Addictive Behaviors 32 (2007) 881 – 887 Short Communication Using the Extrinsic Affective Simon Test as a measure of implicit attitudes towards alcohol: Relationship with drinking behavior and alcohol problems Peter J. de Jong a,⁎, Reinout W. Wiers b,c , Marten van de Braak c , Jorg Huijding d a b d University of Groningen, The Netherlands Radbout University Nijmegen, The Netherlands c University of Maastricht, The Netherlands Erasmus University Rotterdam, The Netherlands Abstract In apparent contrast to the alleged importance of positive alcohol expectancies in alcohol (ab)use, a series of studies using the Implicit Association Test (IAT; [Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J.L.K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480]), found that heavy and light drinkers display more negative implicit attitudes toward alcohol than toward sodas (e.g., [Wiers, R. W., van Woerden, N., Smulders, F. T. Y., & de Jong, P. J. (2002). Implicit and explicit alcoholrelated cognitions in heavy and light drinkers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 648–658]). One explanation for this might be that the negative-alcohol IAT effect reflects an artifact of the IAT procedure and are due to its relative nature and/ or its sensitivity to task recoding strategies. Therefore, the present study used a non-relative measure that has been argued to be robust against participants' task recoding strategies (Extrinsic Affective Simon Test; EAST, [De Houwer, J. (2001). A structural and process analysis of the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 443– 451]) to test heavy (n=16) and light (n=16) drinkers' automatic affective associations with alcohol and sodas. Heavy and light drinkers displayed clear positive associations with sodas and neutral (or ambivalent) automatic associations with alcohol. Importantly, positive automatic alcohol associations predicted unique variance of alcohol (mis)use and was the single best predictor of individuals' alcohol problems, underlining the idea that they do play a role in alcohol (mis)use. © 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Implicit cognition; Attitudes; Alcohol misuse; Alcohol problems ⁎ Corresponding author. Clinical and Developmental Psychology, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen, The Netherlands. Tel.: +31 50 363 6825; fax: +31 50 363 7602. E-mail address: p.j.de.jong@rug.nl (P.J. de Jong). 0306-4603/$ - see front matter © 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.06.017 882 P.J. de Jong et al. / Addictive Behaviors 32 (2007) 881–887 Insight in people's automatic associations may help explaining the irrational aspects of addictive behaviors. Automatic associations are assumed to be related to relatively spontaneous responses, whereas deliberated associations are assumed to be related to more controlled responses (Wilson, Lindsey, & Schooler, 2000). In the absence of sufficient cognitive resources and/or motivation to control, automatic associations are also assumed to determine controllable behaviors. Following this, positive automatic associations may become highly dysfunctional as they may trigger impulsive approach behavior although the individual knows the stimulus is in fact harmful. In apparent contrast to the alleged importance of positive expectancies in motivating alcohol abuse, a series of studies using the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998), found that heavy (and light) drinkers display more negative implicit attitudes toward alcohol than toward sodas (De Houwer, Crombez, Koster, & De Beul, 2004; Wiers, van Woerden, Smulders, & de Jong, 2002). Yet, given its relative nature, the alcohol–soda IAT may at least partly reflect positive associations toward sodas rather than negative associations toward alcohol. Moreover, research investigating the underlying mechanisms of the IAT provided evidence to suggest that the negative alcohol IAT effects may reflect non-associative factors (e.g., salience asymmetries) rather than affective associations (Houben & Wiers, in press). Thus, the earlier findings may well underestimate the actual importance of positive implicit associations in alcohol misuse. Therefore, this study used the Extrinsic Affective Simon Test 1 (EAST; De Houwer, 2001), a non-relative measure designed to assess associations with singular targets and arguably less sensitive to non-associative factors like salience asymmetries. We separately tested heavy and light drinkers' automatic associations with alcohol and sodas, and examined the predictive validity of positive automatic associations for (retrospective) alcohol misuse. 1. Method 1.1. Participants and assessments Participants were 16 heavy drinking students2 (11 women; alcohol use: M = 30.0 standard servings 3 per week, S.D. = 18.5; alcohol problems: M = 12.4, S.D. = 6.4) and 16 light drinkers (11 women; alcohol use: M = 6.5 standard servings per week, S.D. = 4.8; alcohol problems: M = 4.1, S.D. = 4.6). Mean age was 20.3years (S.D. = 1.8). Alcohol use was assessed using the timeline follow-back method (Sobell & Sobell, 1990). Alcohol-related problems were assessed with the 18-item Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index (RAPI; 1 In this study we used the Extrinsic Affective Simon Test as it was originally designed by De Houwer (2001) and was described in Footnote 4 of his paper. More recently, other versions of the EAST have also been designed using a somewhat different format. 2 Heavy and light drinkers were selected based on a screening among 192 undergraduate students who indicated to drink at least one alcoholic consumption per week (n = 163). We selected all available participants scoring in the highest and lowest 10% of this sample on the basis of average alcohol consumption (as a function of sex) to guarantee sufficient meaningful difference between the heavy and light drinkers. Mean alcohol use in the eventually selected heavy and light drinkers were comparable to the alcohol use reported by Wiers et al. (2002) for their samples (heavy drinkers: 30 vs. 32 standard servings; light drinkers: 6.5 vs. 5.1 standard servings). 3 In The Netherlands a standard serving is the equivalent of 10 g alcohol. P.J. de Jong et al. / Addictive Behaviors 32 (2007) 881–887 883 Table 1 Measuring affective associations with alcohol and soda using the Extrinsic Affective Simon Test Phase # Trials Category, left hand Category, right hand 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 40 40 120 40 120 40 120 40 120 Solid Positive Solid or positive Fluid Fluid or positive Fluid Fluid or positive Solid Solid or positive Fluid Negative Fluid or negative Solid Solid or negative Solid Solid or negative Fluid Fluid or negative (C1) (R1) (R2) (C2) For half of the critical test phases, a particular target category (e.g., solid) shared the response assignment with the positive attribute category (C1, C2) whereas in the other half of the critical phases the response assignment for a particular target/attribute combination was reversed (R1, R2). Within each group, response assignments were balanced across individuals (half started with solid/positive and half of the participants started with solid/negative; for half of the participants within each initial combination, positive corresponded with left and for half positive corresponded with right). The critical phases are presented in boldface. White & Labouvie, 1989). The lay-out and experimental procedure of the present Extrinsic Affective Simon Test (EAST) are very similar to the IAT. For the outline of the task see Table 1. The attribute categories and exemplars were identical to the IAT used by Wiers et al. (2002). Yet, the target categories were now the neutral categories ‘solid’ and ‘fluid’ rather than ‘soda’ and ‘alcohol’. The crucial ‘fluid’ category consisted of the 5 alcoholic and the 5 non-alcoholic drinks used in the previous studies. The ‘solid’ category consisted of 10 exemplars of food items. Exemplar words were presented randomly in the middle of a screen (ITI 250ms). Participants sorted the words as fast as possible to the category names displayed in the left and right corners by pressing the ‘P’ or the ‘Q’. Phases 1, 2, 4, 6, and 8 were practice stages. During the critical test phases (3, 5, 7, and 9) participants sorted alternately target and attribute stimuli. For half of the test phases alcohol and soda shared the response button with positive, and for the other half with negative attributes. The word ‘fault’ appeared following an incorrect response. To index specific attitudes, we used 100mm Visual Analogue Scales (VAS) asking for each of the stimuli to what extend they matched with positive (0 = not at all, 100 = very good) or with negative. To assess their global attitudes toward alcohol (and sodas), participants indicated for each of the positive and negative attributes whether they considered the drinking of alcoholic drinks (and soda's) to be pleasant, etc., using VASs ranging from 0 (= disagree) to 100 (=agree). 2. Results 2.1. Specific explicit attitudes A 2 Drink × 2 Valence × 2 Group ANOVA showed a Drink by Valence interaction [F(1,30) = 59.4, p < .05], indicating that participants considered soda as more positive than alcohol (see Table 2). This was similar for both groups [F(1,30) < 1]. Subsequent t-tests indicated that participants' positive attitudes with alcohol tended to be weaker than the negative attitudes [t(31) = 1.9, p = .06]. With 884 P.J. de Jong et al. / Addictive Behaviors 32 (2007) 881–887 Table 2 Mean (S.D.) positive and negative explicit associations and global attitudes towards alcohol and soda for light and heavy drinkers Light drinkers Explicit associations (0–100) Positive Negative Global attitudes (0–100) Positive Negative Heavy drinkers Alcohol Soda Alcohol Soda 40.2 (15.4) 59.6 (15.0) 72.6 (12.9) 23.1 (13.0) 45.7 (18.8) 51.1 (23.2) 70.0 (12.4) 22.1 (14.0) 52.8 (15.2) 21.6 (14.9) 69.8 (15.5) 7.1 (4.9) 62.1 (18.3) 16.6 (12.5) 61.3 (12.5) 7.2 (9.6) respect to sodas, positive attitudes were significantly stronger than negative attitudes [t(31) = 11.2, p < .05]. 2.2. Global attitudes A 2 Drink × 2 Valence × 2 Group ANOVA showed a main effect of Valence [F(1,30) = 309.4, p < .05] revealing a generally positive attitude toward all drinks (see Table 2). In addition, a Drink by Valence interaction emerged [F(1,30) = 12.8, p < .05] which was qualified by a Drink by Valence by Group interaction [F(1,30) = 4.2, p < .05]. Subsequent tests showed that the Drink by Valence interaction was only significant for the light drinkers [F(1,15) = 16.8, p < .05], but not for the heavy drinkers [F(1,15) = 1.1]. Heavy drinkers showed a similar positive attitude toward sodas and alcoholic drinks, whereas light drinkers showed a clear preference for sodas. 2.3. Implicit associations 2.3.1. Soda A 2 Response (positive vs. negative) × 2 Group (heavy vs. light) ANOVA pertaining to participants' error rates, showed a significant main effect of Response, F(1,30) = 10.4, p < .05, indicating that individuals were relatively accurate when sodas shared the response button with positive (see Table 3). None of the other effects reached significance. The pattern of RT-data was consistent with that of the errors, yet a similar ANOVA revealed no significant effects. Table 3 Mean reaction times (RT) and error rates (%) for Alcohol and soda trials during the Extrinsic Affective Simon Test (EAST) as a function of response assignment Phase Light drinkers Heavy drinkers RT Error rate RT Error rate EAST–soda Soda + Positive Soda + Negative 740 (175) 740 (174) 8.5 (11.7) 11.9 (9.7) 707 (181) 746 (122) 4.3 (5.3) 13.9 (11.7) EAST–alcohol Alcohol + Positive Alcohol + Negative 748 (196) 708 (144) 10.8 (8.2) 10.0 (9.5) 709 (198) 743 (94) 6.0 (5.3) 6.9 (4.6) P.J. de Jong et al. / Addictive Behaviors 32 (2007) 881–887 885 Table 4 Pearson's correlations between implicit and explicit cognitions and alcohol use and problems 1. EAST-index 2. Explicit Affect 3. Global Attitude (alcohol) 4. Alcohol Problems 5. Alcohol Use (mean) 1 2 3 4 – .27 .19 .38 ⁎ .37 ⁎ .54 ⁎⁎ .08 .17 .33 .47 ⁎⁎ .78 ⁎⁎ The EAST-index was calculated by subtracting the performance on alcohol trials when alcohol shared the response key with negative, from the performance on trials of when alcohol shared the response key with positive. Hence high scores indicate relatively positive alcohol associations. Explicit Affect was calculated as the mean association scores for alcohol exemplars and positive minus the mean scores of alcohol exemplars and negative. Global Attitude was calculated as the mean of the negative attributes minus the mean of the positive attributes. Alcohol Problems represent the score on the Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index. Alcohol Use (mean) reflects the average weekly alcohol consumption. ⁎ p < .05. ⁎⁎ p < .01. 2.3.2. Alcohol 2 Response × 2 Group ANOVAs pertaining to participants' error rates and RTs, revealed no main effects of Response [F(1,30) < 1], indicative of a neutral or ambivalent implicit attitude towards alcoholic drinks (see Table 3). The pattern of error rates as well as RTs were similar for both groups [F(1,30) < 1]. To test whether positive implicit attitudes have independent predictive validity over and above the explicit measures, we carried out a regression analysis with alcohol problems (RAPI) as the dependent variable, and EAST-alcohol (RT-index4,5 ), the explicit equivalent of the EAST, and the global attitude measure as the independent variables. The EAST-index was the only variable with independent predictive properties [β = .41; p = .02], indicating that relatively positive implicit associations with alcohol are associated with more negative consequences of alcohol use. A similar analysis with mean alcohol use as the dependent variable indicated that the global attitude measure best predicted mean retrospective alcohol use [β = .41, p = .01], whereas the EAST-index showed additional predictive power [β = .30, p = .06], although the effect did not reach the conventional level of significance. Pearson's p–m correlations are shown in Table 4. 3. Discussion The present findings support the idea that the previously reported negative IAT effects may at least partly reflect positive associations with soda rather than negative associations with alcohol. Both heavy and light drinkers displayed unambiguously positive implicit associations with sodas, whereas with 4 The EAST-effect was calculated by subtracting the performance on alcohol trials when alcohol shared the response key with negative, from the performance on trials of when alcohol shared the response key with positive. Hence high scores indicate relatively positive alcohol associations. 5 To index the reliability of inter-individual variance we computed for both the alcohol–EAST effect and the soda–EAST effect Cronbach's alphas based on split-half EAST scores. For alcohol α = .48, and for soda RT α = .48. 886 P.J. de Jong et al. / Addictive Behaviors 32 (2007) 881–887 respect to alcoholic drinks the positive and negative automatic affective associations were of similar strength. This is consistent with the idea that people have an ambivalent rather than a negative implicit attitude towards alcohol (cf. De Houwer et al., 2004). Such an interpretation is also in agreement with the results of the explicit measures showing clear positive and clear negative attitudes with alcohol. In support of the idea that relatively positive automatic associations are involved in alcohol (mis)use, the EAST showed a positive relation with alcohol-related problems and mean alcohol use. In addition, the EAST was found to have predictive validity over and above explicit measures with respect to individuals' mean alcohol use and was the single best predictor of individuals' alcohol problems. Although these findings are also consistent with the idea that positive associations are a consequence rather than a cause of alcohol (ab)use, it seems not particularly convincing to argue that the negative consequences of alcohol use might have resulted in positive implicit alcohol associations. Some limitations should be acknowledged. First, the EAST contained bipolar attribute categories. Therefore, the present study cannot differentiate between the possibility that alcohol use is related to relatively weak negative and/or to relatively strong positive automatic alcohol associations. Second, although the pattern of EAST-results was in line with the idea that heavy drinkers are characterized by relatively positive automatic alcohol associations, the difference between light and heavy drinkers did not reach significance. One explanation might be that since our heavy drinkers consisted of undergraduates rather than treatment seeking people, the level of alcohol problems may not have been sufficiently extreme and homogeneous to reliably differentiate between groups in a factorial analysis. Consistent with this, the correlational analyses did reach significance (see also Wiers et al., 2002). To conclude, the present pattern of results strongly suggests that the negative implicit alcohol associations that were found in previous studies were due to the specific characteristics of the alcohol–soda IAT. The finding that the EAST predicted unique variance concerning the indices of alcohol misuse not only sustains the alleged importance of relatively positive evaluative alcohol associations in drinking behavior, but also underlines the importance of implicit assessments of alcohol-related cognitions. Acknowledgement The authors are grateful to Annie Raven for her assistance during data acquisition, Bert Hoekzema for his technical assistance, and Jan De Houwer for his comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. References De Houwer, J. (2001). A structural and process analysis of the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 443−451. De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., Koster, E. H. W., & De Beul, N. (2004). Implicit alcohol-related cognitions in clinical samples of heavy drinkers. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35, 275−286. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464−1480. Houben, K., & Wiers, R. W. (in press). A test of the salience asymmetry interpretation of the Alcohol–IAT. Experimental Psychology. Sobell, L. C., & Sobell, M. B. (1990). Self-report issues in alcohol abuse: State of the art and future directions. Behavioral Assessment, 12, 77−90. P.J. de Jong et al. / Addictive Behaviors 32 (2007) 881–887 887 White, H. R., & Labouvie, E. W. (1989). Towards the assessment of adolescent problem drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 50, 30−37. Wiers, R. W., van Woerden, N., Smulders, F. T. Y., & de Jong, P. J. (2002). Implicit and explicit alcohol-related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 648−658. Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101−126.