Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2006, Vol. 115, No. 3, 567–579 Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 0021-843X/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.3.567 Affect Regulation and Food Intake in Bulimia Nervosa: Emotional Responding to Food Cues After Deprivation and Subsequent Eating Birgit I. Mauler Alfons O. Hamm and Almut I. Weike Christoph-Dornier-Foundation for Clinical Psychology Muenster, Germany University of Greifswald, Germany Brunna Tuschen-Caffier University of Bielefeld, Germany Emotional responding to salient food cues and effects of food deprivation and consumption were investigated in 32 women with bulimia and 32 control women. One half of each group was food deprived before viewing unpleasant, neutral, pleasant, and food-related pictures. Then participants could eat from a buffet before viewing a parallel picture set. Women with bulimia showed a substantial potentiation of startle responses during viewing of food cues relative to control women. This startle potentiation was attenuated by food deprivation and augmented by increased food consumption. These data support the affective regulation model suggesting that food cues prompt negative affective states in women with bulimia, who are overwhelmed by fasting. The resulting deprivation increases the incentive value of food cues and may thus trigger binge eating. Keywords: bulimia nervosa, startle response, deprivation, affect regulation, binge eating From the perspective of self-regulation (see Mann & Ward, 2004, for a recent discussion), dieting is the attempt of persons with bulimia to reduce the negative affect elicited by cues that are associated with food intake and the fear of gaining weight. Such an idea seems to be nonintuitive, because food cues normally have reinforcing, appetitive qualities in healthy volunteers. Indeed, external food cues potentiate appetitive reflexes (e.g., salivation; Konorski, 1967; Wooley & Wooley, 1981) and slightly inhibit protective reflexes such as the startle response, a cranial to caudal spreading wave of flexor movements elicited by abruptly occurring sensory events (Bradley, Codispoti, Cuthbert, & Lang, 2001). Deprivation even seems to increase the appetitive motivational state that can be triggered by food cues. Food cues are rated as more pleasant by deprived relative to nondeprived persons (e.g., Lavy & van den Hout, 1993) and are rated as being more attractive than other reinforcers (Bulik & Brinded, 1994; Raynor & Epstein, 2003). In addition, deprivation leads to increased salivation during confrontation with food cues (Staats & Hammond, 1972) as well as to increased blood flow in the amygdala and the associated inferotemporal cortical regions (LaBar et al., 2001). On the other hand, emotional responses of women with bulimia nervosa to food cues seem to differ from those of control subjects without bulimia, although data in this area of research are scarce. In controlled studies, when purging was rendered impossible, women with bulimia reported negative affect and anxiety after eating (Buree, Papageorgis, & Hare, 1990; Leitenberg, Gross, Peterson, & Rosen, 1984). Additionally, they reported fear of gaining weight, tenseness, or being irritated and depressed (e.g., Staiger, Dawe, & McCarthy, 2000), supporting the idea that dieting might be used by the women with bulimia to avoid the negative affect associated with food exposure and eating. Studies, in which attempts were made to relate these verbal report data to physiological responses elicited by food cues (e.g., salivation, heart rate, The most salient aspect of bulimia nervosa is an abnormal eating pattern. Arguably, the cardinal feature of this disordered eating is bouts of uncontrolled food intake (binges). During bingeing large amounts of food are usually ingested while the person experiences an irresistible food craving and lack of control over his or her eating behavior. Additionally, there is recurrent inappropriate behavior to prevent weight gain including purging behavior directly after bingeing (e.g., vomiting or use of laxatives), excessive exercise, and restrained eating or fasting between periods of binge eating. There seems to be general consensus in the literature that such dieting is a key factor in the development and maintenance of bulimic psychopathology for the majority of the patients (Brewerton, Dansky, Kilpatrick, & O’Neal, 2000; Bulik, Sullivan, Carter, & Joyce, 1997; Polivy & Herman, 2002; Stice, 2002). Food restriction can trigger binge eating (Davis, Freeman, & Garner, 1988; Rosen, Tacy, & Howell, 1990) and maintains the bulimic eating pattern (Fairburn, Stice, et al., 2003; Leon, Fulkerson, Perry, & Early-Zald, 1995; Patton, Johnson-Sabine, Wood, Mann, & Wakeling, 1990). Birgit I. Mauler, Christoph-Dornier-Foundation for Clinical Psychology, Muenster, Germany; Alfons O. Hamm and Almut I. Weike, Department of Psychology, University of Greifswald, Germany; Brunna TuschenCaffier, Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Bielefeld, Germany. This research was supported by the Christoph-Dornier-Foundation for Clinical Psychology and by grants from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (German Research Foundation) to Alfons O. Hamm (Ha 1593/6-2 and Ha 1593/10-2). We thank Dr. Carmen Hamm for taking the blood samples of all participants. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Alfons O. Hamm, Department of Psychology, University of Greifswald, FranzMehring-Strasse 47, 17487, Greifswald, Germany. E-mail: hamm@unigreifswald.de 567 568 MAULER, HAMM, WEIKE, AND TUSCHEN-CAFFIER and electrodermal responses), though, showed discouraging results. In most studies no reliable relationships between measures of autonomic arousal and verbal report were obtained (Buree et al., 1990; Carter & Bulik, 1996; Leitenberg et al., 1984; Williamson, Kelley, Davis, Ruggiero, & Veitia, 1985). This does not come as a surprise, because discordance and desynchrony between different response systems are often observed during emotional responding (Cacioppo, Klein, Berntson, & Hartfield, 1993; Lang, 1985). One reason for this modest correlation is that different indices of emotions, of course, serve different functions. Verbal report is used to communicate affective experiences to others and is therefore biased by intentions of self-presentation or by specific situational demands. Autonomic measures (such as heart rate and skin conductance) are primarily linked to the intensity of affective stimulation (see Lang, Greenwald, Bradley, & Hamm, 1993), and these measures of autonomic arousal alone might not be the best choice to investigate the emotional impact of food cues for individuals with bulimia. In contrast, the modulation of the acoustic startle reflex seems to specifically index the motivational potency of a cue. The magnitude of the startle eyeblink response elicited by a brief acoustic probe stimulus is augmented during viewing of unpleasant and specifically threatening pictures and is reduced while viewing pleasant images (Vrana, Spence, & Lang, 1988; see Bradley, 2000, for review). This effect has been replicated several times (e.g., Cook, Davis, Hawk, Spence, & Gautier, 1992; Hamm, Cuthbert, Globisch, & Vaitl, 1997; Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, 1993). The phenomenon has been explained by the emotional priming model (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1998), stating that independently evoked defensive and protective reflexes are augmented if the organism is in a defensive motivational state and reduced if the organism is in an appetitive state. Emotional priming of the startle reflex operates on a very fundamental level outside of subjects’ awareness and is mediated by the amygdala, a subcortical limbic structure located in the anterior medial temporal lobe (see Davis, 2000). Drobes et al. (2001) used the startle probe methodology for the first time to investigate the motivational impact of salient food cues in participants who report habitually deviant eating patterns. As expected, food cues prompted comparable startle response magnitudes relative to other pleasant, food unrelated visual cues in control participants. By contrast, those participants who have experienced eating binges in the past showed a relative potentiation of their startle response magnitudes during viewing of food cues (compared with other pleasant nonfood cues), suggesting that these usually appetitive cues did not activate positive affect in this group of subjects. However, the same effect was found in the deprived control group. This finding is surprising, because food cues normally should have a positive incentive value in deprived participants. Accordingly, Hawk, Baschnagl, Ashare, and Epstein (2004) found a stronger reduction of the startle reflex magnitude in deprived participants during viewing of their favorite food cues. In contrast to the study of Drobes et al. (2001), participants in the study of Hawk et al. (2004) were given access to their favorite food after the experiment. The experimental context of the study of Drobes et al. (2001) did not allow for immediate food consumption. Therefore, these authors interpreted the relative startle potentiation as indexing a negative affective state of frustrative nonreward; that is, hungry participants viewed food pictures without getting the opportunity to eat. Deviant eating patterns coupled with deprivation status, however, were not a focus of that study. In the current study, therefore, we focused on affective responses to food cues in women with bulimia nervosa, comparing their physiological, behavioral, and verbal response patterns with those of control women without bulimia. Additionally, standard affective materials (unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant pictures) were presented to investigate the specificity of the emotional responding to food cues in these women with an eating disorder. Moreover, we investigated whether prior food deprivation would influence the motivational relevance of food cues and whether this effect would be modulated by psychopathology. Thus, half of the women with bulimia and half of the control women were food deprived for 24 hours before the experiment. Compliance with the deprivation condition was controlled by the assessment of -hydroxybutyric acid taken from the serum. A second aim of the study was to investigate the modulation of affective responding to food cues by prior food consumption. To this end, all participants were allowed to eat as much as they wanted from a breakfast buffet during a recreational break between the first and second psychophysiological assessment. It was expected that food cues would serve as appetitive stimuli in the control subjects and would consequently elicit an appetitive motivational response pattern, including startle response inhibition. Moreover, if the hypothesis of frustrative nonreward is correct, participants should exhibit increased startle magnitudes during viewing of food cues after 24 hours of food deprivation. If food cues elicit a negative affect in those with bulimia, as predicted by the affect regulation hypotheses, these stimuli should elicit a relative potentiation of the startle reflex in these participants. On the other hand, food cues might also elicit a stronger appetitive response disposition in these participants and might therefore be more potent in eliciting binge eating. If this were the case, those with bulimia should show a stronger reduction in startle magnitudes evoked during viewing of food pictures relative to the control women. Deprivation might increase the incentive value of the food cues and should then increase appetitive motivation and further reduce startle magnitudes during viewing of food cues in the women with bulimia. Method Participants One hundred fifty-six individuals responded to two different advertisements on the university campus and in local newspapers asking for subjects with and without disturbed eating habits. After a short telephone screening of these individuals, asking for symptoms of bulimia, including restrained eating, body weight, height, pregnancy, thyroid malfunctions, and age, 87 of them were invited for a further diagnostic session. After this clinical interview, 66 women agreed to participate in the experiment. Control participants had no history of eating disorders and were not on a weight loss diet at the moment. Bulimia nervosa was diagnosed in 34 participants on the basis of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.) criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) using a standardized clinical interview (Fairburn & Cooper, 1993). The binge frequency of the women with bulimia was 8.06 (⫾ 4.2) per week. Thirty-three of the 34 women with bulimia purged by vomiting. Their mean frequency of self-induced vomiting was 7.34 (⫾ 4.1) times per week. On average, the participants with bulimia reported durations of bulimia nervosa of 6.4 (⫾ AFFECT REGULATION AND BULIMIA NERVOSA 3.7) years. Ten of them showed comorbidity with anxiety or affective disorder.1 All participants completed the German versions of the Restraint Scale (Polivy, Herman, & Howard, 1988) and the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (Stunkard & Messick, 1985). Because the general fear level affects the overall startle response amplitudes (Cook et al., 1992), all participants completed the German version of the State Anxiety Questionnaire (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970). There were no significant differences in the overall state anxiety level between women with bulimia and control women. Group means of all questionnaire scores are presented in Table 1. Furthermore, body mass index and the age range of each group are also shown in Table 1. Because individuals in a normal weight range were sought (most individuals with bulimia are of normal weight), all subjects were selected on the basis of an average body mass index (ranging from 19 to 24 kg/m2). Additionally, they were not taking any medication and did not report pregnancy or thyroid malfunctions. Participants included in the control group (N ⫽ 32) did not report any history of eating disorders and were not currently dieting or identified as restraint eaters (ⱕ9 in the Restraint Scale and ⱕ6 in Scale 1 of the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire). Participants in each diagnostic group were randomly assigned to the deprivation condition, whereas the remaining participants were instructed to proceed with their normal diet. Participants obtained a small monetary reward for their participation. This investigation was approved by the ethics committee of the Christoph-Dornier-Foundation for Clinical Psychology. Procedure and Stimulus Materials Food deprivation. In the deprivation condition subjects were asked to refrain from eating for 24 hours beginning at 10:00 a.m. on the day before the study. On the following day, blood samples were taken at 9:00 a.m. from all participants. -hydroxybutyric acid was analyzed from the serum to assess whether participants followed the instruction.2 As expected, concentrations of this metabolite increased from 84.4 and 86.6 mol/L to 605.8 and 522.7 mol/L after fasting for deprived women with bulimia and control women, respectively. Although participants were informed that the blood test would show whether or not they had eaten during the deprivation interval, two participants with bulimia had to be excluded after the blood test, because the concentration of the metabolite clearly revealed that they had not followed the food deprivation instruction (values were 82.5 and 90.3 mol/L). All control subjects followed the food deprivation instruction. Thus, 16 subjects in each of the 4 groups participated in the experimental sessions. Table 1 Scores in the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TEFQ), the Restraint Scale (RS), the State Anxiety Questionnaire (STAI), and the Mean Body Mass Index (BMI) of the Deprived and Nondeprived Participants With Bulimia and Control Participants Bulimia Control Deprived Nondeprived Deprived Nondeprived (n ⫽ 16) (n ⫽ 16) (n ⫽ 16) (n ⫽ 16) TFEQ (Scale I) RS STAI BMI (kg/m2) Age (range) (years) Note. 12.8 (1.2) 22.8 (1.2) 47.0 (0.6) 21.9 (0.4) 25–35 13.5 (1.2) 21.6 (1.0) 47.5 (0.6) 20.6 (0.4) 21–33 2.4 (0.4) 6.3 (0.6) 47.6 (0.4) 20.9 (0.4) 20–28 Scores are presented as means and (standard errors). 2.3 (0.4) 6.4 (0.5) 45.9 (0.6) 21.3 (0.3) 20–30 569 First experimental session. The first experimental session in the laboratory began at 10:00 a.m., that is, 1 hour after the blood sample was taken. Participants in the nondeprived groups were instructed to eat breakfast (about 450 kcal) before coming to the laboratory. Those who had not complied with this instruction (n ⫽ 2) were asked to consume a standard breakfast (450 kcal) before starting the experiment. Physiological sensors were attached while the participant reclined in a comfortable chair in a sound-attenuated and dimly lit room. At the beginning of the session, four acoustic startle eliciting stimuli [a 50-ms burst of broadband 105 dB(A) white noise with instantaneous rise/fall time; S81-02, Coulbourn] were presented binaurally through headphones (MDR-CD 170; Sony). Two probes were administered while participants viewed pictures of landscapes, and two probes were presented without visual foreground. Afterward the signals were checked, and all participants were instructed that a series of pictures would be presented and that each picture should be viewed for the entire time it appeared on the screen. In addition, they were told that occasional noises heard over headphones could be ignored. Then each participant viewed one of two sets of 32 color pictures,3 8 pictures containing explicit food cues and 8 pictures each selected from the three standard valence categories (i.e., pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant) based on normative ratings (Hamm & Vaitl, 1993). Forty-nine of the 64 pictures were selected from the International Affective Picture System4 (Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, 1995). Fifteen of the 16 food pictures were created for that study and were rated for valence, arousal, and dominance in a pilot study. Picture contents were presented in perceptually random order, counterbalanced across participants. Each picture was presented for 6 s. On 6 of the 8 picture presentations per picture category, the acoustic startle probe was administered either 4.0, 4.5, or 5.0 s after picture 1 Because affective startle reflex modulation is impaired in patients with affective disorders (Allen, Trinder, & Brennan, 1999), Mann–Whitney U tests were performed to test whether emotional responses of the 6 women with bulimia with comorbid affective disorder differed from those of the other women with bulimia. Emotional responses to the affective stimuli did not differ in this comparison; Mann–Whitney U (N ⫽ 32) ⫽ 92.00, ns, 82.00, ns, for Sessions 1 and 2, respectively. One has to keep in mind that Allen et al. (1999) investigated medicated inpatients with severe major depression, and deviant affective startle modulation was only obtained for patients with a Beck Depression Inventory score ⬎30. 2 The normal range of -hydroxybutyric acid, a metabolite of the fatty acids that can pass the blood– brain barrier, varies between 30 and 120 mol/L. During fasting the level of this metabolite increases substantially up to 500 –700 mol/L but rapidly returns to the baseline level after food intake. Thus, -hydroxybutyric acid is a good biological index for assessing compliance with a deprivation instruction (Webber & Macdonald, 1994). The secretion of -hydroxybutyric acid also follows a definite circadian rhythm. Therefore, the blood samples were always taken at the same time in the morning. Moreover, to control for interindividual differences a second baseline measurement of -hydroxybutyric acid level was taken either 1 week before or after the experiment. No deprivation instructions were given this time, and blood samples were again taken at 9:00 a.m. 3 Stimulus materials were arranged in two sets (A and B) of 32 slides each, which were similar for picture contents and normative valence and arousal ratings. Half of the subjects viewed Set A in the first and Set B in the second experimental session and vice versa. 4 Numbers of the International Affective Picture System pictures are as follows: Set A neutral, 2200, 5500, 7050, 7010, 7100, 7550, 7820, 7130, pleasant, 1710, 2040, 2080, 4430, 4608, 4680, 8030, 8200, unpleasant, 1120, 3000, 3100, 3120, 6210, 6230, 9250, 6312, food, 7330; Set B neutral, 2210, 5510, 7020, 7090, 7170, 9070, 7830, 7500, pleasant, 1750, 2050, 2250, 4490, 4660, 4690, 8490, 8080, unpleasant, 1300, 3010, 3130, 3150, 6190, 6200, 9050, 6313. 570 MAULER, HAMM, WEIKE, AND TUSCHEN-CAFFIER onset. In addition, 8 startle probes were presented during the intertrial intervals, which varied between 8 and 21 s. After each picture was presented once, sensors and headphones were removed. The participant was instructed that she could now view the same pictures as long as desired using a button press to terminate picture presentation. Viewing times were recorded in milliseconds as a behavioral measure of interest. After picture offset, each participant rated her subjective experience of valence and arousal, using the computerized version of the Self-Assessment–Manikin (Hodes, Cook, & Lang, 1985). Participants then rated their interest in each picture and, only after watching food pictures, their general desire to eat using a 20-point computerized line rating. Food consumption. After the picture rating task, participants were taken to another room, where they were allowed to eat as much as desired from a breakfast buffet consisting of various rolls, cereals, and so forth. Participants were alone in the room but could be observed through a one-way mirror. A radio and newspapers were available to create a pleasant atmosphere. A total amount of 20 min of food access was given. Any food remaining after this period of time was removed, weighted, and counted. The amount of calories consumed was calculated using a computer program (PRODI, Version 4). Second experimental session. After food consumption, participants were led back to the laboratory, physiological sensors were attached again, and the same procedure was applied as in the first experimental session. Now each participant viewed the other of the two sets of 32 color pictures. Acoustic startle probes were administered in the same order during and between picture viewing as in the first session. After the pictures were viewed once and physiological responses were recorded, the same pictures were presented again and free viewing times and subjective ratings were obtained. Apparatus The eyeblink component of the startle response was measured by recording the electromyographic (EMG) activity over the left orbicularis oculi muscle beneath the eye using Ag/AgCl miniature surface electrodes (Sensormedics, Yorbahinda, CA) filled with electrolyte (Marquette, Hellige, Freiburg, Germany). The raw EMG signal was amplified and filtered through a 30 –1,000 Hz bandpass using a Coulbourn S75-01 bioamplifier. The signal was then rectified and integrated on line by a Coulbourn S76-01 contour-following integrator with a time constant of 10 ms. Digital sampling with a rate of 1000 Hz started 100 ms before and lasted until 400 ms after the onset of the acoustic startle stimulus. Corrugator EMG activity was recorded above the left eye with Ag/AgCl miniature surface electrodes (Sensormedics) filled with electrolyte, using the placement recommended by Fridlund and Cacioppo (1986). Amplification and filtering of the raw signal were identical to those described for the orbicularis oculi. The signal was then rectified and integrated on line using a Coulbourn S76-01 contour-following integrator with a time constant set at 500 ms. Digital sampling at 10 Hz began 3 s before picture onset and ended 3 s after picture offset. Skin conductance was recorded with Ag/AgCl standard electrodes (8 mm diameter; Marquette Hellige) filled with a 0.05 M sodium chloride electrolyte medium. Electrodes were placed adjacently on the hypothenar eminence of the palmar surface of the participant’s nondominant hand. A Coulbourn S71-22 skin conductance coupler provided a constant voltage of 0.5 V across electrodes and processed the signal with a resolution of 0.01 S. Digital sampling at 10 Hz started 3 s before picture onset and continued until 3 s after picture offset. A lead II electrocardiogram was obtained using Ag/AgCl standard electrodes (Marquette Hellige) filled with Hellige electrolyte. The signal was amplified and filtered with a Coulbourn S75-01 bioamplifier. The analogue signal was digitized with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz starting 3 s before picture onset and continuing until 3 s after picture offset. A peak trigger served for online registration of the R-wave within the analogue electrocardiogram signal. Interbeat intervals were converted to heart rate in beats per minute in half-second bins, following the recommendations of Graham (1978). Visual stimuli were presented using a Kodak Ektagraph slide projector situated in the room adjacent to the experimental chamber. The pictures were presented on a white wall 1.5 m in front of the participant. The size of the visible picture was 120 ⫻ 80 cm. After completion of the study, all participants were debriefed, and those with bulimia were offered treatment in the outpatient facility of the Christoph-Dornier-Foundation. Data Reduction and Analysis The reflex eyeblink data were reduced and scored off line using a computer program (Globisch, Hamm, Schneider, & Vaitl, 1993) that identified latency of blink onset (in milliseconds) and peak amplitude (in microvolts). Responses starting 20 –100 ms after startle probe onset and reaching peak amplitude within 150 ms after probe onset were identified as startle eyeblinks. Trials with clear movement artifacts or excessive baseline activity were rejected (1.81%), and trials in which no response could be detected were scored as zero magnitude. The results of various distribution analyses (assessment of skewness and kurtosis and the Kolmogorov– Smirnoff test of normal distribution) suggested that startle response magnitudes should be standardized to correct for individual differences. There were no significant differences between the four groups in the overall raw startle magnitude, F(1, 60) ⬍ 1. Therefore, responses from each participant were transformed to z scores and converted to T scores (i.e., 50 ⫹ [z ⫻ 10]). All picture and intertrial interval startle responses were used as the reference distribution for these computations. Skin conductance responses to the pictures were scored according to the recommendations of Prokasy and Kumpfer (1973). The first interval response was defined as the largest increase in conductance occurring between 0.9 and 4 s after stimulus onset. Because the earliest probe was administered 4 s after picture onset, the first interval response was not confounded by the acoustic probe stimulus. Logarithms of these values were computed before statistical analyses to normalize the distribution (Venables & Christie, 1980). Again, to reduce interindividual variability and to facilitate comparisons of different picture contents, the log values were range corrected by dividing each individual score by the participant’s maximum response (Lykken & Venables, 1971). One nondeprived control participant was excluded from further analyses because of electrodermal nonresponding (i.e., maximum response ⫽ 0). Digital values of corrugator EMG activity were converted to microvolts, and corrugator responses were scored by subtracting the 1-s prestimulus level from the average change in the 6-s picture viewing interval. Corrugator responding of one nondeprived control participant was lost because of equipment malfunction. Baseline heart rate (1 s before picture onset) was subtracted from average heart rate for every half-second during the 6-s picture viewing period, and these phasic heart rate changes were averaged for statistical analyses. Cardiac responses to food cues showed an overall deceleration. Accordingly, component scores did not reveal any additional information and therefore are not reported. Five control participants (three deprived and two nondeprived) had to be excluded from heart rate analyses because of excessive artifact recordings during one of the experimental sessions. Following prior research with this paradigm, a first step in the analysis was to replicate responses to the three standard affective categories and verify that the overall response pattern did not vary across the different groups. A mixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA) for each measure involved Pathology (participants with bulimia vs. control participants) and Deprivation (deprived vs. nondeprived participants) as between-subjects variables and Category (unpleasant vs. neutral vs. pleasant) as a withinsubjects variable. After these preliminary tests, further data analyses fo- AFFECT REGULATION AND BULIMIA NERVOSA cused on the food pictures. These analyses used a mixed design (ANOVA) again involving Pathology and Deprivation as between-subjects variables and Category as a within-subjects variable. Because food cues are normally rated as emotionally pleasant stimuli (Bradley et al., 2001), the main contrast food versus (standard) pleasant pictures was tested in the analyses. Unless otherwise noted, all statistical tests used the .05 level of statistical significance. Greenhouse–Geisser adjustments of degrees of freedom were used where appropriate. Partial eta squared values are reported as a measure of effect size. Results First Experimental Session Standard Affective Categories As expected, differences among the standard pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant pictures were observed for physiological, behavioral, and self-report measures (see Table 2).5 Most important, the modulation of the physiological, behavioral, and self-report measures as a function of affective valence of these nonfood cues did not vary for the four groups. Neither the overall Category ⫻ Pathology (participants with bulimia vs. control participants) nor the Category ⫻ Deprivation (deprived vs. nondeprived participants) interaction was significant for any of these different measures. Thus, pictures that were unrelated to food prompted the same affective responses in all four groups. Food Cues Startle response magnitudes. As predicted, the overall analysis of food and pleasant pictures resulted in a significant Category ⫻ Pathology interaction, F(1, 60) ⫽ 102.4, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .63. The effect was also found for food cues compared with neutral pictures, Category ⫻ Pathology, F(1, 60) ⫽ 70.1, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .54. Participants with bulimia showed a significant potentiation of their startle responses to food cues relative to other pleasant pictures, F(1, 30) ⫽ 241.4, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .89, and neutral pictures, F(1, 30) ⫽ 83.4, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .74. By contrast, blink magnitudes of the control participants were significantly reduced when elicited during viewing of food cues relative to neutral pictures, F(1, 30) ⫽ 11.4, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .28, whereas no differences were observed for food cues compared with the standard pleasant pictures, F(1, 30) ⬍ 1. Moreover, startle responses elicited during viewing of food cues were significantly larger for participants with bulimia than for control participants, F(1, 60) ⫽ 65.9, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .52, but blink magnitudes did not differ between groups for pleasant or neutral pictures, F(1, 60) ⬍ 1. Mean blink response magnitudes elicited during viewing of food cues and unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant pictures for deprived and nondeprived participants with bulimia and control participants are depicted in Figure 1. The effect of food deprivation on the startle response magnitudes during viewing of food cues varied significantly between participants with bulimia and control participants, Deprivation ⫻ Pathology, F(1, 60) ⫽ 32.3, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .35. Although fooddeprived control participants exhibited elevated startle response magnitudes during viewing of food cues relative to nondeprived control participants, F(1, 30) ⫽ 5.4, p ⬍ .05, 2 ⫽ .15, participants with bulimia showed the reverse response pattern. In the bulimia group, food-deprived participants exhibited significantly smaller 571 startle responses during viewing of food cues than nondeprived participants, F(1, 30) ⫽ 29.8, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .50. Food-deprived control participants showed a potentiation of their startle reflex during viewing of food cues relative to other pleasant materials whereas such potentiation did not occur for nondeprived control participants, Category ⫻ Deprivation, F(1, 30) ⫽ 7.6, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .20. By contrast, this startle potentiation was smaller for food-deprived participants with bulimia relative to those who were not food-deprived before the experiment, Category ⫻ Deprivation, F(1, 30) ⫽ 55.2, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .65. This differential responding to food cues of participants with bulimia and control participants as a function of deprivation is also supported by a significant Category ⫻ Deprivation ⫻ Pathology interaction, F(1, 60) ⫽ 49.2; p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .45. Corrugator facial muscle responses. The overall comparison of corrugator responses to food cues and pleasant pictures revealed a significant Category ⫻ Pathology interaction, F(1, 59) ⫽ 9.3, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .14. Again, this effect also occurred if corrugator responses to food cues were compared with those elicited by neutral picture contents, Category ⫻ Pathology, F(1, 59) ⫽ 9.4, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .14. Viewing of food cues resulted in a significant increase of corrugator muscle activity relative to viewing of pleasant pictures in participants with bulimia, F(1, 30) ⫽ 19.1, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .39, whereas corrugator responses to food cues overall were comparable with those evoked by other pleasant materials in control participants, F(1, 29) ⬍ 1. Again, participants with bulimia showed overall significantly larger corrugator responses during viewing of food pictures relative to control participants, F(1, 59) ⫽ 11.7, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .17, whereas no group differences were observed for the other stimuli, F(1, 59) ⬍ 1. Deprivation significantly reduced the corrugator responses to food cues, F(1, 59) ⫽ 10.6, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .15, for both groups, Deprivation ⫻ Pathology F(1, 59) ⬍ 1. Mean corrugator responses elicited during viewing of food cues and unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant pictures for deprived and nondeprived participants with bulimia and control participants are shown in Table 2. Skin conductance, heart rate, and viewing times. Mean skin conductance and heart rate responses, viewing times, and interest ratings of food cues, unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant pictures for food-deprived and nondeprived participants with bulimia and control participants are also shown in Table 2. Skin conductance and heart rate responses to food cues as well as viewing times of these stimuli did not vary for participants with bulimia and control participants (all statistical tests of the Pathology factor were not significant). By contrast, food deprivation had a significant effect on these measures. Viewing times of food cues were significantly longer for deprived relative to nondeprived participants, F(1, 60) ⫽ 9.8, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .14, and this effect did not vary for Pathology, F(1, 60) ⬍ 1. Moreover, for deprived participants viewing times of food cues were significantly longer than for neutral pictures whereas nondeprived participants viewed food 5 Replicating previous findings, blink magnitudes, corrugator EMG, and pleasure ratings were significantly modulated by the valence of the nonfood cues. By contrast, skin conductance responses, viewing times, interest, and arousal ratings varied with the arousal level of the nonfood pictures, replicating previous research. MAULER, HAMM, WEIKE, AND TUSCHEN-CAFFIER 572 Table 2 Physiological, Behavioral, and Subjective Responding to the Standard Affective and Food Pictures During the First and Second Picture Viewing Session in Deprived and Nondeprived Participants With Bulimia and Control Participants Session 1 Unpleasant Neutral Session 2 Pleasant Food Unpleasant Neutral Pleasant Food Startle blink (T scores) Bulimic Deprived Nondeprived Control Deprived Nondeprived 58.7 (1.6) 62.9 (1.5) 48.4 (0.8) 48.7 (0.9) 46.5 (0.8) 46.7 (1.0) 50.8 (0.6) 58.9 (1.3) 53.3 (1.1) 49.2 (1.2) 45.2 (0.5) 45.6 (1.0) 43.2 (0.5) 44.4 (0.9) 51.2 (0.9) 46.4 (1.6) 60.2 (1.2) 59.2 (1.4) 48.4 (0.8) 51.3 (1.0) 46.6 (0.4) 47.0 (1.0) 48.4 (0.9) 45.6 (0.8) 58.0 (1.3) 56.4 (1.8) 47.4 (0.8) 47.1 (0.8) 43.9 (0.6) 45.5 (0.7) 45.8 (1.0) 45.2 (1.0) Corrugator (⌬ V) Bulimic Deprived Nondeprived Control Deprived Nondeprived .50 (.21) .71 (.17) .21 (.09) .27 (.11) .05 (.06) ⫺.09 (.07) .19 (.11) .45 (.09) .79 (.21) .53 (.22) .30 (.15) .40 (.11) .14 (.12) .10 (.13) .45 (.14) .21 (.08) .63 (.31) .80 (.31) .28 (.16) .16 (.12) .01 (.15) ⫺.04 (.07) ⫺.27 (.13) .17 (.09) .60 (.23) 1.11 (.37) .34 (.11) .12 (.11) .11 (.09) ⫺.01 (.15) .22 (.20) .36 (.17) Skin conductance (log S) Bulimic Deprived Nondeprived Control Deprived Nondeprived .21 (.05) .10 (.03) .09 (.03) .01 (.01) .14 (.04) .06 (.03) .10 (.03) .01 (.01) .04 (.03) .03 (.02) .02 (.02) .01 (.01) .02 (.02) .02 (.02) .02 (.02) .00 (.00) .18 (.07) .24 (.06) .09 (.05) .09 (.03) .15 (.05) .19 (.05) .08 (.04) .06 (.03) .05 (.05) .06 (.02) .04 (.04) .03 (.02) .04 (.04) .07 (.03) .03 (.03) .01 (.01) Heart rate (⌬ beats/min) Bulimic Deprived Nondeprived Control Deprived Nondeprived ⫺0.91 (.49) ⫺0.17 (.60) ⫺0.87 (.29) ⫺0.64 (.33) ⫺0.21 (.55) ⫺0.65 (.50) ⫺0.03 (.50) ⫺0.63 (.54) ⫺0.92 (.42) ⫺1.18 (.57) ⫺1.04 (.27) ⫺0.38 (.35) ⫺0.63 (.48) ⫺0.74 (.41) ⫺0.19 (.44) ⫺0.39 (.31) ⫺1.57 (.60) ⫺2.51 (.85) ⫺1.29 (.58) ⫺2.13 (.82) ⫺1.02 (.52) ⫺1.64 (.69) ⫺0.11 (.44) ⫺2.15 (.50) ⫺1.63 (.61) ⫺1.06 (.85) ⫺1.51 (.83) ⫺1.25 (.47) ⫺1.68 (.53) ⫺1.00 (.39) ⫺1.60 (.48) ⫺0.70 (.59) Viewing time (s) Bulimic Deprived Nondeprived Control Deprived Nondeprived 5.9 (1.0) 5.6 (0.5) 6.0 (1.1) 5.9 (0.6) 9.9 (1.7) 6.9 (0.5) 8.1 (1.4) 5.1 (0.4) 5.1 (1.0) 4.7 (0.7) 5.1 (0.9) 4.6 (0.6) 7.5 (1.1) 6.4 (0.7) 5.9 (1.0) 4.9 (0.7) 6.3 (1.0) 6.1 (0.6) 6.5 (1.1) 5.8 (0.6) 9.3 (1.4) 9.2 (0.8) 7.9 (1.3) 4.5 (0.4) 5.0 (0.7) 4.6 (0.6) 5.1 (0.8) 4.5 (0.5) 7.1 (1.1) 6.9 (0.8) 5.7 (0.9) 5.0 (0.5) Pleasure rating (0–20) Bulimic Deprived Nondeprived Control Deprived Nondeprived 4.2 (0.2) 4.0 (0.2) 11.9 (0.7) 11.8 (0.5) 17.2 (0.3) 17.2 (0.3) 17.3 (0.3) 13.0 (0.3) 4.6 (0.3) 4.7 (0.3) 12.4 (0.7) 10.3 (0.7) 16.3 (0.3) 16.0 (0.3) 13.3 (0.4) 12.5 (0.4) 3.9 (0.2) 4.0 (0.2) 11.5 (0.3) 11.8 (0.4) 17.9 (0.2) 17.5 (0.3) 16.5 (0.2) 13.7 (0.3) 4.8 (0.3) 4.3 (0.3) 12.4 (0.7) 10.9 (0.7) 16.0 (0.4) 15.9 (0.2) 13.8 (0.5) 12.6 (0.2) Arousal rating (0–20) Bulimic Deprived Nondeprived Control Deprived Nondeprived 16.3 (0.4) 16.7 (0.3) 6.9 (0.3) 6.4 (0.3) 11.7 (0.2) 10.9 (0.3) 11.0 (0.3) 11.8 (0.2) 14.4 (0.5) 14.1 (0.5) 7.4 (0.3) 7.6 (0.3) 10.7 (0.3) 10.8 (0.3) 9.3 (0.8) 8.0 (0.8) 17.2 (0.2) 16.8 (0.3) 6.6 (0.3) 7.1 (0.4) 12.6 (0.4) 12.1 (0.2) 13.5 (0.4) 4.9 (0.4) 14.8 (0.3) 14.7 (0.5) 7.3 (0.2) 7.2 (0.4) 10.8 (0.2) 10.7 (0.3) 4.2 (0.4) 4.1 (0.3) Interest rating (0–20) Bulimic Deprived Nondeprived Control Deprived Nondeprived Note. 14.2 (.3) 14.1 (.2) 7.7 (0.4) 7.8 (0.5) 14.5 (0.3) 14.3 (0.2) 15.5 (0.3) 12.7 (0.2) 13.3 (0.6) 13.2 (0.4) 9.3 (0.5) 8.8 (0.3) 13.4 (0.4) 13.1 (0.4) 11.6 (0.5) 11.6 (0.4) 14.3 (.3) 14.2 (.2) 8.6 (0.3) 8.1 (0.3) 15.4 (0.2) 15.0 (0.3) 15.3 (0.3) 7.5 (0.3) 13.2 (0.3) 13.4 (0.4) 9.1 (0.3) 9.0 (0.4) 13.4 (0.3) 12.8 (0.3) 8.4 (0.3) 7.8 (0.3) Data are presented as means and (standard errors). AFFECT REGULATION AND BULIMIA NERVOSA 573 Figure 1. Mean blink response magnitudes (standardized scores; ⫾ SE) for startles presented during viewing of unpleasant, neutral, pleasant, and food cues for deprived and nondeprived participants with bulimia (left) and control participants (right) during the first session of picture viewing. cues as long as neutral materials, Category ⫻ Deprivation, F(1, 60) ⫽ 53.1, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .47. Again, this effect did not vary with psychopathology. Deprivation also significantly affected the heart rate responses elicited by food cues. Replicating previous findings, deprived participants showed significantly less heart rate deceleration to food cues relative to nondeprived participants, F(1, 55) ⫽ 6.8, p ⬍ .05, 2 ⫽ .11. Electrodermal orienting responses to food cues were also larger for fooddeprived relative to nondeprived participants (see Figure 2), although this difference fell short of statistical significance, F(1, 59) ⫽ 3.2, p ⫽ .08, 2 ⫽ .05. But again, these response patterns did not vary between participants with bulimia and control participants. Mean skin conductance, heart rate responses, viewing times, interest ratings, and ratings of the desire to eat while processing food cues for deprived and nondeprived participants are depicted in Figure 2. Interest ratings, desire to eat, and affective judgments. In agreement with the autonomic measures and viewing times, deprived participants also reported a significantly larger desire to eat during viewing of food pictures, F(1, 60) ⫽ 511.7, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .90, and rated these stimuli as significantly more interesting, pleasant, and arousing than nondeprived participants, Fs(1, 60) ⫽ 384.9, 134.7, and 140.6, ps ⬍ .01, s2 ⫽ .87, .69, and .70 for interest, valence, and arousal ratings, respectively. In contrast to the physiological and behavioral data, however, deprivation status changed verbal reports differently in participants with bulimia and control participants. Among control participants, deprivation caused a stronger increase in the rated desire to eat than in participants with bulimia, suggesting that the verbal reports of participants with bulimia were less explicitly influenced by the internal state of hunger than those of the control participants, Deprivation ⫻ Pathology, F(1, 60) ⫽ 32.93, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .35. The interest ratings also revealed a significant Deprivation ⫻ Pathology interaction, F(1, 60) ⫽ 84.3, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .58. Nondeprived participants with bulimia rated food cues as more interesting and arousing than nondeprived control participants, whereas no such differences occurred for deprived participants, Deprivation ⫻ Pathology, Fs(1, 60) ⫽ 84.3 and 210.3, ps ⬍ .01, 2s ⫽ .58 and .78, for interest and arousal ratings, respectively (see Table 2). In addition, participants with bulimia described food cues more often as disgusting and fearful relative to control participants, 2(4, N ⫽ 64) ⫽ 38.5; p ⬍ .01. Of the participants with bulimia, 53% rated 1–3 food cues as fear evoking, and 50% rated 1– 4 pictures as disgusting. Only 6.3% of the control participants judged 1 of the 8 food cues as being disgusting, and 9.4% rated 1 of these pictures as fearful. Food Consumption As expected, deprived participants consumed significantly more food than nondeprived participants, F(1, 60) ⫽ 20.6, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .26. The mean amounts of calories consumed by each group were 568 (SE ⫽ 82) and 587 kcal (SE ⫽ 59) for deprived participants with bulimia and control participants, respectively, and 205 (SE ⫽ 51) and 308 kcal (SE ⫽ 85) for nondeprived participants with bulimia and control participants, respectively. Food consumption did not vary between participants with bulimia and control participants in this environment, Deprivation ⫻ Pathology, F(1, 60) ⬍ 1. Second Experimental Session Standard Affective Categories The modulation of the physiological, behavioral, and self-report measures as a function of the affective valence and arousal of these nonfood stimuli replicated the findings from the first session. 574 MAULER, HAMM, WEIKE, AND TUSCHEN-CAFFIER Figure 2. Mean (⫾ SE) skin conductance responses (range-corrected log transformation), heart rate changes (during 6 s of picture viewing), viewing times (in seconds), ratings of interest, and the desire to eat (a line rating on a scale from 0 to 20) during viewing of food cues for deprived and nondeprived participants with bulimia (always left) and control participants (always right) during the first session of picture viewing. FIR ⫽ first interval response. Again, the results did not vary as function of psychopathology or the prior status of deprivation.6 For nondeprived participants with bulimia, the pronounced startle potentiation during viewing of food cues compared with viewing of pleasant materials during the first experimental session was Food Cues Startle response magnitudes. Participants with bulimia continued to exhibit significantly larger startle responses when elicited during viewing of food cues compared with control participants, Pathology, F(1, 60) ⫽ 8.1, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .12. Although control participants showed comparable blink magnitudes during viewing of food cues and other pleasant materials, participants with bulimia continued to show a significant potentiation of their startle eyeblinks elicited during viewing of food cues relative to other pleasant pictures, F(1, 30) ⫽ 25.4, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .46, or neutral pictures, F(1, 30) ⫽ 13.0, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .30. This pattern of results was confirmed by significant Category ⫻ Pathology interactions for these comparisons, F(1, 60) ⫽ 12.3 and 15.8, ps ⬍ .01, 2s ⫽ .17 and .21, for the food versus pleasant content and the food versus neutral content comparisons, respectively. Figure 3 illustrates the changes of the startle response potentiation (food minus other pleasant pictures) for participants with bulimia and control participants according to their prior status of deprivation. 6 Although startle blink and skin conductance response magnitudes were generally reduced in the second relative to the first experimental session (Session, F(1, 60) ⫽ 30.9, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .34; F(1, 59) ⫽ 26.6, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .31, for startle blink and skin conductance responses, respectively); the modulation of these measures by the affective content of the pictures remained unchanged during the second phase of the experiment. Again, a comparable pattern was observed for corrugator activity and pleasure ratings. Accordingly, although viewing times and interest ratings also generally decreased across sessions (Session, Fs(1, 60) ⫽ 31.9 and 18.3, ps ⬍ .01, 2s ⫽ .35 and .23, for viewing times and interest ratings, respectively), pleasant and unpleasant pictures were viewed again for a longer period of time and were rated as more interesting than neutral pictures. The same pattern of results was obtained for the arousal ratings. All interactions between Category ⫻ Psychopathology and Category ⫻ Deprivation were again not significant with one exception. Control participants had a stronger potentiation of their startle responses during viewing of unpleasant pictures relative to neutral materials than participants with bulimia in this second experimental session. AFFECT REGULATION AND BULIMIA NERVOSA Figure 3. Differences of the mean startle responses (⫾SE) elicited during viewing of food cues and other pleasant pictures during the first session and the second session of picture viewing for deprived and nondeprived participants with bulimia and control participants. The second picture viewing session followed food consumption from a breakfast buffet. Note that deprived participants ate significantly more calories from the buffet than nondeprived participants, irrespective of the psychopathology. strongly reduced during the second session, whereas the amount of potentiation increased for the previously deprived participants with bulimia, Deprivation ⫻ Session, F(1, 30) ⫽ 29.5, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .50. These participants with bulimia, however, also consumed significantly more food during the recreational break than the previously nondeprived participants with bulimia, suggesting that the affective valence of food cues changed as a result of eating. No such interaction occurred for the control participants, Deprivation ⫻ Session, F(1, 30) ⬍ 1. This pattern of results was also supported by a significant Deprivation ⫻ Session ⫻ Pathology interaction, F(1, 60) ⫽ 22.0, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .27 (see Figure 3). These findings were further supported by a correlational analysis of the linear relationship between food consumption and change in startle potentiation from Session 1 to Session 2. Although no significant correlation between food consumption and change in startle potentiation was found for control participants, (r ⫽ ⫺.08, ns), a significant correlation between these variables was obtained for participants with bulimia (r ⫽ .39, p ⬍ .03). Corrugator facial muscle responses. Corrugator responses showed the same pattern of results, although the effects were less pronounced than for startle blink magnitudes. As observed for startle responses, corrugator activity to food cues increased for previously deprived participants with bulimia and decreased for previously nondeprived participants with bulimia, Deprivation ⫻ Session, F(1, 30) ⫽ 11.6, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .28. Again, no such interaction was observed for control participants, F(1, 29) ⬍ 1. In contrast to the startle results, the three-way interaction was not significant for corrugator responses in the between-group comparison. 575 Skin conductance and viewing times. As expected, skin conductance to food cues as well as viewing times of these pictures decreased significantly in the second session, Session, Fs(1, 59/ 60) ⫽ 13.2 and 9.0, ps ⬍ .01, 2s ⫽ .18 and .13, for skin conductance and viewing times, respectively. Viewing times of food cues were still increased compared with those for neutral pictures, Category, F(1, 60) ⫽ 11.5, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .16, but were now significantly shorter than those for other pleasant materials, Category, F(1, 60) ⫽ 61.1, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .50. A similar pattern of results was observed for skin conductance. Electrodermal orienting responses elicited by food cues were no longer more pronounced than those evoked by neutral pictures but were now significantly smaller than those evoked by other pleasant materials, Category, F(1, 59) ⫽ 8.75, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .13. As during the first experimental session, psychopathology did not modulate this pattern of results; that is, these effects did not vary for participants with bulimia and control participants. Interest ratings, desire to eat, and affective judgments. In line with behavioral and physiological data, all participants rated food cues as less interesting, arousing, and pleasant and reported a reduced desire to eat in the second compared with the first experimental session, Session, Fs(1, 60) ⫽ 128.1, 96.8, 139.2, and 304.6, ps ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .68, .62, .70, and .84, for interest, pleasure, arousal, and desire to eat ratings, respectively. Moreover, these changes were stronger for previously deprived than for nondeprived participants, Session ⫻ Deprivation, Fs(1, 60) ⫽ 96.6, 36.6, 22.9, and 193.7, ps ⬍ .01, 2s ⫽ .62, .38, .28, and .76, for interest, pleasure, arousal, and desire to eat ratings, respectively. In contrast to behavioral and physiological data, the verbal report data again clearly discriminated between participants with bulimia and control participants. Participants with bulimia, although no longer hungry, still reported significantly higher interest in food cues and rated these cues as more arousing relative to satiated control participants, Pathology, Fs(1, 60) ⫽ 82.3 and 53.1; ps ⬍ .01, 2s ⫽ .58 and .47 for interest and arousal ratings, respectively, and also reported a significantly higher desire to eat (M ⫽ 6.4, SE ⫽ 0.5) than control participants (M ⫽ 4.1, SE ⫽ 0.4), Pathology, F(1, 60) ⫽ 14.6, p ⬍ .01, 2 ⫽ .20. Discussion Emotional Responses to Food Cues: The Influence of the Bulimic Psychopathology Although emotional responses to standard affective materials were comparable for participants with bulimia and control participants, food cues prompted a different response pattern in both groups. Food cues prompted an appetitive and positive affective state in normal control participants as indicated by an inhibition of the startle response comparable to that induced by other pleasant stimuli. In contrast, participants with bulimia exhibited a substantial potentiation of their blink response magnitudes, almost comparable to the augmentation that could be observed during viewing of frankly unpleasant emotional stimuli. These data suggest that food cues, although having generally appetitive qualities in healthy volunteers, prompted negative affect in those with bulimia and also support findings indicating that individuals with bulimia consistently show a decreased initial salivary response to food cues (Bulik, Lawson, & Carter, 1996; Wisniewski, Epstein, Marcus, & 576 MAULER, HAMM, WEIKE, AND TUSCHEN-CAFFIER Kaye, 1997). Corrugator facial muscle activity also supports the conclusion that food cues activate an aversive emotional response pattern in participants with bulimia. In these individuals with eating disorders, food cues elicited a strong increase in corrugator muscle activity, whereas the control participants’ corrugator responses were comparable to those elicited by other pleasant cues. Indeed, corrugator activity has been shown to have a very close relationship with the negative valence of visual materials and is specifically increased during aversive states of disgust (Bradley et al., 2001; Hamm, Schupp, & Weike, 2003; Lang et al., 1993). An interesting finding is that participants with bulimia rated the food pictures overall as being as pleasant and interesting as did the control participants. This suggests that the participants with bulimia may not have been aware of the negative affect elicited by the pictures. On the other hand, if participants with bulimia are instructed to label their feeling states associated with food cues, these participants rated food cues more often as disgusting and fearful than did control participants, thus revealing a discrepancy between the dimensional and categorical judgments. The reason for this discrepancy might be that during the dimensional ratings, the participant is more directed to the food cue itself, whereas the categorical judgment task might rather direct the participant to rate her internal state. In any event, the categorical ratings support other research in which individuals with bulimia report more negative feelings than do others during eating or while looking at, smelling, or touching food (Bulik et al., 1996; Buree et al., 1990; Legenbauer, Vögele, & Rüddel, 2004; Neudeck, Florin, & Tuschen-Caffier, 2001; Staiger et al., 2000). These negative feelings increase if purging is rendered impossible after eating a test meal (Rosen, Leitenberg, Gross, & Willmuth, 1985) and decrease after purging (Powell & Thelen, 1996). In the same vein deprivation and subsequent eating affected emotional responding to food cues in a different way in participants with bulimia and control participants in the current experiment. Emotional Responses to Food Cues: The Influence of Food Deprivation Deprivation increased viewing times and electrodermal orienting responses to food cues in both groups. Moreover, deprived participants rated food cues as being more interesting and arousing, suggesting that food cues become more salient when participants are hungry. More important, this increased incentive value of food cues as a result of the motivational drive was not modulated by psychopathology, indicating that the regulation of hunger motivation was comparable for both groups. In contrast, deprivation affected the emotional impact of the food cues in qualitatively different ways in the two groups. Healthy control participants showed an increase in startle response magnitudes to food cues when they were deprived, supporting the between-groups findings of Drobes et al. (2001). Drobes and colleagues argued that food cues might prompt a state of frustrative nonreward in deprived subjects, because the experimental context did not allow immediate food consumption. Therefore, food cues might activate a negative affective state of frustration in hungry participants, and the startle data of the current experiment support these conclusions. It has to be mentioned, however, that corrugator activity was reduced during viewing of food cues in deprived control participants replicating the findings of Drobes et al. (2001). Such a facial action pattern is normally associated with positive valence and would suggest that food cues indeed became more attractive after deprivation. A possible interpretation of these differences might be, comparable to the differences in the verbal report measures, that startle modulation primarily indexes the subcortically mediated internal affective disposition, whereas corrugator activity is more driven by the attractiveness of the external cues. In contrast with control women, deprived women with bulimia exhibited significantly less startle potentiation during viewing of food cues than did nondeprived women with bulimia. In deprived women with bulimia, startle response magnitudes elicited during viewing of food cues were comparable to those evoked during viewing of neutral pictures. These data indicate that deprivation reduced the startle potentiation to food cues in women with bulimia. This finding supports an affect regulation model, suggesting that the negative affect induced by food cues might motivate individuals with bulimia utilizing fasting and restrained eating to reduce the negative affect associated with food items. This interpretation was supported by the startle modulation during viewing of food cues after food intake. Food Intake and Affect Regulation in Bulimia Nervosa Deprived control participants and participants with bulimia consumed significantly more food from the breakfast buffet than did nondeprived participants. As expected, food consumption resulted in a significant reduction of electrodermal orienting, viewing times, interest, and arousal ratings of food cues, again supporting the idea that these indices are related to the increased attention that is allocated to food cues as a result of the motivational state of hunger. Although autonomic orienting and behavioral data did not differ between both groups, participants with bulimia still rated food cues as being more arousing and interesting than did control participants. Moreover, startle responses elicited during viewing of food cues varied substantially between participants with bulimia and control participants after food consumption. Whereas for the control group startle response magnitudes were not affected by the amount of food consumption, blink magnitudes during viewing of food cues were significantly affected by the previous eating behavior in the bulimia nervosa group. Nondeprived women with bulimia, who were not hungry and therefore consumed fewer calories at the breakfast buffet, showed a pronounced decrease in startle response magnitudes elicited during food cues: Blink magnitudes were now comparable to those elicited during viewing of other pleasant pictures. This restrained food intake might have induced a conviction of control that subsequently reduced the aversive emotional quality of the food cues. This interpretation was also supported by the results of corrugator activity. An opposite response pattern was observed for the previously deprived participants with bulimia, who consumed a larger amount of food because they were hungry. This group did not show a decrease but exhibited a significant increase in startle response magnitude during viewing of food cues instead. These data are in line with the hypothesis that the deprivation-induced incentive properties of the food cues might now have been blunted by satiation or the effects of perceived overeating. AFFECT REGULATION AND BULIMIA NERVOSA Implications for the Psychopathology of Bulimia Nervosa In the present study we tested the emotional responding of deprived and nondeprived women with bulimia to salient food cues. Moreover, we investigated whether these affective responses are modified by eating. The data clearly show that food cues evoke negative emotional responses in individuals with bulimia comparable with those elicited by other unpleasant stimuli. This finding has important implications for understanding the psychopathology of bulimia nervosa. In his dual pathway model of bulimic pathology, Stice (2001) hypothesized that elevated pressure to be thin fosters body dissatisfaction in individuals with bulimia. This increased body dissatisfaction then promotes negative affect. Our data support and extend this model by demonstrating that food cues in the environment effectively prime this negative affect. The present data also suggest that the negative affect primed by food items may motivate those with bulimia utilizing fasting and restrained eating to ameliorate the negative affect associated with food cues. In a sense, individuals with bulimia make food less frightening or threatening by demonstrating control over their consumption of it. According to the dual pathway model, however, deprivation further increases negative affect. Actually, prospective studies revealed that fasting predicted overall enhancement of negative affect (Stice & Bearman, 2001). Accordingly, deprivation might produce the same negative affect that has been found to be the motivational core of the drug withdrawal syndrome (see Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie, & Fiore, 2004). From this perspective, it is surprising that the food-induced potentiation of the startle response was reduced in individuals with bulimia after deprivation. Deprivation, however, not only induces general distress but also inflates the incentive value of food cues (see Baker et al., 2004). There is evidence from data obtained from former prisoners of war that after severe food deprivation, these individuals not only eat beyond metabolic requirements when given free access to food but also show a strong preoccupation with food (Polivy, Zeitlin, Herman, & Beal, 1994; for discussion of these effects see also Baker et al., 2004). The significant reduction of the protective startle reflex during viewing of food cues in deprived participants with bulimia is in line with this interpretation. Supporting this view, greater activation of the amygdala and related paralimbic structures was found during viewing of food-related stimuli when participants were hungry (LaBar et al., 2001). This increased incentive value of the food cues together with the overall negative affective state induced by deprivation might explain why contact with specific food cues could trigger binge-eating episodes in individuals with bulimia, almost comparable to the motivational basis of drug cues triggering relapse (Baker et al., 2004). Recent data from Mann and Ward (2004) also demonstrate that food consumption is significantly increased, if food cues are made more salient. Of course, deprivation might reduce startle among individuals with bulimia for another reason; a period of successful deprivation might create a sense of control over food and this may render it less threatening. If the restrained eating pattern collapses, that is, if individuals with bulimia fail to maintain tight control over their eating, food once again becomes threatening. Participants with bulimia who ate a greater number of calories from the breakfast buffet (i.e., the previously deprived participants) subsequently showed a strong potentiation of their startle reflex during viewing of food cues, 577 suggesting that these stimuli now prompted a defensive motivational state. This pattern of results is in line with cognitive models of bulimic pathology suggesting that core beliefs such as “control over eating” are central for bulimia nervosa (Cooper, Wells, & Todd, 2004). Rigid self-regulation seems to be fundamental to the self-esteem of women with bulimia nervosa (Fairburn, Cooper, & Shafran, 2003). If this self-regulation is violated, the negative affect induced by food cues seems to be increased as suggested by the data of the current study. If, however, the women with bulimia had the conviction that they were able to control their eating behavior (e.g., as manifest in modest food intake during the breakfast or by fasting during the pre-experimental deprivation period), startle responses evoked by food cues decreased. The data of the current experiment not only reveal some new aspects in the psychopathology of bulimia nervosa, but they may also have some implications for the treatment of this disorder. According to the present results, the effectiveness of cue exposure therapy might be increased if exposure is conducted after the consumption of a normal meal, because the patient with bulimia has to cope with the negative affect associated with the violation against the restrained eating pattern, that is, the increased (but normal) food intake. Moreover, this paradigm might also be suitable for assessments of treatment effects beyond the level of verbal report on the level of the basic motivational potency of food cues during hungry and satiated states. References Allen, N. B., Trinder, J., & Brennan, C. (1999). Affective startle modulation on clinical depression: Preliminary findings. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 542–550. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Baker, T. B., Piper, M. E., McCarthy, D. E., Majeskie, M. R., & Fiore, M. C. (2004). Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111, 35–51. Bradley, M. M. (2000). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 602– 642). New York: Cambridge University Press. Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation. I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276 –298. Brewerton, T., Dansky, B., Kilpatrick, D., & O’Neal, P. (2000). Which comes first in the pathogenesis of bulimia nervosa: Dieting or binging? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 259 –264. Bulik, C. M., & Brinded, E. C. (1994). The effect of food deprivation on the reinforcing value of food and smoking in bulimic and control women. Physiology & Behavior, 55, 665– 672. Bulik, C. M., Lawson, R. H., & Carter, F. A. (1996). Salivary reactivity in restrained and unrestrained eaters and women with bulimia nervosa. Appetite, 27, 15–24. Bulik, C. M., Sullivan, P., Carter, F., & Joyce, P. (1997). Initial manifestations of disordered eating behavior: Dieting versus binging. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 22, 195–201. Buree, B. U., Papageorgis, D., & Hare, R. D. (1990). Eating in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: An application of the tripartite model of anxiety. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 22, 207–218. Cacioppo, J. T., Klein, D. J., Berntson, G. G., & Hartfield, E. (1993). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 119 –142). New York: Guilford Press. Carter, F., & Bulik, C. M. (1996). Cue reactivity and bulimia nervosa: 578 MAULER, HAMM, WEIKE, AND TUSCHEN-CAFFIER Refining and standardizing methodology. Behavior Change, 13, 98 – 111. Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention (CSEA). (1995). The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) [Photographic slides/CD]. Gainesville FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. Cook, E. W., Davis, T. L., Hawk, L. W., Spence, E. L., & Gautier, C. H. (1992). Fearfulness and startle potentiation during aversive visual stimuli. Psychophysiology, 29, 633– 645. Cooper, M., Wells, A., & Todd, G. (2004). A cognitive model of bulimia nervosa. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 1–16. Davis, M. (2000). The role of the amygdala in conditioned and unconditioned fear and anxiety. In J. P. Aggleton (Ed.), The amygdala (pp. 213–287). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Davis, R., Freeman, R., & Garner, D. (1988). A naturalistic investigation of eating behavior of bulimia nervosa. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 273–279. Drobes, D., Miller, E. J., Bradley, M. M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Food deprivation and emotional reactions to food cues: Implications for eating disorders. Biological Psychology, 57, 153–177. Fairburn, C. G., & Cooper, Z. (1993). The Eating Disorder Examination (12th ed.). In C. G. Fairburn & G. T. Wilson (Eds.), Binge eating. Nature, assessment, and treatment (pp. 317–360). New York: Guilford Press. Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., & Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: A “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 509 –528. Fairburn, C. G., Stice, E., Cooper, Z., Doll, H., Norman, P., & O’Connor, M. (2003). Understanding persistence in bulimia nervosa: A 5-year naturalistic study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 103–109. Fridlund, A. J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567–589. Globisch, J., Hamm, A. O., Schneider, R., & Vaitl, D. (1993). A computer program for scoring reflex eyeblink and electrodermal responses written in PASCAL [Abstract]. Psychophysiology, 30(Suppl.), S30. Graham, F. K. (1978). Constraints in measuring heart rate and period sequentially through real and cardiac time. Psychophysiology, 15, 492– 495. Hamm, A. O., Cuthbert, B. N., Globisch, J., & Vaitl, D. (1997). Fear and the startle reflex: Blink modulation and autonomic response patterns in animal and mutilation fearful subjects. Psychophysiology, 34, 97–107. Hamm, A. O., Schupp, H. T., & Weike, A. I. (2003). Motivational organization of emotions: Autonomic changes, cortical responses, and reflex modulation. In R. J. Davidson, K. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 187–211). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Hamm, A. O., & Vaitl, D. (1993). Emotionsinduktion durch visuelle Reize: Validierung einer Stimulationsmethode auf drei Reaktionsebenen [Emotion induction by visual stimuli: Validation of a method to evoke emotion using three response systems]. Psychologische Rundschau, 44, 143–161. Hawk, L. W., Baschnagl, J. S., Ashare, R. L., & Epstein, L. H. (2004). Craving and startle modification during in vivo exposure to food cues. Appetite, 43, 285–294. Hodes, R. L., Cook, E. W., III, & Lang, P. J. (1985). Individual differences in autonomic response: Conditioned association or conditioned fear? Psychophysiology, 22, 545–560. Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative activity of the brain: An interdisciplinary approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. LaBar, K. S., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., Kim, Y-H., Nobre, A. C., & Mesulam, M. M. (2001). Hunger selectively modulates corticolimbic activation to food stimuli in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 493–500. Lang, P. J. (1985). The cognitive psychophysiology of emotion: Fear and anxiety. In A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 131–170). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1998). Emotion, motivation, and anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 1248 –1263. Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–273. Lavy, E. H., & van den Hout, M. A. (1993). Attentional bias for appetitive cues: Effects of fasting in normal subjects. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 21, 297–310. Legenbauer, T., Vögele, C., & Rüddel, H. (2004). Anticipatory effects of food exposure in women diagnosed with bulimia nervosa. Appetite, 42, 33– 40. Leitenberg, H., Gross, J., Peterson, J., & Rosen, J. C. (1984). Analysis of an anxiety model and the process of change during exposure plus response prevention treatment of bulimia nervosa. Behavior Therapy, 15, 3–20. Leon, G. R., Fulkerson, J. A., Perry, C. L., & Early-Zald, M. B. (1995). Prospective analysis of personality and behavioral vulnerabilities and gender influences in the later development of disordered eating. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 140 –149. Lykken, D. T., & Venables, P. H. (1971). Direct measurement of skin conductance: A proposal for standardization. Psychophysiology, 8, 656 – 672. Mann, T., & Ward, A. (2004). To eat or not to eat: Implications of the attentional myopia model for restrained eaters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 90 –98. Neudeck, P., Florin, I., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2001). Food exposure in patients with bulimia nervosa. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 70, 193–200. Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the criminal psychopath: Startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 82–92. Patton, G., Johnson-Sabine, E., Wood, K., Mann, A., & Wakeling, A. (1990). Abnormal eating attitudes in London schoolgirls—A prospective epidemiological study: Outcome at twelve month follow-up. Psychological Medicine, 20, 383–394. Polivy, J., Herman, C. P., & Howard, K. I. (1988). The restraint scale: Assessment of dieting. In M. Hersen & A. Bellack (Eds.), Dictionary of behavioral assessment techniques (pp. 377–380). New York: Pergamon Press. Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2002). Causes of eating disorders. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 53, 187–213. Polivy, J., Zeitlin, S. B., Herman, C. P., & Beal, A. L. (1994). Food restriction and binge eating: A study of former prisoners of war. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 409 – 411. Powell, A., & Thelen, M. (1996). Emotions and cognitions associated with bingeing and weight control behavior in bulimia. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 40, 317–328. Prokasy, W. F., & Kumpfer, K. L. (1973). Classical conditioning. In W. F. Prokasy & D. C. Raskin (Eds.), Electrodermal activity in psychophysiological research (pp. 157–202). New York: Academic Press. Raynor, H., & Epstein, L. (2003). The relative-reinforcing value of food under differing levels of food deprivation and restriction. Appetite, 40, 15–24. Rosen, J., Leitenberg, H., Gross, J., & Willmuth, M. (1985). Standardized test meals in the assessment of bulimia nervosa. Advances in Behavior Research and Therapy, 7, 181–197. Rosen, J., Tacy, B., & Howell, D. (1990). Life stress, psychological symptoms and weight reducing behavior in adolescent girls: A prospective analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 9, 255–267. Spielberger, C., Gorsuch, R., & Lushene, R. (1970). Manual of the State- AFFECT REGULATION AND BULIMIA NERVOSA Trait Anxiety Inventory (self-evaluation questionnaire). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Staats, A., & Hammond, O. (1972). Natural words as physiological conditioned stimuli: Food-word-elicited salivation and deprivation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 96, 206 –208. Staiger, P., Dawe, S., & McCarthy, R. (2000). Responsivity to food cues in bulimic women and controls. Appetite, 35, 27–33. Stice, E. (2001). A prospective test of the dual-pathway model of bulimic pathology: Mediating effects of dieting and negative affect. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 124 –135. Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: A meta-anlaytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 825– 848. Stice, E., & Bearman, S. (2001). Body image and eating disturbances prospectively predict growth in depressive symptoms in adolescent girls: A growth curve analysis. Developmental Psychology, 37, 597– 607. Stunkard, A. J., & Messick, S. (1985). The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 29, 71– 81. Venables, P. H., & Christie, M. J. (1980). Electrodermal activity. In I. Martin & P. H. Venables (Eds.), Techniques in psychophysiology (pp. 3– 67). Chichester, England: Wiley. 579 Vrana, S. R., Spence, E. L., & Lang, P. J. (1988). The startle probe response: A new measure of emotion? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 487– 491. Webber, J., & Macdonald, I. A. (1994). The cardiovascular, metabolic, and hormonal changes accompanying acute starvation in men and women. British Journal of Nutrition, 71, 437– 447. Williamson, D., Kelley, M., Davis, C., Ruggiero, L., & Veitia, M. (1985). The psychophysiology of bulimia nervosa. Advances in Behavior Research and Therapy, 7, 163–172. Wisniewski, L., Epstein, L. H., Marcus, M. D., & Kaye, W. (1997). Differences in salivary habituation to palatable foods in bulimia nervosa patients and controls. Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 427– 433. Wooley, O., & Wooley, S. (1981). Relationships of salivation in humans to deprivation, inhibition, and the encephalization of hunger. Appetite, 2, 331–350. Received October 1, 2004 Revision received May 31, 2005 Accepted January 10, 2006 䡲