R E S P O N S I B I L I T I E S ethics & L E A D E R S H I P Making Independence Decisions under the Code of Professional Conduct Understanding and Controlling Common Cognitive Biases By James Schmutte and James R. Duncan C onsider the hypothetical case of Robert Jones, CPA. Jones, an audit partner in the firm Jones & Smith (J&S), feels quite pleased with himself because he just spoke with the controller of Ace Manufacturing, a nonpublic entity and long-term audit client, about an engagement for J&S to assist in revising the company’s financial reporting system. J&S has performed several nonattest engagements for the company; in addition, J&S has performed three special internal audit projects this year and is scheduled to prepare the corporate tax returns. Jones is already contemplating next year’s audit and how to address the AICPA’s new ethics guidance defining the preparation of financial statements as a nonattest service. Like many CPAs, Jones is faced with the challenge of serving audit clients with an expanding array of nonattest services, while maintaining the independence required for core attestation services. Before accepting a nonattest engagement, Jones consults the AICPA’s Code of Professional Conduct to determine whether the service is permitted under the independence standards. If the specific service is not prohibited, Jones understands that he is required to exercise professional judgment in the consideration of additional factors before accepting the engagement. But CPAs should be aware of some common cognitive biases that can impact one’s decision making with respect to independence. Independence Requirements The AICPA’s Code of Professional Conduct requires CPAs to remain independent in fact (and in mind) and in appearance for all attest engagements. Independence of mind means that the CPA can perform the attest service without being affected by influ- 68 ences that compromise professional judgment; this enables a professional to act with integrity and exercise objectivity and professional skepticism. In essence, the CPA holds no preconceived notions about the outcome of the attest function as a result of personal biases or other circumstances. Independence in appearance is the avoidance of circumstances that would cause a reasonable and informed third party that has knowl- Decisions made using a structured framework are more likely to be successful. edge of all relevant information (including safeguards applied) to reasonably conclude that the integrity, objectivity, or professional skepticism of a firm or a member of the attest engagement team had been compromised. In other words, neither the firm nor the attest team members are viewed as engaged in any conflict of interest regarding the client. Independence in fact and appearance do not mean a practitioner must be free of any and all influences (i.e., a state of absolute independence); rather, independence means that the practitioner is not engaged in a situation or relationship presenting an unacceptable risk that integrity, objectivity, and professional skepticism will not be exercised (or perceived as being unable to be exercised) by a reasonable and informed third party. The AICPA’s Code of Professional Conduct defines certain client relationships as presenting unacceptable risks to independence that cannot be mitigated—and therefore are prohibited (Interpretation 101-3, “Performance of Nonattest Service”). The AICPA’s Code of Professional Conduct does not (and cannot) address all possible nonattest services and other client relationships. In 2006, the Code of Professional Conduct incorporated a conceptual framework for independence that CPAs are required to consider when making decisions on independence matters not explicitly addressed by the code. The conceptual framework is based on a “threats and safeguards” model and requires a “risk-based” application (Exhibit 1). The Government Accountability Office’s (GAO) 2013 “Revision of Government Audit Standards” incorporates a comparable independence conceptual framework. (For a discussion of a conceptual framework model, see Mark Myring and Robert Bloom, “ISB’s Conceptual Framework for Auditor Independence,” The CPA Journal, January 2003.) Common to both the AICPA’s and GAO’s independence standards is the added requirement to evaluate independence threats not only individually, but also collectively. (The AICPA’s requirement applies to periods beginning on or after December 15, 2014.) Accordingly, CPAs need to consider both the appropriateness of the added service and its incremental impact on independence in light of existing services or relationships. This can be viewed as analogous to the auditing standards requirement to evaluate the materiality of misstatements, both individually and in aggregate, as well as prior uncorrected misstatements. Decision-Making Biases When a nonattest service and client relationship is not specifically prohibited in the Code of Professional Conduct, a CPA must exercise professional judgment in applying the “threats and safeguards” model to decide whether to accept a nonattest engagement. Research shows that decisions OCTOBER 2014 / THE CPA JOURNAL made using a structured framework are more likely to be successful. Guidance from the Committee of Sponsoring Organizations (COSO), Enhancing Board Oversight: Avoiding Judgment Traps and Biases (2012), illustrates a structured approach: 1) define the problem and identify the fundamental objectives, 2) consider the alternatives, 3) gather and evaluate information, 4) reach a conclusion, and 5) articulate and document the rationale. Using a structured framework, however, does not guarantee that professional judgment will not be compromised. To deal with the number and complexity of daily decisions, humans subconsciously develop mental routines or shortcuts. The social sciences use the term “heuristics” to describe the cognitive strategies that people employ when solving problems or making decisions. Research demonstrates that heuristics can introduce cognitive blinders and biases that corrupt and hamper decision making. For example, “bounded awareness” is a form of cognitive tunnel vision that individuals experience when absorbed in a task. Being overly focused inhibits the ability to seek, see, and process easily information that is relevant to decision making. Exploiting this vulnerability is the secret of every successful theatrical magician. In the hypothetical example above, if Jones is so focused on providing client services and generating firm revenue, he might fail to recognize a potential threat to his independence from providing added nonattest services for Ace Manufacturing. With his attention bounded by client service and business development, he is not alert to the incremental independence threats that an additional nonattest service can present. Professional standards require independence in mind and in appearance; thus, if Jones only focuses on independence in mind, he might be blind to threats to independence in appearance. Bias in a decision-making context does not mean being prejudicial and unwilling to have an open mind; rather, it refers to an unconscious inclination toward a particular outcome or belief that can affect how humans search for and process information. The social sciences identify more than 90 such cognitive biases. Biases can be neatly defined in the literature, but in reality they often overlap and are interrelated, similar to OCTOBER 2014 / THE CPA JOURNAL many other social and psychological phenomena. (For a discussion of cognitive biases in an audit context, see Michael C. Knapp and Carol A. Knapp, “Cognitive Biases in Audit Engagements: Errors in Judgment and Strategies for Prevention,” The CPA Journal, June 2012.) The biases that can impact decision making are unconscious; thus, it is hard to recognize when and how they are impacting decisions. For example, Jones must initially acknowledge certain fundamental biases before they negatively affect his decision making. First, practitioners tend to act or make decisions quickly. Although a quick response time is critical to providing client services, some decisions require more thoughtful processing. Second, people have a natural tendency to be self-serving and to seek outcomes that benefit them or their organization. Adding another line of service can improve Jones’s performance appraisal and compensation. Third, individuals must acknowledge their vulnerability to biases when making decisions. Researchers refer to this blind spot as “illusory superiority”— that is, a tendency to overstate abilities or to think of ourselves as less biased than others. Accordingly, Jones must accept the reality that, as he applies professional judgment in assessing his independence, unconscious forces might influence his decisions. For example, “anchoring bias” is the tendency for one’s first impressions or reactions to become the reference point (or anchor) for subsequent thoughts and actions. When the client proposed the need for an added service, Jones could very easily become subconsciously anchored on the decision to accept the engagement. His subsequent actions might incorporate acceptance of the engagement as the focus point. In a similar vein, people can introduce bias by how they initially “frame” the independence issue. Social science research shows that how individuals phrase or frame a decision question not only anchors an initial position, but also directs subsequent inquiries. For example, if Jones poses the question in terms of whether the engagement will impair his independence, his decision making will likely focus on the independence-in-fact dimension. This will anchor him on a characteristic in which he places great pride and confidence. On the other hand, if he poses the question in terms of whether the engagement will cause others to question his independence, Jones is forced to consider independence from a different perspective. Independence in appearance is a dimension that he might not be as comfortable and confident in assuming. If individuals anchor on a position, confirmatory bias can influence their search for information. People tend to seek evidence that supports their point of view and downplay that which contradicts it. As Jones reviews the professional standards and guidance, he might assign greater weight and importance to information that sustains his decision to take on the new engagement. On the other hand, he might discount any contradictory information he discovers. “Availability bias” refers to the tendency to believe that the information one can easily recall from memory is more relevant or important to the decision at hand. People are especially vulnerable if the information relates to significant or important outcomes of earlier decisions or events. The decision regarding Ace Manufacturing’s current request can be overly influenced by the fact that Jones easily recalls the positive effects to his client, himself, and his firm from the earlier decisions to accept nonattest engagements. The unconscious desire to maintain or support earlier decisions can also introduce biases into decision making. The “status quo bias,” for example, is a motivation for the current decision to result in an action that supports earlier decisions. If Jones decides that the new nonattest engagement does not pose an independence threat, the validity of the independence decisions regarding the earlier engagements would be maintained; on the other hand, deciding otherwise could bring into question his decisions regarding independence on other nonattest engagements. In a similar manner, the “sunk-cost bias” influences current decisions, as individuals unconsciously seek to support the choices they have made in the past. The sunk cost is not a financial cost in an accounting sense; rather, it is the psychological investment one has made in earlier decisions. People are especially prone to the sunk-cost bias when there are negative or unfavorable consequences if earlier decisions are proven wrong. If Jones determines that acceptance of the proposed nonattest engagement creates an independence threat, his indepen- 69 dence on other nonattest engagements might also be called into question. If a practitioner decides to consult with others, biases may still be introduced. The “halo effect bias” refers to incorrectly transferring impressions of a person’s abilities or traits in one area to another unrelated trait or ability. Practitioners naturally seek counsel from those they consider experts. Unfortunately, Jones’s colleagues, who are qualified in accounting and auditing matters, are not necessarily equally competent to make independence determinations. As an additional risk, those with whom Jones seeks counsel might interject their own unconscious biases into the decision-making process. Controlling Biases CPAs can utilize several measures to mitigate the basic cognitive biases that can affect professional judgment when considering the independence implications of performing nonattest services for attest clients (Exhibit 2). In this example, Jones must first EXHIBIT 1 AICPA Code of Professional Conduct Independence Threats n n n n n n n Self-review threat Advocacy threat Adverse interest threat Familiarity threat Undue influence threat Financial interest threat Management participation threat EXHIBIT 2 How to Mitigate the Effects of Bias n Frame questions from both internal and external perspectives n Seek opposing and disconfirming evidence n Question expert opinions n Encourage opposing points of view n Slow down the decision process 70 acknowledge that everyone harbors these biases. Heuristic strategies typically result in efficient and effective decision making, but in some settings they impair professional judgment. Second, Jones must be prepared to turn down the engagement in order to maintain his audit independence. Lastly, Jones needs time to thoughtfully apply a structured decision-making approach, control his biases, and exercise professional judgment. Time allows individuals to broaden their focus, and it is an important strategy to counteract bias threats. How Jones defines or frames the independence issue is critical to decision making. Professional standards require independence in both mind and appearance. Jones should frame the independence question from both internal and external perspectives. He should ask whether performing the nonattest engagement will present an unacceptable risk to his ability to make objective judgments during performance of the audit (an internal perspective). Then, he should ask whether others would perceive performing this nonattest engagement as an unacceptable risk to his independence during the audit (an external perspective). If a risk to independence is unacceptable, Jones should critically consider the effectiveness of the various independence safeguards in mitigating the framed threat. Framing the issue from multiple perspectives helps alleviate the risk that Jones may subconsciously anchor on one dimension or solution. When researching independence issues, Jones should make a conscious effort to search for and consider information that may support or negate audit independence if the nonattest engagement is accepted. One approach would be to separately assign the research tasks: one party to gather information in support of an acceptance decision and another to research why the engagement should be declined. This approach would help reduce availability bias as well as provide a broader search to minimize the risk of bounded awareness. Another effective risk reduction strategy is to reach out and include others in the decision-making process. Caution, however, should be exercised in identifying from whom to seek counsel. Jones might want to look beyond his accounting and auditing colleagues (to avoid any self-serving and halo biases). The ideal candidate would be knowledgeable about the Code of Professional Conduct’s conceptual framework and wouldn’t have a stake in the issue. Such a party would be free of not only self-interest biases, but also residual biases due to earlier decisions. Jones should also look for someone who has license to tell him what he needs to hear instead of what he might want to hear. During the discussions, the consultant should be asked to play the role of devil’s advocate in order to test the strengths of the decision alternatives. In addition, the AICPA Ethics Hotline (888-777-7077) might be a useful resource for some. Once Jones has determined that a nonattest service is not specifically prohibited by the applicable professional standards and does not represent an unacceptable threat to independence (after taking into account appropriate safeguards, if necessary), he would be wise to document his decisionmaking process and supporting rationale. In addition, Jones must satisfy other Code of Professional Conduct requirements. Specifically, he should 1) not assume management responsibilities for the attest client; 2) determine that the client has agreed to assume all management responsibilities, designate an appropriately qualified individual to oversee the services, evaluate the adequacy and results of the services performed, and accept responsibility for the results of the services; and 3) establish and document in writing his understanding with the client. Looking to the Future As the variety of nonattest services continues to expand, the standards in AICPA’s Code of Professional Conduct might not keep pace. Accordingly, CPAs can expect to face more independence-threatening situations that will require their professional judgment. Knowledge, experience, and skepticism are commonly viewed as critical factors of professional judgment. Another component, equally important, is acknowledgment and control of the many cognitive biases that can affect one’s decisions. q James Schmutte, DBA, CPA, is a professor, and James R. Duncan, PhD, CPA, is an associate professor, both in the department of accounting, Ball State University, Muncie, Ind. OCTOBER 2014 / THE CPA JOURNAL Copyright of CPA Journal is the property of New York State Society of CPAs and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.