College Student Conceptions of Geological Time and the Disconnect Between Ordering and Scale Julie C. Libarkin Department of Geological Sciences and Division Science and Mathematics Education, Michigan State University, 206 Natural Sciences Bldg, East Lansing, MI 48824 Josepha P. Kurdziel Department of Ecology & Evolutionary Biology, University of Michigan 830 N. University Ave., Kraus Natural Sciences Bldg., Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1048, josephak@umich.edu Steven W. Anderson MAST Institute, University of Northern Colorado, Greeley, CO 80639 steven.anderson@unco.edu ABSTRACT College student conceptions of the scale of geologic time and the relationships between time and geological or biological events were evaluated through interviews, open-ended questionnaires, and student generated timelines collected from four institutions. Our data indicate students hold a number of alternative conceptions about the Earth's formation and the appearance of life, and these ideas are remarkably consistent across institutions. Transferability of these findings was evaluated via comparison with Geoscience Concept Inventory questions related to geologic time collected from 43 institutions nationwide. Detailed evaluation of student timelines reveals a notable disconnect between the relative relationships between the age of the Earth, the time required for the origin of the first life forms (prokaryotes), and the evolution of dinosaurs and humans. Students generally placed these events in the correct relative order, but had a poor understanding of the scale of time between events. Intriguingly, timelines can be mapped onto ternary diagrams, and the relationship between ternary diagram zoning and specific ideas of geologic time is explored. We found that some students, for example those with a young Earth perspective, map onto specific conceptual zones on ternary diagrams. INTRODUCTION Student understanding of geologic time has been explored in a variety of settings, including elementary (Ault, 1982; Schoon, 1992), high school (Marques and Thompson, 1997; Oversby, 1996; Schoon, 1992), college (DeLaughter et al., 1998; Libarkin et al., 2005; Schoon, 1992), pre-service (Gosselin and Macklem-Hurst, 2002) and in-service teacher (Trend, 2001; Dahl et al., 2005) populations. Researchers have focused on both relative (Trend, 1998; Trend, 2000; Trend, 2001; Dodick and Orion, 2003) and absolute time (Libarkin et al., 2005; DeLaughter et al., 1998; Trend, 2001), and some work shows intriguing relationships between relative positioning of geologic events and understanding of absolute geologic time (e.g., Trend, 2001; Dodick and Orion, 2003; Libarkin et al., 2005). Researchers generally find that students are more comfortable with relative time than absolute ages (Trend, 2001), although these same students may have a poor understanding of absolute ages (Libarkin et al., 2005). In addition, young children tend to have a difficult time extracting analogies about relative ages to relative age dating of geologic strata in general (Ault, 1982). Trend (1998; 2000; 2001) and Dahl et al. (2005) have conducted detailed studies of children, pre-service and in-service educators' ideas about and interest in geologic time. These works employ a similar event-ordering task that requires participants to consider when various events happened in Earth's history, and more specifically, the relative order of geologic events. Events are both biological and physical in nature, and may include the appearance of birds, the opening of the Atlantic Ocean, the appearance of life and the formation of the planet Earth. In general, participants seem to be familiar with the general relative order of biologic events in Earth's history, although geologic events, such as the opening of the Atlantic, do not seem to have a significant place in student or teacher ideas of the Earth through time (Trend, 2001). Trend (2001) suggests that this may be the result of the attention paid to biologic events throughout the K-12 curriculum. In addition, several researchers have observed that participants of all ages, ranging from elementary-aged students to in-service teachers, believe that Earth and life originated at the same time (Marques and Thompson, 1997; Trend, 2001; Libarkin et al., 2005). In addition to fundamental research into student conceptions, significant energy has been put into considering the difficulties students have understanding geologic time, as well as crafting curricular materials that can facilitate instruction. For example, the January 2001 issue of the Journal of Geoscience Education contains a series of papers focusing on the teaching of geologic, or deep, time. Certainly, development and testing of curricula designed to impact student conceptions of geologic time has a long history in geoscience education. Curriculum developers often attempt to correlate geologic time with either human time scales, such as a person's life, or convert geologic time to spatial scales that can be observed in a typical classroom. For example, many teachers use a string to represent geologic time, and have students mark various significant geologic events, like the appearance of multi-celled organisms, along this spatial scale. Richardson (2000) suggests that this string could simultaneously be used to represent a student's lifetime, allowing for a relative comparison of events through time. The hope is that this lifetime vs. geologic time evaluation will help students understand the relative recentness of many geologic events, such as the extinction of the dinosaurs. In a more thorough discussion of a similar geologic time exercise, Hermann and Lewis (2004) discuss an extended three-week exercise designed to link the geologic time scale with a time scale of a relative's life. Hermann and Lewis (2004) advocate for formative evaluation of student work, and in particular argue that teachers should modify their own instruction based upon student input, and this is Libarkin et al. - College Student Conceptions of Geological Time 413 Figure 1. Questionnaire prompting students to place geologic events along a timeline. The interview protocol followed a similar line of questioning. Figure 1). These data were collected during the middle to end of the semester from students enrolled in entry-level biology and geology courses. Qualitative results from these and other data have been discussed previously in Libarkin et al. (2005). We report here on a specific component of the interviews related to creation of a geologic timeline. Students were prompted to complete a timeline by placing four events between Earth's formation and the present: The first appearance of life on Earth, the appearance of dinosaurs, the disappearance of dinosaurs, and the appearance of humans. These events are labeled FIRST LIFE, DINOS, NO DINOS, and MAN on timelines, respectively. Students were given the freedom to interpret the meaning of these events for themselves and were prompted to explain their responses. For example, while terrestrial, four-legged dinosaurs went extinct at the end of the Cretaceous, a lineage of dinosaurs gave rise to modern birds. We allowed students to explain their reasoning either verbally or in writing if they believed dinosaurs still METHODS existed. Students were also asked to provide absolute College student conceptions were probed in interviews numbers for the occurrence of these events, and to (n=15) and through open-ended questionnaires (n=49; provide written responses to several questions related to geologic time. Finally, timeline data were collected over particularly important for difficult subjects like geologic time. Hermann and Lewis' (2004) instructional approach is in agreement with the well-established conceptual change theory of educational practice (e.g., Posner et al., 1982). This study attempts to expand upon existing research about geologic time by documenting college student ideas about geologic time and how we can use these ideas to develop effective curricula. We build upon existing techniques, particularly interviews and a modified event-ordering task, to collect student ideas about time, the relative occurrence of geologic events, and absolute ages. This study presents a simplified approach to gathering student conceptions of geologic time (e.g., Trend, 2001), and applies a new analytical technique that has significant potential for evaluating complex models of, as well as the impact of belief systems on, student ideas. 414 Journal of Geoscience Education, v. 55, n. 5, November, 2007, p. 413-422 Figure 2. Representative student timelines (after Libarkin and Anderson, in press). A) Student indicates absolute dates and relative positioning of events that are close to the scientific model. The words “Permian extinction? 50 mya? Not sure” appear next to NO DINOS [Disappearance of dinosaurs] on the original drawing. This timeline plots very close to the accepted scientific model on both linear and ternary diagrams. B) A blend of scientific and non-scientific ideas. This timeline indicates that life existed when the Earth first formed. C) Creationist perspective of Earth’s history and occurrence of events. This student plots in the young Earth conceptual zone in Figure 5. several years (2002-2004) during data collection for a larger study, and interview and questionnaire protocols changed over time. Although interview prompts and questionnaires related to geologic time were identical across this study, some timelines contained gridlines while others did not (e.g., Figure 2). Comparison of timelines created on gridded and non-gridded paper showed no difference between either student ordering of events or the spacing of events. In effect, the range of timelines observed in this study was present on both types of paper. We concluded that no difference exists between student timelines completed on gridded versus non-gridded paper, and thus we combined the two datasets into the single one discussed here. Study Population - Students (n=63) from four U.S. institutions, a small elite private university in the northeast (A), two large state universities located in the southwest (B) and east (C) and one small public liberal arts college in the Midwest (D), participated in this study. Questionnaires were completed by students enrolled in a general education biology course at B (n=29) and an Honors College natural science field course at C (n=19). Timelines were collected during interviews with introductory geology students at A (n=5) and D (n=10). One student from A provided two alternative timelines, both of which are included here; hence, 64 timelines were analyzed. Demographic data were available for the 44 students from A, B, and D. Participants were overwhelmingly Caucasian (86%; one student abstaining), with four Hispanic, one Asian, and one student of mixed Asian-Caucasian ethnicity participating. In terms of gender and age, 61% of the participants were female, and most were between 18-25 years old, except for six students who were 26-55 years of age. Most students reported having exposure to biology or earth science in high school. Three students were majoring in science or engineering, specifically environmental science, meteorology, and electrical engineering. The remaining participants were all Libarkin et al. - College Student Conceptions of Geological Time 415 Type of Validity or Reliability Transferability Dependability Confirmability Explanation of Validity and Reliability* Approach in this Study Are the research findings significant in other contexts? All study variables need to be defined so that future researchers can make reasonable assumptions about applicability to other settings. Comparison of GCI results to timeline results from this study indicates that findings from the timeline portion of this study have application in other instructional settings. Is the study repeatable, are findings stable over time, and do different measures of the same trait yield similar results? "A careful review of the process of data collection and the research product, especially as time evolves, can help establish reliability. For instance, "comparison of observations carried out by two different researchers, as well as their analyses, can help determine the dependability of the qualitative process and study conclusions." (Libarkin and Kurdziel, 2002). Inter-rater reliability is one approach that can be used to gauge agreement between researchers. Inter-rater reliability: Two people measured student placement of events for a subset of timelines (n=10) and were in agreement within 5% for every measurement. Are study findings independent of the researcher's own personal biases, and therefore objective? Would another researcher reviewing the process of interpretation come to the same conclusion? This is difficult to evaluate, although all three authors were in agreement with the conclusions published here. The methodology is well-documented, allowing for confirmation of these findings by future researchers. Note: Not all aspects of qualitative validity and reliability were evaluated in this study. *Explanations are partially summarized from Lincoln and Guba (1985) and Trochim (2004). Table 1. Validity and Reliability (or Trustworthiness) for this Study non-natural science majors enrolled in diverse fields including creative arts, business, communication, psychology, elementary education, and management information systems. Interviews were transcribed verbatim from audiotapes. Analytical Methodology - Students provided absolute numbers for occurrence of events, relative placement of events on the timeline, and written or verbal explanations of their responses. Student ideas about the Earth's formation, the relationship between appearance of life and extinction events, and absolute ages of events were collected and grouped into categories via standard content analysis. The relative placement of events on timelines was investigated, as well as the spacing between events. The placement of three events, the appearance of life relative to Earth's formation and the disappearance of dinosaurs relative to the appearance of humans, was used as an example for evaluating student ideas about the scale of geologic time. Through analysis of the relative spacing of these events we were able to specifically consider those students who believed life existed when the Earth first formed as well as those students who indicated that humans and dinosaurs co-existed. Quantitative analysis of timeline data was conducted to ascertain the significance of the relative placement of all events along the timeline. Timelines were converted to ordinal scales, where "Earth Forms" = 0 and "Today" = 1.0. Relative positions of two of the four events were plotted on simple x-y graphs and simple regression analysis was performed. Comparison of the position of all events was conducted, although only the most significant results are discussed here. Events were also plotted on ternary diagrams to evaluate any zoning in event placement. All time-lines were first normalized with respect to total time (e.g. total time on each timeline was set to 1, or 100%, as describe in 416 the paragraph above). The percentage of total time is determined for three time steps of interest (for example, the time between 1) EARTH FORMS and FIRST LIFE, 2) FIRST LIFE and MAN, and 3) MAN and TODAY). Those three percentages for a single timeline are then plotted on a ternary diagram as a single point. Zoning of responses on these ternary diagrams was evaluated with a focus on specific subgroups, such as students with a "young Earth" perspective, and comparison with the student population overall. Validity and Reliability - This study utilizes qualitative data, so measures of qualitative validity and reliability (sometimes together called 'trustworthiness') are discussed here. In particular, we address measures to ensure transferability, dependability, and confirmability (Lincoln and Guba, 1985; Trochim, 2004; Table 1). Transferability deals with the question of whether or not research findings will be significant in other contexts, such as at other institutions or for a different population of students. It is particularly important that the demographics of the study population and other variables be well defined, to allow future researchers to ascertain the applicability of this study to future work. Dependability in the context of this study refers to whether or not the research is repeatable as well as the agreement between our findings and outcomes of similar research. Future researchers will be able to repeat this study only if the data collection and analytical methodologies are carefully defined, as has been provided above. In addition, agreement between researchers analyzing a single data set, called inter-rater reliability, provides some sense of the repeatability of a study. Finally, confirmability implies that findings are as objective as possible, and that any unbiased researcher would reach the same conclusions. Journal of Geoscience Education, v. 55, n. 5, November, 2007, p. 413-422 EARTH'S APPEARANCE AT FORMATION Molten or gaseous Desolate or featureless Pristine or undisturbed Different from today, without explicit description of differences PRESENCE OF LIFE AT EARTH'S FORMATION Life did not exist Simple organisms Plants Complex organisms All organisms (may exclude or include humans) REPRESENTATIVE STUDENT QUOTES "The earth was liquid lava when it was formed" "very different-very hot, lots of gases, reducing atmosphere" The Earth would have been "mountainless, oceanless [and with] no biomass" or "very desolate…pretty much a big rock" or "barren [with a] different atmosphere, [and] no life" "The earth would be more rugged. Shapper [sic] mountain peaks, rougher terrain" "I believe the earth would be very similar to now-with the exception of what it looked like b/f the flood…(I cannot put limits on God)." "God created this earth that we live on and gave it the ability to form into something different. I believe the world was different because of Plate tectonics." REPRESENTATIVE STUDENT QUOTES "the earth [at formation] is not fully developed enough to sustain life". Life would not have existed because "the atmosphere was not yet formed and there was no water yet" "bacteria" "There would be little life. Nothing would have been very diverse and complex. Animal evolution began after the cambrian [sic] explosion." "there would be bacteria, maybe some plants and one-celled organisms" "there was the vegetation. There was lots of vegetation- lush, green" "there would be living things, but probably less and not like the ones today. I think the organisms I'd encounter would be things like fish, cockroaches…" "…in Genesis it says that God created all living things-plants, animals & humans…Dinosaurs which became extinct". Table 2. Student ideas about life and the Earth at the time of the Earth’s formation Transferability - Data from this study have been compared to Geoscience Concept Inventory (GCI; Libarkin and Anderson, 2005; Libarkin and Anderson, 2006; Libarkin and Anderson, in press) results collected from 3845 students enrolled in 59 courses at 43 institutions nationwide. The GCI is a valid and reliable multiple choice concept test designed for use in geoscience classrooms. In particular, response frequencies for two questions related to the age of the Earth and geologic time are discussed here and compared to timeline results (Figure 1). This allows us to compare the small population included in our timeline study to the population of students taking entry-level courses nationwide. In general, timeline data and GCI data are in agreement although important differences that impact transferability of our findings do exist. Details of the relationship between this study and GCI data are discussed below. Dependability - A complete description of the data collection and analytical approach have been provided above, allowing for future researchers to evaluate the repeatability of this study for similar and other study populations. In addition, inter-rater reliability, the agreement of different researchers in their analysis of qualitative data sets, was evaluated with respect to the relative measurement of events placed on student timelines. Two people measured student events along a subset of timelines (n=10) and were in agreement within 5% for every measurement. Confirmability - Although difficult to evaluate, the level of agreement between all authors in development of study findings is an important gauge of the objectivity of a study. After discussion of the data and study conclusions, all three authors were in strong agreement with the findings presented here, as well as the implications. The analytical methodology is also well documented, allowing for confirmation of these findings by future researchers. RESULTS Student timelines represented a wide variety of ideas about Earth's formation and mechanisms for the origin and evolution of life on Earth (Figure 2). Student ideas ranged from strict creationist to the scientific, and incorporated a wide range of alternative conceptions about the Earth's formation and life on Earth that can be categorized into major groups (Table 2). Students had a range of ideas about the age of the Earth, although most believed the earth was quite old. 70% (n=44) of students self-reported that the Earth is billions of years old, with 57% (n=25) of these students assigning an age between 4 billion and 5 billion years. Seven students assigned ages between 4 million and 800, one student described the Earth as "trillions of years old", and six students reported the age of the Earth as 500,000 years old or less. The remaining three students did not assign an age to the Earth, although they used language in their timelines that suggested a young Earth perspective, such as "God lives" and "Creation of the Earth as described in the Bible". Six students indicated that dinosaurs and humans coexisted by placing the appearance of humans before the disappearance of dinosaurs. This idea was only observed among students enrolled at B and D, and existed along with a wide range of ideas about the age of the Earth. Students with conceptions of the Earth's age of 10,000 years, 4000-6000 years ("2000-4000 BC"), and 2100 years ("100 B.C.") held the co-existence perspective. The Libarkin et al. - College Student Conceptions of Geological Time 417 Figure 3. Graph of relative positioning of two events, appearance of first life and of dinosaurs, on student timelines. The black square corresponds to the scientific model. Other symbols are for each of four schools as shown in the legend. Students falling along the black diagonal line believe that life and dinosaurs appeared at the same time in Earth’s history. Open symbols are those students who indicated a belief that the earth is 500,000 years old or less (n=6) plus three students who wrote “God lives”, “When man first migrated to the American continents” and “As in Bible” on their timelines. first student's timeline depicts seven days and a flood, with "light" and the appearance of life on Day 1, "animals, reptiles (dino)" on Day 5, and humans on Day 6. The Flood "wiped out most life", although dinosaurs continued to exist until "2000 B.C.E.". The second student indicated that all life existed at "creation", and that the "flood" was related to the disappearance of the dinosaurs. The student who believed the Earth formed 2100 years ago held an interesting multifaceted view. For example, this student indicated that life existed when the Earth first formed and that evolution requires time. In particular, "there would be living organisms, such as plants and insects. There would not be many because the earth has just formed and nothing has had a chance to evolve." At the same time, this student explained that traveling to the time when dinosaurs were still alive would require "travel to the years befor [sic] Christ lived", implying a creationist perspective of Earth's formation. The remaining three students indicated that the Earth ranged in age from 200 million to five billion years old. One student (Earth age of 200 million years) held a complex idea where dinosaurs actually appeared before first life, and dinosaurs and humans co-existed, with dinosaurs disappearing at "1 million years past man". The remaining two students described the Earth as 4 and 5 billion years old. The first student indicated that humans appeared slightly before dinosaurs and this co-existence, according to the student's scale, lasted for almost a billion years. The second student held a number of scientific ideas in addition to the belief that dinosaurs and humans co-existed. For example, the student explained that when the Earth formed "There would be no living things because earth was too hot at this time". This student also indicated that life first appeared ~3.8 billion years ago, writing, "I remember that the oldest fossil dates back this far". This student also recognized 418 Figure 4. Graph of relative positioning of two events, disappearance of dinosaurs and appearance of man, on student timelines. Symbols as in Figure 3. Data below the solid line are those students who believe that dinosaurs and man co-existed. Dashed line is a polynomial linear regression through data from C students. that scientists age date features at the Earth's surface "by radioactive decay". Seven students in this study were unique in that they marked all events except for the disappearance of dinosaurs on their timelines. Two of these students indicated that dinosaurs do not exist today, but simply failed to mark NO DINOS on the timelines. One of these students marked "Big Bang Theory" which from the interview transcript appears to correspond to dinosaur extinction, "Supposedly there are these theories about the big bang theory, and that's when they ceased to exist. There's lots of theories. Nothing is like a for sure deal. So how long ago? I'll just write big bang theory". Remaining students suggested that dinosaurs do exist today, usually in the form of crocodiles or other reptiles. For example, one student, the same one who marked "God lives" and had no events marked on his timeline, expressed this conception by writing, "Crocodiles are dinosaurs still living today". Another student recognized that birds are dinosaurs while simultaneously holding an alternative conception that "there are dinosaurs living today-crocodiles, alligators and some think birds are descendants". Normalization of Student Timelines - In addition to qualitative evaluation of student ideas of geologic time, student timelines were evaluated through analysis of relative positioning of events on timelines. As described in the methods, timelines were normalized to identical scales, and the position of events was recorded relative to the Earth's formation and the present day. These student data were compared to scientifically- accepted values, and across institution and student demographic data. In general, a wide distribution of event positioning was observed, and this distribution, although independent of student demographics, was sometimes related to institution. The correlation between placements of two events on student timelines indicates both qualitative and quantitative relationships that are either not as obvious or not evident through simple content analysis of timelines. For example, comparison of the origin of life Journal of Geoscience Education, v. 55, n. 5, November, 2007, p. 413-422 Figure 5. Ternary diagram of spacing between four events on student timelines. Axes correspond to spacing between Earth’s formation and Appearance of first life (EARTH FORMS-FIRST LIFE), Appearance of first life and Appearance of humans (FIRT LIFE-MAN), and Appearance of humans and today (MAN-TODAY). Axes have been normalized such that spacing between Earth’s formation and today corresponds to 100%. Symbols as in Figure 3; recall that open symbols are those students who believe the Earth is 500,000 years old or less. In contrast with Figure 5, the thick, dashed black line demarcates a “conceptual zone” for those students with a young Earth perspective and the dashed gray line includes most of the students from C. Notice that all but one student with a young Earth view falls within the young Earth zone; see text for additional explanation. and the appearance of dinosaurs show quantitatively that very few students (n=4) believe dinosaurs originated when life did; these students fall on the black diagonal line in Figure 3. As would be expected, few students (n=2) place dinosaurs before the first appearance of life, indicating that students generally understand the timeline task and are not randomly placing events along the timeline. Those students who hold a young Earth view of Earth's age (open symbols) generally place both events in the first half of Earth's history. Almost all students, regardless of idea of geologic time held, plot far from the position of the accepted scientific model (black square in Figure 3). Most students place the appearance of first life early in Earth's history, in alignment with the scientific idea, but do not comprehend the significant amount of time that passed before dinosaurs appear in the fossil record. While the placement of the origin of life is not predictive of the placement of dinosaurs on the timeline task, other events do appear to be highly dependent upon one another, particularly for certain groups of students. The relationship between the disappearance of dinosaurs and the appearance of humans indicates that a predictive relationship (R2~0.6) exists between these two events (Figure 4). As indicated by standard content analysis of timelines and written or verbal responses discussed above, very few students (n=6) believe that dinosaurs and humans co-existed. It is possible that students who graphically represented humans and dinosaurs co-existing did not actually hold this belief, but were rather unable to depict events on the timeline correctly. As our research methodology does not allow us to distinguish between the two possibilities, we analyze the data assuming minimal graphing skills, although future analysis of student graphing ability might be warranted. Students with a young Earth model (open symbols) are not necessarily those students with a dinosaur-man coexistence model, as only two of the six students that fall below the black diagonal line in Figure 4 hold a young Earth model. In general, placement of the disappearance of dinosaurs early in geologic times suggests that students believe humans also appeared early in time, with a similar relationship for events placed later in time. Students from C, enrolled in an Honors College course, position these events in the latter half of geologic time (except for one student with a young Earth view) and in a way that suggests a highly predictive relationship. A polynomial regression (dashed line; y =-1.66x2+3.14x-0.50, R2=0.94) through the C data indicates that for this population of students the placement of a single event on the timescale can be used as a predictor of the placement of a second event. Finally, unlike Figure 3, fourteen students plot very close to the accepted scientific position that indicates that both events happened late in Earth's history (Figure 4). DISCUSSION The analysis of qualitative data through traditional content analysis coupled with investigation of conceptual zoning on x-y graphs provides valuable insight into student ideas about geologic time. The majority of students in this study recognize that the Earth is quite old, with a small subset of students holding a "young Earth" perspective. In addition, most students are able to place four events, the origin of life, the appearance of dinosaurs and humans, and the extinction of dinosaurs, in their correct order. On the other hand, very few students created timelines that were similar to the scientifically accepted one, indicating that the relative spacing of events by students is non-scientific. A few students believe dinosaurs and humans coexisted, although in our study this belief is not correlated to a young Earth perspective as reported by Schoon (1992). Some discrepancy between timeline representations of geologic time and written or verbal descriptions was observed. This was particularly evident for student conceptions of when life first appeared on Earth, and is most likely indicative of instability in student ideas. In essence, students may have been taught that life did not exist when the Earth first formed, but may want or need to believe that life did exist. As one student put it, "life must have existed, [otherwise] where did we come from?" Representation of timeline data on ternary diagrams through normalization of the spacing between four events provides significant insight into student ideas of geologic time (Figure 5). The timeline distances between three pairs of events, normalized to the sum of these distances, generates a ternary diagram from which conceptual zoning can be identified. For example, Figure 5 illustrates a ternary diagram generated by evaluating the distances between Earth's formation and the appearance of first life (EARTH FORMS-FIRST LIFE), the appearances of first life and humans (FIRST LIFE-MAN), and the appearance of humans and the present day (MAN-TODAY). Few students plot near the accepted scientific idea (black square), although some sub-groups Libarkin et al. - College Student Conceptions of Geological Time 419 GCI Question from Fall 2002 pre-instruction pilot If you could travel back to the time when the Earth first formed as a planet, what type(s) of life do you think you might encounter? (A) There would be no life on Earth (B) Simple, one-celled organisms only (C) Plants only (D) Animal and plant life in water, but none on land (E) All types of life in water and on land, except people (F) All types of life in water and on land, including people Which of the figures above do you think most closely represents changes in life on Earth over time? * Response A Response B Response C Response D Response E Number (n) of respondents and % students responding n = 2481 32% 47% 3% 3% 8% 6% n = 1089 10% 9% 37% 36% 8% *Refer to Libarkin and Anderson (in press) for more details. Table 3. Geoscience Concept Inventory (GCI) questions used for comparision with this study. Correct responses are in italics. of students do plot in distinct conceptual zones. In particular, the majority of students enrolled in the Honors College course at C plot close to the FIRST LIFE MAN to EARTH FORMS-FIRST LIFE axis. This indicates that these students recognize that humans appeared very late in Earth's history. Interestingly, C students are spread out along this axis, suggesting a range of ideas about when life appeared on Earth. Those C students who plot close to the EARTH FORMS-FIRST LIFE corner believe that life did not exist on Earth for most of Earth's history. A second conceptual zone, labeled "young Earth zone" on the diagram, encompasses eight of the nine students who have either indicated that the earth is <500,000 years, or provided information that suggests a young Earth view (Figure 5). The single young Earth student who plots outside of this zone described the formation of the Earth as occurring "when man first migrated to the American continents". It is unclear what the student meant by this, and this was clearly a different model than that held by other students with a young Earth perspective. Two students who believed the Earth was less than 500,000 years old explicitly demonstrated creationist perspectives coinciding with their young Earth view. These students wrote "Earth formed 6000 years ago. All life appeared shortly after during 420 'Creation'", and "7 days of creation" on their timelines. Other students, also classified in the young Earth view, indicated that the Earth formed during "creation", but were unsure of the exact age of creation. The presence of conceptual zones for both C students and students from all schools with a "young Earth" perspective raises an interesting question about where we would expect students with scientific models of geologic time to plot on ternary diagrams. Comparison with GCI Data - Libarkin and Anderson (2005) provide information about the entrenchment of some geologic ideas despite instruction. As a population, students nationwide seem to be familiar with the age of the Earth, but are unclear about techniques used to determine absolute age. In general, students tend to call upon relative dating techniques, rather than radioactivity, to explain how we know the Earth's age. In addition, those students who do call upon radioactive age-dating tend to focus on carbon-14 rather than isotopes with longer half-lives. GCI data related to the origin of life relative to Earth's formation show a link between our timeline data and the student population nationwide (Table 3). In an earlier study, Libarkin and Anderson (2005) report that 47% of pre-instruction students (n=2481) believe that Journal of Geoscience Education, v. 55, n. 5, November, 2007, p. 413-422 simple, one-celled organisms existed when the Earth first formed, and this idea did not change significantly after instruction. Only 32% of students completing the GCI (Table 3) recognize that life did not exist when the Earth first formed, in alignment with the 31% of students from B and D described in the present study. The GCI data are, however, in significant contrast to data from C and A students; 88% of these students recognized that life did not exist until at least 10% of geological time had passed. The C students are clearly a population that holds a clear, scientific conception of when life first originated in Earth's history, and may simply reflect the educational level and conceptual understanding required to place them in an honors program. In the nationwide GCI data, fourteen percent of students believe that life on the early Earth was very similar to today. This is in perfect agreement with the nine students (14%) in our study who appeared to have a young Earth perspective, some of whom described all life being "created" at the same time. Finally, 37% of students completing the GCI believe that dinosaurs came into existence about halfway through geologic time (Libarkin and Anderson, 2005; Table 3). This is similar to the data reported here; 30% of the 61 students who plotted the appearance of dinosaurs on their timeline placed this event between 0.4 and 0.7 on the normalized timeline. CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS FOR RESEARCH AND TEACHING The analysis of student conceptions of geologic time via timeline analysis provides insight into college student ideas of the Earth through time. Overall, the majority of college students understand the relative order of major biological events in Earth's history, while misunderstanding the time span between events. In particular, students generally posit too much time between the appearance and disappearance of terrestrial dinosaurs, and too little time between the appearance of the first life on Earth and the appearance of dinosaurs. This suggests that the timescale of evolution is difficult for students to comprehend, and needs to be addressed explicitly in instruction. This includes discussion of both the time required for the origin and evolution of simple and complex organisms, as well as the scale of organismal changes throughout Earth's history. The importance of deep time in the development of geological phenomena suggests that conceptions about geologic time may directly impact ideas in other areas of geology. The difficulties in understanding deep time evident through analysis of interviews, time lines, and ternary diagrams suggest that geoscience educators face a huge challenge in improving conceptual understanding of the many concepts that use deep time as a foundation, such as plate tectonics, erosion, metamorphism, stratigraphy and sedimentation, geologic structures, geologic resources, and global change. For example, a sampling of ten introductory geology textbooks found on one of the author's bookshelves showed that all contained entire chapters devoted to deep time, but only six of the ten covered time in the introductory chapter. Of the six that did devote part of the introductory chapter to deep time, detailed chapters on deep time were placed, in general, mid-way through the text. In fact, four of these six had Geologic Time as Chapter 8, with total chapters ranging from 20-22 chapters. Another placed it as chapter 9 of a 20 chapter book, and the final one placed it dead last in a 20 chapter book. We suggest that a thorough discussion of time as a basic concept in geology must occur in the introductory stages of introductory courses. More importantly, the effects of deep time on specific concepts within geology must be reiterated throughout the course to help students discover the influence of time on many aspects of the geological sciences. Placement of topics in textbooks clearly influences the timing of the discussion of these topics in college courses, and we also suggest that textbook authors reevaluate the positioning of chapters on geologic time within textbooks. Further investigation of the interaction between conceptions of geologic time and other geologic phenomena can provide additional insight into how students develop ideas about the Earth. In addition, the growing body of conceptions research in the Earth Sciences points towards a need for change in how we teach geology in K-12 classrooms. Consideration of the role that deep time has to play in shaping our geologic conceptions should be an important point to consider when developing new curricula or pedagogies. Future studies could investigate the relationship between ideas about geologic time and how students build models of Earth phenomena, compare the timescale of events in the Earth's past, today, and in the future, and evaluate the impact of time-related curricula on students' Earth-related conceptual models. The comparison between GCI data collected nationwide and timeline data discussed in this study indicates that some of the study findings are transferable to the larger population of students while others are not. Careful evaluation of data from future studies, including comparison with results from existing studies, will be required before conclusions about the larger population can be made. Fortunately, the analytical approach, both x-y and ternary plotting, used in comparing relative positioning of events is applicable to any population as this approach is independent of the specific ideas held by students. The use of ternary diagrams in investigating student conceptual understanding of geologic time has potential as a predictive agent and method for classifying students. This approach can be used for both research and pre-instruction testing to aid faculty in designing curricula that best fit students' pre-existing conceptions of geologic time. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank all students who graciously participated in this research as well as undergraduate students at Ohio University who assisted in normalization of timelines. This study was partially funded by the National Science Foundation through an Assessment of Student Achievement in Undergraduate Education grant to Libarkin and Anderson (DUE-0127765). Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. REFERENCES Ault, C.R., 1982, Time in geological explanations as perceived by elementary school students, Journal of Geological Education, v. 30, p. 304-309. Libarkin et al. - College Student Conceptions of Geological Time 421 Dahl, J., Anderson, S.W., and Libarkin, J.C., 2005, Digging into Earth Science: Alternative conceptions held by K-12 teachers: Journal of Science Education, v. 12, p. 65-68. DeLaughter, J.E., Stein, S., Stein, C.A., and Bain, K.R., 1998, Preconceptions about earth science among students in an introductory course, EOS, v. 79, p. 429-432. Dodick, J., and Orion, N., 2003, Cognitive factors affecting students understanding of geological time, Journal of Research in Science Teaching, v. 40, p. 415-442. Gosselin, D.C., and Macklem-Hurst, J.L., 2002, Pre-/Post-Knowledge assessment of an earth science course for elementary/middle school education majors, Journal of Geoscience Education, v. 50, p. 169-175. Hermann, R., and Lewis, B., 2004, A formative assessment of geologic time for high school Earth Science students, Journal of Geoscience Education, v. 52, p. 231-235. Libarkin, J.C., Anderson, S., Dahl, J., Beilfuss, M., Boone, W., and Kurdziel, J., 2005, College students' ideas about geologic time, Earth's interior, and Earth's crust, Journal of Geoscience Education, v. 53, p. 17-26. Libarkin, J.C., and Anderson, S.W., 2005, Assessment of learning in entry-level geoscience courses: Results from the Geoscience Concept Inventory, Journal of Geoscience Education, v. 53, p. 394-401. Libarkin, JC., and Anderson, S.W., 2006, The Geoscience Concept Inventory: Application of Rasch Analysis to concept inventory development in higher education, In Liu, X., and Boone, W., eds., Applications of Rasch Measurement in Science Education, JAM Publishers, p. 45-73. Libarkin, J.C., and Anderson, S.W., in press, Development of the Geoscience Concept Inventory, Proceedings of the National STEM Assessment Conference (Assessment of Student Achievement)", Washington DC, October 19-21, 2006, sponsored by 422 the National Science Foundation and Drury University. Lincoln, Y.S., and Guba, E.G., 1985, Naturalistic Inquiry, Beverly Hills, Sage Publishing, 416 p. Marques, L., and Thompson, D., 1997, Portuguese students' understanding at ages 10-11 and 14-15 of the origin and nature of the earth and the development of life, Research in Science and Technological Education, v. 15, p. 29-51. Oversby, J., 1996, Knowledge of the earth science and the potential for its development, School Science Review, v. 78, p. 91-97. Posner, G.J., Strike, K.A., Hewson, W.P., and Gertzog, W.A., 1982, Accommodation of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change, Science Education, v. 66, p. 211-227. Richardson, R.M., 2000, Geologic time (clothes) line, Journal of Geoscience Education, v. 48, p. 584. Schoon, K.J., 1992, Students' alternative conceptions of Earth and space, Journal of Geological Education, v. 40, p. 209-214. Trend, R.D., 1998, An investigation into understanding of geological time among 10- and 11-year-old children, International Journal of Science Education, v. 20, p. 973-988. Trend, 2000, Conceptions of geological time among primary teacher trainees, with reference to their engagement with geoscience, history, and science, International Journal of Science Education, v. 22, p. 539-555. Trend, 2001, Deep time frameworks: A preliminary study of U.K. primary teachers' conceptions of geological time and perceptions of geoscience, Journal of Research in Science Teaching, v. 38, p. 191-221. Trochim, W.M., 2004, The Research Methods Knowledge Base, 2nd Edition. Internet WWW page, at URL: <http://trochim.human.cornell.edu/kb/index.htm > (version current as of August 16, 2004 ). Journal of Geoscience Education, v. 55, n. 5, November, 2007, p. 413-422