use as defined by the Commission. These components do not have a sufficient evidence base to justify their use. 20 Table 1: Typical periods subjects considered under the influence and maximum detection period (or ranges) for saliva and urine tests Drug Under the Influence* (depends on dose and route of admin.) Amphetamine < 12 hours Maximum detection times (ranges) Saliva Amphetamines <24 to < 50 hours Urine Amphetamines < 34 hrs to < 9 days Methamphetamines – Methamphetamines – <24 to <48hr < 6 days MDMA - < 24hr MDMA - < 4 days Cannabis < 4 hours after smoking THC - <30 to <34hr <3 days to <5 weeks (depends on frequency of use) Cocaine < 2 hours < 3 to < 22 days Opiates < 6 hours Cocaine - <8 to <24hr; Benzoylecgonine(metabolite) <24hr Morphine - < 24hr; Heroin - <8 to <24; Codeine - <36hr PCP < 6 hours (smoked, Unknown (Cone & Phencyclidine snorted or orally Huestis, 2007) ingested) 18 hours (injected) 21 Morphine - < 48hr to 11 days; Heroin - < 54hr <10 days References Berghaus, G., & Guo, B.L. (1995). Medicines and driver fitness - findings from a metaanalysis of experimental studies as basic information to patients, physicians and experts. In: Kloeden, C.N., McLean, A.J. (Eds.), Alcohol, Drugs, and Traffic Safety - T’95: proceedings of the 13th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety, Adelaide, pp. 295-300. Bierness, D.J., LeCavalier, J. & Singhal, D. (2007). Evaluation of the drug evaluation and classification program: a critical review of the evidence. Traffic Injury Prevention, 8, 368-376. Blencowe, T., Pehrsson, A., Lillsunde, P., Vimpari, K., Houwing, S., Smink, B., Mathijssen, R., Van der Linden, T., Legrand, S., Pil, K., & Verstraete, A. (2011). An analytical evaluation of eight on-site oral fluid drug screening devices using laboratory confirmation results from oral fluid. Forensic Science International, 208, 173-179. Blomberg RD, Peck RC, Moskowitz H, Burns M, Fiorentino D. The Long Beach/Fort Lauderdale relative risk study. Journal of Safety Research. 2009;40(4):285-92. Butler,B. & Tranter, D. (1994) Behavioral tests to assess performance. In: Macdonald, S. and Roman, P. (Eds). Research Advances in Alcohol and Drug Problems. New York: Plenum Press. Brunet, JR (2002) Employee drug testing as social control. Review of Public Personnel Administration. 22(3): 193-215. A Carpenter C. S. Workplace drug testing and worker drug use. Health Services Research 2007; 42: 795–810. Clarke, J., & Wilson, J.F. (2005). Proficiency testing (external quality assessment) of drug detection in oral fluid. Forensic Science International, 150, 161-164. Coambs, R.B., & McAndrews, M.P. (1994). The effects of psychoactive substances on workplace performance. In: Macdonald, S & Roman, P. (Eds.), Drug testing in the workplace. New York: Plenum Press. Compton, R.P., Blomberg, R.D., Moskowitz, H., Burns, M., Peck, R.C. & Fiorentino, D. (2002). Crash risk of alcohol impaired driving. Proceedings of the 16th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety (CD-ROM). Montreal, Canada. Cone, E., Johnson, R.E., Darwin, W.D., Yousefnejad, D., Mell, L.D., & Paul, B.D. (1987). Passive inhalation of marijuana smoke: urinalysis and room air level of delta-9 tetrahydrocannabinol. Journal of Analytical Toxicology, 11: 89-96. Cone, E.J. (2001). Legal, workplace, and treatment drug testing with alternate biological matrices on a global scale. Forensic Science International, 121, 7-15. 22 Cone, E., Huestis, M. (2007) Interpretation of oral fluid tests for drugs of abuse. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1098, 51-103. Crant, MJ & Bateman, TS (1990) An experimental test of the impact of drug testing programs on potential job applicants’ attitudes and intentions. Journal of Applied Psychology 75(2) : 127-131. Crouch, D., Vebb, D., Peterson, L., Buller, P., & Rollins, D. (1989). A critical evaluation of the Utah Power and Light Company's substance abuse management program: absenteeism accidents, and costs. In: Gust SW, Walsh JM, eds. Drugs in the workplace: research and evaluation data, NIDA Research Monograph, No. 91. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office. Drummer, O.H. (2006). Drug testing in oral fluid. The Clinical Biochemist Reviews, 27, 147-159. Dyer, K.R., & Wilkinson, C. (2008). The detection of illicit drugs in oral fluid: another potential strategy to reduce illicit drug-related harm. Drug and Alcohol Review, 27, 99-107. Feinauer D. M., Haviovic S. J. (1993) Drug testing as a strategy to reduce occupational accidents: a longitudinal analysis.J Safety Res 24: 1–7. Ferrara, S.D., Giorgetti, R., & Zancaner, S. (1994). Psychoactive substances and driving: State of the art and methodology. Alcohol, Drugs and Driving, 10, 1-55. Frone, MR (2006) Prevalence and distribution of illicit drug use in the workforce and in the workplace: Findings and implications from a US national survey. Journal of Applied Psychology 91(4) , 856-869. Gerber J., Yacoubian G. (2001) Evaluation of drug testing in the workplace: study of the construction industry. Journal of Construction Engineering Management 127: 438–44. Gerber, J., Jensen, EL, Schreck, M Babcock, GM (1990) drug testing and social control: Implications for state theory. Contemporary Crisis, 14: 243-258. Grotenhermen, F. (2003) Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of cannabinoids. Clinical Pharmacokinetics, 42(4), 328-360. Grotenherman F., Leson G., Berghaus G., Drummer O., Krüger H., Longo M., et al. Developing limits for driving under cannabis. Addiction 2007; 102: 1910-17. Hart, CL, Marvin, CB, Silver, R & Smith, EE (2012) is cognitive functioning impaired in math and amphetamine users? A critical review. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37(3), 586-608. Hayden, J.W. (1991). Passive inhalation of marijuana smoke: a critical review. Journal of Substance Abuse, 3, 85-90. 23 Health Canada (2011) Canadian Alcohol and Drug Use Monitoring Survey Summary of Results for 2011, http://www.hc-sc.gc.ca/hc-ps/drugs-drogues/stat/_2011/summarysommaire-eng.php Hingson, R., Lederman, R., & Walsh, D. (1985). Employee Drinking Patterns and Accidental Injury: A Study of Four New England States, 46(4), 298-303. Hoffman, J.,& Larison, C. (1999) Drug use, workplace accidents and employee turnover. Journal of Drug Use, 29(2), 341-364. Huestis, M.A., & Cone, E. (1995). Drug test findings resulting from unconventional drug exposure. In: R.H. Liu, & B. Goldberger eds. Handbook of Workplace Drug Testing. Washington, DC, AACC Press: 289-320. in Clinical False Positives.... Jaffee, W.B., Trucco, E., Teter, C., Levy, S., & Weiss, R.D. (2008). Ensuring Validity in Urine Drug Testing. Psychiatric Services, 59, 140-142. Jacobson M. (2003). Drug testing in the trucking industry: the effect on highway safety. Journal of Law and Economics, 46, 131-56. Jones, J. P. (1990). Drug Testing Did Not Reduce Southern Pacific's Accident Rate. Forensic Urine Drug Testing, 2-4. Kadehjian, L. (2005). Legal issues in oral fluid testing. Forensic Science International, 150, 151-160. Kapur, B. (1994) Drug testing methods and interpretations of drug testing results. In: Macdonald, S. and Roman, P. (Eds). Research Advances in Alcohol and Drug Problems. New York: Plenum Press. Kraus, J.F. (2001). The effects of certain drug-testing programs on injury reduction in the workplace: an evidence based review. International Journal of Occupational and Environmental Health, 7(2), 103-108. Last, J. M. (1995). A dictionary of epidemiology (3rd ed). New York: Oxford University Press. Longo, M.C., Hunter, C.E., Lokan, R.J., White, J.M., & White, M.A. (2000a). The prevalence of alcohol, cannabinoids, benzodiazepines and stimulants amongst injured drivers and their role in driver culpability. Part I: The prevalence of drug use in drivers and characteristics of drug positive group. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 32, 613-622. Longo, M.C., Hunter, C.E., Lokan, R.J., White, J.M., & White, M.A. (2000b). The prevalence of alcohol, cannabinoids, benzodiazepines and stimulants amongst injured drivers and their role in driver culpability. Part II: The relationship between drug prevalence and drug concentration, and driver capability. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 32, 623-632. 24 Macdonald, S., Lothian, S. and Wells, S. (1998) Evaluation of an employee assistance program at a transportation company. Evaluation and Program Planning, 20(4):495-505. Macdonald, S. (1995). The role of drugs in workplace injuries: Is drug testing appropriate? Journal of Drug Issues, 25(4), 703-722. Macdonald, S. (1997). Work-place alcohol and other drug testing: A review of the scientific evidence. Drug and Alcohol Review, 16, 251-259. Macdonald, S., Csiernik, R., Durand, P., Rylett, M. and Wild, T.C. (2006) The prevalence and factors related to Canadian workplace health programs. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 97(2):121-125. Macdonald, S., Hall, W., Roman, P., Stockwell, T., Coghlan, M. and Nesvaag, S. (2009). Testing for cannabis in the workplace: A review of the evidence. Addiction, 105(3), 408-416. Mann RE, Macdonald S, Stoduto G, Bondy S, Jonah B, Shaikh A. The effects of introducing or lowering legal per se blood alcohol limits for driving: an international review. Accident Analysis & Prevention. 2001;33(5):569-83. Marquet, P., Delpla, P-A., Kerguelen, S., Bremond, J., Facey, F., Garnier, M., Guery, B., Lhermitte, M., Mathé, D., Pelissier, A-L., Renaudeau, C., Vest, P., & Seguela, JP. (1998). Prevalence of drugs of abuse in urine of drivers involved in road accidents in France: a collaborative study. Journal of Forensic Science, 43(4), 806-811. McKim, W. (1986). Drugs and behavior. Prentice-Hall, New Jersey. McLeod, J (2010) The effectiveness of workplace counseling: A systematic review. Counseling and Psychotherapy Research, 10(4): 238-248. R Miller T. R., Zaloshnja E., Spicer R. S. (2007) Effectiveness and benefit-cost of peerbased workplace substance abuse prevention coupled with random testing. Accid Anal Prev; 39: 565-73. Moeller, K.E., Lee, K.C., & Kissack, J.C. (2008). Urine drug screening: practical guide for clinicians. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 83(1), 66-76. Morantz, A.D. & Mas, A. (2008). Does post-accident testing reduce injuries? evidence from a large retail chain. American Law and Economics Review, 10(2), 246-302. Mule, S.J., & Casella, G.A. (1988). Active and realistic passive marijuana exposure tested by three immunoassays and GC/MS in urine. Journal of Analytical Toxicology, 12, 113-116. Newcomb, M.D. (1994). Prevalence of alcohol and other drug use on the job: Cause for concern or irrational hysteria? Journal of Drug Issues, 24(3), 403-416. 25 Niedbala, S., Karados, K., Salamone, S., Fritch, D., Bronsgeest, M., & Cone, E.J. (2004). Passive cannabis exposure and oral fluid testing. Journal of Analytical Toxicology, 28, 546-552. Niedbala, S. Karados, K.W., Fritch, D.F., Kunsman, K.P., Blum, K.A., Newland, G.A., Waga, J., Kurtz, L., Bronsgeest, M. & Cone, E.J. (2005). Passive cannabis smoke exposure and oral fluid testing II: two studies of extreme cannabis smoke exposure in a motor vehicle. Journal of Analytical Toxicology, 29, 607-615. Niedbala, R. S., Kardos, K.W., Fritch, D.F., Kardos, S., Fries, T., Waga, J., Robb, J., & Cone, E.J. (2001a) Detection of marijuana use by oral fluid and urine analysis following single-dose administration of smoked and oral marijuana. Journal of Analytical Toxicology, 25, 289-303. Niedbala, R.S., Kardos, K., Fries, T., Cannon, A., & Davis, A. (2001b). Immunoassay for detection of cocaine/metabolites in oral fluids. Journal of Analytical Toxicology, 25, 62-68. Normand, J., Salyards, S.D., & Mahoney, J.J. (1990). An evaluation of pre-employment drug testing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 629-39. Normand, J., Lempert, R., O, & O'Brien, C. P. (Eds.). (1994). Under the Influence? Drugs and the American Work Force. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press. Ontario Law Reform Commission (1992) Report on drug and alcohol testing in the workplace. Toronto: Law Reform Commission. Ozminkowski, R. J., Mark, T.L., Goetzel, R.Z., Blank, D., Walsh, J.M. & Cangianelli, L. (2003). Relationships Between Urinalysis Testing for Substance Use, Medical Expenditures, and the Occurrence of Injuries at a Large Manufacturing Firm. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 29, 151–167. Porath-Waller, A.J., Bierness, D.J., & Beasley, E.E. (2009). Toward a more parsimonious approach to drug recognition expert evaluations. Traffic Injury Prevention, 10, 513-518. Quest Diagnostics Incorporated, (2012) Drug Testing Index Ramaekers, J.G., Berghaus, G. van Laar, M., & Drummer, O.H. (2004). Dose related risk of motor vehicle crashes after cannabis use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 73(2), 109-19. Ryan, J., Zwerling, C., & Jones, M. (1992). The Effectiveness of Preemployment Drug Screening in the Prediction of Employment Outcome. Journal of Occupational Medicine, 34(11), 1057-1063. 26 Shipp, E., Tortolero, S, Cooper, S., Baumler, E., Weller, N. (2005) Substance use and occupational injuries among high school students in South Texas. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 31(2),253-265. Smiley, A. (1999). Marijuana: On-road and driving-simulator studies. In: Kalant, H., Corrigall, W., Hall, W., Smart R. (Eds.), The health effects of cannabis, Addiction Research Foundation: Toronto, Ontario, pp. 173-191. Smith, J.A., Hayes, C.E., Yolton, R.L., Rutledge, D.A., & Citek, K. (2002). Drug recognition expert evaluations made using limited data. Forensic Science International, 130, 167-173. Spicer, R.A., Miller, T.R., & Smith, G.S. (2003). Worker substance use, workplace problems and the risk of occupational injury: a matched case-control study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 64(4), 570-578. Taggert, R. Results of the drug testing program at Southern Pacific Railroad.Gust, S. W., & Walsh, J. M. (1989). Drugs in the Workplace: Research and Evaluation Data. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse, NIDA Monograph 91. U.S. Department of Transportation, (2004) Drugs and Human Performance Fact Sheets, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, DOT HS 809 725. Verstraete, A.G. (2004). Detection times of drugs of abuse in blood, urine, and oral fluid. Therapeutic Drug Monitoring, 26, 200-206. Walsh, JM (2008) New technology and new initiatives in US workplace testing. Forensic Science International, 174, 120-124. Wickizer, T., Kopjar, G., Jutta, J (2004) Do drug-free workplace programs prevent occupational injuries? Evidence fro Washington State. Health Services Research 39(1), 91-110. Zwerling, C., Ryan, J., & Orav, E.J. (1990). The efficacy of pre-employment drug screening for marijuana and cocaine in predicting employment outcome. JAMA, 264(20), 2639-2643. 27