Developmental Psychology 2002, Vol. 38, No. 6, 993–1003 Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0012-1649/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.6.993 Children Use Whole–Part Juxtaposition as a Pragmatic Cue to Word Meaning Megan M. Saylor Mark A. Sabbagh University of Oregon Queen’s University at Kingston Dare A. Baldwin University of Oregon When parents label novel parts of familiar objects, they typically provide familiar whole-object terms before offering novel part terms (e.g., “See this cup? This is the rim.”). Such whole–part juxtaposition might help children to accurately interpret the meaning of novel part terms, but it can do so only if they recognize the conjunction as a potential cue to part meaning. Two studies examined (a) whether 3- to 4-year-olds use whole–part juxtaposition to accurately interpret novel part terms and (b) how they might do so. Study 1 confirmed that children indeed use juxtaposition to guide learning of novel part terms. Furthermore, 2 control conditions clarified that children’s use of juxtaposition was not simply due to memory effects, such as the facilitation of lexical access, nor to recognition of the grammatical frame that typically accompanies juxtaposition. Study 2 revealed that children readily use juxtaposition in a novel, gestural format. Such flexibility in recognizing and utilizing novel variants of juxtaposition strongly suggests that pragmatic understanding lies at the heart of children’s sensitivity to whole–part juxtaposition. ences to novel parts with references to familiar whole objects (Masur, 1997; Ninio, 1980; Ninio & Bruner, 1978; Shipley, Kuhn, & Madden, 1983). For example, a parent in Masur’s study presenting a novel part term to her child said, “That’s a bird . . . bird with a big beak” (p. 433). Juxtaposing novel part terms with familiar whole-object labels in this way provides a possible cue to children that reference to something other than the whole object is intended, and parts may represent a salient alternative candidate meaning. But do young children recognize the significance of this cue for guiding their hypotheses about word meanings? If children can capitalize in this way on the juxtaposition that parents offer, language learning would be greatly expedited. Children’s ability to draw on juxtaposition when faced with novel part terms has been hinted at in previous research investigating the role of semantic constraints in early word learning. In particular, research concerning the mutual exclusivity assumption has provided preliminary evidence that children are sensitive to whole–part juxtaposition as a cue to part-term meaning. Mutual exclusivity, a default assumption children may bring to wordlearning situations, enables them to avoid attaching more than one label to a given object (e.g., Golinkoff, Mervis, & Hirsh-Pasek, 1994; Markman, 1992, 1994; Markman & Wachtel, 1988). This bias could facilitate part-term learning by guiding children to search for an alternative to the whole object for a novel label’s referent when such a label (e.g., thorax) is offered in connection with a familiar object (e.g., butterfly). Along these lines, part interpretations represent one salient alternative (e.g., Markman & Wachtel, 1988; Merriman & Bowman, 1989; see also Clark, 1987, 1993; Mervis & Bertrand, 1994; Mervis, Golinkoff, & Bertrand, 1994). Novel words referring to parts of objects, such as thorax and handle, pose potential difficulties for learners that are characteristic of word learning more generally. In particular, when children hear novel part terms, they face the challenge of determining that parts of objects are meant from among the logically infinite possibilities (e.g., Quine, 1960). Fortunately, parents typically offer cues in their speech that could help children resolve these inductive complexities. Specifically, parents tend to juxtapose refer- Megan M. Saylor and Dare A. Baldwin, Department of Psychology, University of Oregon; Mark A. Sabbagh, Department of Psychology, Queen’s University at Kingston. Portions of these data were presented at the October 1998 Interdisciplinary Conference on Intentionality, Eugene, Oregon; the October 1998 Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston; and the April 1999 meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, New Mexico. This study was supported in part by a grant from the John Merck Scholars Fund, by National Science Foundation (NSF) New Young Investigator Award 9458339 to Dare A. Baldwin and by an NSF graduate fellowship to Mark A. Sabbagh. The article was prepared while Dare A. Baldwin was a fellow at the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences; we thank the William T. Grant Foundation for financial support underlying this fellowship. We give special thanks to Raisa Alam, Allison Archer, Dawn Delgado, Katie Gallerani, Brooke Hughes, Margot Holdstein, Christopher Pagnani, and Joseph Penrod for help with coding and data preparation, and to Jodie A. Baird and Kate Harkness for helpful comments on a draft. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Megan M. Saylor, who is now at Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University, GPC, Box 512, Nashville, Tennessee 37203. E-mail: m.saylor@vanderbilt.edu 993 994 SAYLOR, SABBAGH, AND BALDWIN One way to demonstrate children’s adherence to mutual exclusivity for interpreting new words is to compare their inferences about word meaning when a novel label is applied to a known object versus a novel object. If mutual exclusivity is operative, then children should be more likely to interpret the novel label as referring to a part (rather than the whole object) when the novel label is applied to a known object relative to a novel object. Markman and Wachtel (1988) tested this hypothesis with 3- and 4-year-olds in an elegant series of studies. Interestingly, in addition to addressing their intended hypothesis, their research may have also inadvertently supplied a first test of the role that juxtaposition between familiar and novel terms plays in part-term acquisition. To illustrate this requires describing their methodology in some detail. In one test of mutual exclusivity, Markman and Wachtel (1988, Study 3) used the same picture (e.g., a lung) as both a novel and familiar object for different groups of children: One group of children heard lung (as the name for the whole object) without any previous experience with the object, but the other group was taught lung prior to introducing them to a novel part term (e.g., trachea). During the comprehension procedure children were asked to identify the novel part of the object (e.g., “What’s a trachea?”). To ensure that the newly trained lung indeed functioned as a familiar label for children in the familiar-object condition, the researcher reminded children of the label (e.g., “Remember this lung?”) directly before asking the test question (e.g., “What’s a trachea?”). In line with the predictions of the mutual exclusivity assumption, children were more likely to respond with the appropriate part of the object if the lung had been previously labeled, and was therefore familiar, than if they had not been taught the label, and lung was therefore unfamiliar. Although the reminder of the newly trained whole-object term (e.g., “Remember this lung?”) in Study 3 was introduced to equate the familiarity of the whole-object labels used in the novel and familiar conditions, it may also have provided a crucial bit of information to children in the form of whole–part juxtaposition. The phrase children heard in the familiar-object condition (e.g., “Remember this lung? What’s the trachea?”) was similar to parents’ juxtaposition of familiar and novel labels when they introduce novel part terms to children (Masur, 1997). Thus, children’s part responses in Markman and Wachtel’s (1988) Study 3 may have been guided, in part, by their sensitivity to such juxtaposition. In another study examining the role of mutual exclusivity, Markman and Wachtel (1988, Study 2) did not provide children with immediate juxtaposition of whole-object terms and novel part terms (e.g., “What’s a boom?”). Instead, children heard familiar whole-object labels some time prior to seeing the object being asked about and hearing the novel part terms. Under these conditions the juxtaposition cue would have been considerably weakened. Although children did show some evidence of using mutual exclusivity in Study 2, their selection of parts in response to novel labels applied to familiar objects was substantially lower than when the whole-object term immediately preceded the novel label in Study 3 (57% vs. 85%, respectively). Furthermore, children’s part responding in Study 2 did not exceed chance levels, suggesting that the juxtaposition information offered in Study 3 may have facilitated children’s part interpretations of novel words. From the present perspective, the pattern of data across the two Markman and Wachtel (1988) studies suggests that children may benefit from clear juxtaposition when faced with learning novel part terms. Unfortunately, Markman and Wachtel’s findings are not conducive to a direct comparison between Studies 2 and 3, because their goals for the research did not necessitate that they use the same objects in the familiar-object conditions across the two studies. This speaks to the need for research specifically designed to investigate children’s ability to capitalize on whole– part juxtaposition. The present studies directly targeted this issue. It is important to recognize that the current research did not attempt to pit children’s use of whole–part juxtaposition against their use of other resources for part-term learning, such as mutual exclusivity, or other general cognitive skills, such as covariation tracking or sensitivity to salience. Rather, the studies reported here investigated whether juxtaposition informs children’s judgments about a novel part term over and above the assistance that might be supplied by general cognitive skills and mutual exclusivity alone. Study 1 The primary goal of Study 1 was to test children’s reliance on whole–part juxtaposition in part-term learning. We did so by comparing children’s level of part-term learning across two conditions: The juxtaposition condition, which included whole–part juxtaposition, and the no-juxtaposition condition, in which juxtaposition was absent. Study 1 also examined two mechanisms that might underlie children’s sensitivity to juxtaposition. Recall that juxtaposition in the context of introducing a new part term involves a known whole object label that is juxtaposed with a novel term (e.g., “That’s a bird . . . bird with a big beak.”). One possibility is that juxtaposition simply enhances children’s retrieval of the name for the whole object, which in turn more effectively triggers the operation of the mutual exclusivity assumption, hence increasing part hypotheses for new labels. If this retrieval facilitation is the sole mechanism driving children’s use of juxtaposition, then aids to lexical access that do not involve juxtaposition—such as simply introducing a delay to provide more time for lexical access—should likewise facilitate part-term learning. The delay control condition tested this possibility. An alternative possibility is that children use structural information that is correlated with whole–part juxtaposition (e.g., Gleitman, 1990; Naigles & Hoff-Ginsberg, 1995) to infer a part meaning for the novel term. Parents typically produce novel part terms in the following general frame: “Look, a [known whole-object label]. See the/its [novel term]” (e.g., Masur, 1997). Perhaps over repeated exposure to such juxtaposition children come to link the associated grammatical frame with part meanings. If so, children might take the presence of the grammatical frame itself as a predictor of part meaning. This possibility is plausible given previous work confirming that children reliably use information about the distribution of novel terms across labeling phrases when drawing inferences about the meaning of new words (Hall, Waxman, & Hurwitz, 1997; Mintz & Gleitman, 1998; Prasada, 1993; Taylor & Gelman, 1988). For example, property terms, such as glittery can appear either as adjectival predicates (e.g., “It’s glittery!”) or as modifiers on a noun (e.g., “A glittery one”). The relative position of these terms appears to give children information relevant to the likely meaning of the new term, namely, that the new term is more likely to be a label for a property than an object. Likewise, if WHOLE–PART JUXTAPOSITION AS A PRAGMATIC CUE children are able to rely on grammatical structure when learning part terms, then the presence of the frame itself, without the accompanying whole-object term (and hence without clear-cut juxtaposition information), should trigger part interpretations. The frame control condition tested this possibility. Evaluating children’s knowledge of part-term meanings is a notoriously difficult enterprise. One cannot simply ask children to point to a part in response to an inquiry about a novel part term (e.g., “What’s a thorax?”) because the reference of pointing gestures is ambiguous between parts and whole objects. For instance, a pointing gesture may refer to the particular part that it comes closest to contacting, or it may refer to the whole object. Removing parts of objects from their parent whole objects is also unsatisfactory because it is not clear whether the part continues to be a part when it is separated from the whole object—it may become another independent whole object (Tversky & Hemenway, 1984). To circumvent these problems we chose to use a color identification task as our index of part-term learning. We showed children objects that were constructed such that the major portion of the whole object was in one color, and a salient part was in another (e.g., a red butterfly with a green thorax) and asked children questions such as “See this butterfly? What color is the thorax?”. These objects allowed us to probe children’s part-term knowledge via their color-term production, without having to remove the salient part. It also allowed for a fairly unambiguous interpretation of children’s color responses. Of course, this methodology depended on children’s ability to produce color terms. Fortunately, by 3 years of age, children have an impressive command of color terms. Typically, they are able to produce and comprehend the basic color terms (e.g., red, blue, green, yellow) and some others besides (e.g., purple, beige; Shatz, Behrend, Gelman, & Ebeling, 1996). Using the color question methodology enabled us to construct questions across the four conditions that contrasted quite minimally in terms of surface form. Crucially, however, only the juxtaposition condition involved genuine whole–part juxtaposition. For example, in the juxtaposition condition children were asked, “See this butterfly? What color is the thorax?” whereas in the no-juxtaposition condition children were asked “See this? What color is the thorax?”. The delay control condition (“See this? [delay] See it? [delay] What color is the thorax?”) and the frame control condition (“See this thing? What color is the thorax?”) were variants of the no-juxtaposition and juxtaposition conditions, respectively. The former controlled for lexical access facilitation and the latter for grammatical form. 995 If genuine sensitivity to whole–part juxtaposition facilitates children’s part-term learning, they should show enhanced ability to provide the color of the novel part in response to test questions in the juxtaposition condition relative to all three of the other conditions. Method Participants Forty-eight children (28 girls and 20 boys) ranging in age from 3 years 0 months to 4 years 6 months (mean age ⫽ 3 years 9 months) participated. All children were developing normally and came from predominantly White, middle-class, monolingual, English-speaking families. Because color-term production was the dependent measure, a short pretest was included to assess children’s production of the six color terms used in the present study (i.e., red, green, blue, yellow, orange, purple). Only children who could produce all six color terms when asked were included. An additional 27 children participated but their data were omitted because of the following: inability to produce all six color terms during the pretest (n ⫽ 19), experimenter error (n ⫽ 2), parental interference (n ⫽ 1), and noncompliance (n ⫽ 5). Materials The stimuli were 12 pictures of familiar objects, each with a salient part. The objects were selected on the basis of existing norms regarding children’s early vocabularies, so that all objects had whole-object labels likely to be in young children’s productive and receptive vocabularies (Fenson et al., 1994). The pictures were created out of construction paper, and a black marker was used to highlight contours (e.g., the line between the head and neck) and defining object features (e.g., the eyes on an animal and the windows on a house). Half of the objects had a salient novel part (e.g., thorax on a butterfly), and half had a salient familiar part (e.g., wheel on car). Each object was presented so that the largest portion was in one color and a salient part of the object was in another (see Table 1). The six colors used were chosen on the basis of previous research indicating that they are within the productive repertoire of 3- to 4-year-old children (Shatz et al., 1996). Half of the pictures were of animate creatures and half were of inanimate objects. Including stimuli of both types enabled us to ascertain whether any effects would generalize across an ontological distinction that has been shown to affect children’s inferences about word meaning (e.g., Jones & Smith, 1998). The experimental sessions were videotaped. This enabled later coding of children’s responses. Design and Procedure Pretesting. After a brief warm-up session, children were seated at a small table facing the experimenter. First, children participated in a differ- Table 1 Stimuli Used in Study 1 Stimuli Animacy Animates Inanimates Familiar parts Novel parts Orange pig with a purple ear Green cat with a red tail Yellow bunny with a blue eye Purple cup with an orange handle Red car with a green wheel Blue house with a yellow door Purple spider with an orange pedicel Red butterfly with a green thorax Blue fish with a yellow dorsal Orange train with a purple coupler Green flower with a red stamen Yellow shoe with a blue instep 996 SAYLOR, SABBAGH, AND BALDWIN ent study with a 10-min long procedure that did not involve any of the same stimuli or color terms used in the present study. Following this, children were presented with a pretest in which they were shown color swatches and asked to identify each of the six colors used on the test objects. The colors were presented individually in a fixed order. Only the data from children who could produce all six color terms when asked were included in the analyses. Test questions. After pretesting, children were told that they were going to be asked questions about the colors of some pictures, and they were asked to respond with just one color to each test question. Children were then shown each test object individually and either asked about the color of a whole object or its part in one of four conditions: (a) In the juxtaposition condition, the whole-object term was presented before the part term (e.g., “See this butterfly? What color is the thorax?”), in a manner analogous to parents’ introduction of novel part terms (e.g., Masur, 1997; Ninio, 1980; Ninio & Bruner, 1978; Shipley et al., 1983). (b) In the no-juxtaposition condition, the test question was identical to that used in the juxtaposition condition except that the whole-object term was not included (e.g., “See this? What color is the thorax?”). (c) In the delay control condition, the no-juxtaposition condition was augmented by two types of aids to lexical retrieval in the form of two 3-s long delays and a reorienting utterance (e.g., “See this? [delay] See it? [delay] What color is the thorax?”).1 (d) In the frame control condition, children were offered a labeling phrase identical to that offered in the juxtaposition condition except that the whole-object label was replaced with the neutral term thing (e.g., “See this thing? What color is the dorsal?”). This study was conducted in three phases. During the first phase, children were randomly assigned to either the juxtaposition (n ⫽ 12, mean age ⫽ 4 years 0 months) or no-juxtaposition (n ⫽ 12, mean age ⫽ 3 years 11 months) condition, with the constraint that there were roughly equal numbers of boys and girls in each condition. During the second and third phases, respectively, two new groups of children participated in the delay control (n ⫽ 12, mean age ⫽ 3 years 8 months) and frame control (n ⫽ 12, mean age ⫽ 3 years 7 months) conditions.2 The test pictures were presented in the center of a standard-sized sheet of white paper in one of six orders. The six orders were determined randomly with the following constraints: no more than two pictures of one ontological kind appeared in immediate sequence (e.g., no more than two consecutive animate pictures), and no more than two whole objects of the same color appeared in a row (e.g., no more than two consecutive orange whole objects). Children also received no more than two questions of a particular type in a row (e.g., no more than two consecutive whole-object questions). Children were asked one test question about each object. If children did not respond to the test question the first time, it was repeated. If they did not respond the second time, the experimenter skipped to the next item but returned to the picture at the end of the session. All children who passed the pretest answered all of the test questions. Children were asked 12 questions in total. Six of the questions probed knowledge of labels for familiar whole objects and six probed knowledge of part terms. Three of the part questions probed knowledge of labels for familiar parts and three probed knowledge of labels for novel parts. Familiar item test questions. The familiar whole and familiar part test questions provided a baseline measure of children’s ability to answer each of our test questions appropriately. If our queries about colors indexed knowledge of the relevant labels, then children should offer the color of the whole object when asked about the whole object and the color of the part when asked about the part. All children were asked six familiar whole test questions and three familiar part test questions in a manner that matched the modeling of the novel part labels in the experimental condition in which they participated. For example, children in the juxtaposition condition heard both the whole and part labels when being asked about familiar parts and wholes (e.g., “See this pig? What color is the ear/pig?”), and children in the frame control condition were asked about the familiar part with the term thing in the place of the whole-object term (e.g., “See this thing? What color is the ear/pig?”). Novel part test questions. The novel part test questions (e.g., for the juxtaposition condition: “See this butterfly? What color is the thorax?”) probed children’s ability to accurately interpret novel part terms across the four conditions. Half of the children in each condition received novel part test questions about animate creatures, and half received novel part test questions about inanimate objects. All children were asked three novel part test questions. Coding. Children’s responses were coded as referring to either a whole object (when they offered the color of the whole object) or a part (when they offered the color of the salient part). Children occasionally gave two or more colors in answer to a single test question. Multicolor responses fell into two general types. One type was a list of colors that included at least two of the colors in a given object (e.g., whole-object color, part color, the color of the black contour lines, or the color of the white page) without any observable pause between the colors mentioned. For example, one child responded “blue and black and yellow” when asked about the yellow bunny with the blue eye. These responses were coded as whole-object responses. In another type of multicolor response, children offered one color and then after a clear and protracted pause offered another, almost as an afterthought. For example, children responded “yellow . . . and the house is blue” in response to an inquiry about the color of the door, which was yellow. These responses were coded by noting the first response offered. If the first response was a part color, as in the example, children were coded as giving a part response. If the first color was a whole-object color (e.g., “Blue . . . and yellow,” in response to a question about the color of the fish, which was blue), children were coded as giving a whole-object response. Children received a score of 1 for part responses and 0 for whole-object responses for each test question. Thus, scores for familiar part and novel part test questions each ranged between 0 and 3, and scores for the familiar whole test questions ranged between 0 and 6. Note that for part questions (i.e., familiar part and novel part test questions) a score of 3 would indicate that children responded to each test question appropriately, whereas for familiar whole test questions a score of 0 would indicate they responded appropriately to each test question. Results and Discussion Familiar Item Test Questions The first question addressed was whether children were able to respond appropriately to our test questions. Specifically, could they respond with the whole-object color when asked about a familiar whole object and the color of the part when asked about a familiar part? To address this question, in the first analysis we examined responses children made to the nine test questions involving familiar terms. Children excelled at these questions. In fact, not a single child made an error in response to questions about familiar parts across 1 Previous work on children’s recall of familiar words indicated that a total delay period of 6 s would be more than adequate time for retrieval of the whole-object term from memory (Cirrin, 1984). Furthermore, the orienting utterance should work to refocus their attention on the stimulus object during the delay. 2 Across both studies, analyses of children’s performance on test questions in each condition by age (3 vs. 4) via a Mann–Whitney U test revealed no differences in responding between the two age groups. Furthermore, independent samples t tests revealed no differences in mean age in months across any of the conditions. WHOLE–PART JUXTAPOSITION AS A PRAGMATIC CUE the four experimental conditions (all Ms ⫽ 3.00, SDs ⫽ 0).3 In addition, children’s responses to questions about familiar whole objects were highly accurate. Specifically, children rarely responded with the color of the part; this was true across conditions (juxtaposition: M ⫽ 0.17, SD ⫽ 0.39; no juxtaposition: M ⫽ 0.67, SD ⫽ 1.23; delay and frame control: both Ms ⫽ 0.17, SD ⫽ 0.58). Goodness-of-fit tests comparing individual response patterns to the distribution predicted by chance revealed that part responding to familiar whole test questions was below chance in all conditions, 2s(6, Ns ⫽ 12) ⱖ 329.38, ps ⬍ .01.4 Clearly, children had little difficulty with our color question format. Of course, the primary goal of the current study was to investigate the difference in children’s part-term responding to novel part test questions across the four conditions. However, before examining this it was important to establish that any condition differences that might be revealed in responses to novel part test questions did not generalize to areas where a difference in responding would not be expected, namely, in children’s responses to familiar item test questions. That is, the presence or absence of juxtaposition should not affect children’s responses to familiar item test questions. Children’s responses to familiar part test questions clearly did not differ across the four experimental conditions: In all conditions their responding was at ceiling. A little variability across conditions emerged in children’s responses to familiar whole test questions, but these differences were not significant in a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), F(3, 44) ⫽ 1.29, ns. The results from this analysis indicate that a comparison of children’s responses to novel part test questions across the four conditions is potentially meaningful in that any differences arising across the four conditions cannot be attributed to general, cross-condition demand characteristics. Novel Part Test Questions Regarding the novel parts, the focal questions were whether and how juxtaposition enabled children to interpret the novel term as referring to the part of the object. Preliminary analyses revealed no significant effects of sex. Therefore, this variable was dropped from this and all further analyses. The research questions were evaluated with a 4 (condition: juxtaposition, no juxtaposition, delay control, frame control) ⫻ 2 (animacy: animate, inanimate) ANOVA. The analysis revealed no significant main effect of animacy and no significant Condition ⫻ Animacy interaction. The analysis revealed a significant main effect of condition, F(3, 40) ⫽ 4.47, p ⬍ .01. In line with our predictions, contrast analyses revealed that children in the juxtaposition condition gave significantly more part responses to novel part test questions (M ⫽ 2.33, SD ⫽ 0.89) than did children in the no-juxtaposition condition (M ⫽ 1.33, SD ⫽ 0.89), LSD, p ⬍ .05. When items, rather than subjects, were treated as the random variable, the same pattern of findings emerged, paired ti(5) ⫽ 3.46, p ⬍ .02. Furthermore, contrast analyses revealed that neither retrieval facilitation nor the grammatical frame alone could account for children’s tendency to offer more part responses in the juxtaposition than in the nojuxtaposition condition. In particular, children’s responses to novel part test questions did not differ across the no-juxtaposition and delay control (M ⫽ 1.25, SD ⫽ 0.97) and frame control (M ⫽ 0.92, SD ⫽ 1.00) conditions (LSDs, ns). These effects were maintained 997 when items rather than subjects served as the random variable, paired tis(5) ⱕ 2.00, ns. Furthermore, when both subjects and items were used as the random variable (LSDs, ps ⬍ .05) paired tis(5) ⱖ 3.73, ps ⬍ .05, the analyses revealed that children gave significantly more part responses in the juxtaposition than in the delay control or frame control conditions. An additional question concerned how systematic children were in giving part responses to the novel-word questions in the four different conditions. Goodness-of-fit tests comparing children’s patterns of responses to novel part test questions (see Table 2) against the distribution predicted by chance revealed that children reliably gave more part responses than would be expected by chance only in the juxtaposition condition, 2(3, N ⫽ 12) ⫽ 23.56, p ⬍ .01. Interestingly, in the frame control condition, children’s part responding was significantly below chance, 2(3, N ⫽ 12) ⫽ 9.78, p ⬍ .03. This finding clarifies that the frame alone was not helpful in aiding children’s part interpretations, and it perhaps even hindered inferences about part meaning. Furthermore, in the no-juxtaposition and delay control conditions, children’s responses to novel part test questions were not distinguishable from chance, 2s(3, Ns ⫽ 12) ⬍ 2.71, ns. Taken together, these findings indicate two things: First, children were able to use juxtaposition to make part interpretations, and second, children were not systematic in their judgments about meaning when juxtaposition was absent. For the next analyses, children in each condition were categorized as either whole-object responders (zero part responses) or part responders (all part responses) on the basis of their responses to novel part test questions. In the juxtaposition condition, not a single child qualified as a whole-object responder, whereas 7 qualified as part responders. A quite different pattern emerged when juxtaposition was absent. In the no-juxtaposition condition, the majority of children responded unsystematically: Just 2 children qualified as whole-object responders, and only 1 qualified as a part responder. A chi-square test of independence revealed that children in the juxtaposition condition were likely to be part responders but that children in the no-juxtaposition condition were not, 2(1, N ⫽ 24) ⫽ 14.00, p ⬍ .01. Children’s patterns of responding in the delay control (3 whole responders, 1 part responder) and frame control (5 whole responders, 1 part responder) conditions did not differ from patterns of responding in the nojuxtaposition condition, 2s(1, Ns ⫽ 24) ⱕ 0.85, ns. However, the analysis of children’s pattern of responding in the juxtaposition versus the delay control and frame control conditions revealed that children were more likely to be part responders in the juxtaposition condition than in either of the two control conditions, 2s(1, Ns ⫽ 24) ⱖ 17.70, p ⬍ .01. These analyses provided further evidence that in the juxtaposition condition children reliably interpreted the novel label as a part term. 3 In this and all cases where children’s performance was at ceiling levels, their responding was greater than chance (binomial p ⬍ .01). Additionally, when children’s performance was at floor levels, their responding was below chance (binomial p ⬍ .01). 4 In this and all cases where the direction of children’s chance responding is discussed, an examination of the distribution of children’s responses was conducted to determine whether their pattern of responding was best described as being above or below chance (Table 2 displays these distributions). SAYLOR, SABBAGH, AND BALDWIN 998 Table 2 Number of Children Giving Zero, One, Two, or Three Part Responses in Response to Novel Part Test Questions Part responses Condition 0 1 2 3 Part responses expected by: Chance ( fe) Juxtaposition (Study 1) No-juxtaposition Delay control Frame control Gesture juxtaposition (Study 2) Gesture no-juxtaposition Two-gesture control 1.5 0 2 3 5 2 4 4 4.5 3 5 4 4 3 6 3 4.5 2 4 4 2 2 2 3 1.5 7 1 1 1 5 0 2 Note. fe ⫽ expected frequency. Delay Control Condition Manipulation Check Recall that the delay control was introduced to test whether whole-object label retrieval aids other than juxtaposition might enhance children’s part responding. To evaluate the success of the delay control, an important first question was whether the delay control condition indeed succeeded in facilitating children’s retrieval of the whole-object label. In the absence of such confirmation it would be difficult to interpret children’s failure to learn novel part terms in the delay control. Two pieces of data confirmed such retrieval facilitation. First, 8 of the 12 children in the delay control condition spontaneously blurted out the whole-object label of one or more of the three novel part items during one of the delays (e.g., children blurted “butterfly” during delay following the experimenter’s “See this?”). In the no-juxtaposition condition, which lacked the delay retrieval aid, such blurting of the wholeobject label never occurred. It is true that children in the nojuxtaposition condition were given no window of time in which to blurt, but it is certainly not unheard of for children of this age to produce speech that overlaps with adult speech. The latency to children’s test-question responses provides an even more convincing case that the delay control condition facilitated lexical access. From the videotapes, two coders judged the latency (in seconds) to children’s correct responses to questions pertaining to familiar whole-object labels (the clearest test case of lexical access to the familiar whole-object label) in the nojuxtaposition (M ⫽ 2.20 s, SD ⫽ 0.50) and delay control conditions (M ⫽ 1.82 s, SD ⫽ 0.36). Five participants were independently evaluated by both coders. The mean difference in latency scores was only 0.45 s, and the coders’ measurements of the latency duration were highly intercorrelated (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .83). For the statistical analyses, scores that were more than 2.5 SDs from the mean were omitted. An independent samples t test revealed that children in the delay control condition gave correct responses to familiar whole-object test questions at shorter latencies than did children in the no-juxtaposition condition, t(22) ⫽ 2.18, p ⬍ .05. Hence, the retrieval cues (delays plus orienting utterance) appeared to facilitate children’s lexical retrieval in the delay control condition relative to the no-juxtaposition condition. Given that the delay control condition effectively facilitated children’s retrieval of the whole-object label, the question of primary interest—whether such facilitation translated into higher levels of part responding in the delay control condition than in the juxtaposition condition— can be addressed. The analyses reported previously clarify that children in the delay control condition were largely unsuccessful at correctly interpreting the novel part terms and displayed significantly fewer part interpretations than did children in the juxtaposition condition. Furthermore, even the children in the delay control condition who blurted out the familiar whole-object label (hence providing incontrovertible evidence of having accessed that label systematically) responded unsystematically: Children blurted out the whole-object label on 16 occasions, but gave part responses on only 54% of those occasions. Taken together, the findings indicate at least two things. First, aids to retrieval in the delay control were effective in promoting recall of whole-object labels. Second, such recall was not sufficient to engender systematic part responding. Summary Study 1 supplied clear-cut evidence that children readily utilize whole–part juxtaposition to interpret novel part terms. Children were better able to interpret a novel label as referring to a part when the novel part label was juxtaposed with a known wholeobject label than when no juxtaposition was provided. In fact, children reliably linked the new term with a part only when a novel part term was juxtaposed against a familiar whole-object label. Children’s success at generating part interpretations of novel words in the presence of juxtaposition was not merely the result of facilitated retrieval of the whole-object label and subsequent triggering of mutual exclusivity. When children’s retrieval of the whole-object term was facilitated without juxtaposition, they were unsuccessful at generating part interpretations for novel labels. Additionally, the structure of the juxtaposition frame was not sufficient, in and of itself, to guide children to part interpretations of novel words. Taken together, these findings indicate that the presence of the full juxtaposition phrase containing both wholeobject and novel labels is critical for children to achieve part interpretations of novel terms. Having ruled out retrieval facilitation and the grammatical frame as the source of children’s successful part responding in the juxtaposition condition, a third possible explanation rose to the fore: children’s use of pragmatic information. Perhaps children spontaneously attempt to infer speakers’ communicative intentions and rely on cues that speakers offer in their utterances to help unravel such intentions. In particular, children might take the juxtaposition of a novel term (e.g., beak) against a known term (e.g., bird) as a cue to the adult’s intention to refer to something different with the new term relative to what had just been referred to with the known term. The pragmatic account gains plausibility from a range of recent research clarifying children’s ability to exploit nonverbal pragmatic cues for word-learning purposes, including gestures (Carpenter, Nagell, & Tomasello, 1998), discourse novelty (Akhtar, Carpenter, & Tomasello, 1996; Diesundruck, Markson, Akhtar, & Reudor, 2001), and line-of-regard and related attentional cues (e.g., Baldwin, 1993a; Hollich, HirshPasek, & Golinkoff, 2000; Moore, Angelopoulous, & Bennett, 1999; Tomasello, 1999). WHOLE–PART JUXTAPOSITION AS A PRAGMATIC CUE Study 2 In Study 2, we directly targeted the pragmatic account of children’s use of juxtaposition. If children indeed use juxtaposition for pragmatic reasons, they may be able to recognize the juxtaposition cue even when it is provided in novel ways (e.g., Diesundruck & Markson, 2001; Tomasello & Akhtar, 1995). Providing juxtaposition in the gestural modality represents one such novel format (e.g., Namy & Waxman, 1998). In Study 2, we tested a new group of children in a gesture juxtaposition condition that differed from the Study 1 verbal juxtaposition condition only with respect to modality. Juxtaposition in the gestural format involved juxtaposing (a) a gesture tracing a circle around an object, indicating the whole object (analogous to the whole-object label used in the Study 1 juxtaposition condition), with (b) a referentially ambiguous point, hovering a few inches over the part of an object (analogous to the novel, and hence ambiguous, part term used in Study 1). The pointing gesture used in the present study was ambiguous in the sense that, taken on its own, it could indicate either the whole object or the part. If children remain sensitive to juxtaposition when it is provided through gestures, they should be able to make use of the juxtaposition between the circling gesture and the pointing gesture to disambiguate the reference of the pointing gesture. Two comparison conditions were included in Study 2 in addition to the gesture juxtaposition condition. The first was a gesture no-juxtaposition condition. In this condition, only the referentially ambiguous point was included, analogous to the presence of the novel part term in the verbal no-juxtaposition condition in Study 1. Children’s responses in the gesture no-juxtaposition condition provided information about their success in interpreting the novel term as a part term in the absence of any juxtaposition information. Put another way, this condition clarified whether the single point gesture was indeed ambiguous in meaning for children. The second comparison condition was a two-gesture control condition. In this condition, children were exposed to two identical, referentially ambiguous pointing gestures. In this condition, no juxtaposition information was supplied by the two gestures, because they were identical. The two-gesture control condition was included to control for the possibility that children might show increased part responding in the gesture no-juxtaposition condition relative to the gesture juxtaposition condition simply because they saw two gestures (in the gesture juxtaposition condition) as opposed to just one (in the gesture no-juxtaposition condition). If children increased their part responses merely as a result of the presence of two gestures, then they should show an increased selection of parts in the two-gesture control condition relative to the gesture no-juxtaposition condition, despite the fact that no juxtaposition information was provided in the two-gesture control condition. The juxtaposition information in Study 2 was provided only in the gestural modality. In particular, no juxtaposition information was provided verbally. Thus, any differences observed in children’s part responding to questions about novel parts would be due to the gestural, not the verbal, input. The utterances selected to accompany the gestural information were those previously used in the no-juxtaposition condition in Study 1. Precisely the same utterances were used across the three gesture conditions (e.g., “See this? What color is the thorax?”). Recall that the results from 999 Study 1 indicated that this linguistic information did not assist children in identifying parts as referents of novel labels. Thus, if children displayed more part responses in the gesture juxtaposition condition than in the gesture no-juxtaposition and two-gesture control conditions, this could only be due to an appreciation for juxtaposition in the gestural modality. Method Participants Thirty-six children (17 girls and 19 boys) ranging in age from 3 years 1 month to 4 years 6 months (mean age ⫽ 3 years 8 months) participated. Children were recruited, compensated, and pretested as in Study 1. An additional 14 children participated but their data were omitted because of their inability to produce all six color terms (n ⫽ 10) and experimenter error (n ⫽ 4). Materials The stimulus materials for this study were 12 pictures of familiar objects with a salient part. The pictures were created and presented as in the previous study. A new stimulus set was constructed so that the relevant parts were always placed near the center of the whole object. This modification prevented children from noticing over trials that the point always occurred directly above the distinctly colored part, which might lead to the inference that the point referred to the part, independently of any juxtaposition information. In all other respects, the objects were identical to those used in the previous study. For each of the new objects, except for two (orange boat with a purple crank, green duck with a red wing), the familiar whole-object labels used were the same as in the previous study (see Table 3). Design and Procedure Pretesting. Pretesting was the same as in Study 1. Test questions. After pretesting, children were told that they were going to be asked questions about a few pictures. As in the previous study, they were asked to respond with only one color to the test questions. Children’s interpretation of novel part terms was assessed in one of three conditions: (a) In the gesture juxtaposition condition, children were offered a circling gesture indicating the whole object during the first half of the test question and an ambiguous point located several inches above the part during the second half of the test question (e.g., “See this [circling gesture]? What color is the thorax [ambiguous pointing gesture]?”). (b) In the gesture no-juxtaposition condition, the experimenter only provided an ambiguous point during the test question (e.g., “See this [no gesture]? What color is the thorax [ambiguous pointing gesture]?”). (c) In the two-gesture control condition, children were offered an additional ambiguous point during the first half of the test question (e.g., “See this [ambiguous pointing gesture]? What color is the thorax [ambiguous pointing gesture]?”). In all three conditions, children were offered the same test questions. Across all the three conditions, the point over the part was approximately 2 in. (5.08 cm) from the page and was held in place until the experimenter had offered the target label. The circling gesture in the gesture juxtaposition condition involved three revolutions around the whole object and made contact with the page. The study was conducted in two phases. In the first phase, children were randomly assigned to either the gesture juxtaposition (n ⫽ 12, mean age ⫽ 3 years 9 months) or gesture no-juxtaposition (n ⫽ 12, mean age ⫽ 3 years 7 months) condition, with the constraint that there be roughly equal numbers of boys and girls in each condition. In the second phase, a new group of children was recruited to participate in the two-gesture control condition (n ⫽ 12, mean age ⫽ 3 years 8 months). SAYLOR, SABBAGH, AND BALDWIN 1000 Table 3 Stimuli Used in Study 2 Stimuli Animacy Animates Inanimates a Familiar parts Novel parts Orange pig with a purple eye Green duck with a red wing Yellow bunny with a blue nose Purple cup with an orange handlea,b Red car with a green door Blue house with a yellow window Purple spider with an orange pedicela Red butterfly with a green thoraxa Blue fish with a yellow ventral Orange boat with a purple crank Green flower with a red stamena Yellow shoe with a blue grommet Used in Study 1 as well. b Handle moved to center of cup. The number and type of questions asked per child was the same as in Study 1, and counterbalancing was identical. In particular, children were also asked questions about familiar whole objects and familiar parts to provide information about the validity of the color questions as an index of children’s label comprehension. A given child received either gestural juxtaposition, gestural no-juxtaposition, or two-gesture control gestures for all three types of test questions (i.e., familiar part, familiar whole, novel part). Coding. The coding was the same as in Study 1. Results and Discussion Familiar Item Test Questions As in the previous study, we first checked children’s responses to test questions concerning familiar items (i.e., familiar wholeobject labels and familiar part labels) to evaluate the validity of the methodology as a way of probing children’s word comprehension. Children were skilled at responding to familiar item test questions. They offered greater-than-chance levels of part responses to questions about familiar parts across all three conditions (gesture juxtaposition: M ⫽ 2.33, SD ⫽ 0.89; gesture no-juxtaposition: M ⫽ 2.67, SD ⫽ 0.49; and two-gesture control: M ⫽ 2.75, SD ⫽ 0.45), 2s(3, Ns ⫽ 12) ⱖ 18.22, ps ⬍ .01. Likewise, children offered lower-than-chance levels of part responses to questions about familiar wholes (gesture juxtaposition: M ⫽ 0.08, SD ⫽ 0.29; gesture no-juxtaposition: M ⫽ 0.08, SD ⫽ 0.29; and two-gesture control: M ⫽ 0.33, SD ⫽ 0.89), 2s(6, Ns ⫽ 12) ⱖ 485.00, ps ⬍ .01. Clearly, as in the previous study, children had no difficulty with our test-question format, clarifying that our innovative color-question methodology provided a solid index of their comprehension of the whole-object and part labels involved. We would not expect children’s pattern of responding to the familiar item test questions to vary across conditions, despite the fact that the gestures accompanying the questions were not the same across conditions. If such differences emerged, this would indicate that the gestural formats themselves altered the general demand characteristics of the situation for children, rendering it difficult to interpret any condition differences that might emerge in further analyses of children’s responses to novel part test questions. However, a 3 (condition: gesture juxtaposition, gesture nojuxtaposition, two-gesture control) ⫻ 2 (familiar item test question: familiar whole, familiar part) mixed within-subjects ANOVA, with familiar item test question as the within-subjects variable, revealed no significant main effect for the condition variable, F(2, 33) ⫽ 0.67, ns. The results from this analysis ruled out any concern about general demand characteristics differing across conditions, making subsequent comparison of children’s responses to the novel part test questions across the three conditions a meaningful enterprise. Novel Part Test Questions A 3 (condition: gesture juxtaposition, gesture no-juxtaposition, two-gesture control) ⫻ 2 (animacy: animate, inanimate) betweensubjects ANOVA was performed to determine whether gestural juxtaposition was effective in facilitating children’s part-term responding. The analysis revealed no significant main effect of animacy and no significant Condition ⫻ Animacy interaction. However, a marginally significant main effect emerged with the condition variable, F(2, 30) ⫽ 3.04, p ⫽ .06. Planned contrast analyses revealed that children in the gesture juxtaposition condition offered significantly more part responses (M ⫽ 1.83, SD ⫽ 1.19) than did children in the gesture no-juxtaposition condition (M ⫽ 0.83, SD ⫽ 0.72), using both subjects, LSD, p ⬍ .03, one-tailed, and items, paired ti(5) ⫽ 2.93, p ⬍ .04, one-tailed, as the random variable. Children in the gesture juxtaposition condition also displayed a marginally higher level of part responses than did children in the two-gesture control condition (M ⫽ 1.25, SD ⫽ 1.14), using both subjects, LSD, p ⫽ .08, one-tailed, and items, paired ti(5) ⫽ 1.94, p ⫽ .06, one-tailed, as the random variable. Finally, children’s responses to novel part test questions did not differ between the gesture no-juxtaposition and two-gesture control conditions, using both subjects, t(22) ⫽ 1.07, ns, and items, paired ti(5) ⫽ 0.881, ns, as the random variable. To ensure that children’s high level of part responding in the gesture juxtaposition condition of the current study was comparable to that of children in the Study 1 juxtaposition condition, we conducted an analysis of the proportion of part responses to novel part test questions for objects that remained the same across Studies 1 and 2 (butterfly–thorax, spider–pedicel, and flower– stamen). The analysis revealed that children’s responses did not differ between the Study 2 gesture juxtaposition condition and Study 1 juxtaposition condition, t(22) ⫽ 0.481, ns. Children in the gesture juxtaposition condition gave part responses to questions about novel parts at greater than chance levels, 2(3, N ⫽ 12) ⫽ 10.22, p ⬍ .01. Interestingly, children in the gesture no-juxtaposition condition tended to give whole-object responses to novel part test questions; their part responding was marginally below chance, 2(3, N ⫽ 12) ⫽ 7.56, p ⫽ .06. Children WHOLE–PART JUXTAPOSITION AS A PRAGMATIC CUE in the two-gesture control condition were unsystematic in their responses to questions about novel parts, 2(3, N ⫽ 12) ⫽ 5.33, ns (see Table 2). In sum, children in the two conditions not including gestural juxtaposition information seemed either to be uncertain about the meaning of the novel term or to tend to interpret the novel term as a label for the whole object. Additionally, on the basis of their responses to novel part test questions, children in the present study were categorized as being either whole-object responders (zero whole-object responses) or part responders (all part responses). In the gesture juxtaposition condition, only 2 children qualified as whole-object responders, whereas 5 qualified as part responders. In contrast, the gesture no-juxtaposition and two-gesture control conditions yielded a different pattern of responding. In the gesture no-juxtaposition condition, 4 children qualified as whole-object responders, and not a single child qualified as a part responder. Similarly, in the twogesture control condition, 4 children qualified as whole-object responders, whereas only 2 qualified as part responders. Three chi-square tests of independence (gesture juxtaposition vs. gesture no-juxtaposition, gesture juxtaposition vs. two-gesture control, and gesture no-juxtaposition vs. two-gesture control) revealed that children’s pattern of responding to the novel part test questions in the gesture juxtaposition and gesture no-juxtaposition conditions differed significantly, 2(1, N ⫽ 24) ⫽ 11.43, p ⬍ .01, with children in the gesture juxtaposition condition tending to be part responders and children in the gesture no-juxtaposition condition tending to be whole-object responders. Interestingly, the twogesture control condition seemed to yield a different pattern of responses from both the gesture juxtaposition, 2(1, N ⫽ 24) ⫽ 3.49, p ⫽ .06, and gesture no-juxtaposition conditions, 2(1, N ⫽ 24) ⫽ 4.00, p ⬍ .01. As predicted by a pragmatic account, children in the gesture juxtaposition condition tended to be part responders, whereas children in the two-gesture control condition did not. At the same time, children were marginally more likely to be whole-object responders in the gesture no-juxtaposition condition than in the two-gesture control condition. This may simply have been because the two identical ambiguous points in the two-gesture control condition confused children, leading them to be largely unsystematic in their responding, whereas children in the gesture no-juxtaposition condition tended to systematically select whole objects. Summary Study 2 revealed that children could capitalize on juxtaposition offered in a novel gesture-based format to learn names for parts of objects. That is, children used juxtaposition flexibly to infer speakers’ part labeling intention, even when linguistic juxtaposition was absent. Moreover, as in the previous study, children supplied systematic part responses only when juxtaposition was available. This pattern suggests that juxtaposition plays a significant role in assisting children’s part-term learning. General Discussion The present series of studies provided the first evidence that preschool-aged children are sensitive to the juxtaposition that parents provide when introducing novel part terms. When juxtaposition was provided, children readily inferred part meanings for 1001 new words. Put another way, juxtaposition facilitated children’s part-term learning over and above any assistance provided by other possible contributors, such as mutual exclusivity, general cognitive skills such as memory and attention, and other conceptual knowledge. These studies also strongly suggest that children’s sensitivity to juxtaposition information can be classified as one aspect of their pragmatic understanding. The control conditions included in Study 1 ruled out two alternative explanations. The delay control clarified that juxtaposition does not simply enhance children’s access to the whole-object label in memory (thereby better triggering the mutual exclusivity assumption). Even when children in the delay control condition blurted out the whole-object label (confirming that they had retrieved it), they responded unsystematically to the question about the novel part term. One implication of these findings is that juxtaposition between the whole-object and part labels aids children in interpreting a novel part term only when the juxtaposition comes from the speaker; it is not sufficient simply for the juxtaposition to be available in children’s own minds. A second control—the frame control condition—further clarified that children do not simply associate the juxtaposition frame with part meanings. Providing children with the structural frame that is typical of juxtaposition in the absence of genuine whole– part juxtaposition left children uncertain about how to interpret the novel part term. The findings from Study 1 left a pragmatic account of children’s sensitivity to juxtaposition as the prime contender. That is, it seemed likely that children infer part meanings when juxtaposition is provided because they appreciate that speakers are using juxtaposition to signal an intention to refer to something other than the whole object. Study 2 provided a more direct test of the pragmatic account and found evidence in its favor. In particular, children were presented with juxtaposition information in a format that was new to them. Specifically, we offered juxtaposition via gestures: (a) a circling gesture that was easily interpreted as indicating the whole object juxtaposed with (b) a pointing gesture otherwise ambiguous in meaning. Although the pointing and circling gestures used in Study 2 likely each appear singly in children’s input, the direct juxtaposition of these gestures is probably rare in parents’ input to children. Despite the novelty of this gestural format for juxtaposition, children took advantage of the juxtaposition between gestures to systematically infer part meanings, whereas they were unsystematic when juxtaposition was absent. Such flexible use of the available information in a novel format—a hallmark of true pragmatic understanding—strongly suggests that children were noting the juxtaposition information and using it to draw online inferences about the speaker’s intended meaning. Pragmatic Understanding and the Mutual Exclusivity Assumption It might seem that the present studies bring into question the role of the mutual exclusivity assumption in part-term learning. After all, children inferred part meanings only when juxtaposition information was available, despite the fact that the mutual exclusivity assumption should have guided them to reject the novel label as a name for the whole object regardless of whether juxtaposition was 1002 SAYLOR, SABBAGH, AND BALDWIN present or absent. However, the mutual exclusivity assumption may nevertheless have played a role in children’s success at part-term learning when juxtaposition was provided. That is, children may be reluctant to activate the mutual exclusivity assumption until they have reason to believe it is applicable, with juxtaposition providing that reassurance. One way to interpret our findings, then, is that pragmatic understanding and the mutual exclusivity assumption work hand-in-hand to facilitate word learning (see Bloom, 2000; Hall & Waxman, in press; Hollich et al., 2000; Woodward & Markman, 1997, for discussions of this kind of integrative account). At the same time, however, our findings nevertheless strongly suggest that mutual exclusivity is triggered only given the appropriate pragmatic conditions— conditions sparking children’s recognition that the speaker intends to label something other than the whole object. A related possibility put forth by Clark (1987) and others (e.g., Baldwin, 1993b; Bloom, 2000; Diesendruck & Markson, 2001) is that mutual exclusivity does not exert its action separately from pragmatic information, but rather the word learning bias is in fact an instantiation of a pragmatic inference. Either way, the emerging picture is that pragmatic information holds some primacy in accounting for children’s success in the discovery of new word meanings. Pragmatic Understanding, Memory, and Attention The findings from these studies are germane to a current debate in the language learning literature. In the last decade there has been a steady influx of work highlighting infants’ and young children’s capacity to capitalize on sociopragmatic cues such as eye-gaze, gesture, and discourse novelty when learning the meanings of new words. Recently, however, Samuelson and Smith (1998) suggested that the findings from all of these studies might be explained by children’s use of low-level cognitive mechanisms such as memory and attention. Our findings argue against this strong claim. In the control studies in Study 1, we found that neither memory nor contextual association (i.e., grammatical cues) alone could account for children’s use of juxtaposition. The data from Study 2 are especially compelling in this regard. Recall that in the gesture no-juxtaposition condition, the experimenter circled the whole object while saying “See this?” and then asked the test question (e.g., “What color is the thorax?”) while providing an ambiguous point. The direct effect of the circling gesture would be to enhance children’s attention to the whole object. Yet, the addition of this whole object circling gesture had the effect of reducing children’s whole object responding. Clearly, children’s success at providing part responses in the gesture juxtaposition condition of Study 2 was not due to simple enhancements of memory or attention. Children’s increased ability to interpret the new word as referring to the part in the Study 2 gesture juxtaposition condition could only have arisen from an appreciation of the pragmatic force behind the experimenter’s juxtaposition of the two gestures. In the face of this juxtaposition children inferred that the speaker signaled an intention to label something other than the whole object. Of course, memory and attention are critical for word learning. Nevertheless, higher level appreciation for cues to speakers’ referential intentions is also clearly operative in children’s success at inferring part meanings in these studies. Pragmatic Understanding as One Skill Among Many The overarching goal of the current set of studies was to examine whether children can use juxtaposition as a cue to speakers’ labeling intentions. To be sure that children were using juxtaposition as a pragmatic cue, the word-learning situation was pared down to highlight the possible role of specific alternative mechanisms. For example, in the Study 1 control conditions, we focused on lexical access and the grammatical frame, respectively, whereas in Study 2 we specifically targeted pragmatic inferences. Our results across studies highlighted the powerful role juxtaposition played in guiding children’s interpretations of novel part terms, and clarified that neither enhanced lexical access nor sensitivity to a syntactic frame were the sole source of children’s use of juxtaposition. Pragmatic inferences, in contrast, appear to be the primary force behind children’s success at interpreting new part terms provided with juxtaposition. However, we think it is nevertheless likely that children recruit lexical access, syntactic cues, and other basic cognitive mechanisms in addition to pragmatic understanding in real-life word-learning situations. It would certainly benefit them to do so. On this view, pragmatic understanding is just one among a powerful complement of skills children can recruit to expedite word learning (e.g., Baldwin, 1995; Bloom, 2000; Hall & Waxman, in press; Hollich et al., 2000; Woodward, 2000; Woodward & Markman, 1997). Conclusion In recent years, a wealth of research has detailed children’s sensitivity to a variety of pragmatic cues, from gaze direction to anchoring information (Baldwin & Tomasello, 1998; Callanan, 1985, 1989). The present research provides clear evidence of one additional facet of children’s pragmatic understanding—the ability to use juxtaposition for part-term learning. These findings provide striking testament to children’s ability to go beyond the obvious to draw inferences about speakers’ referential intentions, much to the benefit of language learning. References Akhtar, N., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (1996). The role of discourse novelty in children’s early word learning. Child Development, 67, 635– 645. Baldwin, D. A. (1993a). Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for cues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 20, 395– 418. Baldwin, D. A. (1993b). Interpersonal inference as a route to reducing the indeterminacy problem in word learning. Working papers from the Conference on Early Cognition and the Transition to Language (pp. 1–9). Austin: Institute for Cognitive Science, University of Texas at Austin. Baldwin, D. A. (1995, April). Understanding relations between constraints and a socio pragmatic account of meaning acquisition. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, Indiana. Baldwin, D. A., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Word learning: A window on early pragmatic understanding. In E. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the 29th annual Child Language Research Forum (pp. 3–23). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Callanan, M. A. (1985). How parents label objects for young children: The WHOLE–PART JUXTAPOSITION AS A PRAGMATIC CUE role of input in the acquisition of category hierarchies. Child Development, 56, 508 –523. Callanan, M. A. (1989). Development of object categories and inclusion relations: Preschooler’s hypotheses about word meaning. Developmental Psychology, 25, 207–216. Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, T. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 21(2, Serial No. 255), 315–330. Cirrin, F. M. (1984). Lexical search speed in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 158 –175. Clark, E. V. (1987). The principle of contrast: A constraint on acquisition. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition: The 20th annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition (pp. 1–34). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L. (2001). Children’s avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology, 37, 630 – 641. Diesendruck, G., Markson, L., Akhtar, N., & Reudor, A. (2001). Two-yearolds’ sensitivity to speakers’ intent: An alternative account to Samuelson and Smith. Manuscript submitted for publication. Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5, Serial No. 242). Gleitman, L. R. (1990). The structural source of verb meanings. Language Acquisition, 1, 3–55. Golinkoff, R. M., Mervis, C. B., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1994). Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language, 21, 125–156. Hall, D. G., & Waxman, S. R. (in press). Weaving a lexicon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Hall, D. G., Waxman, S. R., & Hurwitz, W. M. (1997). How two- and four-year-old children interpret adjectives and count nouns. Child Development, 64, 1651–1664. Hollich, G. J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65(3, Serial No. 123). Jones, S. S., & Smith, L. B. (1998). How do children name objects with shoes. Cognitive Development, 12, 323–334. Markman, E. M. (1992). Constraints on word learning: Speculations about their nature, origins, and domain-specificity. In M. R. Gunnar & M. Maratsos (Eds.), The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 25. Modularity and constraints in language and cognition (pp. 59 –103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Markman, E. M. (1994). Constraints on word meaning in early language acquisition. In L. Gleitman & B. Landau (Eds.), The acquisition of the lexicon (pp. 199 –229). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Elsevier. Markman, E. M., & Wachtel, G. A. (1988). Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 120 –157. Masur, E. V. (1997). Maternal labeling of novel and familiar objects: Implications for children’s development of lexical constraints. Journal of Child Language, 24, 427– 439. Merriman, W. E., & Bowman, L. L. (1989). The mutual exclusivity bias in 1003 children’s word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54(3– 4, Serial No. 220). Mervis, C. B., & Bertrand, J. (1994). Acquisition of the novel name nameless (N3C) principle. Child Development, 65, 1646 –1663. Mervis, C. B., Golinkoff, R. M., & Bertrand, J. (1994). Two-year-olds readily learn multiple labels for the same basic-level category. Developmental Psychology, 65, 1163–1177. Mintz, T., & Gleitman, L. (1998, October). Adjectives really do modify nouns: 24- and 36-month-olds’ acquisition of adjectives. Paper presented at the 23rd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston. Moore, C., Angelopoulous, M., & Bennett P. (1999). Word learning in the context of referential and salience cues. Developmental Psychology, 35, 60 – 68. Naigles, L., & Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1995). Input to verb learning: Evidence for the plausibility of syntactic bootstrapping. Developmental Psychology, 31, 827– 837. Namy, L. L., & Waxman, S. R. (1998). Words and gestures: Infants’ interpretations of different forms of symbolic reference. Child Development, 69, 295–308. Ninio, A. (1980). Ostensive definition in vocabulary teaching. Journal of Child Language, 7, 565–573. Ninio, A., & Bruner, J. S. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling. Journal of Child Language, 5, 1–15. Prasada, S. (1993). Learning names for solid substances: Quantifying solid entities in terms of portions. Cognitive Development, 8, 83–104. Quine, W. V. O. (1960). Word and object. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Samuelson, L., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Memory and attention make smart learning: An alternative account of Akhtar, Carpenter, and Tomasello. Child Development, 69, 94 –104. Shatz, M., Behrend, D., Gelman, S. A., & Ebeling, K. S. (1996). Colour term knowledge in two-year-olds: Evidence for early competence. Journal of Child Language, 23, 177–199. Shipley, E. F., Kuhn, I., & Madden, E. C. (1983). Mother’s use of superordinate category terms. Journal of Child Language, 10, 571–588. Taylor, M., & Gelman, S. A. (1988). Adjectives and nouns: Children’s strategies for learning new words. Child Development, 59, 411– 419. Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Tomasello, M., & Akhtar, N. (1995). Two-year-olds use pragmatic clues to differentiate reference to objects and actions. Cognitive Development, 10, 201–224. Tversky, B., & Hemenway, K. (1984). Objects, parts, and categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 30, 161–191. Woodward, A. (2000). Constraining the problem space in early word learning. In R. Golinkoff, K. Hirsh-Pasek, L. Bloom, L. B. Smith, A. Woodward, N. Akhtar, et al., Becoming a word learner. A debate on lexical acquisition. (pp. 81–114). London: Oxford University Press. Woodward, A., & Markman, E. M. (1997). Early word learning: In W. Damon, D. Kuhn, & R. Seigler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 371– 410). New York: Wiley. Received June 25, 2001 Revision received June 13, 2002 Accepted June 13, 2002 䡲