Children's Television Viewing: Attention and Comprehension of Auditory versns Visual Infomiation Kathy Pezdek Claremont Graduate School Eileen F. Hartman California State College, San Bernardino PEZDEK, KATHY, and HARTMAN, EILEEN F . Children's Television Viewing: Attention and Comprehension of Auditory versus Visual Information. CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1983, 54, 10151023. This study examines the relationship between children's attention and comprehension of auditory and visual infonnation on television. 60 5-year-olds viewed a videotape of "Sesame Street" and were then asked comprehension questions. Equal numbers of children viewed the television with (a) toys available to play with (the visual attention manipulation); (b) a record playing in the room (the auditory attention manipulation); or (c) no toys or record available (the control condition). All children viewed the same television sequence, which consisted of (a) visual segments, (b) auditory segments, and (c) mixed modality segments. The major results were that the children effectively distributed their attention such that they could process auditory and visual information from television while performing other activities. Further, the children were sensitive to which segments required visual attention and which did not, and they were able to spontaneously adjust their pattern of visual attention appropriately. These results indicate that children utilize a fairly sophisticated cognitive processing strategy while watching television. The large majority of research on the effects of television on children has examined the social impact of television on children's behavior (e.g., aggression and presocial behavior). Although it seems obvious that social lessons cannot be learned unless the lessons are understood and remembered, surprisingly little research has examined these cognitive processing aspects of children's television viewing. The present study investigates the relationship between children's attention and comprehension of infonnation presented visually versus auditorily on television. . J ,^ ^ . ,, . „ ,. u t 1 Adults typically appear to watch television by following the action auditorily (often while performmg some household task) and looking at the screen periodically simply to confirm their comprehension. This is consistent with the description of television as a "radio with pictures" (cf. Bechtel, Achelpohl, & Akers, 1972; Szalai, 1972). However, this method of watching television assumes a fairly sophisticated cognitive processing ability. It assumes that a person can (a) follow the plot from the auditory channel while participating in some other activity, (b) develop hypotheses about what is likely to happen next, so that he or she can (c) look at the television when it is necessary to catch a particularly interesting or important visual event. Current research on children's attention indicates that children are generally inefficient at selective, divided, and maintained attention (Lipps Birch, 1976; Pick, Frankel, & Hess, 1975; Strutt, Anderson, & Well, 1975). These findings suggest age differences in the pattern of television processing, particularly regarding ^j^^ relationship between watching and listening to television, Results from several studies suggest that children's attention to television is more strongly correlated with audio than with visual attributes of television programs (Anderson & Levin, 1976; Wartella & Ettema, 1974). These results appear to suggest that children process more of the auditorily than This research was supported by a grant to the first author from the National Institute of Education. We thank Ed Teyber and Dan Anderson for their helpful comments on this manuscript. Reprint requests and correspondence should be sent to Kathy Pezdek, Department of Psychology, Claremont Craduate School, Claremont, Califomia 91711. [Child Development, 1983,54,1015-1023. © 1983 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/83/5404-0020$01.00] 1016 Child Development visually presented information on television. However, a number of studies have suggested that, although children's attention is drawn to television by auditory attributes of programs, they actually remember visually presented information better than auditorily presented information (Hayes & Birnbaum, 1980; Zuckennan, Ziegler, & Stevenson, 1978; Pezdek, Note 1). Lorch, Anderson, and Levin (1979) examined 5-year-old children's television processing using intact "Sesame Street" programs. They manipulated children's visual attention to the television by including two conditions of viewing. One group watched television with a variety of toys available for play. A second group watched television without the toys. In the no-toys group, visual attention to the television averaged 87%, whereas visual attention for the group with toys averaged only 44%. Interestingly, despite the visual attention differences, (a) the two groups did not differ in accuracy on comprehension test questions, and (b) in the toys group, there was a significant and substantial positive correlation between visual attention at the time the infonnation was given on questions and comprehension of those questions, including questions based on information presented only auditorily. This latter result suggests that comprehension of all aspects of the program was better during periods of visual attention. When children looked away from the television, their comprehension of even the auditorily presented information declined. A model of children's television viewing was derived from these data and further elaborated by Anderson (Note 2). This model posits that, when children watch television, they can effectively divide their attention between the television and other activities. When they are looking at the television, they are semantically processing both the auditory and visual channels. When children look away from the television, they monitor the auditory channel only at a level sufficient to detect auditory cues signaling the need to return full attention to the television. By at least age 4, children seem to have learned the nonsemantic auditory features (e.g., lively music, change of voice, sound efFects, etc.) that are predictive of important content (Anderson & Levin, 1976). The auditory cues thus guide visual attention when children look away from the television. The aim of the present study is, first, to test the replicability of the results of Lorch et al. (1979). Second, this study extends Lorch et al.'s (1979) model of the relationship between attention and comprehension of television to account for auditory attention and comprehension processes. In addition to the control and toys conditions, the present study includes a record condition to manipulate children's auditory attention to television. Thus, variations in auditory as well as visual attention can be related to comprehension of television. Third, the Lorch et al. (1979) model was developed on the basis of television segments in which the auditory and visual channels were well integrated. With segments of this type, auditory cues are available to guide visual attention. The present study examines children's pattern of television viewing using auditory segments and visual segments as well as the mixed modality segments similar to those utilized by Lorch et al. (1979). In the visual segments, most of the plot-relevant content was presented visually, and the visual content was not supported by the typical auditory cues. In the auditory segments, most of the plotrelevant content was presented auditorily and not visually. The type of television segment was manipulated in this study to test whether children are sensitive to variations in the presentation modality of television segments and whether they differentially adjust their viewing as a function of which channel the principal information is presented on. Two experiments were conducted. Experiment 1 was designed to test and verify that the three different types of segments— auditory, visual, and mixed modality—were in fact modality specific as indicated. Experiment 2 tests the major issues of interest in this study. Experiment 1 The purpose of the first experiment is to validate the distinctions among the three types of segments. Subjects viewed all three types of segments in one of three presentation conditions: (a) only the auditory track was presented; (b) only the visual track was presented; or (c) both the auditory and visual tracks were presented. Several predictions follow from the definition of the three types of program segments. First, the auditory segments are accu- Pezdek and Hartman rately defined as "auditory" if comprehension of these segments is significantly better in the auditory-only than visual-only presentation condition and nonsignificantly different in the auditory-only and both auditory and visual conditions. Second, visual segments are accurately defined as visual if comprehension of these segments is significantly better in the visual-only than auditory-only presentation condition, and nonsignificantly different in the visual-only condition compared with the both auditory and visual condition. Finally, the mixed modality segments involve processing both auditory and visual information. Although comprehension of an entire segment of this type required processing both auditory and visual information, specific questions were derived that tapped largely auditorily presented information or largely visually presented information. For the auditory questions from the mixed modality segments, it was predicted that comprehension would be significantly less in the visual-only presentation condition than in either of the other two conditions. For the visual questions from the mixed modality segments, it was predicted that comprehension would be significantly less in the auditory-only presentation condition than in either of the other two conditions. Method Subjects and design.—Thirty 5-year-old children were recruited from kindergarten classes in public schools in Claremont, California. The experiment utilized a 3 (segment type) x 3 (presentation condition) mixed factorial design. All subjects viewed a "Sesame Street" sequence that contained approximately equal amounts of three types of segments. The central infonnation in each of the three types of segments was presented visually, auditorily, or on both the visual and auditory channels. Ten subjects (five males and five females) were randomly assigned to view the television sequence with only the auditory track presented, only the visual track presented, or both the auditory and visual tracks presented. The dependent variable was recall accuracy on comprehension questions. 1017 domly ordered segments. The segments were rated by three adult viewers as containing principally auditory, visual, or mixed modality information. A segment was labeled visual if the central information was presented via the visual channel. An example of a visual segment would be a film, with a constant musical background and no verbalization, showing how to grow beans. A segment was labeled auditory if the central information was presented via the auditory channel. An example of an auditory segment would be Bert and Ernie (two Muppets) talking to each other in one setting. They nod to each other as they talk, but the central content is in their conversation, not in their movement. A segment was labeled as mixed modality if, throughout the segment, central information was presented on the auditory and the visual channels, and the complete segment could be understood only with both the auditory and visual channels on. Most television segments are of this type. An example would be Big Bird talking with Maria about repairing a radio while they are doing so together in the shop. It should be pointed out that, although the auditory segments and visual segments are not typical of most television shows, they are not in fact uncommon. In selecting the segments for the present study we found at least one auditory and one visual segment in each of 10 segments of "Sesame Street" viewed. Of the 11 segments utilized in the present study, three were auditory (6 min and 30 sec), five were visual (6 min and 45 sec), and three were mixed modality (7 min). Procedure.—Each child was tested individually in a comfortably furnished room on the campus. No toys or distractors were available. The children were instructed to pay careful attention to the television because they were going to be asked some questions afterward. The experimenter remained in the room during the session to encourage the children to maintain attention to the television. Ten subjects were presented with the sequence with the visual track turned ofF(the auditory-only condition). Ten subjects were presented with the sequence with the auditory track turned off (the visual-only condition). Ten subjects were presented the sequence in a normal fashion with both the auditory and visual tracks on. Stimulus materials.—The sequence of "Sesame Street" segments was edited by Immediately following the television the experimenters for this study. The complete sequence was approximately 20 min sequence, the comprehension test was long, was in color, and consisted of 11 ran- administered. Thirty-five comprehension 1018 Child Development questions were selected to query specific aspects of the three types of segments. Nine questions were based on information judged to have been presented auditorily. Thirteen questions were based on information judged to have been visually presented in the segments. Two types of test questions were developed for the mixed modality programs: seven questions were based on the auditorily presented content, and six questions were based on the visually presented content. Questions followed the order in which the corresponding segments had been presented. The comprehension test utilized children's recall. Each answer was scored on the following three-point scale: two points if the child answered correctly, one point if a verbal prompt from the experiment was necessary before the child gave the correct answer, and no points if the child did not give the correct answer even with the help of the prompt. Prompts were essentially restatements of the original questions but with an additional piece of information given. For example, one of the original questions was, "What was Big Bird making?" The correct answer was "a model of Big Bird" or "a bird." If the child could not answer this question or answered incorrectly, a prompt was offered, "What was Big Bird making out of a box?" If the child did not answer correctly when prompted, the experimenter gave the correct response before proceeding. The acceptable "correct answers" for each question were specified in advance, making the scoring of answers quite clear-cut. Most correct answers were only two or three words long. Results and Discussion The specific predictions in Experiment 1 were tested with four separate one-way analyses of variance. Comprehension accu- racy (the range of scores = 0-2) as a function of presentation condition was examined for responses to visual segments, auditory segments, auditory questions from mixed modality segments, and visual questions from mixed modality segments. The data are presented in Table 1. The rejection region for all analyses is p < .05. With the auditory segments, comprehension accuracy significantly varied with presentation condition, F(2,27) = 26.35, MS^. = .130. A priori t tests yielded significantly better comprehension in the auditory-only condition (1.37) than in the visual-only condition (.43). Comprehension accuracy did not differ between the combined auditory and visual condition (1.51) and the auditory-only condition. These results verify that the information needed to answer questions from the auditory segments was available on the auditory track, but insufficient infonnation was available on the visual track. Comprehension accuracy for visual segments also varied significantly with presentation condition, F(2,27) = 32.33, MS,. = .087. A priori t tests yielded significantly better comprehension in the video-only condition (1.36) than in the audio-only condition (.47). Comprehension accuracy did not differ between the combined auditory and visual condition (1.41) and the visualonly condition. Therefore, it can be concluded that the information needed to answer questions from the visual segments was available on the visual track but not on the auditory track. Comprehension accuracy for auditory questions from the mixed modality segments significantly varied with presentation condition, F(2,27) = 7.43, MS, = .152. Comprehension was significantly less in the TABLE 1. MEAN CODED COMPREHENSION SCORES IN EACH CONDITION IN EXPERIMENT 1 (0-2 Range) QUESTION TYPE FROM EACH SECMENT PRESENTATION CONDITION Auditory only Visual only Auditory and visual Auditory Visual Mixed Modality (Auditory) Mixed Modality (Visual) 1.37 43 .47 1.36 1.09 .23 .53 1.00 1.51 1.41 1.08 1.26 Pezdek and Hartman 1019 visually attended to the television and (2) recall accuracy on comprehension questions. Setting and procedure.—Each child individually viewed a "Sesame Street" sequence in a comfortably furnished room. In the toys condition, several toys were available in the room for the child to play with. In the record condition, a children's record was playing on a phonograph in the back of the room. The record was "Peter and the Wolf" with its musical background. The volume of the record player was set slightly lower than that of the television (detennined from where most children sat) and was constant for all subjects. The children were instructed to watch television just as they would if they were in their own home. They were told that they could play with the toys (if present) or The results of Experiment 1 validate the listen to the record (if playing) if they materials for use in Experiment 2. The non- wanted to. They were also told that they zero response rate for the auditory questions would be asked a few questions about the in the visual-only condition and for visual television sequence when it was finished. questions in the auditory-only condition can After the study was explained the child be accounted for by three factors. These factors are (a) the extent to which the correct was left alone in the viewing room. The paranswer could be inferred from information ent was taken to another room during the in the other modality, (b) previously ac- session and instructed to fill out a questionquired knowledge of and familiarity with naire on the television-viewing habits of the child. Two observers behind a one-way mir"Sesame Street," and (c) guessing. ror recorded the child's visual attention to the television. (One of the two observers was Experiment 2 blind to the predictions of the experiment.) Experiment 2 specifically examines the The observers depressed a button attached relationship between children's attention to a timer every time the child looked at and comprehension of auditory and visual the television and released it when the child information on television. looked away. The duration of attention in seconds and the cumulative frequency of Method Subjects, design, and materials.—The glances were recorded for each of the 11 insubjects were 60 5-year-old children re- dividual television segments. The observers cruited from kindergarten classes in public noted the beginning and end of each segschools in the San Bernardino, Califomia, ment on a video monitor in the observation metropolitan area. The children were room. The two observers produced an interobserver reliability correlation of .98. brought to the campus by a parent. visual-only condition (.23) than in the auditory-only condition (1.09) or the combined auditory and visual condition (1.08). This is consistent with the definition of these items. Finally, comprehension accuracy for visual questions from the mixed modality segments also significantly varied with presentation condition, F(2,27) = 8.28, MS^ = .143. Comprehension was significantly less in the auditory-only condition (.53) than in the visual-only condition (1.00) or the combined auditory and visual condition (1.26). The visual-only condition and the combined condition did not significantly differ from each other. This is consistent with the definition of these items. The experiment utilized a 3 (television-viewing condition) x 3 (segment type) X 2 (sex of subject) mixed factorial design. All subjects viewed the "Sesame Street" television sequence pretested in Experiment 1. This sequence included the same random ordering of visual, auditory, and mixed modality segments. Ten female and 10 male subjects were randomly assigned to view the television sequence with either toys available for play (the visual attention manipulation), a record playing in the room (the auditory attention manipulation), or no toys or record available (the control condition). The dependent variables were (1) the percent of the total time for each segment that subjects Immediately following the television sequence, the experimenter returned to the viewing room for the comprehension test. The 35 comprehension questions pretested in Experiment 1 were utilized in this experiment. These included auditory questions from the auditory segments, visual questions from the visual segments, auditory questions from the mixed modality segments, and visual questions from the mixed modality segments. Questions followed the order in which the corresponding segments had been presented. Each answer was scored from 2 (correct answer without prompt) to 0 (no correct answer even with prompt). 1020 Child Development Results The principal measures were the amount of visual attention and comprehension accuracy. Separate analyses were carried out on these measures. The rejection region for all analyses was p < .05. Sex of subject was included as a factor in all analyses. However, because no significant sex effects resulted in the analyses, the data reported have been collapsed across sex. children's visual attention to the television. Auditory attention may have been reduced, but there was no way to observe this except to infer attention differences from comprehensive differences. A significant effect for type of television segment also resulted, F(2,108) = 30.52, MS^ = .008. Post hoc comparisons revealed that subjects' visual attention to the television was significantly higher with the visual segments (73%) and the mixed modality segments (76%) than with auditory segments (64%). The effect of the type of segment did not interact with the television viewing condition. Visual attention.—An analysis of variance was performed on the percent of time that each child visually attended to television as a function of the television-viewing condition, type of segment, and sex of subject. These data are presented in Table 2. Comprehension accuracy.—This study probes whether the conditions that produce differences in attention also result in differences in comprehension. An analysis of variance was performed on the coded comprehension scores (0-2 range). The factors in the analysis were television-viewing condition, type of question, and sex of subject. There were four types of comprehension test questions, as described in Experiment 1. Mean comprehension scores are presented in Table 3. Presenting toys did effectively reduce visual attention to the television relative to the other two viewing conditions. The effect of the television-viewing condition was significant, F(2,54) = 72.62, MSe = .048. Post hoc comparisons indicated that subjects' visual attention to the television program was significantly higher in the control condition (88%) and in the record condition (82%) than in the condition with toys (44%). Attention in the record condition and the control condition did not differ. It should be noted that the measure of attention utilized was the Comprehension did not significantly vary as a function of the television-viewing TABLE 2 MEAN PERCENTAGE OF VISUAL ATTENTION FOR EACH TELEVISION-VIEWINC CONDITION AS A FUNCTION OF THE TYPE OF SEGMENT IN EXPERIMENT 2 TELEVISIONVIEWING CONDITION TYPE OF SEGMENT Auditory Visual Mixed Modality Mean .81 .37 .74 .89 .44 .87 .93 .50 .86 .88 .44 .82 Control . . . . Toys Record TABLE 3 MEAN CODED COMPREHENSION SCORES IN EACH CONDITION IN EXPERIMENT 2 (0-2 Range) QUESTION TYPE FROM EACH SEGMENT TELEVISIONVIEWING CONDITION Control Toys Record . Auditory Visual Mixed Modality (Auditory) 1.31 1.17 1.09 1.31 .92 1.20 1.06 .98 .82 Mixed Modality (Visual) 1.17 1.08 , 1.17 Mean 1.21 1.04 1.07 Pezdek and Hartman 1021 condition, despite differences in visual attention in these conditions. However, a significant effect for type of question was observed, F(3,162) = 7.94, MS, = .081. Comprehension was lower with auditory questions from mixed modality segments than with the other three conditions.^ The more interesting effect, however, was the significant interaction of television-viewing condition x type of question, F(6,162) = 2.99, MSe = -081. This result suggests a relationship between attention and comprehension that is specific within the visual modality and the auditory modality. Dunnett's post hoc contrasts were perfonned to compare the control condition with each of the other viewing conditions for each of the four types of questions. Comprehension of auditory segments was significantly reduced in the record condition (1.09) but not in the toys condition (1.17) relative to the control condition (1.31). With visual program segments, comprehension was significantly reduced in the toys condition (.92) but not in the record condition (1.20) relative to the control condition (1.31). With auditory questions from mixed modality segments, comprehension was significantly reduced in the record condition (.82) but not in the toys condition (.98) relative to the control condition (1.06). No significant differences resulted with visual questions from the mixed modality segments. Correlational data.—The relationship between visual attention and comprehension of segments was further examined. Correlations were calculated between the mean percent of visual attention and the mean comprehension score for subjects across all program segments. The overall correlation was low but significant, r = -I-.30, t{58) = 2.35. To more closely observe the rela- tionship between visual attention and comprehension, we computed separate correlations for auditory and visual segments in each television-viewing condition. Only auditory segments and visual segments were examined here since it was not possible to determine for the mixed modality segments at what particular part of the show the child was visually attending. Significant correlations between visual attention and comprehension resulted for visual program segments in the control condition (r = +.43), in the toys condition (r = +.61), and in the record condition (r = +.36).^ No significant correlations resulted for auditory segments in any of the viewing conditions. Television-viewing questionnaire.— Parents completed a questionnaire on the television-viewing habits of their child. Children in this sample watched an average of 2.75 hours of television a day (with a range from 1 hour to 7 hours). These results are consistent with national norms for children reported by Lyle and Hoffman (1972a, 1972b). This confirms that the children in this study were typical television viewers. General Discussion The results are discussed in terms of three specific issues. First, were the results of Lorch et al. (1979) replicated in the comparable conditions in the present study? The answer is yes. In the mixed modality segments, attention was significantly reduced in the toys condition relative to the control condition, but there were no significant differences in comprehension accuracy between these two conditions. The explanation offered by Lorch et al. (1979) for this result is that, although the children were attending to the television less in the toys condition than in the control condition, they were sensitive to when it was necessary to return attention ' The reader is cautioned against interpreting the performance differences among the segment types in terms of modality differences in recall. For example, the equal recall scores for auditory and visual segments in the control condition might appear to contradict numerous findings supporting the visual superiority effect. However, in the present study, these auditory and visual scores are based on different test questions, and no attempt was made to equate the difficulty of these questions. * The correlation coefficients hetween visual attention and comprehension reported here are considerably smaller than those reported hy Lorch et al. (1979). However, the coefficients reported by Lorch et al. (1979) were calculated for each question from continuously recorded attention data. They thus were ahle to measure the percent visual attention during the precise time that the information needed to answer each question was given and the comprehension accuracy on each question. These authors have indicated that, when they calculated correlation coefficients on overall percent visual attention per child and comprehension accuracy per child, smaller positive correlation coefficients resulted, more similar to those reported in the present study (Anderson, Note 3). 1022 Child Development to the television. The cues for when they needed to return full attention to the television were in the nonsemantic features of the accompanying auditory track. But subjects were semantically processing the auditory information primarily during periods of visual attention. This interpretation by Lorch et al. (1979) is strengthened by their result that there was a substantial positive correlation between visual attention and comprehension of both auditory and visual infonnation. Lorch et al. (1979) utilized a continuous method of recording attention data so that they were able to measure the percent of visual attention during the precise interval that the information needed to answer each question was given. This method was not utilized in the present study, and thus these correlations are not available for comparison with the Lorch et al. (1979) study. However, the parallel findings in terms of both the attention data and the comprehension data suggest that subjects were performing similarly with the mixed modality segments in the two studies. An additional test of the Lorch et al. (1979) model is offered in the present study by including visual segments in which no supporting auditory cues are available to signal important visual content. With these visual segments, in contrast to the mixed modality segments, comprehension was significantly reduced in the toys condition relative to the control condition. When subjects looked away from the television to play with toys, there were no auditory cues to rely on to redirect attention to the television. However, the record did not significantly reduce comprehension of visual segments because there were no important auditory cues to be interfered with by the competing auditory stimulus. The second purpose of the present study is to examine children's processing of segments that are primarily auditory or primarily visual. The results of Experiment 1 suggest that children did not have to look at auditory segments to comprehend them, nor did they have to listen to visual segments to comprehend them. The question of interest is whether, in the process of viewing a series of segments, children are sensitive to which segments require visual attention and which segments do not. To examine this issue, subjects' attention and comprehension of auditory segments and visual segments are examined. If children are sensitive to when visual attention to the television is neces- sary, then attention should be less with the auditory segments than with the visual segments. This prediction was supported. In all viewing conditions, subjects visually attended to the auditory segments less than to the visual segments. This result is particularly impressive given that, in this experiment, the type of segment was manipulated within subjects, and subjects were viewing a randomly arranged series of 11 segments, each only a few minutes long. Thus, these 5-year-old children were able to rather quickly evaluate the presentation modality of segments and spontaneously adjust their pattern of visual attention appropriately. The third purpose of the present study is to extend Lorch et al.'s (1979) model of the relationship between attention and comprehension to account for auditory attention and comprehension processes. As can be seen in Tables 2 and 3, presenting the toys effectively reduced visual attention to auditory segments without reducing comprehension of the segments relative to the control condition. Further, the overall correlation between comprehension and attention of auditory segments did not approach significance in any of the viewing conditions. Subjects were thus semantically processing the auditory information even during periods of low visual attention to the television. This differs from the results reported by Lorch et al. (1979) with mixed modality segments. They reported a positive correlation between visual attention and comprehension of both auditory and visual information. The results of the present study suggest that, although children may ordinarily semantically process auditory information primarily during periods of visual attention, they have altemative processing strategies available to them. For example, when an entire segment requires auditory but not visual processing, it appears that children can semantically process the auditory channel without visually attending to the television. They can listen without looking. As discussed above, apparently the children could discriminate between those segments that did not require processing visual information and those that did require visual processing. The result that the record reduced comprehension of auditory segments can be attributed to modality-specific interference. An alternative explanation of the above results exists. Perhaps the subjects were not semantically processing the auditor)' seg- Pezdek and Hartman ments during periods of visual inattention but were, rather, using cues in the auditory track to redirect full attention to the visual track and then were only semantically processing the auditory track during periods of visual attention. This interpretation follows from the suggestion by Lorch et al. (1979) that semantic processing of auditory information occurs primarily during periods of visual attention. However, this conclusion was based on mixed modality segments in which there were auditory cues that accompanied important visual content. This strategy is unlikely with the auditory segments utilized in the present study, because there was no plot-relevant visual content and therefore no auditory cues suggesting a need to direct visual attention to the television. The present results extend our understanding of the relationship between children's attention and comprehension of information on television. These findings suggest that the cognitive processing ability of 5-year-old children is much more sophisticated than is suggested by laboratory research on children's attention (e.g., Lipps Burch, 1976; Strutt, Anderson, & Well, 1975). When the television is used as a stimulus, it appears that 5-year-olds are able to effectively distribute their attention such that they can differentially process auditory and visual aspects of television while performing other activities (e.g., playing with toys or listening to a record). These results run counter to the position that children passively absorb the information bombarding them from television. It is hoped that continued research in this area will lead to a better understanding of children's true cognitive processing abilities as well as to the production of television programs that are more comprehensible to children. Reference Notes 1. Pezdek, K. Memory for visual and auditory 2. 3. information on television. Paper presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Philadelphia, Novemher 1981. Anderson, D. Active and passive processes in children's television viewing. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, New York, September 1979. Anderson, D. Personal communication, Novemher 1980. 1023 References Anderson, D. R., & Levin, S. R. Young children's attention to "Sesame Street." Child Development, 1976, 47, 80&-811. Bechtel, R. B., Achelpohl, C , & Akers, R. Correlates hetween observed hehavior and questionnaire responses on television viewing. In E. A. Rubinstein, G. A. Comstock, & J. P. Murray (Eds.), Television and social behavior (Vol. 4): Television in day-to-day life: Patterns of use. Washington, D.C: Government Printing Office, 1972. Hayes, D. S., & Bimhaum, D. W. Preschoolers' retention of televised events: Is a picture worth a thousand words? Developmental Psychology, 1980, 16, 410-416. Lipps Birch, L. Age trends in children's timesharing performance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1976, 22, 331-345. Lorch, E. P., Anderson, D. R., & Levin, S. R. The relationship of visual attention to children's comprehension of television. Child Development, 1979, 50, 722-727. Lyle, J. L., & Hoffman, H. R. Children's use of television and other media. In E. A. Rubinstein, G. A. Comstock, & J. P. Murray (Eds.), Television and social behavior (Vol. 4): Television in day-to-day life: Patterns of use. Washington, D.C: Government Printing Office, 1972. (a) Lyle, J. L., & Hoffman, H. R. Explorations in patterns of television viewing by preschool-age children. In E. A. Rubinstein, G. A. Comstock, & J. P. Murray (Eds.), Television and social behavior (Vol. 4): Television in dayto-day life: Patterns of use. Washington, D.C: Government Printing Office, 1972. (h) Pick, A. D., Frankel, D. G., & Hess, V. L. Children's attention: The development of selectivity. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Review of child development research (Vol. 5). Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1975. Strutt, G. F., Anderson, D. R., & Well, A. D. A developmental study of the effects of irrelevant information on speeded classification. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1975, 20, 127-135. Szalai, E. The use of time: Daily activities of urban and suburban populations in twelve countries. The Hague: Mouton, 1972. Wartella, E., & Ettema, J. S. A cognitive developmental study of children's attention to television commercials. Communication Research, 1974, 1, 69-87. Zuckerman, P., Ziegler, M., & Stevenson, H. W. Children's viewing of television and recognition memory of commercials. Child Development, 1978, 49, 96-104.